Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 19 May 2015
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Trends in Neuroendocrinology View all 13 articles

Oxytocin during Development: Possible Organizational Effects on Behavior

  • 1Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and Behavior, Department of Biological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA
  • 2School of Biomedical Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA

Oxytocin (Oxt) is a neurohormone known for its physiological roles associated with lactation and parturition in mammals. Oxt can also profoundly influence mammalian social behaviors such as affiliative, parental, and aggressive behaviors. While the acute effects of Oxt signaling on adult behavior have been heavily researched in many species, including humans, the developmental effects of Oxt on the brain and behavior are just beginning to be explored. There is evidence that Oxt in early postnatal and peripubertal development, and perhaps during prenatal life, affects adult behavior by altering neural structure and function. However, the specific mechanisms by which this occurs remain unknown. Thus, this review will detail what is known about how developmental Oxt impacts behavior as well as explore the specific neurochemicals and neural substrates that are important to these behaviors.

Introduction

In the landmark paper by Phoenix et al. (1), the organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexual behavior in guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) were established and resulted in the formulation of the “organizational/activational hypothesis of sexual differentiation.” This hypothesis, which states that perinatal exposure to gonadal steroids is important for the sexual differentiation of the brain and behavior, is part of the foundation on which the field of behavioral neuroendocrinology has been built. Given the importance of this concept, it is perhaps not surprising that in the last 55 years this hypothesis has been extended to include another critical time period – puberty – as well as other hormones and behaviors, with one of these hormones being oxytocin (Oxt) (24).

Oxytocin is a mammalian neurohormone composed of nine amino acids, known for its peripheral effects on parturition and lactation (57), as well as its central neuromodulatory effects on social behaviors such as affiliative, aggressive, and parental behaviors (8, 9). While much of the work on Oxt has focused on its involvement in the acute modulation of behavior, there is also evidence that exposure to Oxt during early life is important for the proper development of neural pathways and subsequent sex-specific behaviors. These latter observations have led researchers to hypothesize that Oxt has organizational effects on the brain (3, 10). This is an exciting possibility as developmental exposure to Oxt appears to impact many of the species-specific and sex-specific social behaviors it is known to modulate in adulthood – thus, a reevaluation and broadening of our understanding of Oxt’s effects seems warranted. To encourage this shift in paradigm, in this review we will highlight recent research on how developmental exposure to Oxt affects behavior as well as the specific neurochemicals and neural substrates that underlie these behaviors.

Oxytocin’s Postnatal and Peripubertal Effects on Behavior

Noonan and colleagues (10) were the first to hypothesize that Oxt might have organizational effects on the brain and behavior. Their work demonstrated that a single intracisternal injection of Oxt in postnatal day (PND) 3 rats (Rattus norvegicus) significantly increases novelty-induced grooming at 4 months of age (10) [grooming is known to be directly enhanced by Oxt administration in adults (11)]. However, when a study by Boer and colleagues failed to replicate the aforementioned findings in an open-field test (12), investigations into the organizational effects of Oxt largely fell by the wayside. Yet, in the last 12 years, there has been a resurgence of research in this area, with studies in numerous species providing converging evidence that Oxt during development can have permanent effects on the brain and behavior (3, 1329) (see Table 1). The implications are profound, as they are changing the way that we think about how Oxt works – no longer as just a neuromodulator, but rather as a neurohormone that contributes to the development of behaviors that are essential for survival.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. A summary of the design and findings of studies investigating how developmental manipulation of oxytocin activity affects long-term behavioral expression.

Sexual, Affiliative, and Aggressive Behaviors

Work in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) has shown that there are clear behavioral consequences when Oxt is manipulated during early postnatal development, and that these effects are sex specific; this latter observation is perhaps not surprising since it is well known that Oxt expression is sexually dimorphic in numerous species, including prairie voles (3034). In female prairie voles, a single injection of Oxt on PND1 results in increases in intrasexual aggression in adults, suggesting strengthened pair bond formation (18), whereas an injection of both Oxt and an Oxt receptor antagonist (OTA), also on PND1, decreases the frequency of mating bouts in both adult males and females (20). A single postnatal injection of Oxt also increases intrasexual aggression in adult female mandarin voles (Lasiopodomys mandarinus) after exposure to a male (35). In male prairie voles, an injection of Oxt at both low (1 mg/kg) and high (4 mg/kg) doses on PND0 increases partner preference and social contact in adults compared to controls (19, 36); however, a dose of 2 mg/kg does not (36). Male mandarin voles that receive a single postnatal injection of Oxt increase their mounting behavior at PND60 (37). So, at least in voles, developmental Oxt appears to increase pair bond formation in females, and increase affiliative behaviors in males, whereas the effects on sexual behavior appear to be stimulatory in both males and females; though they may be dose dependent in a manner that is not linear.

In mice (Mus musculus), there are sex differences in the effects of neonatal Oxt manipulation on affiliative behaviors, though the findings differ from observations in prairie voles. Female mice administered an OTA (3 μg/20 μL) on the day of birth have decreases in social approach when tested at 8–15 weeks (29) as measured in a three chambered apparatus based on that developed by Crawley (38). On the other hand, male mice administered an OTA on PND0 display social approach behaviors similar to what is observed in the control conditions (29). So, in mice it appears that Oxt exposure in neonate females may promote affiliative behaviors, while having no effect in males. Exploration of why there may be no effect in male mice can be found in the section titled “Potential Effects of Oxytocin During Fetal Development.”

The aforementioned impact of developmental Oxt on behavior is not limited to rodents. Postnatal intranasal Oxt administration in 2.5- to 8-week-old pigs (Sus scrofa) increases intrasexual aggressive behavior and decreases social contact (39), which is similar to what is observed in female prairie voles (18). [Pigs were selected as an experimental model because their neuroanatomy, as well as their physiology and development, is more similar to humans than rodent models (40).] This particular study is unusual in that it is the only one to use an ungulate animal model, as well as one of only a few studies to use intranasal Oxt administration. Because of the uniqueness of this species, it is not known if these findings are broadly applicable to other species.

