Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 June 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00301

Social modulation of decision-making: a cross-species review

  • 1Department of Organismal Animal Physiology, Faculty of Science, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • 3Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Centre for Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, UMC St. Radboud, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Taking decisions plays a pivotal role in daily life and comprises a complex process of assessing and weighing short-term and long-term costs and benefits of competing actions. Decision-making has been shown to be affected by factors such as sex, age, genotype, and personality. Importantly, also the social environment affects decisions, both via social interactions (e.g., social learning, cooperation and competition) and social stress effects. Although everyone is aware of this social modulating role on daily life decisions, this has thus far only scarcely been investigated in human and animal studies. Furthermore, neuroscientific studies rarely discuss social influence on decision-making from a functional perspective such as done in behavioral ecology studies. Therefore, the first aim of this article is to review the available data of the influence of the social context on decision-making both from a causal and functional perspective, drawing on animal and human studies. Also, there is currently still a gap between decision-making in real life where influences of the social environment are extensive, and decision-making as measured in the laboratory, which is often done without any (deliberate) social influences. However, methods are being developed to bridge this gap. Therefore, the second aim of this review is to discuss these methods and ways in which this gap can be increasingly narrowed. We end this review by formulating future research questions.

Introduction

Decision-making plays a pivotal role in daily life and comprises a complex process of assessing and weighing short-term and long-term costs and benefits of competing actions. The output of the decision-making process, i.e., which action is to be taken, is determined by an interaction between impulsive or emotionally based systems, responding to immediate (potential) rewards and losses or threats, and reflective or cognitive control systems controlling long-term goals (Bechara, 2005; de Visser et al., 2011). Decision-making is influenced by many factors. However, whereas factors such as sex, age, genotype, and personality have been extensively investigated and discussed (reviews; Crone and van der Molen, 2004; Overman, 2004; Overman et al., 2004; de Visser et al., 2011; Homberg, 2012; van den Bos et al., 2013a), relatively little attention has been paid to the crucial moderating effect of social context on decision-making. This is all the more surprising given that decisions in real life are often strongly influenced by the social environment and involve direct and indirect social interactions.

The social environment may affect decision-making in different ways. For instance, decisions may directly involve social partners such as when deciding to share knowledge or goods with others or to provide support (review; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011). Furthermore, subjects may adjust their decisions depending on who is with them or who they consider as their reference-point at the time of the decision. For instance, in the case of so called “conformity behavior,” subjects change their behavior to match that of the rest of the group (Morgan and Laland, 2012). Finally, the social environment may influence decisions globally by “setting the atmosphere.” For instance, the social environment may breathe a tense or relaxed atmosphere, which influences the individual's emotional state and thereby its decisions (review; Starcke and Brand, 2012). While studies in the field of behavioral ecology have provided elaborate understanding of functional aspects of the social context of decision-making behavior, studies in the field of neuroscience have begun to provide information on the causal aspects and the neural substrate underlying decision-making behavior in a social context. Still, crosstalk between these fields rarely occurs. Researchers in both fields may benefit from insights from both domains that will enable progress toward a common understanding of the social modulating role on decision-making. Therefore, the first aim of our review is to discuss the influence of the social context on decision-making both from a causal and functional perspective, drawing on animal and human studies.

Currently, there is still a gap between decision-making in real life where influences of the social environment are extensive, and decision-making as measured in the laboratory, which is often done without any (deliberate) social influences. Subjects may for instance be less disturbed by stressful conditions when in company of friends or relatives with thereby little effect on their decisions in real life, while showing high levels of stress and concomitant effects on decision-making in the laboratory when tested singly. While these laboratory findings may be important for studying basic mechanisms of e.g., the effects of stress on decision-making (Preston et al., 2007; Lighthall et al., 2009; van den Bos et al., 2009), they hamper for instance assessing the value and general applicability of laboratory findings to the functioning of people, such as patients, in daily life. Furthermore, they miss out the important impact the social environment may normally play on individual and group decision-making. However, a major obstacle to assess the role of the social environment in decision-making under laboratory conditions in humans is that it is difficult to create ecologically valid conditions. Therefore, monitoring real life effects of the social environment on decision-making would be a significant step forward. In rodents, home-cage experimental set-ups, which allow for careful manipulation of brain-behavior relationships in social settings, have been developed as means of bridging precisely this gap. Therefore, a second aim of our review is to discuss these developments in methodology to address the question of the effect of the social environment on decision-making.

Given the foregoing, in the following sections we will discuss how the social environment may modulate decision-making and how this can be incorporated in experimental studies. In section Decision-making in a social context, we will discuss direct and indirect social influences on decision-making, while in section Social stress and decision-making the effects of social stress on decision-making are addressed. Where possible we link a causal and functional perspective and discuss underlying neural substrates. In section Laboratory studies and real-life studies we will (briefly) discuss ways to incorporate the social environment into studies of decision-making. We end this review (section Concluding remarks) with a brief summary of the main issues addressed and define (some) future questions.

Decision-Making in a Social Context

Humans are an exceptionally successful species, both in the number of individuals and in our flexibility to expand to the range of environments and situations in which we live. A major factor underlying this success boils down to our complex social life as we have the ability to acquire valuable knowledge and skills from others through social learning and teaching and build upon this generation after generation (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Laland et al., 2011). In our daily life we constantly make decisions based on our personal information and experience as well as that of others, i.e., social learning. Our behavior may be restricted through social conformity (Asch, 1956), or promoted or enhanced through facilitation (Zajonc, 1965). Furthermore, often the decisions of multiple individuals may result in collective behavior, such as the synchronization of applause (Néda et al., 2000), or have to be made jointly to reach a consensus (Conradt and Roper, 2005; Dyer et al., 2008). Living with others comes with the potential benefit of cooperation (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004a) as well as costs of competition when resources are limited (Davies et al., 2012). Finally, an individual's decisions may be indirectly influenced by the social environment, by affecting an individual's emotional state. Importantly, the modulating role of the social environment is strongly affected by an individual's characteristics and personality as well as that of its group mates (Webster and Ward, 2011).

To fully understand the role of social modulation on decision-making, it is important to consider it from both a causal and functional perspective (Tinbergen, 1963; see e.g., Morgan and Laland, 2012). In neuropsychology, functional explanations are rarely taken into account while this behavioral ecological perspective may help to understand how the behavior of individuals is adapted to the social environment in which they live (Davies et al., 2012). A growing list of behaviors once described as uniquely human have now been described in a range of animals, such as teaching (Franks and Richardson, 2006; Thornton and McAuliffe, 2006), culture (see Laland, 2008; Laland et al., 2011), and conformity (Whiten et al., 2005; Galef and Whiskin, 2008; Jolles et al., 2011), which provide us with new insights into our own behavior. Therefore, the next few sections are focused on a behavioral ecological perspective with links to relevant human and animal laboratory studies. However, as the human literature on social decision-making has been reviewed elsewhere, we limit ourselves to the most relevant human experimental studies (see e.g., Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004a,b; Lieberman, 2007; Frith and Singer, 2008; Behrens et al., 2009; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011).

Social Learning

For social species, like humans, the social environment plays a critical role in day-to-day decision-making, such as where to live, what to eat and with whom to mate, and may affect their emotional state (see section Observational fear learning). Decisions can be based on either personal experience and/or information gathered by others (Danchin et al., 2004) and through “social learning,” individuals may for example learn how (observational learning) to deal with a resource or where it is located (local enhancement; Thorpe, 1956; Webster and Laland, 2012). Although social learning may involve several different learning mechanisms (Laland, 2008) only some rely on advanced cognitive abilities (Galef, 1988; Heyes, 1994) and most cases appear to result from very simple processes (Galef, 1988). Indeed, although social learning may seem particular to humans, animals from a broad range of species gather and exploit information generated by others (review; Galef, 1988; Heyes, 1994; Heyes and Galef, 1996).

A considerable part of the social learning literature has been performed with rats (review; Galef and Giraldeau, 2001; Galef, 2007) and has shown that rats use information from others to learn where, what, how and even when to eat (Galef and Giraldeau, 2001). Both the social information provided by visual and olfactory cues from conspecifics provide a strong basis for individual foraging decisions. Just by observing conspecifics, rats quickly locate food and join to feed with them (see Galef and Giraldeau, 2001). This is further intensified by deposited olfactory cues on both the food and the location of the food (Galef, 2007), which may for example enable young rats to learn what foods are best to eat as they may not be able to figure this out by themselves (see Galef, 2007). In particular the olfactory cues via the breath of conspecifics may result in these socially induced food preferences that may overrule personal preferences (Galef and Whiskin, 2008; Jolles et al., 2011) and even reverse learned aversions to foods (Galef, 1986).

To accurately make decisions, individuals need to constantly weigh the costs and benefits of private and social information and need to be selective when and whom to copy (Galef, 1995; Laland, 2004). Social learning may be beneficial as it allows individuals to acquire relevant information without having the risk or costs associated with individual learning. However, social information may be outdated, for example when the environment is highly variable, or less valuable, when the environment is very stable (Boyd and Richerson, 1985). Thus, relative reliance on social and individual learning can be viewed as involving a trade-off between accuracy and cost (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Laland, 2004; Kendal et al., 2005). For example, Dally and colleagues (2008) showed that rooks selectively consumed the same food as a demonstrator when the foods were novel, but not when the foods were familiar. Likewise, Galef and Whiskin showed that the greater the discrepancy between private and social information, the less likely the subject is to behave in accord with the socially acquired information (Galef and Whiskin, 1998). Moreover, Brown and colleagues (2008) showed that personal and social information about spatial choices are combined in a rat's working memory and both the quality of the food available and the memory of a familiar conspecific's behavior affect an individual's tendency to visit spatial locations in a radial-arm maze.

The trade-off between accuracy and costs is nicely illustrated by the difference in public information use of two closely related species of sticklebacks. Coolen and colleagues (Coolen et al., 2003) showed that while nine-spined sticklebacks exploited public information and foraged at the areas they observed others to have better feeding rates, three-spined sticklebacks ignored this information and relied in their decisions on their own experience. This difference in social information use may be explained by the relative difference in costs of self-acquired information between the two stickleback species. The robust defenses that three-spined but not nine-spined sticklebacks have, such as large spines and armored body plates, allows them to sample alternative food patches directly in relatively better safety, as reflected by the increased time nine-spines spent hidden amongst vegetation (Laland, 2008).

When the presence of group mates affects the behavior of an individual, allowing or causing them to engage in certain behaviors at a different rate, or to perform behaviors that they would not perform at all if they were alone, this is called social facilitation (Zajonc, 1965). For example, in animals it has been shown that the presence of others may result in higher activity (Griffiths and Foster, 1998; Webster et al., 2007), increased foraging (Webster et al., 2007; Dally et al., 2008) and provide scrounging opportunities (review; Giraldeau and Caraco, 2000). For example, conform to human work, studies on rats have shown that the greater the number of models and the greater their uniformity in behavior, the more likely a naive subject will act in accord with the information that conspecifics provide (Galef and Whiskin, 1995). These changes in behavior can probably be ascribed to proximate mechanisms such as greater anti-predator benefits of larger groups (review; Krause and Ruxton, 2002), investment in vigilance and/or increased competition (review; Beauchamp, 2003). This is nicely illustrated by two studies in ravens (Stöwe et al., 2006a,b) which showed that when individuals were alone compared to in a group, they approached a novel object faster but spent less time close to and manipulating it. Although the social group enabled individuals to decrease time investment in vigilance, they may have a higher approach latency because individuals might wait for the other to take the risk and lead.

