Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 18 March 2015
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 7 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00028

Differences in temporal order memory among young, middle-aged, and older adults may depend on the level of interference

Lindsay J. Rotblatt1 Catherine A. Sumida1 Emily J. Van Etten1 Eva Pirogovsky Turk2,3 Jerlyn C. Tolentino1 Paul E. Gilbert1,4*
  • 1Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
  • 3Veterans Affairs San Diego Health Care System, Research Service, San Diego, CA, USA
  • 4San Diego State University—University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, CA, USA

Age-related changes in temporal order memory have been well documented in older adults; however, little is known about this ability during middle age. We tested healthy young, middle-aged, and older adults on a previously published visuospatial temporal order memory test involving high and low interference conditions. When interference was low, young and middle-aged adults did not differ, but both groups significantly outperformed older adults. However, when interference was high, significant differences were found among all three age groups. The data provide evidence that temporal order memory may begin to decline in middle age, particularly when temporal interference is high.

Introduction

Age-related deficits in memory for the temporal order of items or events in a sequence have been well documented in older humans (Newman et al., 2001; Kessels et al., 2007; Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008; Ulbrich et al., 2009; Blachstein et al., 2012; Tolentino et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2014). Two recent studies demonstrated that temporal interference may play a key role in the ability of older adults to remember sequences of stimuli (Tolentino et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2014). As reviewed by Kesner and Hopkins (2006), studies have shown that items occurring further apart in a temporal sequence are easier to remember than items that are temporally adjacent using various tests in both humans and animals. This is hypothesized to occur because there is more interference between temporally proximal stimuli in a sequence than temporally distant stimuli (Gilbert et al., 2001; Kesner and Hopkins, 2006; Tolentino et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2014).

Tolentino et al. (2012) demonstrated that temporal order memory for fixed and random sequences of visuospatial stimuli was impaired in non-demented older adults compared to young adults, particularly when stimuli were close together in the sequence and temporal interference was high. A recent study by Roberts et al. (2014) tested young and older adults on a novel temporal discrimination test involving visually distinct objects with a wider range of temporal distances between stimuli relative to the study by Tolentino et al. (2012). Although there was individual variability among older adults, Roberts et al. (2014) reported that older adults were impaired relative to young adults on the temporal order task at a group level. One group of older adults was found to show a general impairment in temporal order memory compared to young adults when interference was moderate or low. However, the other group was impaired only when interference was moderate. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that the ability of older adults to remember sequences of stimuli may depend on the level of temporal interference.

Although age-related changes in temporal order memory have been studied rather extensively in older adults, little is known about temporal order memory abilities during middle age. A study by Stark et al. (2013) found that the ability to discriminate between highly similar visual objects begins to decrease in middle age. Based on this finding, it is possible that middle-aged adults also may experience difficulty when discriminating between stimuli close together in time due to increased interference. An impaired ability to remember sequences of stimuli and/or events when temporal interference is increased could have a significant impact of a variety of daily tasks. Therefore, the present study sought to examine the effects of interference on temporal order memory in young, middle-aged, and older adults. The present study tested two specific a priori hypotheses. First, it was hypothesized that when temporal interference is high, temporal order memory would be significantly better in young adults compared to both middle-aged and older adults. Second, it was hypothesized that when interference is low, temporal order memory would be significantly better in young and middle-aged adults compared to older adults. However, no differences were expected between young and middle-aged adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants consisted of 60 young adults 18–25 years of age, 43 middle-aged adults 40–55 years of age, and 43 non-demented older adults 65 years of age or older (demographic data summarized in Table 1). A subset of the data was included in previous publications (Tolentino et al., 2012; Nicoll et al., 2013; Woods et al., 2013). The average Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis, 1976) score for older adults was 135.52 (SE = 1.13) and all older adults scored above 123, providing evidence that none of the participants showed symptoms of dementia. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant trend for years of education completed, F(2,143) = 4.467, p = 0.06. As shown in Table 1, the older adults completed the highest years of education, followed by middle-aged adults. Therefore, age-related deficits in temporal order memory in middle-aged or older adults are not due to lower education levels in these groups. A chi-square analysis revealed a slight trend for gender difference among the three groups, χ2 (2, N = 146) = 4.94, p = 0.09. When gender or education was entered into the analyses discussed below, the variable was not significant in the model nor did the variable interact with age group; therefore, it was not included in the final analyses. All participants provided informed consent prior to participation approved by San Diego State University and the University of California, San Diego.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean (standard error of the mean) demographic variables for young adults (YA), middle-aged adults (MA), and older adults (OA).

