Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 26 February 2014
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Programming HPA-axis by early life experience: Mechanisms of stress susceptibility and adaptation View all 14 articles

Mother–Pup Interactions: Rodents and Humans

  • 1Departamento de Fisiologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 2Departamento de Genética, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil

In order to survive after birth, mammalian infants need a caretaker, usually the mother. Several behavioral strategies have evolved to guarantee the transition from a period of intense caregiving to offspring independence. Here, we examine a selection of literature on the genetic, epigenetic, physiological, and behavioral factors relating to development and mother–infant interactions. We intend to show the utility of comparisons between rodent and human models for deepening knowledge regarding this key relationship. Particular attention is paid to the following factors: the distinct developmental stages of the mother–pup relationship as relating to behavior; examples of key genetic components of mammalian mother–infant interactions, specifically those coding for the hormones oxytocin and vasopressin; and the possible functions of gene imprinting in mediating interactions between genetics and environment in the mother–infant relationship. As early mother–infant attachment seems to establish the basic parameters for later social interactions, ongoing investigations in this area are essential. We propose the importance of interdisciplinary collaboration in order to better understand the network of genes, gene regulation, neuropeptide action, physiological processes, and feedback loops essential to understand the complex behaviors of mother–infant interaction.

Introduction

A social interaction is a dynamic process, composed not of a single set of behaviors at a given moment, but rather of the relationship of a series of behaviors over time. The exchange of information between subjects progressively modifies them. Due to its fundamental role in development, the effects of mother–infant interaction last throughout life.

In this review, we examine research on behavioral strategies that have evolved to guarantee the transition from intense caregiving to offspring independence. Infants display preference for social stimuli, suggesting evolutionary pressure in favor of these stimuli. In humans, the mechanisms involved in mother–infant attachment are still insufficiently understood. Nevertheless, a few areas of research are offering promising insight into the determination of this complex interaction. First, we examine studies that demonstrate olfactory stimuli from the mother to be crucial for infant latching and suckling, a determining factor in mother–infant attachment, feeding, and infant survival (1). Next we offer case studies on the genetic basis of two neurotransmitters, oxytocin (OXT) and vasopressin, which facilitate social recognition by acting on several brain regions, such as the olfactory bulb. In placental mammalians, both OXT and vasopressin are highly involved in key social interactions, including social recognition, pair bonding, and parental behavior. Finally, we consider the possible role of imprinting of candidate genes in determining features of the mother–infant interaction. These fundamental early-life physiological, genetic, and epigenetic processes suggest why, in many ways, mother–infant attachment establishes basic parameters for later social interactions.

The Actors: Mother and Infant

In rodents and humans, mother–pup interaction involves two essential types of actors: the mother and the infant, or several infants at the same time. Many other elements may be involved and/or affect this interaction: the father, the environment, siblings, and predators (2). However, here we focus on mother and infant. This particular relationship involves two organisms in different developmental stages. In this sense, it is an asymmetric relationship.

Moreover, this interaction occurs over time and is constantly adjusted, especially due to the fact that one organism – the infant – changes dramatically over the course of the relationship. Motherhood also involves behavioral changes, such as long-term memory of mothering mediated by OXT (3) and selectively reduced stress responses (4, 5) and anxiety (6).

Animal models have been developed to analyze the effects of maternal deprivation on the development of offspring (1, 7). Due to the complexity of the neuroendocrine and behavioral peculiarities of this relationship (8), the alteration of one element can induce changes of several orders, many of them not directly related to the original disturbance. Two changing organisms interact in a complex and balanced dynamic (9). Attempts to replace a missing mother or to repair an affection poor childhood (10) are rather difficult. This difficulty probably depends on the fact that there are at least two organisms that are necessarily changed by the interaction.

A mother–infant interaction is unique and depends on many factors (11). For instance, even in large litters, as in rats, mothers show different maternal behaviors toward male and female offspring (12). There are several elements that should be taken into consideration: the genetic background of the mother and the infant; the past experiences of the mother and the prenatal environment of the infant; the neuroendocrine profile of mother and infant; the stress copying style of the mother; and the environment (social and resource availability), among others. To consider such a wide variety of factors is a challenging task. As such, it is essential for the neuroscientist to collaborate with geneticists and physiologists in order to understand the origins of relevant psychopathologies (13).