Conformity Behaviour

Social learning theory suggest that in most circumstances where natural selection favors reliance on social learning, conformity is favored and individuals, both humans and other animals, should adopt the behavior of the majority (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Laland, 2004). This particular form of social modulation on decision-making is especially important as it has been argued to be a major mechanism in human cultural evolution (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Efferson et al., 2008). One of the earliest described studies on human conformity was performed by Asch (1955, 1956). In a very influential paper, Asch (1955) described how adults would be willing to abandon their own perceptual judgment in a simple visual task and go with the overtly false alternative as a result of group normative behavior. Since then a huge number of studies has replicated these kinds of findings (see Bond and Smith, 1996; Morgan and Laland, 2012). Interestingly, the extent of conformity behavior seems to be strongly dependent on the situation. Namely, if a participant has to make a public response and is face-to-face with the majority, there is a strong normative influence of conformity, whereas it is weaker when participants make a private response and indirectly communicate with the majority (Bond, 2005). Furthermore, conformity behavior may be dependent on task difficulty and its importance (Baron, 1996), group size (Asch, 1955; Bond, 2005) and culture (Bond and Smith, 1996) among others.

Recently, several studies have addressed the neurobiological basis of conformity (see also Morgan and Laland, 2012). For instance, studies using mental rotation and auditory tasks (Berns et al., 2005, 2010) showed that social information may affect brain regions classically associated with perception as well as the processing areas associated with each task, suggestion that social information was affecting the subjects' perception as well as decision-making (see Morgan and Laland, 2012). Moreover, it has been shown that while cingulate areas are involved in monitoring the difference between private and public information (Klucharev et al., 2009), the ventral striatum is involved in the tendency to adjust one's behavior to the social information (Burke et al., 2010; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010), which may be related to rewarding aspects of being in line with the behavior of others (Klucharev et al., 2009; Burke et al., 2010; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010).

Conformity has been described in a wide range of animal species including fish (Laland and Williams, 1998; Day et al., 2001; Pike and Laland, 2010), rats (Galef and Whiskin, 2008; Jolles et al., 2011) and primates (Whiten et al., 2005; Dindo et al., 2009) (see Webster and Ward, 2011 for a review). For example, Laland and Williams (1997) showed that guppies preferentially chose a foraging route they had previously observed demonstrators use despite an equally valid available alternative. Individuals may base these kind of conformity decisions on heuristic rules of social attraction (Webster and Laland, 2012) such as to approach others (e.g., Laland and Williams, 1997), to approach larger over smaller groups (e.g., Lachlan et al., 1998; Day et al., 2001) and to approach groups that produce cues indicative of higher foraging success (e.g., Coolen et al., 2003, 2005). These tendencies are likely to benefit animals in most cases as it allows them to detect food without having to pay the costs of sampling the environment directly (see e.g., Pitcher et al., 1982; Day et al., 2001). However, sometimes this conforming to the behavior of others may come with “opportunity costs.” For example, individual fish may discover a visually isolated food patch faster and exploit it for longer than when a group of conspecifics is present (Webster and Laland, 2012), and smaller groups may discover a hidden food patch more quickly than larger ones (Day et al., 2001). The reliance on social information may sometimes even result in individuals to base their decisions on maladaptive information, such as rats consuming less palatable and sodium-deficient diets based on the breath of conspecifics (Galef, 1986), and even after the source of information is removed, such as guppies that kept on using energetically costly routes to food patches despite shorter alternatives available (Laland and Williams, 1998).

Although conformity of the basic “follow the majority” kind has been demonstrated in a variety of species of which Pike and Laland's (2010) study on public information use in sticklebacks provides compelling evidence, only a few animal studies (Whiten et al., 2005; Galef and Whiskin, 2008; Jolles et al., 2011) have investigated the situation where conformity overrides the discovery of valid alternative means (cf. Asch, 1955, 1956). In a two-action diffusion study in chimpanzees, Whiten and colleagues (2005) showed that although some individuals discovered an alternative technique to free trapped food items to the one seeded in their group, they later re-converged on the norm of their group, demonstrating conformity in the face of discovering a functional alternative. Two recent studies also suggest the existence of this type of conformity in rats (Galef and Whiskin, 2008; Jolles et al., 2011). Rats were given the opportunity to learn that two diets differed in palatability. They were subsequently exposed to a demonstrator that had eaten the less palatable food and were thereafter exposed to the same diets again. By simply being exposed to the odors in the breath of a conspecific for 30 min, individuals considerably decreased their preference for the more palatable food. Interestingly, despite similar initial preferences and similar social information, some rats were more resistant to changing their preference in relation to private and social information than others (Jolles et al., 2011), suggesting a different sensitivity to conflicting information (cf. Klucharev et al., 2009).

Collective Behaviour and Group Decision-Making

Both humans and many group living animals exhibit complex, coordinated, group patterns, such as lanes of traffic flow in human crowds (Helbing and Molnar, 1995) and the three-dimensional movements of fish shoals (Couzin and Krause, 2003). Through collective action, individuals can enhance their capacity to detect and respond to salient features of the environment, resulting in more accurate decision-making (Couzin, 2009) without the need of explicit signals or complex communication (Couzin et al., 2005; Dyer et al., 2008). The common property of these phenomena is self-organization, suggesting that much of complex group behavior may be coordinated by relatively simple interactions among the members of the group (review; Couzin and Krause, 2003). Indeed, recently studies have begun to reveal that collective decision-making mechanisms across animal species, from insects to birds and even humans, seem to share similar functional characteristics (Couzin and Krause, 2003; Conradt and Roper, 2005; Sumpter, 2006). For example, Helbing and colleagues (Helbing and Molnar, 1995; Helbing et al., 2000) have shown that simple rules such as “try to minimize travel time,” “avoid collisions” and “move in the direction of other people” may help explain pedestrian movements on busy streets and in life-threatening situations. Similar patterns have been described for non-human animals including the spectacular trails of ants on foraging trips (Couzin and Franks, 2003), the collective movements of starlings (Ballerini et al., 2008), and social interactions in shoaling fish (Herbert-Read et al., 2011).

In some cases group decisions are the result of a consensus reached by the individuals in the group (Conradt and Roper, 2005). Humans make these kinds of decisions all the time, from agreements in groups of a few people, to large-scale international conventions and political elections. However, also amongst non-human animals consensus decision-making is very common, such as travel routes in navigating birds use and the timing of activities (review; Conradt and Roper, 2003, 2005). In many situations conflicts may exist between the preferences of different individuals (Couzin et al., 2005). However, all individuals in the group have to decide on the same action because the group will fall apart unless a consensus is reached (Conradt and Roper, 2005), resulting in a loss of many of the advantages of group living (review; Krause and Ruxton, 2002). In line with theoretical predictions (Couzin et al., 2005), it has now been demonstrated that only a small proportion of knowledgeable individuals is needed to influence the direction of movement of the whole group, such as has been shown for nest site choice in social insect colonies (Franks et al., 2003; Seeley, 2003), the foraging movements in golden shiner fish (Reebs, 2000), and humans moving to a target without the use of verbal communications or obvious signaling (Dyer et al., 2008).

Cooperation and Competition

An important way to understand social decision-making in humans and other social animals is to look at it in terms of costs and benefits, not only to the actor as indicated above, but also to the recipient in the social context (Hamilton, 1964; West et al., 2007; Davies et al., 2012). For this it is important to keep in mind that via natural selection those genes are favored that increase an organism's ability to survive and reproduce (fitness). Therefore, individuals will often attempt to act in such a way as to receive immediate, selfish benefits, which may often result in competition or mutualistic cooperation. This is nicely illustrated by the Prisoner's Dilemma (PD; Axelrod and Hamilton, 1981) in which individuals can either cooperate or defect. Both individuals would benefit from mutual cooperation but both are also tempted to cheat, as it would be more rewarding to the individual. Therefore, irrespective of the other player's choice, it pays to defect. This raises the problem why cooperation is so common among human and animal societies (see West et al., 2007) and why individuals not act selfishly all the time and exploit the cooperative behavior of others (see Davies et al., 2012). In many cases, the cooperating individual simply acts selfishly and gains an immediate benefit, but thereby provides by-product benefits to its group mates, such as the benefits of an increased group size, i.e., reduced chance of predation, due to helping behavior in meerkats (Clutton-Brock, 2002). When on the other hand cooperation is altruistic—costly to the cooperator and beneficial to the recipient—cooperating individuals may still gain selfish benefits in the long term by using conditional strategies (Stevens and Hauser, 2004), such as cooperating only with relatives (kin selection; Hamilton, 1964), interacting only with those that have cooperated previously (reciprocity; Trivers, 1971; see Clutton-Brock, 2009), or under enforcement (Frank, 2003).

Individuals may help relatives as this may increase their genetic representation in future generations, and thus their fitness, as relatives share genes by common descent (see further Hamilton, 1964; West et al., 2007; Davies et al., 2012). If individuals preferentially help those that have helped them or those that help others, also known as reciprocity, the short-term cost of being cooperative is outweighed by the long-term benefit of receiving cooperation (Trivers, 1971). Although the PD has shown that when individuals meet only once it is better for individuals to defect than to cooperate, some form of cooperation may be stable if there is a chance both players will meet again because the long-term benefits of cooperation may outweigh the short-term benefit of defecting (Axelrod and Hamilton, 1981). Indeed, experimental work on both humans (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2003) and rodents (Rutte and Taborsky, 2007, 2008; Viana et al., 2010) has shown that individuals cooperate at higher levels in repeated interactions. For example, Rutte and Taborsky showed that rats that were trained to pull a stick in order to produce food for a partner pulled more often for an unknown partner after they were helped than if they had not received help before (generalized reciprocity; Rutte and Taborsky, 2007) and more often from a partner they received help from (direct reciprocity; Rutte and Taborsky, 2008). Furthermore, Schneeberger and colleagues (2012) showed that, similar to human PD studies, rats provided more food to cooperative partners than to defectors and that furthermore, this was dependent on costs: when rats experienced experimentally increased resistance to pull the stick of the apparatus and deliver food to the social partner, they reduced their help. It remains unclear, however, to what extent these behaviors may potentially be ascribed to simpler processes such as conditioned place preference. For example, rats have been shown to prefer a social partner over an empty space (Trezza et al., 2009) and to cooperate 80% of the time if they have the choice to act either alone or in cooperation with a social partner to obtain food pellets (Tsoory et al., 2012). Indeed, although reciprocity has attracted a huge amount of attention, it is thought to be generally unimportant outside humans (Hammerstein, 2003; Stevens and Hauser, 2004) as in most cases cooperation can be explained by more simple mechanisms such as by-product-benefits (Hammerstein, 2003; Clutton-Brock, 2009). Nevertheless, it shows that (lab) rodents may provide a good model system to investigate the mechanisms and development of cooperation (Łopuch and Popik, 2011).

Finally, enforcement or punishment may alter the benefit/cost ratio of helping and thereby favor cooperation (Frank, 2003). The consequences of punishment are nicely illustrated in cleaner fish. Cleaner fish remove parasites on the body of other species of fish that cannot remove the parasites themselves. Although, the cleaner fish prefer to eat parts of their clients' tissue they rarely perform this cheating behavior as their hosts may punish them by chasing them or by swimming away (Bshary and Grutter, 2002). What may be special about human cooperation is that we have the capacity to establish and enforce social norms (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2003, 2004a,b) because our societies are based on large-scale cooperation among genetically unrelated individuals (Henrich et al., 2003). For example, human research investigating the conditional cooperation on social norms has shown that subjects increase their contribution to the public good if the average contribution of the other group members increases (see Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004a). Moreover, third-party punishment experiments in which the PD is extended with a passive third party has shown that these individuals punish not-cooperating players despite a cost to themselves and that moreover, defection was punished much more severely if the other player cooperated than if they both defected (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004b).

