Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 15 April 2014
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 6 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00072

Vitamin D in older adults: the need to specify standard values with respect to cognition

imageCédric Annweiler1,2* imageOlivier Beauchet1
  • 1Department of Neuroscience, Division of Geriatric Medicine and Memory Clinic, Angers University Hospital, UPRES EA 4638, University of Angers, UNAM, Angers, France
  • 2Department of Medical Biophysics, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, Robarts Research Institute, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

Besides its classical function of bone metabolism regulation, vitamin D exhibits multiple biological targets mediated by its nuclear hormone receptor, the vitamin D receptor (VDR) (Holick, 2007; Kalueff and Tuohimaa, 2007; Annweiler et al., 2010a, 2011b). Specific actions on target organs such as the central nervous system (CNS) have been described, providing evidence for a neurosteroid action of vitamin D (Kalueff and Tuohimaa, 2007; Annweiler et al., 2010a). Consistently, older adults with lower serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25OHD) concentrations exhibit more often and more severe cognitive decline (Annweiler et al., 2009, 2013a,b; Balion et al., 2012; Etgen et al., 2012). At that point, an important issue to be clarified is to determine what concentration of 25OHD is associated with adverse effects in the brain.

Most Older Adults have Low Levels of Vitamin D

At least one billion people have hypovitaminosis D worldwide (Holick, 2007). Even if all adults can be affected by hypovitaminosis D, older adults have the greatest risk, especially those living in institution (Annweiler et al., 2011b). For illustration, in Europe and the United States, up to 90% of older adults have hypovitaminosis D (Annweiler et al., 2011b). This high prevalence of hypovitaminosis D in the elderly is due to the high frequency of mechanisms leading to hypovitaminosis D, first and foremost the reduction of cutaneous synthesis (decreased exposure to sunlight due to loss of functionality, and decreased capacity of skin synthesis due to the 25% reduction of 7-dehydrocholesterol compared to younger adults) (Holick et al., 1989). Hypovitaminosis D may also result from the alteration of the metabolism of vitamin D (hepatic and kidney failures) (Dusso et al., 2005), inadequate food intake (Hollis and Wagner, 2004), reduced bioavailability (malabsorption syndrome, obesity with sequestration of vitamin D in the fat) (Lo et al., 1985), increased catabolism (regular use of antiepileptics, glucocorticoids, immunosuppressive drugs) (Zhou et al., 2006), and urinary loss of vitamin D (nephrotic syndrome) (Dusso et al., 2005).

The high prevalence of hypovitaminosis D in elderly population is also explained by the choice of the threshold defining hypovitaminosis D, which may vary between 10 and 30 ng/mL.

Threshold of “Normality” for Serum Vitamin D Concentration

Vitamin D status is usually estimated by measuring serum 25OHD (Holick, 2007; Annweiler et al., 2011b). In general, there are two ways to establish reference values for a biological variable. The first one is based on the use of “population-based reference values,” which comprises measuring a parameter in the reference population size and calculating the reference interval in which there is 95% of the population. In the case of 25OHD, the reference values could not be generalized because they depend on non-modifiable environmental factors (e.g., season, local climate, latitude), modifiable life habits (e.g., clothing, eating habits, sun exposure), and non-modifiable parameters (e.g., ethnicity, skin pigmentation, skin thickness, and age). Moreover, it should be kept in mind that, in clinical practice, the dosage of 25OHD is prescribed to determine if patients need vitamin D supplementation to maintain their health. These values have therefore little interest for the clinicians.