Temporal Order Memory Test

Temporal order memory for random sequences of visuospatial stimuli was assessed using a well-characterized, computer-based task (Pirogovsky et al., 2009; Tolentino et al., 2012; Nicoll et al., 2013; Woods et al., 2013). Participants viewed a computerized radial eight-arm maze (approximately 30 cm in diameter) on a monitor. The maze consisted of eight arms extending from a center, similar to spokes on a wheel. Participants were told that a circle would appear at the end of each arm, one at a time, in a random order. The experimenter instructed the participants to remember the sequence in which the circles were presented on the arms.

Each trial consisted of a sample phase followed by a choice phase. During the sample phase, a gray circle (3-cm diameter) appeared randomly at the end of one of the eight arms on the maze. The circle appeared for 2 s, followed by a 2 s gray mask to eliminate after-image effects. Then another gray circle appeared at the end of a different, randomly selected arm for 2 s followed by another 2 s gray mask. This procedure continued until a circle was presented at the end of each of the eight arms once. The random sequence varied for each trial. Following the sample phase, the participant was presented simultaneously with two circles for 5 s, one at the end of one study phase arm and the other at the end of another study phase arm. The participant was asked to indicate which circle appeared earlier in the sequence. Temporal separation lags involving high (0 lag and 2 lag) and low (4 lag and 6 lag) levels of interference were randomly selected for each choice phase. The temporal lag represented the number of circles that appeared during the sample phase sequence between the two circles presented in the choice phase. For example, a 4 lag separation trial would consist of two choice phase circles that occurred with four circles between them during the sample phase sequence (e.g., 1st circle vs. 6th circle presented). Following each sample phase sequence, three choice phases were conducted involving three of the four temporal separations that were counterbalanced across sequences. Sixteen different sample phase sequences were presented with three choice phases for each sequence. There were a total of 12 choice phase trials for each of the four temporal separations. A 15 s inter-trial interval was implemented between each trial.

As reviewed above, it was hypothesized that there was more interference and a greater need to separate temporally proximal circles on 0 and 2 lag trials than temporally distant circles on 4 and 6 lag trials. In order to compare the performance of the three age groups on high interference and low interference trials, 0 and 2 lag trials were averaged and constituted the high interference condition and 4 and 6 lag trials were averaged to constitute the low interference condition.

Statistical Analyses

ANOVA tests were conducted to examine the performance of young, middle-aged, and older adults on the high and low interference conditions. A Bonferroni correction was applied to control for multiple comparisons and the p-value for significance was set at p < 0.025. Tukey post hoc comparison tests were used to analyze group differences and effect size estimates were calculated using Cohen’s d.

Results

In the low interference condition, the analysis revealed a significant difference among the age groups, F(2,143) = 10.943, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.13. As shown in Figure 1, a post hoc Tukey test revealed that at low levels of interference, both the young (p < 0.05, d = 0.80) and middle-aged (p < 0.05, d = 0.59) groups significantly outperformed the older adult group. However, there was not a significant difference between the performance of the young and the middle-aged groups. In the high interference condition, the analysis also revealed a significant difference among the age groups, F(2,143) = 14.497, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.17. As shown in Figure 1, a Tukey test revealed that the performance of all three groups differed at high levels of interference. The young group significantly outperformed both the middle-aged (p < 0.05, d = 0.53) and older groups (p < 0.05, d = 1.28). In addition, the middle-aged group significantly outperformed the older group (p < 0.05, d = 0.65).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean percent correct responses for young, middle-aged, and older adults on low and high temporal interference trials of the temporal order memory task. *Indicates significance difference of p < 0.001.