When individuals act selfishly under situations of limited resources, competition may occur between individuals. Competing individuals have to weigh the competitive efforts against expected benefits as well as the intensity of the conflict. Individuals may compete by exploitation and/or by resource defense (Davies et al., 2012). Importantly, the best way for an individual to behave often depends on what its competitors are doing (review; Davies et al., 2012), which will therefore result in a stable outcome of competition, also known as the evolutionary stable strategy (EES; Maynard Smith and Price, 1973). Under ideal free distributions in which individuals are free to go where they want and have complete information about the availability of resources (Fretwell, 1972), individuals will distribute themselves in such a way that all individuals have the same rate of resource acquisition. For example, people queuing at the checkout area of the supermarket will often decide to choose the shorter and faster queues, ultimately resulting in all queues being of more or less equal length. However, in most cases individuals may not be free to go where they want as better competitors will occupy the richer habitats. This situation is very common in the natural world (see Davies et al., 2012). For example, although ducks have been shown to occur in stable distributions of individuals among foraging sites (Harper, 1982), some ducks were better competitors than others and grabbed most of the food (Harper, 1982). Importantly, defense of a resource has costs as well as benefits and individuals should only behave territorial when the benefits are greater than the costs. This may also help explain why often variable competitive behavior can be found within a population, such as producers and scroungers in a foraging context (Giraldeau and Caraco, 2000), as the costs and benefits may be different between individuals.

Insight in the neural mechanisms underlying cooperation and competition is increasing (see Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Huettel and Kranton, 2012). For example, a neuroimaging study of the Prisoner's Dilemma has shown that mutual cooperation led to increased activation in reward regions (Rilling et al., 2002), potentially explaining how cooperative social relationships may be sustained while inhibiting the impulse to act selfishly. Many social decision-making studies have used the Ultimatum Game in which two players split a sum of money, one player proposes a division, and the other can accept or reject this. For example, it has been shown that both unfair offers and their rejection elicited activity in brain areas related to emotion, such as the anterior insular cortex, suggesting an important role for emotions in social decision-making related to cooperation (Sanfey et al., 2003). Furthermore, alpha- and theta-oscillations in prefrontal areas have been found to be sensitive to social risk and to underlie fine-tuning regulation of social decisions (Billeke et al., 2012). A study investigating whether punishment of unfair offers might be affected by the relationship between the players has shown that when the proposer was a friend rather than an unknown person, unfair offers were much less frequently rejected. The anterior prefrontal cortex plays an important role in these kind of interpersonal economic interactions (Campanhã et al., 2011).

Rodent work has also provided interesting insights into the emotional and neurobiological bases of competition. For example, water-deprived rats in a pair competing for a single source of water quickly establish a firm relationship during which one rat drinks consistently more (the dominant rat) than the other (the submissive rat). However, interestingly, when the animals are exposed to severe stress, the dominants becomes less dominant, and when their submissive cagemates are administered anxiolytics, they increase their access to resources at the expense of that obtained by dominants (Joly and Sanger, 1991). One brain area in particular seems to play a central role in the cost-benefit decision making related to competition: the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). For example, the ACC is implicated in action selection and action outcome and effort monitoring, as well as signaling the use of social information (Rudebeck et al., 2006). Hillman and Bilkey (2012) provided rats with a choice whether to physically compete with a peer for a large food reward or not to compete and to obtain a small reward. It was found that ACC neurons electrophysiologically responded to competitive effort costs, assisting the rats in goal-directed decision making under social competitive conditions (Hillman and Bilkey, 2012).

Observational Fear Learning

Decision-making can be strongly influenced by the way the social environment affects an individual's emotional state. An important example of this is social learning of fear (reviewed by Olsson and Phelps, 2007). Learning about potentially harmful stimuli and events is important in shaping adaptive behavior, which may be less risky if learned socially through observation and social communication. Experimentally, social fear learning can be assessed by subjecting an observer to another individual who is undergoing cued threatening experiences, which may elicit physiological and behavioral responses in the observer as if undergoing the threat him/herself. Fear responses acquired through conditioning and observation of a distressed model were expressed to both seen and unseen (backwardly masked) conditioned stimuli, whereas, fear responses acquired through verbal communication were expressed only to seen conditioned stimuli (Olsson and Phelps, 2004). This indicates that the route of social information transmission affects how information is perceived. Also genotype affects social fear learning. Carriers of the low activity variant of the common serotonin transporter polymorphism displayed more cued fear responses compared to high activity variant carriers when subjected to an observational fear learning paradigm in which subjects had to view a movie in which models received shocks in the presence of a conditioned stimulus (Crişan et al., 2009). Furthermore, personality has been investigated as modulator of social fear learning using a paradigm in which participants watched mock panic attacks while emotional (e.g., fear and panic) and skin conductance levels were assessed. It was found that emotional avoidance and anxiety sensitivity were positively associated with more self-reported fear and more severe panic symptoms to the challenge procedure (Kelly and Forsyth, 2009). Similarly, Hooker et al. (2008) found that trait neuroticism enhanced social fear learning. Finally, there are sex differences in observational fear conditioning using modeled “mock” panic attacks as an unconditioned stimulus and an associated neutral cue as conditioned stimulus, as women reported more distress to the conditioned stimulus (Kelly and Forsyth, 2007). Mechanistically, social fear learning shares neural features with classical conditioned fear, including the involvement of the amygdala, but also requires higher-level reflective mental state attribution, like involvement by the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula (see Olsson and Phelps, 2007; Olsson et al., 2007; Olsson and Ochsner, 2008).

Next to humans, observational fear learning has been shown in a large number of species (see Olsson and Phelps, 2007) but most animal studies have been performed with rodents, showing that both visual, auditory as well as olfactory stimuli play an important role in social transfer of fear. For example, Jeon et al. (2010) demonstrated that mice observing demonstrators undergoing foot-shock stress displayed increased contextual conditioned freezing when subsequently placed in the observing chamber. This process was reduced, but not occluded, when an opaque partition was placed between the observer and demonstrator. In rodents in particular, olfactory cues play an important role, especially related to alarm pheromones. These may change autonomic activity and increase defensive and risk assessment behaviors (Kiyokawa et al., 2004, 2006) and are excreted in the rat's perianal region, especially by allogrooming, as seen during the social interaction between the demonstrator and observer rats (Knapska et al., 2010). Also, distress vocalizations affect fear learning. For example, when a conditioned stimulus was coupled to aversive 22 KHz ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), observers displayed conditioned freezing (Chen et al., 2009) and the number of 22 KHz-USVs emitted by a fearful demonstrator was positively associated with the conditioned freezing response displayed by the observer (Wöhr and Schwarting, 2008). In line with human studies, familiarity between the observer and demonstrator results in higher observational fear learning (Chen et al., 2009; Jeon et al., 2010). Interestingly, not only fear or distress itself can be socially transmitted amongst rats and mice, also the predictive value of the conditioned stimulus itself. Bruchey and colleagues (2010) demonstrated that observer rats acquire a freezing response by observing fear-conditioned demonstrators, i.e., being exposed to the conditioned stimulus in the absence of the foot-shock. Thus, the observers responded to the conditioned stimulus as if they had experienced foot-shocks themselves. Whereas fear can be socially transmitted by social interaction between a previously stressed demonstrator and a naive conspecific, it has also been demonstrated that observation of a non-fearful demonstrator mouse inhibited subsequent recall of a context-shock association in observers (Guzmán et al., 2009). Thus, it seems that previous experience with a fear-naive demonstrator ‘buffered’ fear conditioning in observers (Panksepp and Lahvis, 2011), providing strong evidence for socio-emotional influences on the behavioral response to threat.

Role of Individual Characteristics and Personality Differences

Although the mere presence of others may affect the decisions an individual makes, such as via facilitation and conformity, this modulating effect is strongly influenced both by the characteristics of the individual as well as that of its group mates, for instance by social status (Nicol and Pope, 1999), familiarity (see above; Swaney et al., 2001; Jeon et al., 2010), sex (see Choleris and Kavaliers, 1999; Piyapong et al., 2010) and social relationships between individuals in the group (e.g., Beauchamp, 2000; Schwab et al., 2008; Jolles et al., 2013b). Furthermore, consistently expressed behavioral differences between individuals that are otherwise similar to one another in terms of age, size and sex—also known as personality types or coping styles (Réale et al., 2007; Koolhaas et al., 2010)—may play a particular large role on individual decision-making (Webster and Ward, 2011). For example, bold compared to shy individuals have been found to be less responsive to changes in their social environment (Magnhagen and Bunnefeld, 2009) and their partner's behavior (Harcourt et al., 2009; Schuett and Dall, 2009), have a lower tendency to join and follow conspecifics (Ward et al., 2004), base their decisions less on social information (Kurvers et al., 2010) and display greater initiative in leadership (Harcourt et al., 2009). It is especially the interplay between these personality traits, individual characteristics and the relationships between individuals that affects an individual's decisions (e.g., van Oers et al., 2005; Schuett and Dall, 2009; Jolles et al., 2013b). Importantly, in this way individual characteristics and heterogeneity within groups may ultimately impact the dynamics of group decisions and behavior and affect the way in which the group as a whole functions in relation to the environment (Webster and Ward, 2011). For example, individual differences in risk-taking strongly affect social feedback between individuals (Harcourt et al., 2009), individuals may not be uniformly distributed within groups (Jolles et al., 2013a), and certain individuals may take leadership positions and thereby determine group decisions (King et al., 2009; Nagy et al., 2010). Surprisingly, few studies have considered the impact of individual characteristics and personality traits on the social modulation of decision-making. For example, although sex differences have been described in a wide range of cognitive and behavioral processes, investigations of sex differences in social learning are still largely neglected (review: Choleris and Kavaliers, 1999). Furthermore, despite the surge of interest in personality traits in animals, only in recent times have studies started to consider personality in the context of the crucial moderating effect of the social environment (review: Webster and Ward, 2011). Finally, both human and non-human studies as well as models on group behavior still seldom consider the impact of such heterogeneity on the rules underlying their coordination (but see e.g., Jolles et al., 2013a).

Social Stress and Decision-Making

The social environment in which humans and animals live is not devoid of psychosocial stress. Stressors may entail among others potential or actual conflicts with conspecifics either in the context of dominance-submission or in competition over (valuable) resources, the sheer performance of a task in front of conspecifics, and experiencing or witnessing aggression and violence. To assess the effects of social stressors on decision-making in the laboratory, tests are needed which produce reliable and reproducible stress-related effects. One such psychosocial test is the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) and its variants (e.g., group-wise TSST; Von Dawans et al., 2011).