The second method for calculating reference values of 25OHD is to define hypovitaminosis D as 25OHD levels for which there are some adverse health effects. These reference values are called “health-based reference values.” There is an international consensus to use this kind of threshold in the case of 25OHD (Annweiler et al., 2011b). The determination of such a threshold remains yet complex because of the multiplicity of disorders caused by hypovitaminosis D. Thus, historically, “normality” was defined by the avoidance of bone adverse effects. It is classically recognized that there is no rickets or osteomalacia with serum 25OHD above 10 ng/mL (Basha et al., 2000), and no secondary hyperparathyroidism with serum 25OHD above 20 ng/mL (Basha et al., 2000). For now, the threshold at 10 ng/mL (25 nmol/L) remains consensual to define vitamin D deficiency (i.e., severe hypovitaminosis D) (Holick, 2007; Annweiler et al., 2011b), and the threshold at 20 ng/mL (50 nmol/L) is the one used by the World Health Organization to define vitamin D insufficiency (World Health Organization, 2003). However, more recently, it has been elegantly reported that serum 25OHD above 30 ng/mL (75 nmol/L) is required to prevent a number of non-bone effects (Bischoff-Ferrari et al., 2006).

However, it is not clear thus far which of these thresholds matches the eviction of neurological adverse effects of hypovitaminosis D. This would require confronting these three thresholds with the neurological effects that have been described in relation to vitamin D.

Vitamin D and the Central Nervous System: Preclinical Evidence

Vitamin D is able to enter the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and brain by crossing the blood–brain barrier via passive diffusion and additional specific carriers in the cerebral capillaries or the blood–CSF barrier in the plexus choroideus (Holmøy et al., 2009). The concentration of 25OHD in the CSF positively correlates with that in the serum under physiological conditions (Holmøy et al., 2009). In situ, vitamin D exerts most of its actions through its nuclear hormone receptor, VDR, expressed in neuronal and glial cells of the CNS, especially the hippocampus, hypothalamus, cortex, and subcortex (Kalueff and Tuohimaa, 2007; Annweiler et al., 2010a). The binding of vitamin D on the VDR triggers neuronal protection against several degenerative processes, including anti-inflammatory action (Moore et al., 2005), antioxidant effect (Ibi et al., 2001), control of calcium homeostasis by regulating the concentration of intracellular calcium in hippocampal neurons (Brewer et al., 2001), anti-atrophic effect by regulating neurotrophic agents (Brown et al., 2003), and attenuation of Aβ42 peptide accumulation (Yu et al., 2011) by stimulating the phagocytosis of Aβ peptide (Masoumi et al., 2009) together with enhancing brain-to-blood Aβ efflux transport at the blood–brain barrier (Ito et al., 2011). Moreover, vitamin D regulates the genetic expression of numerous neurotransmitters in the brain, including acetylcholine, dopamine, serotonin, and γ-aminobutyric acid, notably in the hippocampus (Kalueff and Tuohimaa, 2007). These experimentally described neurosteroid properties of vitamin D may help, in the case of normalized vitamin D status, to address against the decline of brain function in older adults, especially against cognitive decline (Annweiler and Beauchet, 2011).

Vitamin D and Cognition: Epidemiological Evidence

Most studies on this topic have been conducted in the past decade. It has become clear that older adults with Alzheimer’s disease have lower vitamin D concentrations than others (Balion et al., 2012; Annweiler et al., 2013a). Similarly, hypovitaminosis D is associated with the presence of dementia in cross-sectional studies (Buell et al., 2010). Prospective longitudinal cohorts in older adults have also reported that hypovitaminosis D predicted increased incidence of dementia after 7 years of follow-up (Annweiler et al., 2011a). In contrast, high dietary intake of vitamin D (≥800 UI/day reduced the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease after 7 years (Annweiler et al., 2012a). The relationship between vitamin D and dementia is highlighted during the advanced stages of the disease (Annweiler et al., 2011), but also from the prodromal stage (mild cognitive impairment, MCI) (Annweiler et al., 2012b) even though this minor neurocognitive disorder does not diminish the functional autonomy. Finally, the relationship between vitamin D and cognition was also found in people without dementia, with a linear relationship (Annweiler et al., 2009). The lower the concentration of vitamin D, the more impaired the cognitive performance (Oudshoorn et al., 2008). This direct association was found with both the global cognitive performance (Etgen et al., 2012), the memory, and especially with the executive functions (Annweiler et al., 2013b). In other words, vitamin D is associated with cognitive performance in older adults even before the onset of dementia. It is thus crucial to appreciate, in older adults with or without dementia, what level of vitamin D is associated with impaired cognitive scores.