Time: Lifecycles and the Development of the Mother–Infant Interaction

The mother–infant interaction depends upon several elements to establish attachment: the product of genes such as hormones and/or neurotransmitters (e.g., OXT, vasopressin, serotonin, noradrenaline), cytokines (14), opioids (15, 16), and dopamine (17), as well as epigenetic (18) and environmental factors (19). In mammals, the uterus is the first site of mother–infant interaction (20, 21). In both rodents and humans, pregnant females undergo physiological and behavioral changes to support gestation (22); and several studies have shown that the infant responds to the mother from within the uterus (23, 24). During the neonatal period, mother–infant attachment develops and filial bonds form (25, 26). This period seems crucial for behavioral development and the establishment of the infant’s stress coping style (27). The mother’s behavior conveys important information about the environment in which the newborn will live (28), through sensory stimulation of pups by the mother (29). Additionally, the mother–infant interaction involves the behavior of the mother in synchrony with that of the infant (30). Predictability of mother’s behavior and thus a secure attachment is crucial for the neurocognitive development of offspring (31).

Consistent demonstrations in rats have shown that mother–infant interaction is based on infant learning processes, expressed by increased CREB phosphorylation (19) and the development of a memory of the mother (4). This is a simple mechanism involving the noradrenergic activity of the locus coeruleus on the olfactory bulb, where the memory of the mother begins (32). Two aspects seem important: the physiology of the locus coeruleus, whose activity is less inhibited by its noradrenergic inhibitory autoreceptors during infancy than later in life; and plastic changes induced by the learning process. Environmental stimuli (odor and varying forms of body contact with the mother) act on the neural system of the infant, which is particularly receptive and prepared to react to those stimuli; the system has a very low threshold during this specific period. All of these interactions exert long-lasting impact on the development of the infant.

Origins of the Mother–Infant Interaction: Nature versus Nurture

The innateness of behaviors is controversial. Some authors suggest that behaviors presented by an individual early in life are essentially based on the genome and are innate (2, 33, 34). The behaviors would be already “wired” and triggered by specific stimuli, during sensitive period (35). However, consistent studies have shown that mother–infant interaction involves learning mechanisms (4, 11, 36). Physical contact is crucial to establish bonding. In rats, the mother’s licking induces olfactory memory that would be the basic mechanism of the mother–infant interaction. This interaction is by its nature dynamic and induces permanent reorganizations of the neuroendocrine systems and behaviors of both mother and offspring. Moreover, the system is open to the environment and can be affected by environmental stimuli in several ways, inducing short-term and/or long-term changes (37).

For the mother–infant interaction, it seems plasticity is not exclusively related to specific learning periods, as in other functions. For comparison, we might look at the classic neurophysiological example of ocular dominance columns in the visual system (38). These columns exist in rudimentary form at birth, but need to be stimulated by specific stimuli during a set period to fully develop their function. In the case of the mother–infant interaction, we would predict that plasticity is not time-limited as in the development of ocular dominance columns, but rather is cumulative. The constant learning processes that occur over time in the mother–infant relationship provoke related plastic changes. Thus the development of this interaction and the consequent attachment would not be an all-or-nothing phenomenon. Nevertheless, there are periods of greater impact, such as birth and the initial contact directly following birth.

In order to conceptualize the origins of the mother–infant interaction, let us consider an analogy between that interaction and the rhythmic activity of locomotion. Although these two behaviors are very different in complexity, it is interesting to imagine a similar basic working model. In this model, for locomotion, the output of a hierarchical neural system is the result of the activity of several circuits working in coordination. This system could be triggered by specific environmental stimuli. The generation and fine control of locomotion involves a basic central pattern generator in the spinal cord modulated by several other hierarchical disposed structures (39). In the case of the mother–infant interaction, this model would imply that several “layers” of interconnected neuroendocrine structures are the basis for behavior. The central pattern generator would first need to be properly triggered and then modulated by the interaction that it generates. Feedback is essential in social behaviors; the interaction generated by a behavior can be modulated by the response of the partner. However, in its very early stage, a supposed innate “central pattern generator” would require feed-forward action. Based on this model of the development of the mother–infant interaction, the functional characteristics of the infant locus coeruleus during the period after birth would be an innate system, prepared to be stimulated by the mother.