There is also evidence that Oxt’s developmental effects on sociability may not be limited to the perinatal period, but may also extend into peripubertal development (2). Bowen and colleagues administered daily Oxt injections to male rats from PND33 to PND42 and then tested them in a social interaction test on PND55. Oxt-treated males spent more time in close proximity to conspecifics and made more active social contacts than controls (41). Researchers from this same research group later conducted an experiment in which male rats were given Oxt injections every 3 days from PND28 to PND55. When tested in a social interaction test at PND70, their behavior appeared similar to what was described above, with their spending more time in close proximity to conspecifics than control animals (42). There have also been studies investigating the effects of peripubertal intranasal Oxt on behavior in rodents. Male prairie voles administered low, medium, and high doses of intranasal Oxt daily during the approximate time between weaning and sexual maturity, from PND21 until 42, display increases in social contact during the treatment window, and those given low and medium doses increase their preference for strangers when tested from PND43–60 (43). In female BTBR mice (a model of autism spectrum disorders), daily intranasal Oxt treatment from PND21 to 50, followed by testing on PND55, increases time spent sniffing a novel mouse over a novel object, essentially rescuing “sociability” to levels observed in wild-type mice (44). Another study found that chronic intranasal Oxt from 12 to 23 weeks reduced social behavior in adult male mice when they were tested 1 h after Oxt administration (45). While the studies described above may differ somewhat in terms of the details of their findings, it does appear that intranasal Oxt treatment during peripubertal development facilitates social interactions in both males and females. However, it is not clear how long lasting these effects are. Given that the peripubertal period is another critical developmental window for the organizational effects of hormones (2), more work focused on these types of questions is needed, especially as intranasal Oxt is being considered as a treatment for various neurodevelopmental conditions (4648), in particular children and adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders (4952).

Parental Behavior

In male prairie voles, treatment with an OTA in neonates decreases alloparental behaviors. Specifically, males injected with an OTA within 24 h of birth and later tested on PND21 decrease their parental care, as measured by fewer retrievals, less time spent huddling over pups, and increases in pup-directed attacks. These effects also appear to be transient, as they are not observed when the same animals are tested again on PND60 (14). In laboratory mice, treatment with an OTA also reduces alloparental care, but in both sexes. Specifically, treatment with an OTA on PND0 decreases the total number of pups retrieved by females and increases pup retrieval latencies in males (29). Oxt treatment also increases the responsiveness of females to pups, as measured by approach times, although this effect is dose dependent, with the lowest dose of Oxt resulting in longer approach times compared to saline controls (16). Thus, it appears that Oxt signaling is important for normal displays of alloparental care, which is consistent with its role in lowering the threshold for maternal care in rodents (5356).

Work by Keebaugh and Young has utilized viral vectors to overexpress the Oxtr and study the organizational effects of Oxt on behavior during puberty. By injecting an adeno-associated viral vector into the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) shell of female prairie voles at PND21 they were able to facilitate alloparental care, as measured by reductions in approach times and increases in time spent licking and grooming pups compared to controls (57). This gene delivery approach has helped to identify a specific neural substrate – the NAcc – on which Oxt may act. The NAcc, which is a part of the brain’s reward circuit, is known to be important in numerous motivated behaviors – it not only expresses the Oxtr but is also one of the regions in which the Oxtr is more highly expressed in biparental prairie voles compared to non-monogamous species that do not exhibit biparental care (5862).

Other Behaviors

While only a few studies have investigated Oxt’s organizational effects on other behaviors, these studies confirm that Oxt’s developmental effects are not limited to its impact on social behavior. In rats, repeated administration of Oxt between PND10 and PND14 results in weight gain in both males and females, increases in the gut hormone cholecystokinin, and longer withdrawal latencies in the tail-flick test at PND60, which suggests an increase in pain threshold (63). Male and female mice with a genetic disruption that models Prader–Willi syndrome usually exhibit a lethal feeding deficiency. This feeding deficiency can be rescued with an injection of Oxt on PND0, or induced in wild-type mice by injecting an OTA 1–1.5 h after birth (64). These observations are consistent with what is observed in Oxt and Oxtr knockout (−/−) mice. These mice develop late-onset obesity in the absence of hyperphagia (65, 66). However, these developmental effects of Oxt differ from what is observed in adults, where Oxt is hypothesized to have anorexigenic effects (67). Since developmental Oxt appears to impact aspects of energy homeostasis, which in turn can affect behavior, additional work in this area is warranted.

Finally, in the study by Bowen and colleagues (41) mentioned in the previous section, peripubertal Oxt administration also affects anxiety-like behavior and ethanol consumption later in life. Male rats given daily Oxt injections from PND33 to PND42 and tested in an emergence test on PND50 traveled further and spent more time in the open-field compared to controls, which would suggest that Oxt had an anxiolytic effect. When tested on PND72 and later, Oxt-treated males consumed significantly less alcohol (i.e., beer) than controls, while water consumption remained unaffected (41).

Summary

It is apparent that Oxt in early postnatal development and during puberty can result in long-lasting changes in behavior, including behaviors that have traditionally been associated with Oxt’s acute neuromodulatory effects, such as affiliative and sexual behaviors, as well as non-social behaviors, such as nociception and ethanol consumption. While there is a lack of consensus in the data, this is in part a reflection of the difficulty of these types of studies, as there are many experimental possibilities that could result in very different outcomes. Particularly crucial is the timing window for Oxt administration, the dose of Oxt, and the behavioral endpoints measured. It is also important to note that Oxt’s organizational effects appear to be sex- and species-specific, which is consistent with the complexity of its neuromodulatory role in adulthood. Thus, future behavioral work will need to continue to take a broad approach to identify Oxt’s potential organizational effects, as some of these behavioral changes, such as energy metabolism and anxiety, could affect a variety of other behaviors. In the meantime, rigorous investigation of Oxt’s organizational effects on neural structure and function may help clarify mechanisms by which Oxt affects brain development and ultimately behavior.