Analytical Study of the Threshold of Serum 25OHD Concentration Related to Cognitive Disorders

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the studies that have explored the association of cognitive scores with hypovitaminosis D defined as serum 25OHD concentration less than either 10, 20, or 30 ng/mL.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the observational studies examining the association between cognitive scale scores and hypovitaminosis D using a threshold at 10, 20, or 30 ng/mL.

As illustrated, the results were mixed, with some studies having found an association between hypovitaminosis D and cognitive disorders (Wilkins et al., 2006; Buell et al., 2009; Annweiler et al., 2010b; Llewellyn et al., 2010, 2011; Hansen et al., 2011; Menant et al., 2012; Slinin et al., 2012), while others reported no association (Aung et al., 2006; Buell et al., 2009; Wilkins et al., 2009; Hansen et al., 2011; Menant et al., 2012). Of note, it was primarily the threshold at 10 ng/mL that was associated with cognition, whereas this was not the case with the threshold at 20 ng/mL. Additionally, the convincing results with the threshold at 30 ng/mL were constantly found in comparison with concentrations lower than 10 ng/mL, which reinforces the clinical value of the latter threshold with respect to cognition.

Highlighting that the threshold of 25OHD at 10 ng/mL is linked to cognition makes sense. Indeed, the brain is able to withstand degenerative lesions for a long time before expressing an objectified cognitive decline (Jack et al., 2010). In other words, occurrence of cognitive disorders means that the brain is already the seat of advanced neuronal damages. Precisely, since hypovitaminosis D occurs gradually, presenting with 25OHD concentration lower than 10 ng/mL means that hypovitaminosis D is chronic (Annweiler et al., 2011b), and has probably led to brain dysfunction for a long time. In line with this, it has already been shown that the lower the 25OHD concentration, the more severe the chronic diseases (Beauchet et al., 2012).

Implications for Practice and Research

The existing body of evidence provides proof that the threshold of 25OHD associated with cognitive status is around 10 ng/mL, the people with 25OHD concentration <10 ng/mL having a greater risk of cognitive disorders than those with 25OHD >10 ng/mL, and an even greater risk compared to those with 25OHD >30 ng/mL. The implications for practice and research are manifold. First, this finding supports the idea that chronic hypovitaminosis D is a risk factor for cognitive disorders, and may partially explain the onset of dementia among older adults. Second, it means that older adults with cognitive disorders likely have very low 25OHD concentrations and, thus, should receive vitamin D supplementation in clinical routine to prevent both skeletal and non-skeletal adverse consequences of hypovitaminosis D. Third, this finding is interesting for the conduct of future clinical trials testing the cognitive efficacy of vitamin D supplements. Since it is useless to give vitamin D supplements to people who already have a satisfactory rate (Annweiler and Beauchet, 2013), it appears appropriate to include in future trials only older participants who have 25OHD concentrations <10 ng/mL. Conversely, including participants with higher initial rates might mask the cognitive effects of supplementation (if any). What is more, it is legitimate to propose a supplement plan designed to achieve a final 25OHD concentration >30 ng/mL.

Conclusion

In conclusion, older adults commonly have low serum vitamin D concentrations. For clinicians, it is useful to determine the level of vitamin D required to prevent the development of diseases. Regarding cognition, existing literature provides evidence that the threshold of 25OHD associated with cognitive disorders is somewhere around 10 ng/mL. Unfortunately no study has tested yet the three classical 25OHD thresholds simultaneously in relation to cognition. Prospective multi-center population-based cohort studies are desirable to address this issue specifically with a satisfactory level of evidence.