Discussion

The finding that older adults are impaired relative to young adults on a temporal order task is consistent with prior studies (Newman et al., 2001; Kessels et al., 2007; Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008; Ulbrich et al., 2009; Blachstein et al., 2012; Tolentino et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2014). However, not all studies have found temporal order memory to be impaired in older adults (Perlmutter et al., 1981; Sekuler et al., 2006). As discussed in a recent paper published by Roberts et al. (2014), there are a number of differences between these studies that may have contributed to the discrepant findings (e.g., response times, exposure times, types of stimuli, temporal distance between pairs). The current findings also are consistent with recent studies by Tolentino et al. (2012) and Roberts et al. (2014), suggesting that temporal order memory abilities in older adults may be influenced by the level of interference involved in the task. The present findings demonstrate that older adults are impaired relative to young adults on a temporal order task when interference is high or low. However, the effect size associated with group differences between young and older adults was 62.5% higher in the high interference condition (d = 1.28) relative to the low interference condition (d = 0.80). This finding provides evidence that temporal order memory in older adults is poorer when interference is high.

The findings also provide novel insight into temporal order memory abilities during middle age. The data demonstrate that temporal order memory does not differ between middle-aged and young adults when temporal interference is low. However, deficits in temporal order memory can be detected in middle-aged adults, relative to young adults, when temporal interference is high using the current measure. The finding that middle-aged adults did not differ from young adults in the low interference condition, coupled with the greater effect associated with differences between young and older adults in the high interference condition compared to the low interference condition demonstrate that temporal order memory was differentially affected by interference levels. Taken together, these findings provide evidence that the observed age-related differences were not due solely to a general memory deficit.

Temporal order memory is thought to depend on frontal and temporal lobe function (Devito and Eichenbaum, 2011; Ekstrom et al., 2011). Humans with damage to the frontal (Milner et al., 1985; Shimamura et al., 1990; Daum and Mayes, 2000) or temporal lobes (Hopkins et al., 1995; Mayes et al., 2001; Spiers et al., 2001; Downes et al., 2002) show deficits on temporal order tasks. In addition functional neuroimaging studies have recorded activation in the frontal (Cabeza et al., 2000; Rowe and Passingham, 2001; Hayes et al., 2004; Knutson et al., 2004) and temporal lobes (Ekstrom and Bookheimer, 2007; Lehn et al., 2009) when humans perform tasks involving sequences of stimuli. Neuroimaging data indicate that the prefrontal cortex may be more involved in encoding of temporal information relative to the retrieval (Duarte et al., 2010). A recent study reported that cognitive decline during middle age may be mediated by gray matter changes in brain regions including the frontal lobes (Ferreira et al., 2014). Taken together, it is hypothesized that age-related changes in the frontal lobes of middle-aged adults may result in poorer encoding of temporally ordered stimuli resulting in impairments on the present task when stimuli are closer together in a sequence and temporal interference is high. However, when temporal interference is lessened and choice stimuli are further apart in the sequence, the encoding in middle-aged adults may be sufficient enough to make more accurate recency judgments.

Pattern separation is a mechanism for separating partially overlapping patterns of activation so that one pattern may be retrieved as separate from other patterns. A pattern separation mechanism may reduce interference among similar memory representations and increase the likelihood of accurate encoding and retrieval (Gilbert and Brushfield, 2009). The dentate gyrus (DG) and CA3 hippocampal subregions have been reported to support pattern separation (Kesner, 2007; Gilbert and Brushfield, 2009; Rolls, 2010; Yassa and Stark, 2011; Schmidt et al., 2012). Age-related changes in these subregions have been hypothesized to result in less efficient pattern separation due to strengthened processing of stored information at the expense of processing new information (Wilson et al., 2006; Yassa and Stark, 2011). Neurogenesis within the DG subregion also has been hypothesized to play a role in pattern separation (Aimone et al., 2011; Sahay et al., 2011; Luu et al., 2012) and there is evidence that the reductions in neurogenesis in old animals may be related to decreased hippocampal volume and impaired performance on hippocampal dependent tasks (Driscoll et al., 2006). One possible interpretation of the present findings is that pattern separation for temporal sequences is less efficient in middle-aged and older adults. Recent studies have hypothesized that pattern separation for temporal information may be impaired in older adults (Tolentino et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2014). In the current study, the pattern separation demands are hypothesized to be greatest in the high interference condition, involving trials with the shortest temporal separation lags. Given that both middle-aged and older adults showed significant deficits relative to young adults in the high interference condition, the data provide evidence that less efficient pattern separation may contribute to age-related changes in temporal order memory. There is a growing body of literature providing evidence that older adults may be impaired relative to young adults on behavioral tasks hypothesized to tax pattern separation (Toner et al., 2009; Stark et al., 2010, 2013; Yassa et al., 2011a,b; Holden et al., 2012; Tolentino et al., 2012; Ly et al., 2013; Reagh et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2014). In addition, a recent study by Stark et al. (2013) reported middle-aged adults were impaired on a behavioral pattern separation task involving visual objects. The present findings provide preliminary evidence that middle-aged adults also may show impairments consistent with a pattern separation deficit on a temporal order memory test.