Oxytocin’s Developmental Effects on Neurochemicals and Neural Substrates

The behavioral effects of postnatal and peripubertal Oxt must be rooted in structural and functional modifications to neurons, such as changes in gene expression, axonal guidance, or cell morphology. Therefore, in this section, we review what is known about how Oxt may be impacting these systems.

Effects on Estrogen Receptor Alpha

It is well established that gonadal steroids play a significant role in regulating Oxt activity (6871). While androgens and progesterone modulate Oxt and Oxtr expression (7275), it is the estrogens that seem to have the greatest impact on the Oxt system (68, 70, 74, 7678). Further, these effects are not unidirectional, with the Oxt system altering gonadal steroid systems, specifically the expression of estrogen receptor α (ERα). Neonatal injections of Oxt increase ERα expression in the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) of adult females and Oxt treatment on PND0 increases the number of ERα-immunoreactive (ERα-ir) cells in the VMH of 3-week-old prairie voles (26). These effects appear to be rapid since neonatal prairie voles treated with Oxt also have increases in ERα-ir (24, 79) and ERα mRNA expressions (79). Similar to prairie voles, rats that are repeatedly administered Oxt from PND0 until PND7 have increases in ERα-ir at PND75 (28) and neonatal Oxt manipulation increases the expression of ERα in the hippocampus (79), ventral lateral septum (LS), and central nucleus of the amygdala (24). These results differ from reports in females where a single injection of an OTA on PND1 decreases expression of ERα in the medial preoptic area (MPOA) of adult female prairie voles, as well as increases ERα expression in the BNST, and possibly decreases expression in the medial amygdala (MeA) (26) and repeated administration of an OTA from PND0 to PND7 decreases the expression of ERα in the MPOA of adult female rats (28).

Effects on the Oxytocin and Vasopressin Systems

Early exposure to relevant stimuli is known to permanently alter the responsiveness of a hormone receptor; this phenomenon is known as hormonal imprinting (80). Based on this observation, it is reasonable to suspect that early Oxt exposure could affect the development of the Oxt system itself. Data from several species clearly support this idea; however, the findings are not consistent between sexes and species – in keeping with Oxt’s known intersexual and interspecific variation. In female prairie voles, neonates treated with Oxt and an OTA have increases in Oxt immunoreactivity (Oxt-ir) within the PVN by 3 weeks of age. Yet, in males, Oxt has no effect on Oxt-ir, but treatment with an OTA does decrease arginine vasopressin (Avp) immunoreactivity in the PVN (27).

In addition to changes in peptide expression, neonatal manipulation of Oxt impacts Avp 1a receptor (Avpr1a) binding in a sexually dimorphic manner. Specifically, in female prairie voles, Oxt treatment on PND0 decreases Avpr1a binding in the MPOA, BNST, LS, cingulate cortex (CgCtx), and medial thalamus on PND60, but in males it increases Avpr1a binding in the CgCtx. OTA treatment in females decreases Avpr1a binding only in the BNST and CgCtx, while in males it decreases Avpr1a binding in the MPOA, BNST, and LS (15). Taken together, the delivery of Oxt or an OTA during early postnatal life appears to have nearly opposite effects on adult Avpr1a expression in females and males. Similar to what was discussed in terms of behavioral effects, there does appear to be a “critical period” of organization that extends into peripubertal development, with peripubertal Oxt administration increasing Oxtr mRNA expression in the hypothalamus (41) and plasma Oxt levels (42) in adult male rats.

Other Effects

While studies on the organizational effects of Oxt on other neurochemical systems are few and far between, there is evidence that perinatal exposure to Oxt can influence the functioning of the stress axis. Female prairie voles administered Oxt on PND1 have reductions in baseline plasma corticosterone by PND8 compared to animals treated with saline or an OTA (23). In neonatal pigs, repeated Oxt treatment increases adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) at 8 weeks of age and decreases responsiveness in the dexamethasone suppression test at 11 weeks; indicative of dysregulation of the glucocorticoid response. However, pigs that receive Oxt have less blunting of the cortisol response than controls (39). So, not only is Oxt able to acutely regulate stress responses (81, 82) but appears to also be involved in the development of long-term responsiveness to stress.

There are also reported effects of neonatal Oxt on adrenergic and serotonergic receptors, which may contribute to the aforementioned effects of Oxt on feeding and social behaviors (see Other Behaviors). In rats, chronic neonatal Oxt treatment alters α2 adrenergic receptor (α2r) kinetics in PND130 male rats; these changes are dependent upon the nutrition of the dam to which the pups were born. Specifically, Oxt treatment rescues the affinity (described by the dissociation constant Kd), which is decreased in placebo treated neonates of food-restricted dams compared to offspring of dams fed ad libitum, of the α2r for its ligand within the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) and increases the number of α2r binding sites (described by Bmax) in the hypothalamus and amygdala of offspring born to food-restricted dams compared to controls. In pups born to dams fed ad libitum Oxt treatment decreases the affinity of the α2r for its ligand in the hypothalamus, and increases the number of binding sites in the hypothalamus and NTS (83). In male prairie voles, neonatal Oxt administration also results in increases in serotonergic axon density in the anterior hypothalamus, cortical amygdala, and VMH at PND21 (21), as well as decreases dopamine turnover in the hypothalamus as well as serotonin turnover in the hypothalamus, medulla oblongata, and striatum of 4-month-old female rats (84). Thus, not only does developmental Oxt exposure affect many different neural circuits, these effects can also be highly dependent on the state of the dam. This is in keeping with a large body of research showing that maternal physiology, particularly stress (8587) and nutrition (8890), can greatly impact the neural and behavioral development of offspring. The previously described studies have opened the door to exploring what role developmental Oxt might play in these effects.