Author Contributions

All authors meet all of the following criteria: (1) contributing to the conception and design, or analyzing and interpreting data; (2) drafting the article or revising it critically for important intellectual content; and (3) approving the final version to be published.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Annweiler, C., Allali, G., Allain, P., Bridenbaugh, S., Schott, A. M., Kressig, R. W., et al. (2009). Vitamin D and cognitive performance in adults: a systematic review. Eur. J. Neurol. 16, 1083–1089. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02755.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., and Beauchet, O. (2011). Vitamin D-mentia: randomized clinical trials should be the next step. Neuroepidemiology 37, 249–258. doi:10.1159/000334177

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., and Beauchet, O. (2013). Vitamin D and cognition: recommendations for future trials. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 61, 1049–1050. doi:10.1111/jgs.12287

CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Fantino, B., Le Gall, D., Schott, A. M., Berrut, G., and Beauchet, O. (2011). Severe vitamin D deficiency is associated with advanced-stage dementia in geriatric inpatients. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 169–171. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03166.x

CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Llewellyn, D. J., and Beauchet, O. (2013a). Low serum vitamin D concentrations in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 33, 659–674. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-121432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Montero-Odasso, M., Llewellyn, D. J., Richard-Devantoy, S., Duque, G., and Beauchet, O. (2013b). Meta-analysis of memory and executive dysfunctions in relation to vitamin D. J. Alzheimers Dis. 37, 147–171. doi:10.3233/JAD-130452

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Rolland, Y., Schott, A. M., Blain, H., Vellas, B., and Beauchet, O. (2011a). Serum vitamin D deficiency as a predictor of incident non-Alzheimer dementias: a 7-year longitudinal study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 32, 273–278. doi:10.1159/000334944

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Souberbielle, J. C., Schott, A. M., de Decker, L., Berrut, G., and Beauchet, O. (2011b). Vitamin D in the elderly: 5 points to remember. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 9, 259–267. doi:10.1684/pnv.2011.0288

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Rolland, Y., Schott, A. M., Blain, H., Vellas, B., Herrmann, F. R., et al. (2012a). Higher vitamin D dietary intake is associated with lower risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a 7-year follow-up. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 1205–1211. doi:10.1093/gerona/gls107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Fantino, B., Schott, A. M., Krolak-Salmon, P., Allali, G., and Beauchet, O. (2012b). Vitamin D insufficiency and mild cognitive impairment: cross-sectional association. Eur. J. Neurol. 19, 1023–1029. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03675.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Schott, A. M., Berrut, G., Chauviré, V., Le Gall, D., Inzitari, M., et al. (2010a). Vitamin D and ageing: neurological issues. Neuropsychobiology 62, 139–150. doi:10.1159/000318570

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Annweiler, C., Schott, A. M., Allali, G., Bridenbaugh, S. A., Kressig, R. W., Allain, P., et al. (2010b). Association of vitamin D deficiency with cognitive impairment in older women: cross-sectional study. Neurology 74, 27–32. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181beecd3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aung, K., Burnett, J., Smith, S. M., and Dyer, C. B. (2006). Vitamin D deficiency associated with self-neglect in the elderly. J. Elder Abuse Negl. 18, 63–78. doi:10.1300/J084v18n04_07

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Balion, C., Griffith, L. E., Strifler, L., Henderson, M., Patterson, C., Heckman, G., et al. (2012). Vitamin D, cognition, and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 79, 1397–1405. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826c197f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Basha, B., Rao, D. S., Han, Z. H., and Parfitt, A. M. (2000). Osteomalacia due to vitamin D depletion: a neglected consequence of intestinal malabsorption. Am. J. Med. 108, 296–300. doi:10.1016/S0002-9343(99)00460-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beauchet, O., Hélard, L., Montero-Odasso, M., de Decker, L., Berrut, G., and Annweiler, C. (2012). Hypovitaminosis D in geriatric inpatients: a marker of severity of chronic diseases. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 188–192. doi:10.3275/7838