The TSST has been shown to be very effective in inducing stress as measured by questionnaires regarding stress, mood and anxiety as well as parameters indicative of the activation of the two main stress axes, i.e., hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenocortical axis (HPA-axis; cortisol) and the sympatho-adrenomedullary axis (SAM-axis; (nor)adrenaline) (e.g., Kirschbaum et al., 1999; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2005; Nater et al., 2005, 2006; Starcke et al., 2008; Nater and Rohleder, 2009; van den Bos et al., 2009; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010; Cornelisse et al., 2011; Starcke et al., 2011; Maruyama et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013). This stress effect is related to the social-evaluative and uncontrollable elements of the task (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004): subjects have to deliver a speech as well as do a difficult arithmetic in front of a panel that judges their performance without much a priori knowledge of the procedure. Even anticipating delivery of the speech is already stressful.

The activation of the SAM-axis is often measured by salivary alpha-amylase, while activation of the HPA-axis is often measured by salivary cortisol (Kirschbaum et al., 1999; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2005; Nater et al., 2005, 2006, 2007; van Stegeren et al., 2006, 2008; Nater and Rohleder, 2009; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010; Thoma et al., 2012). While the SAM-axis is strongly activated during the TSST and returns to baseline immediately or quickly thereafter, HPA-axis activity peaks 10–20 min after the TSST and returns to baseline about 60 min thereafter (e.g., Nater et al., 2005, 2006; Cornelisse et al., 2011; Starcke et al., 2011; Maruyama et al., 2012; Thoma et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013). Cortisol levels in men are generally higher than in women, while in women the menstrual cycle and contraceptives in addition have a modulatory effect (Kirschbaum et al., 1999; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2005; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010; Nielsen et al., 2013; but see Kelly et al., 2008). Thus, the TSST seems to be a useful laboratory test to delineate the effects of psychosocial stress on decision-making, when decision-making tasks are delivered after the TSST. It should be noted that the Cold Pressor Test has been used as well to delineate the effects of stress on decision-making. As at first glance the results between this test and the TSST on decision-making were not different its effects on decision-making will be included in the following paragraphs.

Social Stress: Effects on Decision-Making Paradigms

Following the TSST (as well as the Cold Pressor Test) several reward-based decision-making tasks have been shown to be affected (review; Starcke and Brand, 2012), i.e., the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Preston et al., 2007; van den Bos et al., 2009), the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART; Lighthall et al., 2009), the Game of Dice Task (Starcke et al., 2008), delay-discounting (Lempert et al., 2012) and a financial decision-making task (Porcelli and Delgado, 2009). Social stress paradigms have not been tested in animals with respect to reward-based decision-making. However, the data of other types of stress paradigms reveal similar effects as in humans: stress disrupts reward-based decision-making tasks in rats (Graham et al., 2010; Shafiei et al., 2012).

Thus far, only a few studies have been published on the effects of the TSST on social decision-making related paradigms. Social stress had no effects on moral decision-making, although in the stress group it was shown that the higher the salivary cortisol levels the more egoistic, and thus less altruistic, decisions were taken in highly emotional dilemmas (Starcke et al., 2011). Furthermore, social stress induced by the TSST increased pro-social behavior as measured by the Trust Game (reciprocal exchange) and the Dictator Game (altruism) (Takahashi et al., 2007; Von Dawans et al., 2012). Still in the latter game this effect seemed to be dependent of whether money was donated to a person or to an anonymous charity organization as Vinkers and colleagues (2013) observed that people donated less money to an organization following the TSST. Finally, altruistic punishment behavior in the Ultimatum Game was not affected immediately following the TSST (Von Dawans et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013); however, it was affected when the task was administered 75 min after the TSST (Vinkers et al., 2013; see further below).

The overall impression from these studies is that differences are present in the consequences of social stress on paradigms that people play singly and those that involve interaction, even when virtually, with others. Von Dawans and colleagues (2012) suggest that this may be related to the workings of oxytocin, which would be released under stress and modulate the response in social decision-making in the direction of pro-social behavior and social support (see Taylor et al., 2000; Cousino Klein and Corwin, 2002; Heinrichs et al., 2003; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010; Vinkers et al., 2013; see further below). The latter would lower the stress-response (Heinrichs et al., 2003; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010). The data on the stress-related increase in pro-social behavior are in line with the observation that in primate species behaviors like reconciliation and consolation follow conflicts or social tension (e.g., Aureli et al., 1989; Koski et al., 2007; Fraser et al., 2008). These behaviors facilitate recovery from stress and counterbalance the negative consequences of social conflict on group-cohesion and may restore internal group-cohesion (Aureli et al., 1989; Fraser et al., 2008; but see Koski et al., 2007). For, maintaining internal cohesion is crucial as to maintain the benefits from group-living, which are related to increased possibilities to find and exploit food resources as well as lowering predation risk. Interestingly, oxycotin has been shown to promote in-group behavior and increase defensive aggression toward outsiders (De Dreu et al., 2010). To what extent this relates to the observation that altruism in the Dictator Game was enhanced following social stress depending upon whether it was in the context of persons or a charity organization (Takahashi et al., 2007; Von Dawans et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013) remains to be studied. These data thus provide a link between causal mechanisms and functional mechanisms of pro-social behavior following social stress. The biological meaning of the data on reward-based decision-making is discussed in section Timing, coping styles and daily life (coping-styles).

Social Stress: Sex Differences

Studies directed at dissecting sex differences showed that men displayed more risk-taking behavior following stress (IGT and BART), whilst women were more risk-aversive (BART) or became more task-focused (IGT). These studies also showed that sex differences were related to the levels of cortisol. The higher the levels of cortisol, the more risk-taking behavior was shown by men (IGT; van den Bos et al., 2009). Women, on the other hand displayed more risk-aversive or task-focused behavior with increasing levels of cortisol (BART; Lighthall et al., 2009; IGT; van den Bos et al., 2009). Data from the IGT also indicated that women became more risk-taking when levels were too high (van den Bos et al., 2009; see also Witbracht et al., 2012). Thus, overall these data indicate that stress has a different effect on reward-based decision-making in men and women with different underlying effects of cortisol. This was recently confirmed using the Cambridge Gambling Task and a job assessment procedure to induce stress: while salivary cortisol levels were positively correlated with risk-taking behavior in men, they were if anything weakly negatively correlated in women (van den Bos et al., 2013b). Interestingly, this study also revealed a different relationship between salivary alpha-amylase and risk-taking in men and women: while in women a positive relationship was found, a negative relationship existed in men (van den Bos et al., 2013b). These data underline that differences do exist between men and women regarding the relationship between stress, neuro-endocrine changes and decision-making (see also de Visser et al., 2010; van den Bos et al., 2013a).

Studies on social decision-making have been mainly done in male only populations (Takahashi et al., 2007; Von Dawans et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013) or do not mention potential sex-differences in the data set (Starcke et al., 2011), precluding therefore to discuss differences between men and women in this respect. Still, one study using the same social-decision-making tasks and stress protocol as applied in men, did not observe an effect of social stress on social-decision making in women (Koot, unpublished). None of the studies in men reported a relation with cortisol (Von Dawans et al., 2012; Vinkers et al., 2013). Furthermore, while one study reported a correlation between heart-rate and pro-social behavior (Von Dawans et al., 2012), other studies did not observe a correlation between salivary alpha-amylase and pro-social behavior (Takahashi et al., 2007; Vinkers et al., 2013).

The increase in risk-taking behavior in men in reward-related decision-making may be associated with a loss of top-down control of prefrontal over subcortical areas, such as mediated by the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Piazza and Le Moal, 1997; Arnsten, 1998, 2009; Erickson et al., 2003; Stark et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2007; Kern et al., 2008; Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009; Goldstein et al., 2010; Koot et al., 2011, 2013). Furthermore within the limbic system high levels of cortisol may shift the balance of the activity of the ventral striatum (reward-related behavior) and amygdala (punishment-related behavior) toward the ventral striatum (Piazza et al., 1993; Dellu et al., 1996; Piazza and Le Moal, 1997; Pruessner et al., 2004; Mather et al., 2010; Porcelli et al., 2012; see Wager et al., 2008). A recent study showed that increasing noradrenergic activity decreased amygdala activity and processing of fearful faces (Schwabe et al., 2013). Thus, it may be hypothesized that in men the prefrontal-subcortical balance is disrupted by acute stress. In line with this, it was recently observed that systemic injections of corticosterone in male rats in a rodent analogue of the Iowa Gambling Task (de Visser et al., 2011) disrupted decision-making performance, which was associated with changes in activity in prefrontal structures (Koot et al., 2011, 2013). Still it should be noted that such effects of corticosterone were not observed in other studies (Graham et al., 2010; Shafiei et al., 2012). However, as discussed by Koot et al. (2013) this may be due to the way these studies applied corticosterone and/or administered the task following corticosterone injections. In general, studies on stress in male rats have revealed that acute stress sensitizes the reward system (through corticosterone; e.g., Piazza and Le Moal, 1997). As mentioned above it has been suggested that stress induced release of oxytocin may have an effect on the way subjects engage into social decision-tasks. Currently no studies exist which have studied the interaction between stress, changes in neural structures and social decision-making tasks.

As to the underlying neural substrate in women it seems that top-down control may actually be increased under stress, related to levels of cortisol, with among others a lower striatal and a stronger amygdala activity (Stark et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2007; Mather et al., 2010; Porcelli et al., 2012). A recent study showed that increasing noradrenergic activity increased amygdala activity, decreased orbitofrontal activity (thereby decreasing top-down control) and increased processing of fearful faces (Schwabe et al., 2013). It has been suggested that the persistent activity in for instance the anterior cingulate cortex following a stressful experience in women may be associated with the development of depressive symptoms in women related to tendencies of ruminative thinking (Tamres et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2007). The menstrual cycle has a strong effect on the outcome of changes in neuronal activity (Goldstein et al., 2010; Ter Horst et al., 2013). Thus, at present changes in neural activity in women are less clear and straightforward than in men. However, by and large these changes in women seem compatible with a shift toward risk-aversive behavior. Like in men, currently studies in women are lacking which have looked at the interaction between stress, changes in brain structures and social decision-making tasks.

Timing, Coping Styles and Daily Life

For a full understanding of the effects of social stress on decision-making paradigms three issues need further discussion: (1) short-term versus long-term effects of stress (timing), (2) relationship between stress, coping styles and task performance, and (3) consequences for daily life.

While most studies have applied decision-making tasks directly following the TSST, the data of several studies suggest that stress, notably cortisol, may have time-dependent effects on the balance between prefrontal and subcortical functioning. These timing effects may be related to non-genomic, rapid, and delayed, genomic, corticosteroid actions. For instance, when targeting these two time-domains specifically by administering cortisol in human subjects either shortly or several hours before behavioral testing, working memory was found to be improved by slow compared to rapid corticosteroid actions, and this improved performance was linked to enhanced activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Henckens et al., 2011). These and other studies have led to the hypothesis that prefrontal cortical functioning is impaired by corticosteroids acting via rapid non-genomic pathways, but enhanced by slow corticosteroid actions (Joëls et al., 2012). Few studies have targeted these different time-windows thus far. In a recent study using the TSST and a social decision-making task it was shown that male subjects showed more acceptance of ambiguous offers when the task was administered 75 min after the TSST than when administered immediately thereafter, leading the authors to conclude that this may be due to enhanced cognitive control, although it should be mentioned that no direct relationship with cortisol levels was found (Vinkers et al., 2013). Accordingly, it may be suggested that the effect of psychosocial stress on decision-making may be different when tasks are administered immediately following a stressor or sometime thereafter. It is clear that this needs further study.