Summary

The organizational effects of Oxt extend to many different neurochemical systems including the gonadal steroids, the Oxt and Avp systems, and the stress axis, which suggests that Oxt’s effects are widespread and complex (Figure 1). The brain regions most commonly affected by developmental Oxt exposure are the VMH, MPOA, BNST, LS, PVN, and several nuclei of the amygdala. Oxt or the Oxtr is expressed in some of these regions (68, 91), though their expression in these brain areas often varies depending on species, age, and sex. What is particularly interesting about the aforementioned brain nuclei is that many of them are a part of the “social behavioral network,” which is comprised of neuroanatomical areas or “nodes” that are interconnected, express gonadal hormone receptors, appear to be influenced by Oxt, and are important in the regulation of many types of social behaviors (92, 93). Work from Bruce Cushing’s laboratory suggests that the BNST and MeA are particularly important for the developmental effects of Oxt and ERα on social behavior (24, 26, 94) but further investigation into the entire social behavioral network is needed.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Developmental exposure to oxytocin is known to affect many behaviors (left) and neurotransmitter and neurohormone systems (right). These behavioral effects are often species and sex specific, which is consistent with oxytocin’s neuromodulatory role in adults. However, how these behavioral changes are rooted in the observed alterations in neurochemistry remains unknown.

Potential Effects of Oxytocin during Fetal Development

While research into the neonatal and peripubertal developmental effects of Oxt has been ever increasing, an area that remains largely unexplored is the potential for Oxt to have organizational effects during embryonic development. These potential effects are relevant, in part, because of the increased use of Oxtr agonists, such as Pitocin, and antagonists, such as Atosiban, during human pregnancy in order to manage labor timing (95, 96). While the developmental consequences of these interventions have been reviewed elsewhere (95), the possible behavioral implications of exposing human fetuses to exogenous Oxt or Oxtr antagonists should not be ignored. Because of the complexity and ethical considerations in humans our best hope for elucidating the role of fetal Oxt is the use of animal models.

In mice, there is evidence that in utero exposure to Oxt is important for normal intermale aggressive behavior in adulthood. Specifically, male Oxt−/− mice that are born to null mutant dams show heightened aggressive behavior in adulthood (97, 98). While this phenotype cannot be rescued if the pups are cross-fostered to wild-type dams (99), it is not observed when male mice are born to heterozygous dams. One of the key differences between pups born to null mutant dams versus those born to heterozygous dams is the absence or presence of maternal Oxt. The hypothesis that the maternal Oxt may be signaling in the fetal brain is supported by studies using male Oxtr knockout (Oxtr−/−) mice, which lack Oxtr signaling throughout development. These mice also have heightened aggressive behavior in adulthood (99, 100). However, male forebrain Oxtr knockout (Oxtr Fb/Fb) mice (101, 102), in which the Oxtr gene is excised 21–28 days after birth, have normal aggressive behavior in adulthood (100). These data suggest that Oxt signaling via the Oxtr during fetal development might be important for displays of aggressive behavior, and perhaps other behaviors in adulthood.

Unfortunately, very little is known about the developmental time course of the Oxt system in rodents. In rats, Oxt mRNA is observed as early as embryonic day (E) 15.5 in the PVN and E18.5 in the SON (103). The mRNA for the Oxt carrier protein neurophysin-I is available as early as E16 in the PVN and SON, and the Oxt peptide is seen by PND7 in the SON and PVN and E21 in the pituitary (104). Prairie voles also have a postnatal increase in Oxt peptide expression, with the number of Oxt expressing neurons significantly increasing from PND1 to PND21 in both males and females (27). While less is known about the development of the Oxtr, in rats, Oxtr binding has been identified as early as E14 in undifferentiated neurons (104). In mice, a study by Hammock and Levitt (105), which focused primarily on Oxtr binding during postnatal development, found Oxtr binding in the brains of E18.5 C57BL/6J mice. However, this was the only embryonic time point that was examined. It is plausible that the “critical window” for the manipulation of the Oxt system is not the same in males as in females, since there could be sex differences in its development. Therefore, future research in this area should consider the potential for sex differences in the development of the Oxt system, as it may help to inform the conclusions that are drawn from the data.

Conclusion

Compared to what we understand about Oxt in adulthood, research into its role in development is still in its early stages and there is much to do before we really have a handle on its effects on the brain and behavior. For instance, what other behaviors are affected? How conserved are Oxt’s effects across species, or within a particular sex? How are these organizational changes grounded in alterations of neuronal structure and function? How broad is the “critical window” for Oxt’s effects? What are the implications for human offspring? Therefore, examination of the mechanisms that may underlie behavioral changes, including the identification of specific neural substrates, is of the utmost importance.

This is an exciting time in behavioral neuroendocrine research due to increased interest in Oxt and the social brain as well as great advancements in the foundational work, which has methodically examined the neuromodulatory effects of Oxt in animal models. However, there is still much we do not understand about the Oxt system, and filling this “knowledge gap” becomes more vital as the interest in using Oxt in clinical settings continues to increase. For the last several years, intranasal Oxt has been marketed as the “cuddle” or “love” hormone, and is being used in a variety of contexts as a therapeutic agent to promote prosocial behaviors in humans. Much of this work has been performed in the absence of dose response studies, without serious consideration of the potential developmental or long-term effects, and with little attention paid to where in the brain these effects might be mediated. Thus, it is perhaps not surprising that more recent studies in humans suggest that Oxt’s effects are nuanced (as the data from animal models would suggest) and that intranasal Oxt treatment can have undesirable effects (106108). In light of this, basic research on the Oxt system becomes ever more critical, particularly since our understanding of the developmental role of Oxt is expanding. An important first step is to home in on specific circuits, focusing on studies that will shed light on the interactions between numerous brain regions and behaviors. This approach will allow scientists to elucidate the specific mechanisms of Oxt’s organizational effects on behavior – be they genetic, epigenetic, or neuroanatomical – which can then be used not only to inform human studies but also identify any conserved mechanisms between sexes and species.

Author Contributions

Both TM and HC conceived of and drafted the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are thankful to Dr. Colleen Novak and Dr. Wilson Chung for their comments on the manuscript. This work was supported in part by the Department of Biological Sciences and the School of Biomedical Sciences at Kent State University as well as funding to HC from the National Science Foundation (IOS353859).