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bischoff-Ferrari, H. A., Giovannucci, E., Willett, W. C., Dietrich, T., and Dawson-Hughes, B. (2006). Estimation of optimal serum concentrations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D for multiple health outcomes. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 84, 18–28.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Brewer, L. D., Thibault, V., Chen, K. C., Langub, M. C., Landfield, P. W., and Porter, N. M. (2001). Vitamin D hormone confers neuroprotection in parallel with downregulation of L-type calcium channel expression in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 21, 98–108.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Brown, J., Bianco, J. I., McGrath, J. J., and Eyles, D. W. (2003). 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D-3 induces nerve growth factor, promotes neurite outgrowth and inhibits mitosis in embryonic rat hippocampal neurons. Neurosci. Lett. 343, 139–143. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00303-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buell, J. S., Dawson-Hughes, B., Scott, T. M., Weiner, D. E., Dallal, G. E., Qui, W. Q., et al. (2010). 25-Hydroxyvitamin D, dementia, and cerebrovascular pathology in elders receiving home services. Neurology 74, 18–26. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181beecb7

CrossRef Full Text

Buell, J. S., Scott, T. M., Dawson-Hughes, B., Dallal, G. E., Rosenberg, I. H., Folstein, M. F., et al. (2009). Vitamin D is associated with cognitive function in elders receiving home health services. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 888–895. doi:10.1093/gerona/glp032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dusso, A. S., Brown, A. J., and Slatopolsky, E. (2005). Vitamin D. Am. J. Physiol. 289, F8–F28.

Etgen, T., Sander, D., Bickel, H., Sander, K., and Förstl, H. (2012). Vitamin D deficiency, cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 33, 297–305. doi:10.1159/000339702

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hansen, A. L., Dahl, L., Bakke, L., and Thayer, J. F. (2011). Vitamin D and executive function: a preliminary report. Percept. Mot. Skills 113, 677–685. doi:10.2466/02.09.13.15.16.PMS.113.5.677-685

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holick, M. F. (2007). Vitamin D deficiency. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 266–281. doi:10.1056/NEJMra070553

CrossRef Full Text

Holick, M. F., Matsuoka, L. Y., and Wortsman, J. (1989). Age, vitamin D, and solar ultraviolet. Lancet 2, 1104–1105. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(89)91124-0

CrossRef Full Text

Hollis, B. W., and Wagner, C. L. (2004). Vitamin D requirements during lactation: high-dose maternal supplementation as therapy to prevent hypovitaminosis D for both the mother and the nursing infant. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80, 1752S–1758S.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Holmøy, T., Moen, S. T., Gundersen, T. A., Holick, M. F., Fainardi, E., Castellazzi, M., et al. (2009). 25-Hydroxyvitamin D in cerebrospinal fluid during relapse and remission of multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 15, 1280–1285. doi:10.1177/1352458509107008

CrossRef Full Text

Ibi, M., Sawada, H., Nakanishi, M., Kume, T., Katsuki, H., Kaneko, S., et al. (2001). Protective effects of 1 alpha,25-(OH)(2)D(3) against the neurotoxicity of glutamate and reactive oxygen species in mesencephalic culture. Neuropharmacology 40, 761–771. doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00009-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, S., Ohtsuki, S., Nezu, Y., Koitabashi, Y., Murata, S., and Terasaki, T. (2011). 1α,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 enhances cerebral clearance of human amyloid-β peptide(1-40) from mouse brain across the blood-brain barrier. Fluids Barriers CNS 8, 20. doi:10.1186/2045-8118-8-20

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jack, C. R. Jr., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Shaw, L. M., Aisen, P. S., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2010). Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 9, 119–128. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalueff, A. V., and Tuohimaa, P. (2007). Neurosteroid hormone vitamin D and its utility in clinical nutrition. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 10, 12–19. doi:10.1097/MCO.0b013e328010ca18

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Llewellyn, D. J., Lang, I. A., Langa, K. M., and Melzer, D. (2011). Vitamin D and cognitive impairment in the elderly U.S. population. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 59–65. doi:10.1093/gerona/glq185