In men, it seems that high levels of cortisol following a stressor are related to risk-taking: high-cortisol responders show decreased IGT performance, while non/low-cortisol responders do not (van den Bos et al., 2009). However, these data seem to be in contrast with data on coping styles. Male subjects with a pro-active coping style are in general considered to be more risk-taking than male subjects with a reactive coping style (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Coppens et al., 2010). Subjects with a reactive coping style show a higher HPA-axis activity than subjects with a pro-active coping style, while subjects with a pro-active coping style show a higher SAM-axis activity than subjects with a reactive coping style (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Coppens et al., 2010). This apparent contradiction may be resolved when the stress response and the task are considered separately. Thus, the stress response in the TSST is directed to the challenge, i.e., the speech and arithmetical task in front of the panel. The physiological and neural changes would normally allow the organism to cope with this particular challenge. In case of the non/low-cortisol responders, which have a short-lasting SAM-axis activation during the TSST, this would be to take immediate action directed toward this particular stressor with an already learned response or routine (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Coppens et al., 2010). As the SAM-axis is strongly activated during the task with little effect thereafter, there is no after-effect when the IGT is administered. In case of the cortisol responders, which also have a short-lasting SAM-axis activation during the TSST and a long-lasting HPA-axis activation, the coping response is to “freeze” i.e., re-assess the situation by exploration and being sensitive to environmental cues (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Coppens et al., 2010). Given the observed effects of stress and cortisol on neural structures, their brain seems to be in a “exploratory mode,” i.e., a decreased prefrontal activity and an increased ventral striatal (dopaminergic) activity in which risk-taking, as part of exploratory behavior, is included (Fiorillo et al., 2003). This “exploratory mode” seems to be set in motion during the TSST and remains for some time thereafter. This “exploratory mode” may be followed by an enhanced level of cognitive control (Vinkers et al., 2013), likely to be mediated by increased activity in the prefrontal cortex (Henckens et al., 2011), which may serve to store newly obtained information and/or regain homeostasis and cognitive control. Thus, when the IGT is administered shortly after the TSST, the on-going coping response interferes with IGT task-performance. For, the IGT or other decision-making tasks require a delicate balance between cognitive control and reward/punishment-sensitivity or a prefrontal-subcortical balance (de Visser et al., 2011). As the brain of cortisol-responders is in a “exploratory/risk-taking mode” they are more sensitive to the immediately highly rewarding decks of cards, which is indeed the case as judged from their choice behavior (van den Bos et al., 2009). One may speculate that a different pattern emerges when the decision-making task in itself would be stressful. In this case, subjects with a pro-active coping style would probably take more risks than subjects with a reactive coping style as in this case the coping style is directly related to the task. It is clear that this needs further study. Data on rat behavior in resident-intruder paradigms in which pro-active copers show a fight-flight response and reactive copers a freezing response suggest such differences in risk-taking tendencies during interactions (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010).

Discussions on coping styles have nearly exclusively focused on differences in behavior in male subjects (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Coppens et al., 2010). While it has been suggested that male and female subjects in general differ in coping style, for instance fight-flight (pro-active) versus tend-and-befriend (reactive; Cousino Klein and Corwin, 2002) or problem-oriented (pro-active) versus emotion-oriented (reactive; see Tamres et al., 2002 for discussion), this is too limited a view as the differences in coping styles in male subjects already show. The current data rather suggest that a distinction in female subjects may also occur in coping styles, with in all likelihood “tend and befriend” as the most dominant one (Taylor et al., 2000; Cousino Klein and Corwin, 2002; Tamres et al., 2002) and likely to be a reactive coping style (Koolhaas et al., 2010). This coping style is related to the workings of oxytocin (Cousino Klein and Corwin, 2002), directed at caring for the young and/or seeking social support (Taylor et al., 2000; Tamres et al., 2002) and, possibly, leading to a reduction of HPA-axis activity (Cousino Klein and Corwin, 2002). Thus, the cortisol-mediated increase in risk-aversive behavior in reward-based tasks may be related to changes in neural structures toward safety and social support. Yet, it is clear that further studies are needed to substantiate this.

As risk-taking is in general considered to be a more disastrous mode of behavior than risk-aversiveness, the behavior of women following psychosocial stress may be considered as less detrimental than the behavior of men. However, this may not be entirely true. For instance, trying to rescue someone from a burning house with an objectively high risk of death or injury in the course of action is as disastrous as not-rescuing someone from a burning house with an objectively low risk of death or injury in the course of action. So it may be rather the context that gives one behavioral pattern an advantage over another or not than the attitude per se: today's hero may be tomorrow's fool and vice versa.

Laboratory Studies and Real-Life Studies

The laboratory environment offers the growing field of social neuroscience the opportunity to study general principles underlying brain-behavior relationships in a social context by using well-defined tasks tailored to be performed in and outside of scanners (review; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011). Similarly, the laboratory is well suited to study general effects of the social environment on individual behavior as for instance indicated in the previous section and as shown for instance by the effects of peers on risk-taking behavior (Gardner and Steinberg, 2005). However, at the same time it is often difficult, if not impossible, in laboratory settings to arrange the complete array of social contexts and/or social interactions. This becomes especially problematic when the behavior of the individual in relation to the social environment is the focus of attention, especially such as related to understanding the development of behavioral pathologies, intervention strategies and monitoring the success of therapies. For instance, in the case of the development of social conduct disorders probably not all social settings or social partners are equally likely to trigger a response, i.e., they may strongly differ between subjects and even across the life-span of subjects. As argued by others, time, i.e., when events relevant to the individual occur, and context, i.e., where and with whom events occur, are the limiting factors in laboratory studies (Johnson et al., 2009). Therefore, it would be ideal to study social interactions and their effect on the individual in real life as a complement to laboratory studies.

Among the most promising approaches to understanding time-limited behaviors in ecologically valid circumstances involves ambulatory monitoring through mobile technologies. This strategy is known alternately as Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) as well as the Experience Sampling Method (ESM), and it uses devices such as smart phones or other portable microcomputers to collect data at numerous intervals throughout the day. Like all methods, EMA/ESM also has its own limitations that include the necessity for all repeated assessments to remain brief as well as its reliance on subjective reports from the individual. However, extensive validation studies in diverse normal and psychiatric populations have demonstrated the feasibility and validity of this technique as a means of assessing psychological states and behavior in real time and in natural contexts (Granholm et al., 2008; Johnson et al., 2009; Husky et al., 2010). The major contribution of EMA/ESM is that it should provide a bridge between laboratory-based protocols with daily life behaviors that are otherwise inaccessible to the researcher. The value of EMA/ESM in investigating dynamic daily life risk factors has already been demonstrated relative to substance use behavior (Swendsen et al., 2000, 2011; Epstein et al., 2009), depressive cognitions (Swendsen, 1998), and many other “micro” processes of daily life.

Studying the dynamic character of social interactions including its long-term effects on the individual is standard in studies involving primates, either in the wild or in the laboratory. Still, it is clear that opportunities and possibilities for invasive neuroscience research are limited. In rodent studies, however, the opposite pattern seems to be present: while many experiments are directed at understanding the neural basis of for instance decision-making behavior in stand-alone tasks (de Visser et al., 2011), in general relatively little research is done in a long-term social context, an exception being for instance the work by Blanchard and Blanchard (1989), Blanchard et al. (1995, 2001, 2002). Recently home-cage tests have been developed allowing to address decision-making in a more naturalistic setting, i.e., to combine cognitive testing with a social/ecological-like environment (e.g., Automated group-cage (AGC), PhenoSys GmbH, Berlin, Germany; Intellicage (IC), Newbehavior AG, Zurich, Switzerland). In these home-cages, mice or rats are free to move and interact with each other but they can also voluntarily access operant modules situated inside or outside the home-cage. Using the Home Cage Panels (PRS Italia, Rome, Italy) Zoratto and colleagues (2013) developed a setting where animals were pair-housed, but could be tested singly. Furthermore, to assess social behavior in more detail programmes have been developed to analyse social interactions (De Chaumont et al., 2012). Thus, future studies in social neuroscience in rodents should develop protocols that combine new possibilities of studying the role of neural structures in behavior, such as by optogenetics, with well-defined and controlled home-cage social settings. This would allow inducing changes in behavior due to changes in neural structures within a social context in a controlled way.

Concluding Remarks

A large extent of the exceptional success of the human species is related to our complex social life. It is therefore important to properly understand in what way the social environment may modulate decision-making. In the foregoing sections we have highlighted research on this topic, both from human and animal-based neuropsychological studies as well as insights from a behavioral ecology perspective. It is the combination of these top-down and bottom-up approaches that may enable us to fully understand decision-making and the social factors that affect it.

Although humans are outperforming animals regarding social skills, rodents and other animals bear some fundamental aspects of these skills as well, indicating the important role of social influences on decision-making in evolution. These comparisons may therefore not only enable us to better understand our own behavior, they may help us understand the ways in which our behavior may be fundamentally similar or different from that of other animals. However, with a few exceptions—like the observational fear learning task and Prisoner's Dilemma game—these kinds of translations between human and rodent based neuropsychological studies and behavioral ecology studies have rarely been done.

Future studies on social modulation of decision-making can therefore benefit by making the links between both fields of research and taking both a top-down and bottom-up approach. Furthermore, this may enable us to go beyond general social modulating effects and allow to understand how individual characteristics and heterogeneity within groups affect decisions that individuals make and the way this may ultimately affect group functioning, such as can be seen in human society. For instance, future experiments should focus on further dissecting the interaction between social stress, gender and decision-making taking timing, the kind of decision-making task and coping style into account. While in rats social stress has been mainly studied in the context of coping styles and neuro-endocrine changes (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010) as well as long-term consequences related to depression and buffering effects of the social environment hereon (e.g., Blanchard et al., 1995, 2001, 2002; Von Frijtag et al., 2000), no studies exist which have explicitly looked at decision-making either related to food, social partners or otherwise. Clearly such studies are needed in parallel to human studies to unravel short- and long-term effects of social stress in male and female subjects.

As argued, studies are needed in humans and animals under real-life conditions to assess the impact of (stressful) events on subsequent decisions. For instance, Newman and colleagues (2007) showed that daily hassles affected the decision to eat in female subjects in real life. Such studies clearly help in understanding both similarities and discrepancies between findings in real life and laboratory findings and thus enhance the application of laboratory-acquired knowledge in real-life conditions.

For a successful cross-species approach it is mandatory to show that the same principles underlie changes in decision-making behavior regarding social interactions and stress. This is all the more important when focusing e.g., on sex differences (van den Bos et al., 2013a). Thus, this requires understanding social behavior and its underlying principles in the context of behavioral ecology as outlined under section Decision-making in a social context for instance. Furthermore, it requires to use paradigms which in a species-specific way tap-off similar phenomena and/or are matched as closely as possible (see de Visser et al., 2011; van den Bos et al., 2012, 2013a; Jimura et al., 2013).

As a final remark, we do hope that this review may serve as a fruitful starting point for extending current studies and discussions of decision-making by incorporating the social environment.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr. Leonie de Visser for fruitful discussions. Furthermore, we wish to thank the reviewers whose comments helped to improve the focus of a number of sections of this review. The contribution of Jolle W. Jolles was supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC).

References

Arnsten, A. F. T. (1998). The biology of being frazzled. Science 280, 1711–1712. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5370.1711

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. 10, 410–422.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Sci. Am. 193, 31–35.

Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I a minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychol. Monograms 70, 1–70.