References

1. Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology (1959) 65:369–82. doi:10.1210/endo-65-3-369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Schulz KM, Molenda-Figueira HA, Sisk CL. Back to the future: the organizational-activational hypothesis adapted to puberty and adolescence. Horm Behav (2009) 55(5):597–604. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cushing BS. The organizational effects of oxytocin and vasopressin. In: Choleris E, Pfaff DW, Kavaliers M, editors. Oxytocin, Vasopressin and Related Peptides in the Regulation of Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2013). p. 56–72.

Google Scholar

4. Bouret SG. Organizational actions of metabolic hormones. Front Neuroendocrinol (2013) 34(1):18–26. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Blanks AM, Thornton S. The role of oxytocin in parturition. BJOG (2003) 110(s20):46–51. doi:10.1016/S1470-0328(03)00024-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wakerley J, Lincoln D. The milk-ejection reflex of the rat: a 20-to 40-fold acceleration in the firing of paraventricular neurones during oxytocin release. J Endocrinol (1973) 57(3):477–93. doi:10.1677/joe.0.0570477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Frayne J, Townsend D, Nicholson H. Effects of oxytocin on sperm transport in the pubertal rat. J Reprod Fertil (1996) 107(2):299–306. doi:10.1530/jrf.0.1070299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Insel TR, Young LJ. Neuropeptides and the evolution of social behavior. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2000) 10(6):784–9. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00146-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Caldwell HK. Neurobiology of sociability. In: Lopez-Larrea C, editor. Sensing in Nature. New York: Springer (2012). p. 187–205.

Google Scholar

10. Noonan LR, Continella G, Pedersen CA. Neonatal administration of oxytocin increases novelty-induced grooming in the adult rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1989) 33(3):555–8. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(89)90386-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Drago F, Pedersen CA, Caldwell JD, Prange AJ Jr. Oxytocin potently enhances novelty-induced grooming behavior in the rat. Brain Res (1986) 368(2):287–95. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)90573-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Boer GJ, Quak J, De Vries MC, Heinsbroek RP. Mild sustained effects of neonatal vasopressin and oxytocin treatment on brain growth and behavior of the rat. Peptides (1994) 15(2):229–36. doi:10.1016/0196-9781(94)90007-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bales KL, Kim AJ, Lewis-Reese AD, Sue Carter C. Both oxytocin and vasopressin may influence alloparental behavior in male prairie voles. Horm Behav (2004) 45(5):354–61. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bales KL, Pfeifer LA, Carter CS. Sex differences and developmental effects of manipulations of oxytocin on alloparenting and anxiety in prairie voles. Dev Psychobiol (2004) 44(2):123–31. doi:10.1002/dev.10165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bales KL, Plotsky PM, Young LJ, Lim MM, Grotte N, Ferrer E, et al. Neonatal oxytocin manipulations have long-lasting, sexually dimorphic effects on vasopressin receptors. Neuroscience (2007) 144(1):38–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bales KL, van Westerhuyzen JA, Lewis-Reese AD, Grotte ND, Lanter JA, Carter CS. Oxytocin has dose-dependent developmental effects on pair-bonding and alloparental care in female prairie voles. Horm Behav (2007) 52(2):274–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bales KL, Abdelnabi M, Cushing BS, Ottinger MA, Carter CS. Effects of neonatal oxytocin manipulations on male reproductive potential in prairie voles. Physiol Behav (2004) 81:519–26. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bales KL, Carter CS. Sex differences and developmental effects of oxytocin on aggression and social behavior in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Horm Behav (2003) 44(3):178–84. doi:10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00154-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Bales KL, Carter CS. Developmental exposure to oxytocin facilitates partner preferences in male prarie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav Neurosci (2003) 117(4):854–9. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.4.854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cushing BS, Levine K, Cushing NL. Neonatal manipulation of oxytocin influences female reproductive behavior and success. Horm Behav (2005) 47(1):22–8. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Eaton JL, Roache L, Nguyen KN, Cushing BS, Troyer E, Papademetriou E, et al. Organizational effects of oxytocin on serotonin innervation. Dev Psychobiol (2012) 54(1):92–7. doi:10.1002/dev.20566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kramer KM, Choe C, Carter CS, Cushing BS. Developmental effects of oxytocin on neural activation and neuropeptide release in response to social stimuli. Horm Behav (2006) 49(2):206–14. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kramer KM, Cushing BS, Carter CS. Developmental effects of oxytocin on stress response: single versus repeated exposure. Physiol Behav (2003) 79(4–5):775–82. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00175-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kramer KM, Yoshida S, Papademetriou E, Cushing BS. The organizational effects of oxytocin on the central expression of estrogen receptor alpha and oxytocin in adulthood. BMC Neurosci (2007) 8(1):71. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-8-71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Withuhn TF, Kramer KM, Cushing BS. Early exposure to oxytocin affects the age of vaginal opening and first estrus in female rats. Physiol Behav (2003) 80(1):135–8. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00222-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Yamamoto Y, Carter CS, Cushing BS. Neonatal manipulation of oxytocin affects expression of estrogen receptor alpha. Neuroscience (2006) 137(1):157–64. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yamamoto Y, Cushing BS, Kramer KM, Epperson PD, Hoffman GE, Carter CS. Neonatal manipulations of oxytocin alter expression of oxytocin and vasopressin immunoreactive cells in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus in a gender-specific manner. Neuroscience (2004) 125(4):947–55. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Perry AN, Paramadilok A, Cushing BS. Neonatal oxytocin alters subsequent estrogen receptor alpha protein expression and estrogen sensitivity in the female rat. Behav Brain Res (2009) 205(1):154–61. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mogi K, Ooyama R, Nagasawa M, Kikusui T. Effects of neonatal oxytocin manipulation on development of social behaviors in mice. Physiol Behav (2014) 133:68–75. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Uhl-Bronner S, Waltisperger E, Martinez-Lorenzana G, Condes Lara M, Freund-Mercier MJ. Sexually dimorphic expression of oxytocin binding sites in forebrain and spinal cord of the rat. Neuroscience (2005) 135(1):147–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. de Vries GJ. Sex differences in vasopressin and oxytocin innervation of the brain. Prog Brain Res (2008) 170:17–27. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00402-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Coirini H, Johnson A, Schumacher M, McEwen B. Sex differences in the regulation of oxytocin receptors by ovarian steroids in the ventromedial hypothalamus of the rat. Neuroendocrinology (1992) 55(3):269–75. doi:10.1159/000126125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Szot P, Dorsa DM. Differential timing and sexual dimorphism in the expression of the vasopressin gene in the developing rat brain. Dev Brain Res (1993) 73(2):177–83. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(93)90136-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. De Vries G, Panzica G. Sexual differentiation of central vasopressin and vasotocin systems in vertebrates: different mechanisms, similar endpoints. Neuroscience (2006) 138(3):947–55. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.07.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jia R, Tai F, An S, Broders H, Ding X, Kong Q, et al. Effects of neonatal oxytocin treatment on aggression and neural activities in mandarin voles. Physiol Behav (2008) 95(1):56–62. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Carter C, Boone EM, Bales KL. Early experience and the developmental programming of oxytocin and vasopressin. In: Bridges RS, editor. Neurobiology of the Parental Brain. San Diego, CA: Elsevier (2008). p. 417–33.