Animals are born with the capacity to develop complex behaviors (40), but not with behaviors per se: an infant must build them. The constructive nature of this process is analogous to those of other systems; for instance, in cognitive functions, an animal builds internally a unique sensation of a particular stimulus.

Role of Genes

It is well known that behavioral traits such as parent–infant bonding are the product of a sophisticated combination of genetic, environmental, and epigenetic factors. The adequate interplay of these elements, both over the course of the evolutionary trajectory of a species, and during the development and life of an individual, ensures a behavioral repertoire sufficient for survival and reproductive success. Identifying the contribution of genetic factors is the starting point for understanding the formation and expression of a particular phenotype. Although there is still much to learn regarding the genes implicated in specific mammalian behavioral phenotypes, a number of them have been characterized. This is the case of the genes for the OXT and arginine vasopressin (AVP) nonapeptides, which act as hormones and neurotransmitters.

It has been proposed that OXT and AVP-like genes originated due to a tandem duplication of an ancestral gene ~500 million years. This event would explain the presence of at least one homolog in jawed vertebrates (41), although many aspects of the origin and evolution of these genes remain unknown (42). In humans, OXT and AVP are located on the same chromosome, at 20p13, but with opposite orientation (43).

In placental mammalians, OXT and AVP nonapeptides differ by two amino acids at positions 3 and 8 (OXT: Cys–Tyr–Ile–Gln–Asn–Cys–Pro–Leu–Gly, and AVP: Cys–Tyr–Phe–Gln–Asn–Cys–Pro–Arg–Gly) and play an important role during lactation, uterine contractions, offspring care, stress-response, and other reproductive and social behavioral traits (41, 44, 45).

Although they boast a wide spectrum of activity, OXT and AVP are best known for their contribution to the regulation of social behaviors. Pedersen and Prange (46) were pioneers in showing that artificial administration of OXT promoted maternal nurturing in virgin and steroid-primed female rats, indicating that this hormone plays a specific role in the mother–pup interaction. Other authors replicated and extended this initial study in several other mammalian species, including humans [(45) and references therein]. For example, Bosch (47) showed that the OXT and AVP systems are key factors regulating maternal aggression and anxiety in rodents, while Apter-Levi et al. (48) found that both OXT and AVP are associated with human maternal and paternal behaviors in distinct, sometimes overlapping, ways.

Both nonapeptides are highly conserved in placental mammalians, with a few rare exceptions. For example, Lee et al. (49) showed that five New World monkey species had fixed a T > C mutation changing leucine to proline at position 8 of OXT. Other authors described an AVP mutation leading to a substitution of arginine for lysine at position 8 in opossums and pigs, but the phenotypic consequences of these changes remain unknown (42, 50). It is noteworthy that changes at amino acid chain position 8 characterize some non-placental AVP/OXT-like nonapeptides such as mesotocin in marsupials (51, 52).

The oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and the three AVP receptors (A2, V1a, and V1b) are encoded by the OXTR, AVPA2, AVPR1a, and AVPR1b genes located on human chromosomes 3, X, 12, and 1, respectively. These receptors, which use G-proteins as transducer signals across the cell membranes, have seven transmembrane domains, four extracellular, and four intracellular domains. Both OXT and AVP can bind to each one of the four above-indicated receptors, but not with the same affinity, since the most important binding sites differ slightly among the four receptors (53, 54).

Oxytocin and AVP are produced in greatest quantity in the hypothalamus, but their activity outside of the brain depends on their interaction with the receptors produced in various organs and tissues. For instance, the presence of OXTR in the uterus and mammary glands guarantee uterine contraction and milk ejection (42, 55). Hammock and Levitt (56), on the other hand, showed that OXTR is also found in several tissues of the mouse embryo, such as the adrenal glands, brown adipose tissue, and the oronasal cavity.