Aureli, F., van Schaik, C. P., and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1989). Functional aspects of reconciliation among long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Am. J. Primatol. 19, 39–51. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350190105

CrossRef Full Text

Axelrod, R., and Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science 211, 1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ballerini, M., Cabibbo, N., Candelier, R., Cavagna, A., Cisbani, E., Giardina, I., et al. (2008). Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 1232–1237.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Baron, R. (1996). The Forgotten variable in conformity research: impact of task importance on social influence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 915–927.

Beauchamp, G. (2000). The effect of prior residence and pair bond on scrounging choices in flocks of zebra finches, Taenopygia guttata. Behav. Processes 52, 131–140.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Beauchamp, G. (2003). Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav. Processes 63, 111–121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1458–1463.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., and Rushworth, M. F. (2009). The computation of social behavior. Science 324, 1160–64. doi: 10.1126/science.1169694

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berns, G., Capra, C., Moore, S., and Noussair, C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. Neuroimage 49, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berns, G. S., Chappelow, J., Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G., Martin-Skurski, M. E., and Richards, J. (2005). Neurobiological correlates of social conformity and independence during mental rotation. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 245–253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Billeke, P., Zamorano, F., Cosmelli, D., and Aboitiz, F. (2012). Oscillatory brain activity correlates with risk perception and predicts social decisions. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs269. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanchard, D. C., Spencer, R. L., Weiss, S. M., Blanchard, R. J., McEwen, B., and Sakai, R. R. (1995). Visible burrow system as a model of chronic social stress, behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinoly 20, 117–134. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)E0045-B

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanchard, R. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. J. Comp. Psychol. 103, 70–82.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Blanchard, R. J., McKittrick, C. R., and Blanchard, D. C. (2001). Animal models of social stress: effects on behaviour and brain neurochemical systems. Physiol. Behav. 73, 261–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Blanchard, R. J., McKittrick, C. R., Hardy, M. P., and Blanchard, D. C. (2002). “Social stress affects hormones, brain and behavior,” in Hormones and Behavior, eds D. Pfaff, A. Arnold, A. Etgen, S. Fahrbach, R. Rubin (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 735–772.

Bond, R. (2005). Group size and conformity. Group Proces. Intergroup Relat. 8, 331–354. doi: 10.1177/1368430205056464

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bond, R., and Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: a meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychol. Bull. 119, 111–137.

Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

Brown, M. F., Knight-Green, M. B., Lorek, E. J., Packard, C., Shallcross, W. L., Wifall, T., et al. (2008). Social working memory: memory for another rat's spatial choices can increase or decrease choice tendencies. Learn. Behav. 36, 327–340.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bruchey, A. K., Jones, C. E., and Monfils, M. H. (2010). Fear conditioning by-proxy: social transmission of fear during memory retrieval. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 80–84.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bshary, R., and Grutter, A. S. (2002). Asymmetric cheating opportunities and partner control in a cleaner fish mutualism. Anim. Behav. 63, 547–555. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1937

CrossRef Full Text

Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Schultz, W., and Baddeley, M. (2010). Striatal BOLD response reflects the impact of herd information on financial decisions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:48. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campanhã, C., Minati, L., Fregni, F., and Boggio, P. S. (2011). Responding to unfair offers made by a friend: Neuroelectrical activity changes in the anterior medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 15569–15574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1253-11.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Bach, D. R., Roepstorff, A., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2010). How the opinion of others affects our valuation of objects. Curr. Biol. 20, 1165–1170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chen, Q., Panksepp, J. B., and Lahvis, G. P. (2009). Empathy is moderated by genetic background in mice. PLoS ONE 4:e4387. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004387

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Choleris, E., and Kavaliers, M. (1999). Social learning in animals: sex differences and neurobiological analysis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 64, 767–776. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00141-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2002). Breeding together: kin selection, reciprocity and mutualism in cooperative animal societies. Science 296, 69–72. doi: 10.1126/science.296.5565.69

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature 462, 51–57. doi: 10.1038/nature08366

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Conradt, L., and Roper, T. J. (2003). Group decision-making in animals. Nature 421, 155–158. doi: 10.1038/nature01294

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Conradt, L., and Roper, T. J. (2005). Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coolen, I., Van Bergen, Y., Day, R. L., and Laland, K. N. (2003). Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 2413–2419. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2525

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coolen, I., Ward, A. J. W., Hart, P. J. B., and Laland, K. N. (2005). Foraging nine-spined sticklebacks prefer to rely on public information over simpler social cues. Behav. Ecol. 16, 865–870.

Coppens, C. M., de Boer, S. F., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2010). Coping styles and behavioural flexibility: towards underlying mechanisms. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 365, 4021–4028. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0217

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cornelisse, S., van Stegeren, A. H., and Joëls, M. (2011). Implications of psychosocial stress on memory formation in a typical male versus female student sample. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 569–578. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cousino Klein, L., and Corwin, E. J. (2002). Seeing the unexpected: how sex differences in stress responses may provide a new perspective on the manifestations of psychiatric disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 4, 441–448. doi: 10.1007/s11920-002-0072-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Couzin, I. D. (2009). Collective cognition in animal groups. Trends Cogn. Sci 13, 36–43.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Couzin, I. D., and Franks, N. R. (2003). Self-organized lane formation and optimized traffic flow in army ants. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 139–146. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2210

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Couzin, I. D., and Krause, J. (2003). Self-organization and collective behavior in vertebrates. Adv. Stud. Behav. 32, 1–75.

Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., Franks, N. R., and Levin, S. A. (2005). Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516. doi: 10.1038/nature03236

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crişan, L. G., Pana, S., Vulturar, R., Heilman, R. M., Szekely, R., Druğa, B., et al. (2009). Genetic contributions of the serotonin transporter to social learning of fear and economic decision making. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 399–408.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Crone, E. A., and van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision mak- ing: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Neuropsychol. 25, 251–279.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dally, J. M., Clayton, N. S., and Emery, N. J. (2008). Social influences on foraging by rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Behaviour 145, 1101–1124. doi: 10.1163/156853908784474470

CrossRef Full Text

Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J., and Wagner, R. H. (2004). Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487–491. doi: 10.1126/science.1098254

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davies, N. B., Krebs, J. R., and West, S. A. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

Day, R. L., Macdonald, T., Brown, C., Laland, K. N., and Reader, S. M. (2001). Interactions between shoal size and conformity in guppy social foraging. Anim. Behav. 62, 917–925. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1820

CrossRef Full Text

De Chaumont, F., Coura, R. D.-S., Serreau, P., Cressant, A., Chabout, J., Granon, S., et al. (2012). Computerized video analysis of social interactions in mice. Nat. Methods 4, 410–417.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J. J., Shalvi, S., van Kleef, G. A., Baas, M., et al. (2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328, 1408–1411. doi: 10.1126/science.1189047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dellu, F., Piazza, P. V., Mayo, W., Le Moal, M., and Simon, H. (1996). Novelty-seeking in rats - biobehavioral characteristics and possible relationship with the sensation-seeking trait in man. Neuropsychobiology 34, 136–145. doi: 10.1159/000119305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Visser, L., Homberg, J. R., Mitsogiannis, M., Zeeb, F. D., Rivalan, M., Fitoussi, A., et al. (2011). Rodent versions of the Iowa gambling task: opportunities and challenges for the understanding of decision-making. Front. Neurosci. 5:109. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Visser, L., van der Knaap, L. J., van der Loo, A. J. A. E., van de Weerd, C. M. M., Ohl, F., and van den Bos, R. (2010). Trait anxiety affects decision-making differently in healthy men and women: towards gender-specific endophenotypes of anxiety. Neuropsychologia 48, 1598–1606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J. C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., Mesquita, A. R., Cerqueira, J. J., et al. (2009). Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325, 621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dickerson, S. S., and Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130, 355–391.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dindo, M., Whiten, A., and De Waal, F. B. M. (2009). In-group conformity sustains different foraging traditions in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). PLoS ONE 4:e7858. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007858

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dyer, J. R. G., Ioannou, C. C., Morrell, L. J., Croft, D. P., Couzin, I. D., Waters, D. A., et al. (2008). Consensus decision making in human crowds. Anim. Behav. 75, 461–470. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.010

CrossRef Full Text

Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., and Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003

CrossRef Full Text

Epstein, D. H., Willner-Reid, J., Vahabzadeh, M., Mezghanni, M., Lin, J. L., and Preston, K. L. (2009). Real-time electronic diary reports of cue exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin and use. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 88–94. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.509

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K., Drevets, W., and Schulkin, J. (2003). Glucocorticoid regulation of diverse cognitive functions in normal and pathological emotional states. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 233–246. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00033-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fehr, E., and Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature 425, 785–791. doi: 10.1038/nature02043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fehr, E., and Fischbacher, U. (2004a). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 185–190.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fehr, E., and Fischbacher, U. (2004b). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 63–87. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4

CrossRef Full Text

Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N., and Schultz, W. (2003). Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science 299, 1898–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1077349

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foley, P., and Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Human hypothalamus-pituitary- adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frank, S. A. (2003). Perspective: repression of competition and the evolution of cooperation. Evolution 57, 693–705.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Franks, N. R., Dornhaus, A., Fitzsimmons, J. P., and Stevens, M. (2003). Speed versus accuracy in collective decision making. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 2457–2463. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2527

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Franks, N. R., and Richardson, T. (2006). Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature 439, 153. doi: 10.1038/439153a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fraser, D. N., Stahl, D., and Aureli, F. (2008). Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 8557–8562. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804141105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fretwell, S. D. (1972). Populations in a Seasonal Environment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press

Frith, C. D., and Singer, T. (2008). The role of social cognition in decision making. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3875–3886.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Galef, B. (1995). Why behaviour patterns that animals learn socially are locally adaptive. Anim. Behav. 49, 1325–1334. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1995.0164

CrossRef Full Text

Galef, B. G. (1986). Social interaction modifies learned aversions, sodium appetite, and both palatability and handling-time induced dietary preference in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 432–439. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.100.4.432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galef, B. G. (1988). “Imitation in animals: history, definition and interpretation of data form the psychological laboratory,” in Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, eds T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef (Hillsdale: Erlbaum), 3–28.

Galef, B. G. (2007). “Social learning in rodents,” in Rodent Societies. eds P. W. Sherman and J. Wolff (Chicago: University of Chicago Press), 207–215.

Galef, B. G., and Giraldeau, L.-A. (2001). Social influences on foraging in vertebrates: causal mechanisms and adaptive functions. Anim. Behav. 61, 3–15. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1557

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galef, B. G., and Whiskin, E. E. (1995). Learning socially to eat more of one food than of another. J. Comp. Psychol. 109, 99–101. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.1.99

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galef, B. G., and Whiskin, E. E. (1998). Limits on social influence on food choices of Norway rats. Anim. Behav. 56, 1015–1020. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0867

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galef, B. G., and Whiskin, E. E. (2008). “Conformity” in Norway rats. Anim. Behav. 75, 2035–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gardner, M., and Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood, an experimental study. Dev. Psychol. 41, 625–635.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Giraldeau, L.-A., and Caraco, T. (2000). Social Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Goldstein, J. M., Jerram, M., Abbs, B., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Makris, N. (2010). Sex differences in stress response circuitry activation dependent on female hormonal cycle. J. Neurosci. 30, 431–438. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3021-09.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Graham, L. K., Yoon, T., and Kim, J. J. (2010). Stress impairs optimal behavior in a water foraging choice task in rats. Learn. Mem. 17, 1–4.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Granholm, E., Loh, C., and Swendsen, J. (2008). Feasibility and validity of computerized ecological momentary assessment in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 507–514.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Griffiths, R. A., and Foster, J. P. (1998). The effect of social interactions on tadpole activity and growth in the British anuran amphibians (Bufo bufo, B. calamita, and Rana temporaria). J. Zool. Soc/ London 245, 431–437.