Google Scholar

37. Jia R, Tai F, An S, Broders H, Sun R. Neonatal manipulation of oxytocin influences the partner preference in mandarin voles (Microtus mandarinus). Neuropeptides (2008) 42(5):525–33. doi:10.1016/j.npep.2008.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Perez A, Barbaro RP, Johns JM, Magnuson TR, et al. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav (2004) 3(5):287–302. doi:10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Rault J-L, Carter CS, Garner JP, Marchant-Forde JN, Richert BT, Lay DC Jr. Repeated intranasal oxytocin administration in early life dysregulates the HPA axis and alters social behavior. Physiol Behav (2013) 112:40–8. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lind NM, Moustgaard A, Jelsing J, Vajta G, Cumming P, Hansen AK. The use of pigs in neuroscience: modeling brain disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2007) 31(5):728–51. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bowen MT, Carson DS, Spiro A, Arnold JC, McGregor IS. Adolescent oxytocin exposure causes persistent reductions in anxiety and alcohol consumption and enhances sociability in rats. PLoS One (2011) 6(11):e27237. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Suraev AS, Bowen MT, Ali SO, Hicks C, Ramos L, McGregor IS. Adolescent exposure to oxytocin, but not the selective oxytocin receptor agonist TGOT, increases social behavior and plasma oxytocin in adulthood. Horm Behav (2014) 65(5):488–96. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bales KL, Perkeybile AM, Conley OG, Lee MH, Guoynes CD, Downing GM, et al. Chronic intranasal oxytocin causes long-term impairments in partner preference formation in male prairie voles. Biol Psychiatry (2013) 74(3):180–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bales K, Solomon M, Jacob S, Crawley J, Silverman J, Larke R, et al. Long-term exposure to intranasal oxytocin in a mouse autism model. Transl Psychiatry (2014) 4(11):e480. doi:10.1038/tp.2014.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Huang H, Michetti C, Busnelli M, Managò F, Sannino S, Scheggia D, et al. Chronic and acute intranasal oxytocin produce divergent social effects in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39(5):1102–14. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wudarczyk OA, Earp BD, Guastella A, Savulescu J. Could intranasal oxytocin be used to enhance relationships? Research imperatives, clinical policy, and ethical considerations. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2013) 26(5):474–84. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283642e10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Tachibana M, Kagitani-Shimono K, Mohri I, Yamamoto T, Sanefuji W, Nakamura A, et al. Long-term administration of intranasal oxytocin is a safe and promising therapy for early adolescent boys with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol (2013) 23(2):123–7. doi:10.1089/cap.2012.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67(7):692–4. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fujisawa TX, Tanaka S, Saito DN, Kosaka H, Tomoda A. Visual attention for social information and salivary oxytocin levels in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: an eye-tracking study. Front Neurosci (2014) 8:295. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Guastella AJ, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Alvares GA, Tonge BJ, Hickie IB, et al. The effects of a course of intranasal oxytocin on social behaviors in youth diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2015) 56:444–52. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Kumbier E, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Herpertz SC. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on the neural basis of face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2013) 74(3):164–71. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gordon I, Vander Wyk BC, Bennett RH, Cordeaux C, Lucas MV, Eilbott JA, et al. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110(52):20953–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.1312857110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Rich ME, deCardenas EJ, Lee HJ, Caldwell HK. Impairments in the initiation of maternal behavior in oxytocin receptor knockout mice. PLoS One (2014) 9(6):e98839. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Macbeth AH, Stepp JE, Lee HJ, Young WS III, Caldwell HK. Normal maternal behavior, but increased pup mortality, in conditional oxytocin receptor knockout females. Behav Neurosci (2010) 124(5):677–85. doi:10.1037/a0020799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Pedersen CA. Oxytocin control of maternal behavior. Regulation by sex steroids and offspring stimuli. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1997) 807:126–45. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb51916.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Francis DD, Young LJ, Meaney MJ, Insel TR. Naturally occurring differences in maternal care are associated with the expression of oxytocin and vasopressin (V1a) receptors: gender differences. J Neuroendocrinol (2002) 14(5):349–53. doi:10.1046/j.0007-1331.2002.00776.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Keebaugh AC, Young LJ. Increasing oxytocin receptor expression in the nucleus accumbens of pre-pubertal female prairie voles enhances alloparental responsiveness and partner preference formation as adults. Horm Behav (2011) 60(5):498–504. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Smeltzer MD, Curtis JT, Aragona BJ, Wang Z. Dopamine, oxytocin, and vasopressin receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex of monogamous and promiscuous voles. Neurosci Lett (2006) 394(2):146–51. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ross HE, Cole CD, Smith Y, Neumann ID, Landgraf R, Murphy AZ, et al. Characterization of the oxytocin system regulating affiliative behavior in female prairie voles. Neuroscience (2009) 162(4):892–903. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.05.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ross HE, Freeman SM, Spiegel LL, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ. Variation in oxytocin receptor density in the nucleus accumbens has differential effects on affiliative behaviors in monogamous and polygamous voles. J Neurosci (2009) 29(5):1312–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5039-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Liu Y, Wang ZX. Nucleus accumbens oxytocin and dopamine interact to regulate pair bond formation in female prairie voles. Neuroscience (2003) 121:537–44. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00555-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Young LJ, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat Neurosci (2004) 7(10):1048–54. doi:10.1038/nn1327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Uvnäs-Moberg K, Alster P, Petersson M, Sohlström A, Björkstrand E. Postnatal oxytocin injections cause sustained weight gain and increased nociceptive thresholds in male and female rats. Pediatr Res (1998) 43(3):344–8. doi:10.1203/00006450-199803000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Schaller F, Watrin F, Sturny R, Massacrier A, Szepetowski P, Muscatelli F. A single postnatal injection of oxytocin rescues the lethal feeding behaviour in mouse newborns deficient for the imprinted Magel2 gene. Hum Mol Genet (2010) 19(24):4895–905. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddq424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Takayanagi Y, Kasahara Y, Onaka T, Takahashi N, Kawada T, Nishimori K. Oxytocin receptor-deficient mice developed late-onset obesity. Neuroreport (2008) 19(9):951–5. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283021ca9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Camerino C. Low sympathetic tone and obese phenotype in oxytocin-deficient mice. Obesity (Silver Spring) (2009) 17(5):980–4. doi:10.1038/oby.2009.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Blevins JE, Ho JM. Role of oxytocin signaling in the regulation of body weight. Rev Endocr Metab Disord (2013) 14(4):311–29. doi:10.1007/s11154-013-9260-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev (2001) 81(2):629–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