In contrast to the observed OXT and AVP gene conservation, hundreds of mutations in the OXTR, AVPR2, AVPR1a, and AVPR1b genes have been reported in placental mammalians. For instance, in pigs, AVPR1a and AVPR1b mutations have been associated with stress and aggressive behavior through their connection with ACTH, an important component of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (57, 58). In humans, polymorphisms in these receptor genes have also been linked to attachment, generosity, and pair bonding behaviors (59).

These examples illustrate that variations in candidate genes can be related to some intra and inter-specific mammalian behaviors. As seen above, however, other non-genetic factors have important roles in determining behavioral phenotype; this may be the case for the differential care of offspring between father and mother. Although males and females can have the same alleles in genes related to pup-care behaviors, only ~10% of mammalian species show significant male parental care, the majority of which are primates, carnivores, and rodents (60). This suggests that some social behavior candidate genes, located on autosomal chromosomes, can be subject to the imprinting process, an epigenetic phenomenon where changes in gene function can be heritable despite the fact that no alteration in the DNA sequence is detected. If genetic variation in key peptides and their receptors is understood as a first step, the imprinting of genes can be understood as a second step in determining the features of the mother–infant interaction. Below, we will see some examples of imprinted genes.

Imprinted Genes

The term “imprinted genes” refers to genes in which either the maternal or paternal copy is exclusively expressed. Previous study (61) presented evidence that the imprinted gene PEG3 is involved in sexual behavior and it would also be related to maternal care. More recently, Garfield et al. (62) showed that the imprinted gene GRB10 influences, in mice, distinct physiological processes, fetal growth, and adult social behavior, due to actions of the two parental alleles in different tissues. An evolutionary explanation for this parent-of-origin-dependent gene expression is the “conflict hypothesis.” A good example of evidence for this hypothesis, which will also help illustrate the meaning of imprinted genes, is the case of insulin-like growth factor (IGF2) and its receptor (IGF2R). As it is an imprinted gene, organisms that predominantly express the paternal allele of IGF2 show high levels of offspring resource extraction from the mother. However, if selection favors high expression of the maternally inherited allele of IGF2R, the offspring would show low levels of resource extraction from the mother (63). Thus, the balance of these imprinted genes would define a very initial and essential relationship pattern between mother and offspring interaction regarding nurturing.

Nevertheless, the likelihood of epigenetic influence on imprinted genes raises the question of the importance of environmental intervention in determining behavior. In rats and humans, several studies have shown that changes in the expression of candidate genes due to epigenetic mechanisms are connected with early parental care. Furthermore, McGowan et al. (64) suggested that the epigenetic response to maternal care in rats does not involve a single candidate gene, but rather includes changes in the expression of hundreds of additional genes. It is possible to speculate that the same happens with other placental mammalians, including humans.

In conclusion, early-life interactions clearly mold the neural basis for social behaviors (65), and sociability is perhaps the most important natural evolutionary force for mammalian species (66). In this context, it can be assumed that interactions with the mother are essential, not only in quantity, but perhaps most importantly in quality. A crucial question that emerges, among many, is whether it is possible to predict the consequences of the absence of a mother for her offspring, since several studies have indicated a genetic basis for animal behavior.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Raineki C, Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Developing a neurobehavioral animal model of infant attachment to an abusive caregiver. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67(12):1137–45. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

2. Mogi K, Nagasawa M, Kikusui T. Developmental consequences and biological significance of mother–infant bonding. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35:1232–41. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.08.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

3. Lopatina O, Inzhutova A, Pichugina YA, Okamoto H, Salmina AB, Higashida H. Reproductive experience affects parental retrieval behaviour associated with increased plasma oxytocin levels in wild-type and CD38-knockout mice. J Neuroendocrinol (2011) 23:1125–33. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02136.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Sullivan RM, Holman PJ. Transitions in sensitive period attachment learning in infancy: the role of corticosterone. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34(6):835–44. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Windle RJ, Wood SA, Kershaw YM, Lightman SL, Ingram CD. Adaptive changes in basal and stress-induced HPA activity in lactating and post-lactating female rats. Endocrinology (2013) 154(2):749–61. doi:10.1210/en.2012-1779