Guzmán, Y. F., Tronson, N. C., Guedea, A., Huh, K. H., Gao, C., and Radulovic, J. (2009). Social modeling of conditioned fear in mice by non-fearful conspecifics. Behav. Brain Res. 201, 173–178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I and II. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–52.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hammerstein, P. (2003). Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press.

Harcourt, J. L., Ang, T. Z., Sweetman, G., Johnstone, R. A., and Manica, A. (2009). Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers. Curr. Biol. 19, 248–252.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Harper, D. G. C. (1982). Competitive foraging in mallards: ‘ideal free’ ducks. Anim. Behav. 30, 575–558. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80071-7

CrossRef Full Text

Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., and Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–1398.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Helbing, D., Farkas, I., and Vicsek, T. (2000). Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 407, 487–490. doi: 10.1038/35035023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helbing, D., and Molnar, P. (1995). Social force model for pedestrian dynamics. Phys. Rev. E 51, 4282–4286.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Henckens, M. J., van Wingen, G. A., Joëls, M., and Fernández, G. (2011). Time-dependent corticosteroid modulation of prefrontal working memory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 5801–5806.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Henrich, J., Bowles, S., Smith, E. A., Young, P., Boyd, R., Sigmund, K., et al. (2003). “The cultural and genetic evolution of human cooperation,” in Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation, ed P. Hammerstein (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 445–468.

Herbert-Read, J., Perna, A., Mann, R. P., Schaerf, T. M., Sumpter, D. J. T., and Ward, A. J. W. (2011). Inferring the rules of interaction of shoaling fish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 18726–18731.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol. Rev. 69, 207–231.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Heyes, C. M., and Galef, B. G. (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. London: Academic Press.

Hillman, K. L., and Bilkey, D. K. (2012). Neural encoding of competitive effort in the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1290–1297.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Homberg, J. R. (2012). Serotonin and decision making processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 218–336.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Miyakawa, A., Knight, R. T., and D'Esposito, M. (2008). The influence of personality on neural mechanisms of observational fear and reward learning. Neuropsychologia 46, 2709–2724. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huettel, S. A., and Kranton, R. E. (2012). Identity economics and the brain: uncovering the mechanisms of social conflict. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 680–691.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Husky, M. M., Gindre, C., Mazure, C. M., Brebant, C., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Sanacora, G., et al. (2010). Computerized ambulatory monitoring in mood disorders, Feasibility, compliance, and reactivity. Psychiatry Res. 178, 440–442. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeon, D., Kim, S., Chetana, M., Jo, D., Ruley, H. E., Lin, S. Y., et al. (2010). Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 482–488.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jimura, K., Chushak, M. S., and Braver, T. S. (2013). Impulsivity and self-control during intertemporal decision making linked to the neural dynamics of reward value representation. J. Neurosci. 33, 344–357. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0919-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joëls, M., Sarabdjitsingh, R. A., and Karst, H. (2012). Unraveling the time domains of corticosteroid hormone influences on brain activity: rapid, slow, and chronic modes. Pharmacol. Rev. 64, 901–938.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Johnson, E. I., Grondin, O., Barrault, M., Faytout, M., Helbig, S., Husky, M., et al. (2009). Computerized ambulatory monitoring in psychiatry, a multi-site collaborative study of acceptability, compliance, and reactivity. Int. J. Methodol. Psychiatr. Res. 18, 48–57.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jolles, J. W., de Visser, L., and van den Bos, R. (2011). Male Wistar rats show individual differences in an animal model of conformity. Anim. Cogn. 14, 769–773.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jolles, J. W., King, A., Manica, A., and Thornton, A. (2013a). Heterogeneous structure in mixed-species corvid flocks in flight. Anim. Behav. 85, 743–750.

Jolles, J. W., Ostojić, L., and Clayton, N. S. (2013b). Dominance, pair bonds and boldness determine social foraging tactics in rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Anim. Behav. 85, 1261–1269. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.013

CrossRef Full Text

Joly, D., and Sanger, D. J. (1991). Social competition in rats: a test sensitive to acutely administered anxiolytics. Behav. Pharmacol. 2, 205–213.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kelly, M. M., and Forsyth, J. P. (2007). Sex differences in response to an observational fear conditioning procedure. Behav. Ther. 38, 340–349.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kelly, M. M., and Forsyth, J. P. (2009). Associations between emotional avoidance, anxiety sensitivity, and reactions to an observational fear challenge procedure. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 331–338.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kelly, M. M., Tyrkaa, A. R., Anderson, G. M., Pricea, L. H., and Carpentera, L. L. (2008). Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 39, 87–98.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., Van Bergen, Y., and Laland, K. N. (2005). Trade-offs in the adaptive use of social and asocial Learning. Adv. Study Behav. 35, 333–379. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(05)35008-X

CrossRef Full Text

Kern, S., Oakes, T. R., Stone, C. K., McAuliff, E. M., Kirschbaum, C., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Glucose metabolic changes in the prefrontal cortex are associated with HPA axis response to a psychosocial stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 517–529. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

King, A. J., Johnson, D. D. P., and Van Vugt, M. (2009). The origins and evolution of leadership. Curr. Biol. 19, R911–R916. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., and Hellhamer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosom. Med. 61, 154–162.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., and Hellhamer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ - a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiyokawa, Y., Kikusui, T., Takeuchi, Y., and Mori, Y. (2004). Partner's stress status influences social buffering effects in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 798–804.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kiyokawa, Y., Shimozuru, M., Kikusui, T., Takeuchi, Y., and Mori, Y. (2006). Alarm pheromone increases defensive and risk assessment behaviors in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 87, 383–387.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Klucharev, V., Hytönen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A., and Fernández, G. (2009). Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron 61, 140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knapska, E., Mikosz, M., Werka, T., and Maren, S. (2010). Social modulation of learning in rats. Learn. Mem. 17, 35–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Koolhaas, J. M., De Boer, S. F., Coppens, C. M., and Buwalda, B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31, 307–321. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Koot, S., Baars, A., Hesseling, P., van den Bos, R., and Joëls, M. (2011). “Stress and decision-making in rats, effects of corticosterone,” in EBBS Conference Seville Spain September, (Seville) 9-12 2011, Abstract D12–D49.

Koot, S., Baars, A., Hesseling, P., van den Bos, R., and Joëls, M. (2013). Time-dependent effects of corticosterone in a rodent model of the Iowa Gambling Task. Neuropharmacology 70, 306–315. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.02.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koski, S. E., Koops, K., and Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Reconciliation, relationship quality, and postconflict anxiety: Testing the integrated hypothesis in captive chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 69, 1–15.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Krause, J., and Ruxton, G. D. (2002). Living in groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kudielka, B. M., and Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biol. Psychol. 69, 113–132.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kurvers, R. H. J. M., Van Oers, K., Nolet, B., Jonker, R. M., Van Wieren, S. E., Prins, H. H. T., et al. (2010). Personality predicts the use of social information. Ecol. Lett. 13, 829–837.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lachlan, R. F., Crooks, L., and Laland, K. N. (1998). Who follows whom. Shoaling preferences and social learning of foraging information in guppies. Anim. Behav. 56, 181–190. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0760

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Laland, K. N. (2008). Animal cultures. Curr. Biol. 18, R366–R370. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laland, K. N., and Williams, K. (1997). Shoaling generates social learning of foraging information in guppies. Anim. Behav. 53, 1161–1169. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0318

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laland, K. N., and Williams, K. (1998). Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy. Behav. Ecol. 9, 493–499. doi: 10.1093/beheco/9.5.493

CrossRef Full Text

Laland, K. N., Atton, N., and Webster, M. M. (2011). From fish to fashion: experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 958–968. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0328

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lempert, K. M., Porcelli, A. J., Delgado, M. R., and Tricomi, E. (2012). Individual differences in delay discounting under acute stress: the role of trait perceived stress. Front. Psychol. 3:251. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00251

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lighthall, N. R., Mather, M., and Gorlick, M. A. (2009). Acute stress increases sex differences in risk seeking in the balloon analogue risk task. PLoS ONE 4:e6002, 1–6. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Łopuch, S., and Popik, P. (2011). Cooperative behavior of laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus) in an instrumental task. J. Comp. Psychol. 125, 250–253. doi: 10.1037/a0021532

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Magnhagen, C., and Bunnefeld, N. (2009). Express your personality or go along with the group: what determines the behaviour of shoaling perch. Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 3369–3375. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0851

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maruyama, Y., Kawano, A., Okamoto, S., Ando, T., Ishitobi, Y., Tanaka, Y., et al. (2012). Differences in salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol responsiveness following exposure to electrical stimulation versus the Trier Social Stress Tests. PLoS ONE 7:e39375. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039375

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mather, M., Lighthall, N. R., Nga, L., and Gorlick, M. A. (2010). Sex differences in how stress affects brain activity during face viewing. Neuroreport 21, 933–937. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833ddd92

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maynard Smith, J., and Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of animal conflict. Nature 246, 15–18. doi: 10.1038/246015a0

CrossRef Full Text

Morgan, T. J. H., and Laland, K. N. (2012). The biological bases of conformity. Front. Neurosci 6:87. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nagy, M., Akos, Z., Biro, D., and Vicsek, T. (2010). Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature 464, 890–893. doi: 10.1038/nature08891

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nater, U. M., La Marca, R., Florin, L., Moses, A., Langhans, W., Koller, M. M., et al. (2006). Stress-induced changes in human salivary alpha-amylase activity - associations with adrenergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.05.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nater, U. M., and Rohleder, N. (2009). Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 486–496. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nater, U. M., Rohleder, N., Gaab, J., Berger, S., Jud, A., Kirschbaum, C., et al. (2005). Human salivary alpha-amylase reactivity in a psychosocial stress paradigm. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 55, 333–342. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.09.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nater, U. M., Rohleder, N., Schlotze, W., Ehlert, U., and Kirschbaum, C. (2007). Determinants of the diurnal course of salivary alpha-amylase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Néda, Z., Ravasz, E., Brechet, Y., Vicsek, T., and Barabási, A. L. (2000). The sound of many hands clapping. Nature 403, 849–850. doi: 10.1038/35002660

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newman, E., O'Connor, D. B., and Conner, M. (2007). Daily hassles and eating behaviour: the role of cortisol reactivity status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 125–132. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nicol, C. J., and Pope, S. J. (1999). The effects of demonstrator social status and prior foraging success on social learning in laying hens. Anim. Behav. 57, 163–171. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0920

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nielsen, S. E., Segal, S. K., Worden, I. V., Yim, I. S., and Cahill, L. (2013). Hormonal contraception use alters stress responses and emotional memory. Biol. Psychol. 92, 257–266.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Olsson, A., and Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The role of social cognition in emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 65–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 3–11. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olsson, A., and Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of "unseen" faces after Pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychol. Sci. 15, 822–828. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00762.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olsson, A., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1095–1102.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Overman, W. H. (2004). Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 55, 134–147. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00279-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Overman, W. H., Frassrand, K., Ansel, S., Trawalter, S., Bies, B., and Redmond, A. (2004). Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia 42, 1838–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Panksepp, J. B., and Lahvis, G. P. (2011). Rodent empathy and affective neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1864–1875. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piazza, P. V., Deroche, V., Deminiere, J.-M., Maccari, S., Le Moal, M., and Simon, H. (1993). Corticosterone in the range of stress-induced levels possesses reinforcing properties, Implications for sensation-seeking behaviors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 11738–11742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.24.11738