Although future studies are needed with larger samples, the current findings offer unique insight into the effects of interference on temporal order memory during middle and old age. The findings show that age-related temporal order memory deficits may be detectable as early as middle age when a test is used that involves elevated levels of temporal interference. A recent study reported that impaired temporal order memory may be a selective behavioral marker of Alzheimer’s disease (Bellassen et al., 2012). In addition, temporal order memory deficits have been reported in older adults diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (Gillis et al., 2013), which has been described as a transitional stage between normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Therefore, the present findings potentially may have both basic science and clinical implications.

Funding

Grant sponsor: National Institutes of Health; Grant number: 1RO1AG034202.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01AG034202 awarded to PEG. EJVE was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R25AG043364. The authors thank all of the participants for their contributions to this study.

References

Aimone, J. B., Deng, W., and Gage, F. H. (2011). Resolving new memories: a critical look at the dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis and pattern separation. Neuron 70, 589–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellassen, V., Iglói, K., de Souza, L. C., Dubois, B., and Rondi-Reig, L. (2012). Temporal order memory assessed during spatiotemporal navigation as a behavioral cognitive marker for differential Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis. J. Neurosci. 32, 1942–1952. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4556-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blachstein, H., Greenstein, Y., and Vakil, E. (2012). Aging and temporal order memory: a comparison of direct and indirect measures. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 34, 107–112. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.625352

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Houle, S., Mangels, J. A., and Nyberg, L. (2000). Age-related differences in neural activity during item and temporal-order memory retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 197–206. doi: 10.1162/089892900561832

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daum, I., and Mayes, A. R. (2000). Memory and executive function impairments after frontal or posterior cortex lesions. Behav. Neurol. 12, 161–173. doi: 10.1155/2000/327304

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devito, L. M., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Memory for the order of events in specific sequences: contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 3169–3175. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4202-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downes, J. J., Mayes, A. R., MacDonald, C., and Hunkin, N. M. (2002). Temporal order memory in patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome and medial temporal amnesia. Neuropsychologia 40, 853–861. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00172-5

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Howard, S. R., Stone, J. C., Monfils, M. H., Tomanek, B., Brooks, W. M., et al. (2006). The aging hippocampus: a multi-level analysis in the rat. Neuroscience 139, 1173–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.01.040

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duarte, A., Henson, R. N., Knight, R. T., Emery, T., and Graham, K. S. (2010). Orbito-frontal cortex is necessary for temporal context memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1819–1831. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21316

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, A. D., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2007). Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. Learn. Mem. 14, 645–654. doi: 10.1101/lm.575107

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, A. D., Copara, M. S., Isham, E. A., Wang, W. C., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2011). Dissociable networks involved in spatial and temporal order source retrieval. Neuroimage 56, 1803–1813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.033

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, D., Molina, Y., Machado, A., Westman, E., Wahlund, L. O., Nieto, A., et al. (2014). Cognitive decline is mediated by gray matter changes during middle age. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 1086–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.095

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. E., and Brushfield, A. M. (2009). The role of the CA3 hippocampal subregion in spatial memory: a process oriented behavioral assessment. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.03.037