69. Dhakar MB, Stevenson EL, Caldwell HK. Oxytocin, vasopressin and their interplay with gonadal steroids. In: Choleris E, Pfaff DW, Kavaliers M, editors. Oxytocin, Vasopressin and Related Peptides in the Regulation of Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2013). p. 3–36.

Google Scholar

70. Caldwell HK, Young WS III. Oxytocin and vasopressin: genetics and behavioral implications. 3rd ed. In: Lim R, editor. Neuroactive Proteins and Peptides: Handbook of Neurochemistry and Molecular Neurobiology. New York, NY: Springer (2006). p. 573–607.

Google Scholar

71. Tribollet E, Audigier S, Dubois-Dauphin M, Dreifuss JJ. Gonadal steroids regulate oxytocin receptors but not vasopressin receptors in the brain of male and female rats. An autoradiographical study. Brain Res (1990) 511(1):129–40. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)90232-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Johnson AE, Coirini H, Insel TR, McEwen BS. The regulation of oxytocin receptor binding in the ventromedial hypothalaimic nucleus by testosterone and its metabolites. Endocrinology (1991) 128(2):891–6. doi:10.1210/endo-128-2-891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Bale TL, Dorsa DM. Regulation of oxytocin receptor messenger ribonucleic acid in the ventromedial hypothalamus by testosterone and its metabolites. Endocrinology (1995) 136(11):5135–8. doi:10.1210/en.136.1.27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Wathes D, Mann G, Payne J, Riley P, Stevenson K, Lamming G. Regulation of oxytocin, oestradiol and progesterone receptor concentrations in different uterine regions by oestradiol, progesterone and oxytocin in ovariectomized ewes. J Endocrinol (1996) 151(3):375–93. doi:10.1677/joe.0.1510375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Grazzini E, Guillon G, Mouillac B, Zingg HH. Inhibition of oxytocin receptor function by direct binding of progesterone. Nature (1998) 392(6675):509–12. doi:10.1038/33176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. De Kloet E, Voorhuis D, Boschma Y, Elands J. Estradiol modulates density of putative oxytocin receptors’ in discrete rat brain regions. Neuroendocrinology (1986) 44(4):415–21. doi:10.1159/000124680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Richard S, Zingg H. The human oxytocin gene promoter is regulated by estrogens. J Biol Chem (1990) 265(11):6098–103.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