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

6. Lonstein JL. Reduced anxiety in postpartum rats requires recent physical interactions with pups, but is independent of suckling and peripheral sources of hormones. Horm Behav (2005) 47:241–255. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

7. Carlyle BC, Duque A, Kitchen RR, Bordner KA, Coman D, Doolittle E, et al. Maternal separation with early weaning: a rodent model providing novel insights into neglect associated developmental deficits. Dev Psychopathol (2012) 24(4):1401–16. doi:10.1017/S095457941200079X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

8. Moriceau S, Raineki C, Holman JD, Holman JG, Sullivan RM. Enduring neurobehavioral effects of early life trauma mediated through learning and corticosterone suppression. Front Behav Neurosci (2009) 3:22. doi:10.3389/neuro.08.022.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Walker CD, Deschamps S, Proulx K, Tu M, Salzman C, Woodside B, et al. Mother to infant or infant to mother? Reciprocal regulation of responsiveness to stress in rodents and the implications for humans. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2004) 29(5):364–82.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

10. Loman MM, Gunnar MR. Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34(6):867–76. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

11. Barrett J, Fleming AS. All mothers are not created equal: neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2011) 52(4):368–97. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

12. Oomen CA, Girardi CE, Cahyadi R, Verbeek EC, Krugers H, Joels M, et al. Opposite effects of early maternal deprivation on neurogenesis in male versus female rats. PLoS One (2009) 4(1):e3675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003675

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. Jaffee SR, Price TS. The implications of genotype-environment correlation for establishing causal processes in psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol (2012) 24(4):1253–1264. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000685

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. Bilbo SD, Schwarz JM. The immune system and developmental programming of brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol (2012) 33:267–86. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2012.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Resendez SL, Kuhnmuench M, Krzywosinski T, Aragona BJ. Kappa-opioid receptors within the nucleus accumbens shell mediate pair bond maintenance. J Neurosci (2012) 32(20):6771–84. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5779-11.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Lutz PE, Kieffer BL. Opioid receptors: distinct roles in mood disorders. Trends Neurosci (2013) 36(3):195–206. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2012.11.002

CrossRef Full Text

17. Stolzenberg DS, Numan M. Medial preoptic area interactions with dopamine neural systems in the control of the onset and maintenance of maternal behavior in rats. Front Neuroendocrinol (2009) 30:46–64. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.10.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Zhang TY, Labonté B, Wen XL, Turecki G, Meaney MJ. Epigenetic mechanisms for the early environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in rodents and humans. Neuropsychopharmacology (2013) 38:111–23. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.149

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Raineki C, De Souza MA, Szawka RE, Lutz ML, De Vasconcellos LFT, Sanvitto GL, et al. Neonatal handling and the maternal odor preference in rat pups: involvement of monoamines and cyclic amp response element-binding protein pathway in the olfactory bulb. Neuroscience (2009) 159:31–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

20. Mueller BR, Bale TL. Sex-specific programming of offspring emotionality following stress early in pregnancy. J Neurosci (2008) 28(36):9055–65. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1424-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

21. Pankevich DE, Mueller BR, Brockel B, Bale TL. Prenatal stress programming of offspring feeding behavior and energy balance begins early in pregnancy. Physiol Behav (2009) 98(1-2):94–102. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.04.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Brunton PJ, Russell JA. Endocrine induced changes in brain function during pregnancy. Brain Res (2010) 1364:198–215. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Feldman R, Weller A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Levine A. Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol Sci (2007) 18(11):965–70. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. Howerton CL, Morgan CP, Fischer DB, Bale TL. O-GlcNAc transferase (OGT) as a placental biomarker of maternal stress and reprogramming of CNS gene transcription in development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110(13):5169–74. doi:10.1073/pnas.1300065110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Botbol M. Towards an integrative neuroscientific and psychodynamic approach to the transmission of attachment. J Physiol Paris (2010) 104:263–71. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2010.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Stoesz BM, Hare JF, Snow WM. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying affiliative social behavior: insights from comparative research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:123–32. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. Lucassen PJ, Naninck EFG, van Goudoever JB, Fitzsimons C, Joels M, Korosi A. Perinatal programming of adult hippocampal structure and function; emerging roles of stress, nutrition and epigenetics. Trends Neurosci (2013) 36(11):621–31. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.08.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Sheriff MJ, Love OP. Determining the adaptive potential of maternal stress. Ecol Lett (2013) 16:271–80. doi:10.1111/ele.12042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Hofer MA. On the nature and consequences of early loss. Psychosom Med (1996) 58:570–81.

30. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2007) 48(3/4):329–54. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL, Solodkin A, et al. Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169:907–15. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091347

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Moriceau S, Shionoya K, Jakubs K, Sullivan RM. Early-life stress disrupts attachment learning: the role of amygdala corticosterone, locus coeruleus, corticotropin releasing hormone, and olfactory bulb norepinephrine. J Neurosci (2009) 29(50):15745–55. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4106-09.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Kim EJ, Horovitz O, Pellman BA, Tan LM, Li Q, Richter-Levin G, et al. Dorsal periaqueductal gray-amygdala pathway conveys both innate and learned fear responses in rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110(36):14795–800. doi:10.1073/pnas.1310845110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Xu X. Modular genetic control of innate behaviors. Bioessays (2013) 35:421–4. doi:10.1002/bies.201200167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Bateson P. How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Anim Behav (1979) 27:470–86. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(79)90184-2

CrossRef Full Text

36. Robinson GE. Beyond nature and nurture. Science (2004) 304:397–9. doi:10.1126/science.1095766

CrossRef Full Text

37. Keverne EB, Curley JP. Epigenetics, brain evolution and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol (2008) 29:398–412. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.03.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Kandel ER, Jessell T. Early experience and the fine tuning of synaptic connections. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM editors. Principles of Neural Science. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishing (1991). p. 945–58.

39. Guertin PA. Central pattern generator for locomotion: anatomical, physiological, and pathophysiological considerations. Front Neurol (2013) 3:183. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00183

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Marler P. Innateness and the instinct to learn. An Acad Bras Cienc (2004) 76(2):189–200. doi:10.1590/S0001-37652004000200002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

41. Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev (2001) 81:629–83.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

42. Yamashita K, Kitano T. Molecular evolution of the oxytocin-oxytocin receptor system in eutherians. Mol Phylogenet Evol (2013) 67:520–8. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2013.02.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

43. Rao VV, Löffler C, Battey J, Hansmann I. The human gene for oxytocin-neurophysin I (OXT) is physically mapped to chromosome 20p13 by in situ hybridization. Cytogenet Cell Genet (1992) 61:271–3. doi:10.1159/000133420

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

44. McCall C, Singer T. The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior. Nat Neurosci (2012) 15:681–8. doi:10.1038/nn.3084

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

45. Young LJ, Flanagan-Cato LM. Editorial comment: oxytocin, vasopressin and social behavior. Horm Behav (2012) 61:227–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.019

CrossRef Full Text

46. Pedersen CA, Prange AJ Jr. Induction of maternal behavior in virgin rats after intracerebroventricular administration of oxytocin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1979) 76:6661–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.76.12.6661

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

47. Bosch OJ. Maternal aggression in rodents: brain oxytocin and vasopressin mediate pup defence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2013) 368:20130085. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0085

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

48. Apter-Levi Y, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Oxytocin and vasopressin support distinct configuration of social synchrony. Brain Res (2013). doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

49. Lee AG, Cool DR, Grunwald WC Jr, Neal DE, Buckmaster CL, Cheng MY, et al. A novel form of oxytocin in New World monkeys. Biol Lett (2011) 7:584–7. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

50. Ferguson DR. Genetic distribution of vasopressins in the peccary (Tayassu angulatus) and warthog (Phacochoerus aethiopicus). Gen Comp Endocrinol (1969) 12:609–13. doi:10.1016/0016-6480(69)90181-6

CrossRef Full Text

51. Wallis M. Molecular evolution of the neurohypophysial hormone precursors in mammals: comparative genomics reveals novel mammalian oxytocin and vasopressin analogues. Gen Comp Endocrinol (2012) 179:313–8. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.07.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

52. Koehbach J, Stockner T, Bergmayr C, Muttenthaler M, Gruber CW. Insights into the molecular evolution of oxytocin receptor ligand binding. Biochem Soc Trans (2013) 41:197–204. doi:10.1042/BST20120256