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. E., Kesner, R. P., and Lee, I. (2001). Dissociating hippocampal subregions: double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus 11, 626–636. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1077

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillis, M. M., Quinn, K. M., Phillips, P. A., and Hampstead, B. M. (2013). Impaired retention is responsible for temporal order memory deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 143, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. M., Ryan, L., Schnyer, D. M., and Nadel, L. (2004). An fMRI study of episodic memory: retrieval of object, spatial and temporal information. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 885–896. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.885

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holden, H. M., Hoebel, C., Loftis, K., and Gilbert, P. E. (2012). Spatial pattern separation in cognitively normal young and older adults. Hippocampus 22, 1826–1832. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22017

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopkins, R. O., Kesner, R. P., and Goldstein, M. (1995). Item and order recognition memory in subjects with hypoxic brain injury. Brain Cogn. 27, 180–201. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1995.1016

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesner, R. P. (2007). A behavioral analysis of dentate gyrus function. Prog. Brain Res. 163, 567–576. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(07)63030-1

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesner, R. P., and Hopkins, R. O. (2006). Mnemonic functions of the hippocampus: a comparison between animals and humans. Biol. Psychol. 73, 3–18. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessels, R. P., Hobbel, D., and Postma, A. (2007). Aging, context memory and binding: a comparison of “what, where and when” in young and older adults. Int. J. Neurosci. 117, 795–810. doi: 10.1080/00207450600910218

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, K. M., Wood, J. N., and Grafman, J. (2004). Brain activation in processing temporal sequence: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 1299–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehn, H., Steffenach, H. A., van Strien, N. M., Veltman, D. J., Witter, M. P., and Håberg, A. K. (2009). A specific role of the human hippocampus in recall of temporal sequences. J. Neurosci. 29, 3475–3484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5370-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luu, P., Sill, O. C., Gao, L., Becker, S., Wojtowicz, J. M., and Smith, D. M. (2012). The role of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in reducing interference. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 381–391. doi: 10.1037/a0028252

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, M., Murray, E., and Yassa, M. A. (2013). Perceptual versus conceptual interference and pattern separation of verbal stimuli in young and older adults. Hippocampus 23, 425–430. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22110

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattis, S. (1976). Geriatric Psychiatry: A Handbook for Psychiatrists and Primary Care Physicians. New York: Grune and Stratton Inc.

Mayes, A. R., Isaac, C. L., Holdstock, J. S., Hunkin, N. M., Montaldi, D., Downes, J. J., et al. (2001). Memory for single items, word pairs and temporal order of different kinds in a patient with selective hippocampal lesions. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 18, 97–123. doi: 10.1080/02643290125897

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, B., Petrides, M., and Smith, M. L. (1985). Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of memory. Hum. Neurobiol. 4, 137–142.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, M. C., Allen, J. J. B., and Kaszniak, A. W. (2001). Tasks for assessing memory for temporal order versus memory for items in aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 8, 72–78. doi: 10.1076/anec.8.1.72.849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicoll, D. R., Pirogovsky, E., Collazo, A. E., Tierney, S. M., Corey-Bloom, J., and Gilbert, P. E. (2013). The effect of interference on temporal order memory in premanifest and manifest Huntington’s disease. J. Huntingtons Dis. 2, 297–304. doi: 10.3233/JHD-130064

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Old, S. R., and Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Memory for people and their actions: further evidence for an age-related associative deficit. Psychol. Aging 23, 467–472. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.467

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlmutter, M., Metzger, R., Nezworski, T., and Miller, K. (1981). Spatial and temporal memory in 20 to 60 year olds. J. Gerontol. 36, 59–65. doi: 10.1093/geronj/36.1.59

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirogovsky, E., Goldstein, J., Peavy, G., Jacobson, M. W., Corey-Bloom, J., and Gilbert, P. E. (2009). Temporal order memory deficits prior to clinical diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 662–670. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990427

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reagh, Z. M., Roberts, J. M., Ly, M., Diprospero, N., Murray, E., and Yassa, M. A. (2014). Spatial discrimination deficits as a function of mnemonic interference in aged adults with and without memory impairment. Hippocampus 24, 303–314. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22224