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Llewellyn, D. J., Lang, I. A., Langa, K. M., Muniz-Terrera, G., Phillips, C. L., Cherubini, A., et al. (2010). Vitamin D and risk of cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch. Intern. Med. 170, 1135–1141. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2010.173

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lo, C. W., Paris, P. W., Clemens, T. L., Nolan, J., and Holick, M. F. (1985). Vitamin D absorption in healthy subjects and in patients with intestinal malabsorption syndromes. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 42, 644–649.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Masoumi, A., Goldenson, B., Ghirmai, S., Avagyan, H., Zaghi, J., Abel, K., et al. (2009). 1Alpha,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 interacts with curcuminoids to stimulate amyloid-beta clearance by macrophages of Alzheimer’s disease patients. J. Alzheimers Dis. 17, 703–717. doi:10.3233/JAD-2009-1080

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Menant, J. C., Close, J. C., Delbaere, K., Sturnieks, D. L., Trollor, J., Sachdev, P. S., et al. (2012). Relationships between serum vitamin D levels, neuromuscular and neuropsychological function and falls in older men and women. Osteoporos. Int. 23, 981–989. doi:10.1007/s00198-011-1637-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moore, M. E., Piazza, A., McCartney, Y., and Lynch, M. A. (2005). Evidence that vitamin D3 reverses age-related inflammatory changes in the rat hippocampus. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 33, 573–577. doi:10.1042/BST0330573

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oudshoorn, C., Mattace-Raso, F. U., van der Velde, N., Colin, E. M., and van der Cammen, T. J. (2008). Higher serum vitamin D3 levels are associated with better cognitive test performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25, 539–543. doi:10.1159/000134382

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slinin, Y., Paudel, M., Taylor, B. C., Ishani, A., Rossom, R., Yaffe, K., et al. (2012). Association between serum 25(OH) vitamin D and the risk of cognitive decline in older women. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 1092–1098. doi:10.1093/gerona/gls075

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilkins, C. H., Birge, S. J., Sheline, Y. I., and Morris, J. C. (2009). Vitamin D deficiency is associated with worse cognitive performance and lower bone density in older African Americans. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 101, 349–354.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wilkins, C. H., Sheline, Y. I., Roe, C. M., Birge, S. J., and Morris, J. C. (2006). Vitamin D deficiency is associated with low mood and worse cognitive performance in older adults. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 1032–1040. doi:10.1097/01.JGP.0000240986.74642.7c

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

World Health Organization. (2003). Prevention and Management of Osteoporosis: Report of a WHO Scientific Group. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Yu, J., Gattoni-Celli, M., Zhu, H., Bhat, N. R., Sambamurti, K., Gattoni-Celli, S., et al. (2011). Vitamin D3-enriched diet correlates with a decrease of amyloid plaques in the brain of AbetaPP transgenic mice. J. Alzheimers Dis. 25, 295–307. doi:10.3233/JAD-2011-101986

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhou, C., Assem, M., Tay, J. C., Blumberg, B., Schuetz, E. G., and Thummel, K. E. (2006). Steroid and xenobiotic receptor and vitamin D receptor crosstalk mediates CYP24 expression and drug-induced osteomalacia. J. Clin. Invest. 116, 1703–1712. doi:10.1172/JCI27793

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: cognition, dementia, older adults, standard, threshold, vitamin D

Citation: Annweiler C and Beauchet O (2014) Vitamin D in older adults: the need to specify standard values with respect to cognition. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:72. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00072

Received: 12 January 2014; Accepted: 31 March 2014;
Published online: 15 April 2014.

Edited by:

Claudia Perez-Cruz, Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Avanzados, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Carlos Beas-Zarate, Universidad de Guadalajra Mexico, Mexico

Copyright: © 2014 Annweiler and Beauchet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: ceannweiler@chu-angers.fr

Download