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

53. Zingg HH, Laporte SA. The oxytocin receptor. Trends Endocrinol Metab (2003) 14:222–7. doi:10.1016/S1043-2760(03)00080-8

CrossRef Full Text

54. Koshimizu TA, Nakamura K, Egashira N, Hiroyama M, Nonoguchi H, Tanoue A. Vasopressin V1a and V1b receptors: from molecules to physiological systems. Physiol Rev (2012) 92:1813–64. doi:10.1152/physrev.00035.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

55. Kimura T, Tanizawa O, Mori K, Brownstein MJ, Okayama H. Structure and expression of a human oxytocin receptor. Nature (1992) 356:526–9. doi:10.1038/356526a0

CrossRef Full Text

56. Hammock EAD, Levitt P. Oxytocin receptor ligand binding in embryonic tissue and postnatal brain development of the C57BL/6J mouse. Front Behav Neurosci (2013) 7:195. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

57. Muráni E, Ponsuksili S, D’Eath RB, Turner SP, Kurt E, Evans G, et al. Association of HPA axis-related genetic variation with stress reactivity and aggressive behavior in pigs. BMC Genet (2010) 11:e74. doi:10.1186/1471-2156-11-74

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

58. Terenina E, Babigumira BM, Le MignonG, Bazovkina D, Rousseau S, Salin F, et al. Association study of molecular polymorphisms in candidate genes related to stress responses with production and meat quality traits in pigs. Domest Anim Endocrinol (2013) 44:81–97. doi:10.1016/j.domaniend.2012.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

59. Ebstein RP, Knafo A, Mankuta D, Chew SH, Lai PS. The contributions of oxytocin and vasopressin pathway genes to human behavior. Horm Behav (2012) 61:359–79. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

60. Schradin C, Reeder DM, Mendoza SP, Anzenberger G. Prolactin and paternal care: comparison of three species of monogamous new world monkeys (Callicebus cupreus, Callithrix jacchus, and Callimico goeldii). J Comp Psychol (2003) 117:166–75. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.166

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

61. Swaney WT, Curley JP, Champagne FA, Keverne EB. Genomic imprinting mediates sexual experience-dependent olfactory learning in male mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2007) 104(14):6084–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.0609471104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

62. Garfield AS, Cowley M, Smith FM, Moorwood K, Stewart-Cox JE, Gilroy K, et al. Distinct physiological and behavioural functions for parental alleles of imprinted Grb10. Nature (2011) 469(7331):534–8. doi:10.1038/nature09651

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

63. Day T, Bonduriansky R. Intralocus sexual conflict can drive the evolution of genomic imprinting. Genetics (2004) 167:1537–46. doi:10.1534/genetics.103.026211

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

64. McGowan PO, Suderman M, Sasaki A, Huang TCT, Hallett M, Meaney MJ, et al. Broad epigenetic signature of maternal care in the brain of adult rats. PLoS One (2011) 6(2):e14739. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0014739

CrossRef Full Text

65. Wersinger SR. The neuroscience of social behavior. ILAR J (2009) 50(1):1–4. doi:10.1093/ilar.50.1.1

CrossRef Full Text

66. Dunbar RI, Shultz S. Evolution in the social brain. Science (2007) 317(5843):1344–7. doi:10.1126/science.1145463

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: mother–infant attachment, oxytocin receptor gene, innate behavior, social behaviors, behavior synchrony

Citation: Lucion AB and Bortolini MC (2014) Mother–pup interactions: rodents and humans. Front. Endocrinol. 5:17. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00017

Received: 22 December 2013; Paper pending published: 11 January 2014;
Accepted: 08 February 2014; Published online: 26 February 2014.

Edited by:

Nikolaos P. Daskalakis, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, USA

Reviewed by:

Regina M. Sullivan, New York University School of Medicine, USA
Antonios Stamatakis, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece

Copyright: © 2014 Lucion and Bortolini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aldo B. Lucion, Departamento de Fisiologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Rua Sarmento Leite 500, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul 90050-170, Brazil e-mail: alucion@ufrgs.br

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.