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piazza, P. V., and Le Moal, M. (1997). Glucocorticoids as a biological substrate of reward: physiological and pathophysiological implications. Brain Res. Rev. 25, 359–372. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00025-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pike, T. W., and Laland, K. N. (2010). Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks' foraging decisions. Biol. Lett. 6, 466–468. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pitcher, A. T. J., Magurran, A. E., Winfield, I. J., and Pitcher, T. J. (1982). Fish in larger shoals find food faster. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10, 149–151. doi: 10.1007/BF00300175

CrossRef Full Text

Piyapong, C., Krause, J., Chapman, B. B., Ramnarine, I. W., Louca, V., and Croft, D. P. (2010). Sex matters: a social context to boldness in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav. Ecol. 21, 3–8. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp142

CrossRef Full Text

Porcelli, A. J., and Delgado, M. R. (2009). Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychol. Sci. 20, 278–283.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., and Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Front. Neurosci. 6:157. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00157

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preston, S. D., Buchanan, T. W., Stansfield, R. B., and Bechara, A. (2007). Effects of anticipatory stress on decision making in a gambling task. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 257–263.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pruessner, J. C., Champagne, F., Meaney, M. J., and Dagher, A. (2004). Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: a Positron Emission Tomography study using [11C]-Raclopride. J. Neurosci. 24, 2825–2831.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., and Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 82, 291–318.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Reebs, S. G. (2000). Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal. Anim. Behav. 59, 403–409.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rilling, J. K., and Sanfey, A. G. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 23–48.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rilling, J., Gutman, D., Zeh, T., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G., and Kilts, C. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 35, 395–405.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rudebeck, P. H., Buckley, M. J., Walton, M. E., and Rushworth, M. F. (2006). A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science 313, 1310–1312.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rutte, C., and Taborsky, M. (2007). Generalized reciprocity in rats. PLoS Biol. 5:e196. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050196

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rutte, C., and Taborsky, M. (2008). The influence of social experience on cooperative behaviour of rats (Rattus norvegicus): direct vs generalised reciprocity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 499–505.

Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science 300, 1755–1758.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schneeberger, K., Dietz, M., and Taborsky, M. (2012). Reciprocal cooperation between unrelated rats depends on cost to donor and benefit to recipient. BMC Evol. Biol. 12:41. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-41

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schuett, W., and Dall, S. R. X. (2009). Sex differences, social context and personality in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim. Behav. 77, 1041–1050.

Schwab, C., Bugnyar, T., Schloegl, C., and Kotrschal, K. (2008). Enhanced social learning between siblings in common ravens, Corvus corax. Anim. Behav. 75, 501–508.

Schwabe, L., Hoeffken, O., Tegenthoff, M., and Wolf, O. T. (2013). Opposite effect of noradrenergic arousal on amygdala processing of fearful faces in men and women. Neuroimage 73, 1–7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Seeley, T. D. (2003). Consensus building during nest-site selection in honey bee swarms: the expiration of dissent. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53, 417–424.

Shafiei, N., Gray, M., Viau, V., and Floresco, S. B. (2012). Acute stress induces selective alterations in cost/benefit decision-making. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2194–2209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Starcke, K., and Brand, M. (2012). Decision making under stress: a selective review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1228–1248.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Starcke, K., Polzer, C., Wolf, O. T., and Brand, M. (2011). Does stress alter everyday moral decision-making. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 210–219.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Starcke, K., Wolf, O. T., Markowitsch, H. J., and Brand, M. (2008). Anticipatory stress influences decision making under explicit risk conditions. Behav. Neurosci. 122, 1352–1360.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Stark, R., Wolf, O. T., Tabbert, K., Kagerer, S., Zimmerman, M., Kirsch, P., et al. (2006). Influence of the stress hormone cortisol on fear conditioning in humans: evidence for sex differences in the response of the prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 32, 1290–1298.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Stevens, J. R., and Hauser, M. D. (2004). Why be nice. Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 60–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Stöwe, M., Bugnyar, T., Heinrich, B., and Kotrschal, K. (2006a). Effects of group size on approach to novel objects in ravens (Corvus corax). Ethology 112, 1079–1088.

Stöwe, M., Bugnyar, T., Loretto, M.-C., Schloegl, C., Range, F., and Kotrschal, K. (2006b). Novel object exploration in ravens (Corvus corax): effects of social relationships. Behav. Processes 73, 68–75.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sumpter, D. J. T. (2006). The principles of collective animal behaviour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 361, 5–22.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Swaney, W., Kendal, J., Capon, H., Brown, C., and Laland, K. N. (2001). Familiarity facilitates social learning of foraging behaviour in the guppy. Anim. Behav. 62, 591–598.

Swendsen, J. D. (1998). The helplessness-hopelessness theory and daily mood experience: an idiographic and cross-situational perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1398–1408.

Swendsen, J. D., Tennen, H., Carney, M. A., Affleck, G., Willard, A., and Hromi, A. (2000). Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-medication hypothesis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 198–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Swendsen, J., Ben-Zeev, D., and Granholm, E. (2011). Real-time ambulatory monitoring of substance use and symptom expression in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 202–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., and Hasegawa, T. (2007). Social evaluation- induced amylase elevation and economic decision-making in the dictator game in humans. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 28, 662–665.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tamres, L. K., Janicki, D., and Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 2–30.

Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A., and Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107, 411–429.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ter Horst, J. P., Kentrop, J., de Kloet, E. R., and Oitzl, M. S. (2013). Stress and estrous cycle affect strategy but not performance of female C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 241 92–95.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thoma, M. V., Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, J. M., and Rohleder, N. (2012). Acute stress responses in salivary alpha-amylase predict increase of plasma norepinephrine. Biol. Psychol. 91, 342–348.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thornton, A., and McAuliffe, K. (2006). Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313, 227–229.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thorpe, W. I. (1956). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London: Methuen.

Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie 20, 410–433.

Trezza, V., Damsteegt, R., and Vanderschuren, L. J. (2009). Conditioned place preference induced by social play behavior: parametrics, extinction, reinstatement and disruption by methylphenidate. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 19, 659–669.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–57.

Tsoory, M. M., Youdim, M. B., and Schuster, R. (2012). Social-cooperation differs from individual behavior in hypothalamic and striatal monoamine function: evidence from a laboratory rat model. Behav. Brain Res. 232, 252–263.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van den Bos, R., Harteveld, M., and Stoop, H. (2009). Stress and decision-making in humans, performance is related to cortisol-reactivity, albeit differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1449–1458.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van den Bos, R., Homberg, J., and de Visser, L. (2013a). A critical review of sex differences in decision-making tasks, focus on the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav. Brain Res. 238, 95–108.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van den Bos, R., Taris, R., Scheppink, B., Haan, de L., and Verster, J. C. (2013b). Salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase levels during an assessment procedure correlate differently with risk-taking measures in male and female police recruits. Abstract ENP 2013, Lunteren, 29–31 May 2013, Lunteren, The Netherlands. Available online at: http://enpmeeting.org/2013/programme.php

van den Bos, R., Jolles, J. W., van der Knaap, L., Baars, A., and de Visser, L. (2012). Male and female Wistar rats differ in decision-making performance in a rodent version of the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav. Brain Res. 234, 375–379.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van Oers, K., Klunder, M., and Drent, P. J. (2005). Context dependence of personalities: risk-taking behavior in a social and a non-social situation. Behav. Ecol. 16, 716–723.

van Stegeren, A., Rohleder, N., Everaerd, W., and Wolf, O. T. (2006). Salivary alpha amylase as marker for adrenergic activity during stress: effect of betablockade. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 137–141.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van Stegeren, A. H., Wolf, O. T., and Kindt, M. (2008). Salivary alpha amylase and cortisol responses to different stress tasks: impact of sex. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 69, 33–40.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Viana, D. S., Gordo, I., Sucena, E., and Molta, M. A. (2010). Cognitive and motivational requirements for the emergence of cooperation in rat social game. PLoS ONE 5:e8483. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008483

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vinkers, C. H., Zorn, J. V., Cornelisse, S., Koot, S., Houtepen, L. C., Olivier, B., et al. (2013). Time-dependent changes in altruistic punishment following stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology Avilable online at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.012. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., and Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychol. Sci. 23, 651–660. doi: 10.1177/0956797611431576

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., and Heinrichs, M. (2011). The Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G): A new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 514–522. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Von Frijtag, J. C., Reijmers, L. G., Van der Harst, J. E., Leus, I. E., van den Bos, R., and Spruijt, B. M. (2000). Defeat followed by individual housing results in long-term impaired reward- and cognition-related behaviours in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 117, 137–146.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., and Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: evidence for two independent prefrontal-subcortical pathways. Neuron 59, 1037–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006

CrossRef Full Text

Wang, J., Korczykowski, M., Rao, H., Fan, Y., Pluta, J., Gur, R. C., et al. (2007). Gender difference in neural response to psychological stress. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 227–239. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ward, A. J. W., Hart, P. J. B., and Krause, J. (2004). The effects of habitat- and diet-based cues on association preferences in three-spined sticklebacks. Behav. Ecol. 15, 925–929. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh097

CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. M., and Laland, K. N. (2012). Social information, conformity and the opportunity costs paid by foraging fish. Behavi. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 797–809. doi: 10.1007/s00265-012-1328-1

CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. M., and Ward, A. J. W. (2011). Personality and social context. Biol. Rev. 86, 759–773. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00169.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. M., Ward, A. J. W., and Hart, P. J. B. (2007). Boldness is influenced by social context in threespine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behaviour 144, 351–371. doi: 10.1163/156853907780425721

CrossRef Full Text

West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., and Gardner, A. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Curr. Biol. 17, R661–R672. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whiten, A., Horner, V., and De Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437, 737–740. doi: 10.1038/nature04047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witbracht, M. G., Laugero, K. D., Van Loan, M. D., Adams, S. H., and Keim, N. L. (2012). Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task is related to magnitude of weight loss and salivary cortisol in a diet-induced weight loss intervention in overweight women. Physiol. Behav. 106, 291–297.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wöhr, M., and Schwarting, R. K. (2008). Maternal care, isolation-induced infant ultrasonic calling, and their relations to adult anxiety-related behavior in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 122, 310–330.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science 149, 269–274. doi: 10.1126/science.149.3681.269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zoratto, F., Laviola, G., and Adriani, W. (2013). Gambling proneness in rats during the transition from adolescence to young adulthood: a home-cage method. Neuropharmacology 67, 444–454. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.11.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: decision-making, translational research, social environment, stress, psychological, humans, animals

Citation: van den Bos R, Jolles JW and Homberg JR (2013) Social modulation of decision-making: a cross-species review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:301. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00301

Received: 29 March 2013; Accepted: 05 June 2013;
Published online: 26 June 2013.

Edited by:

Agustin Ibanez, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Argentina

Reviewed by:

René San Martín, Duke University, USA
Koji Jimura, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Japan

Copyright © 2013 van den Bos, Jolles and Homberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Ruud van den Bos, Department of Organismal Animal Physiology, Radboud University Nijmegen, Heyendaalseweg 135, NL-6524 AJ Nijmegen, Netherlands e-mail: ruudvdbos@science.ru.nl

Download