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, J. M., Ly, M., Murray, E., and Yassa, M. A. (2014). Temporal discrimination deficits as a function of lag interference in older adults. Hippocampus 24, 1189–1196. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22303

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2010). A computational theory of episodic memory formation in the hippocampus. Behav. Brain Res. 215, 180–196. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.027

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, J. B., and Passingham, R. E. (2001). Working memory for location and time: activity in prefrontal area 46 relates to selection rather than maintenance in memory. Neuroimage 14, 77–86. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0784

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahay, A., Wilson, D. A., and Hen, R. (2011). Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70, 582–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, B., Marrone, D. F., and Markus, E. J. (2012). Disambiguating the similar: the dentate gyrus and pattern separation. Behav. Brain Res. 226, 56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.039

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekuler, R., McLaughlin, C., Kahana, M. J., Wingfield, A., and Yotsumoto, Y. (2006). Short-term visual recognition and temporal order memory are both well-preserved in aging. Psychol. Aging 21, 632–637. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.632

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., and Squire, L. R. (1990). Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 28, 803–813. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90004-8

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiers, H. J., Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., Baxendale, S. A., Hartley, T., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2001). Unilateral temporal lobectomy patients show lateralized topographical and episodic memory deficits in a virtual town. Brain 124, 2476–2489. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.12.2476

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stark, S. M., Yassa, M. A., Lacy, J. W., and Stark, C. E. (2013). A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 51, 2442–2449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.014

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stark, S. M., Yassa, M. A., and Stark, C. E. (2010). Individual differences in spatial pattern separation performance associated with healthy aging in humans. Learn. Mem. 17, 284–288. doi: 10.1101/lm.1768110

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolentino, J. C., Pirogovsky, E., Luu, T., Toner, C. K., and Gilbert, P. E. (2012). The effect of interference on temporal order memory for random and fixed sequences in nondemented older adults. Learn. Mem. 19, 251–255. doi: 10.1101/lm.026062.112

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toner, C. K., Pirogovsky, E., Kirwan, C. B., and Gilbert, P. E. (2009). Visual object pattern separation deficits in nondemented older adults. Learn. Mem. 16, 338–342. doi: 10.1101/lm.1315109

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulbrich, P., Churan, J., Fink, M., and Wittmann, M. (2009). Perception of temporal order: the effects of age, sex and cognitive factors. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 16, 183–202. doi: 10.1080/13825580802411758

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, I. A., Gallagher, M., Eichenbaum, H., and Tanila, H. (2006). Neurocognitive aging: prior memories hinder new hippocampal encoding. Trends Neurosci. 29, 662–670. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, S. P., Hoebel, C., Pirogovsky, E., Rooney, A., Cameron, M. V., Grant, I., et al. (2013). Visuospatial temporal order memory deficits in older adults with HIV infection. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 26, 171–180. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000013

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yassa, M. A., Lacy, J. W., Stark, S. M., Albert, M. S., Gallagher, M., and Stark, C. E. (2011a). Pattern separation deficits associated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus activity in nondemented older adults. Hippocampus 21, 968–979. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20808

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yassa, M. A., Mattfeld, A. T., Stark, S. M., and Stark, C. E. (2011b). Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 8873–8878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101567108

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yassa, M. A., and Stark, C. E. (2011). Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci. 34, 515–525. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: temporal order, interference, aging, middle age, pattern separation

Citation: Rotblatt LJ, Sumida CA, Van Etten EJ, Turk EP, Tolentino JC and Gilbert PE (2015) Differences in temporal order memory among young, middle-aged, and older adults may depend on the level of interference. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:28. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00028

Received: 13 November 2014; Accepted: 23 February 2015;
Published online: 18 March 2015.

Edited by:

P. Hemachandra Reddy, Texas Tech University, USA

Reviewed by:

J. Martin Wojtowicz, University of Toronto, Canada
Qitao Ran, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, USA

Copyright © 2015 Rotblatt, Sumida, Van Etten, Turk, Tolentino and Gilbert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paul E. Gilbert, San Diego State University—University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Court, Suite 103, San Diego, CA 92120, USA Tel: (619) 594-7409 pgilbert@mail.sdsu.edu

Download