78. McCarthy MM, Mcdonald CH, Brooks PJ, Goldman D. An anxiolytic action of oxytocin is enhanced by estrogen in the mouse. Physiol Behav (1997) 60(5):1209–15. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00212-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Carr MS, Papademeteriou E, Schmidt JV, Cushing BS. Oxytocin selectively increases ERα mRNA in the neonatal hypothalamus and hippocampus of female prairie voles. Neuropeptides (2007) 41(1):39–44. doi:10.1016/j.npep.2006.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Csaba G. Phylogeny and ontogeny of hormone receptors: the selection theory of receptor formation and hormonal imprinting. Biol Rev (1980) 55(1):47–63. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1980.tb00687.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Windle R, Shanks N, Lightman SL, Ingram CD. Central oxytocin administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety behavior in rats 1. Endocrinology (1997) 138(7):2829–34. doi:10.1210/en.138.7.2829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Neumann I, Wigger A, Torner L, Holsboer F, Landgraf R. Brain oxytocin inhibits basal and stress-induced activity of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in male and female rats: partial action within the paraventricular nucleus. J Neuroendocrinol (2000) 12(3):235–44. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2000.00442.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Díaz-Cabiale Z, Olausson H, Sohlström A, Agnati L, Narvaez J, Uvnäs-Moberg K, et al. Long-term modulation by postnatal oxytocin of the α2-adrenoceptor agonist binding sites in central autonomic regions and the role of prenatal stress. J Neuroendocrinol (2004) 16(3):183–90. doi:10.1111/j.0953-8194.2004.01146.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hashemi F, Tekes K, Laufer R, Szegi P, Tóthfalusi L, Csaba G. Effect of a single neonatal oxytocin treatment (hormonal imprinting) on the biogenic amine level of the adult rat brain could oxytocin-induced labor cause pervasive developmental diseases? Reprod Sci (2013) 20(10):1255–63. doi:10.1177/1933719113483010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Ward ID, Zucchi FC, Robbins JC, Falkenberg EA, Olson DM, Benzies K, et al. Transgenerational programming of maternal behaviour by prenatal stress. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth (2013) 13(Suppl 1):S9. doi:10.1186/1471-2393-13-S1-S9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Fine R, Zhang J, Stevens H. Prenatal stress and inhibitory neuron systems: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry (2014) 19(6):641–51. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. DiPietro JA. Maternal stress in pregnancy: considerations for fetal development. J Adolesc Health (2012) 51(2):S3–8. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Reyes-Castro L, Rodriguez J, Rodriguez-Gonzalez G, Chavira R, Bautista C, McDonald T, et al. Pre-and/or postnatal protein restriction developmentally programs affect and risk assessment behaviors in adult male rats. Behav Brain Res (2012) 227(2):324–9. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Peleg-Raibstein D, Luca E, Wolfrum C. Maternal high-fat diet in mice programs emotional behavior in adulthood. Behav Brain Res (2012) 233(2):398–404. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Marques AH, O’Connor TG, Roth C, Susser E, Bjørke-Monsen A-L. The influence of maternal prenatal and early childhood nutrition and maternal prenatal stress on offspring immune system development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Neurosci (2013) 7:120. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Tribollet E, Dubois-Dauphin M, Dreifuss J, Barberis C, Jard S. Oxytocin receptors in the central nervous system. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1992) 652(1):29–38. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34343.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Newman SW. The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior. A node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1999) 877:242–57. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Albers HE. Species, sex and individual differences in the vasotocin/vasopressin system: relationship to neurochemical signaling in the social behavior neural network. Front Neuroendocrinol (2015) 36:49–71. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Cushing BS, Kramer KM. Mechanisms underlying epigenetic effects of early social experience: the role of neuropeptides and steroids. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2005) 29(7):1089–105. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kenkel WM, Yee JR, Carter CS. Is oxytocin a maternal-foetal signalling molecule at birth? Implications for development. J Neuroendocrinol (2014) 26(10):739–49. doi:10.1111/jne.12186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Carter CS. Developmental consequences of oxytocin. Physiol Behav (2003) 79(3):383–97. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00151-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Winslow JT, Hearn EF, Ferguson J, Young LJ, Matzuk MM, Insel TR. Infant vocalization, adult aggression, and fear behavior in an oxytocin null mutant mouse. Horm Behav (2000) 37:145–55. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1999.1566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. DeVries AC, Young WS III, Nelson RJ. Reduced aggressive behaviour in mice with targeted disruption of the oxytocin gene. J Neuroendocrinol (1997) 9(5):363–8. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.1997.t01-1-00589.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Takayanagi Y, Yoshida M, Bielsky IF, Ross HE, Kawamata M, Onaka T, et al. Pervasive social deficits, but normal parturition, in oxytocin receptor-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102(44):16096–101. doi:10.1073/pnas.0505312102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Dhakar MB, Rich ME, Reno EL, Lee HJ, Caldwell HK. Heightened aggressive behavior in mice with lifelong versus postweaning knockout of the oxytocin receptor. Horm Behav (2012) 62(1):86–92. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Lee HJ, Caldwell HK, Macbeth AH, Tolu SG, Young WS III. A conditional knockout mouse line of the oxytocin receptor. Endocrinology (2008) 149(7):3256–63. doi:10.1210/en.2007-1710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Lee HJ, Caldwell HK, Macbeth AH, Young WS III. Behavioural studies using temporal and spatial inactivation of the oxytocin receptor. Prog Brain Res (2008) 170:73–7. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00407-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Jing X, Ratty AK, Murphy D. Ontogeny of the vasopressin and oxytocin RNAs in the mouse hypothalamus. Neurosci Res (1998) 30:343–9. doi:10.1016/S0168-0102(98)00017-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Whitnall MH, Key S, Ben-Barak Y, Ozato K, Gainer H. Neurophysin in the hypothalamo-neurohypophysial system. II. Immunocytochemical studies of the ontogeny of oxytocinergic and vasopressinergic neurons. J Neurosci (1985) 5(1):98–109.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

105. Hammock EA, Levitt P. Oxytocin receptor ligand binding in embryonic tissue and postnatal brain development of the C57BL/6J mouse. Front Behav Neurosci (2013) 7:195. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Cardoso C, Ellenbogen MA, Linnen AM. The effect of intranasal oxytocin on perceiving and understanding emotion on the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Emotion (2014) 14(1):43–50. doi:10.1037/a0034314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Ellenbogen MA, Linnen AM, Cardoso C, Joober R. Intranasal oxytocin impedes the ability to ignore task-irrelevant facial expressions of sadness in students with depressive symptoms. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2013) 38(3):387–98. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Bartz J, Simeon D, Hamilton H, Kim S, Crystal S, Braun A, et al. Oxytocin can hinder trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2011) 6(5):556–63. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: affiliative behavior, aggressive behavior, estrogen receptor alpha, maternal behavior, oxytocin receptor, parental behavior, sexual behavior, vasopressin

Citation: Miller TV and Caldwell HK (2015) Oxytocin during development: possible organizational effects on behavior. Front. Endocrinol. 6:76. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2015.00076

Received: 24 November 2014; Accepted: 27 April 2015;
Published: 19 May 2015

Edited by:

Hubert Vaudry, University of Rouen, France

Reviewed by:

Karen Lisa Bales, University of California Davis, USA
Bruce S. Cushing, The University of Akron, USA

Copyright: © 2015 Miller and Caldwell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Heather K. Caldwell, Kent State University, PO Box 5190, 121 Cunningham Hall, Kent, OH 44242, USA, hcaldwel@kent.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.