Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 April 2014
Sec. Psychology of Language

Literacy shapes thought: the case of event representation in different cultures

  • 1Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Münster, Germany
  • 2Department of Experimental Psychology, European Medical School, Carl von Ossietzky University, Oldenburg, Germany
  • 3Institute for Psychology, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Münster, Germany

There has been a lively debate whether conceptual representations of actions or scenes follow a left-to-right spatial transient when participants depict such events or scenes. It was even suggested that conceptualizing the agent on the left side represents a universal. We review the current literature with an emphasis on event representation and on cross-cultural studies. While there is quite some evidence for spatial bias for representations of events and scenes in diverse cultures, their extent and direction depend on task demands, one‘s native language, and importantly, on reading and writing direction. Whether transients arise only in subject-verb-object languages, due to their linear sentential position of event participants, is still an open issue. We investigated a group of illiterate speakers of Yucatec Maya, a language with a predominant verb-object-subject structure. They were compared to illiterate native speakers of Spanish. Neither group displayed a spatial transient. Given the current literature, we argue that learning to read and write has a strong impact on representations of actions and scenes. Thus, while it is still under debate whether language shapes thought, there is firm evidence that literacy does.

The influence of language on perception and thought is one of the most fascinating topics in the cognitive sciences and troubles researchers at least since Sapir and Whorf (Whorf, 1956). In their view, termed linguistic relativity, learning and knowing a language has a profound impact on cognitive domains such as basic perception (for recent reviews see Regier and Kay, 2009; Lupyan, 2012), recognition memory (e.g., Fausey and Boroditsky, 2011) or spatial processing (e.g., Levinson, 2003a,b). These results did not remain undisputed and were objected by other influential researchers (e.g., Pinker, 1994; Li and Gleitman, 2002). Some argue that language follows universal principles and that semantics are based on an innate “language of thought” (Fodor, 1975). Thus, the relation of language and thought is still hotly debated (e.g., Gentner and Goldin-Meadow, 2003), with the general favor swinging back and forth in one or the other direction.

One way to tackle this controversy is to investigate whether representations of actions or static scenes follow a spatial direction and if such bias depends on language, that is, on the (syntactically constrained) order of mention of scene protagonists in the sentence. The idea that action representations have a spatial direction was inspired by an aphasic patient, who assigned the agent role to the left figure in action events, independent whether this figure was the agent or the patient in the action. This finding was corroborated by data from a sentence-to-picture matching task, where his performance was best when the agent was depicted on the left, and the action-transient went from left to right (Maher et al., 1993; Chatterjee et al., 1995). In follow-up studies, this finding generalized to native speakers of English, who displayed a similar left-to-right action transient when depicting action events (Chatterjee et al., 1995, 1999). Thus, with respect to the tight connection between language and space, this agent-left preference might constitute a universal characteristic, present in the vast majority of the world's languages (Chatterjee, 2001) and maybe even influencing artistic works (Chatterjee, 2002) or calling fouls in soccer (Kranjec et al., 2010). Such a left-to-right order was not only found for action events involving animate agents, but also in scenes consisting of arrangements of inanimate objects (Jahn et al., 2007). Jahn and colleagues proposed that listeners build a mental model of first-mentioned objects (e.g., “A table is between the TV and a chair”) and then try to spatially integrate the next-mentioned objects (e.g., “The light is on the left of the TV”). Their data suggest that first-mentioned objects are represented in a left-to-right fashion in the preferred initial model. The authors also assumed that this spatial bias appears as a consequence of the habitual reading and writing direction (RWD). There is already a lot of evidence that RWD has an impact on a rather large number of cognitive processes involving both low-level skills and higher-level representations and processes. Examples are perceptual span (Pollatsek et al., 1981), scanning direction (Chokron and Imbert, 1993), representations of numbers (Dehaene et al., 1993), time (Tversky et al., 1991) and aesthetic preferences (Chokron and De Agostini, 2000; for a more comprehensive list see Román et al., 2013). Presenting a review about directionality effects in various domains and tasks, Vaid (2010) concludes that a motoric account which is influenced by RWD is the most parsimonious explanation.

Obviously, possible confounds of RWD have to be taken into account when investigating the agent-left bias. With tasks very similar to Chatterjee et al. (1995, 1999), Maass and Russo (2003) compared Italian and Arab speakers. Experience of Arab speakers ranged from exclusive exposure to right-to-left RWD (Arabic), to studying in a culture with left-to-right RWD (Italy). The results clearly demonstrated that RWD heavily influences the tendency to conceptualize agents on the left or right of scenes. Italians located them on the left, while Arabs with exclusive right-to-left RWD placed them on the right. The amount of experience with a particular RWD modulated this tendency in an almost linear fashion. Nevertheless, the left-to-right tendency was more strongly expressed in Italians than the right-to-left tendency in Arabs with exclusive experience of this RWD. Thus, spatial predispositions for agents might be innate, but they are strongly modulated by cultural factors. Very similar results were found for Spanish and Arab speakers who had to interpret and draw descriptions of static scenes (Román et al., 2013).

However, the tendency to place agents on the left in cultures with left-to-right RWD and on the right in right-to-left RWD cultures was not reported in all studies. Altmann et al. (2006) presented English and Arabic speakers with action verbs in either active or passive voice (e.g., “chase” or “is chased”), and asked them to subsequently draw stick figures illustrating the action. In contrast to earlier studies, participants from both cultures neither displayed an effect of RWD nor of active/passive voice. English speakers drew the agent more often on the right side when sketching passive verbs, but no other biases were found.

How can this contradictory finding be accounted for? One reason may be the format of linguistic input: single verbs or verbs embedded in sentences. In an earlier study, we only found a spatial transient for verbs embedded in sentence context (Dobel et al., 2011). Thus, to observe a spatial bias, the linguistic input must have the format of a sentence or, alternatively, an ordered list of objects. Altmann et al. (2006) surmised that the lack of a spatial bias was due to their participants interpreting the verbs more statically than dynamically. This seems unlikely given the literature on static scenes, where spatial preferences were reported (e.g., Román et al., 2013). Furthermore, action verbs by themselves are dynamic in nature, as they always imply a change of state (for an overview see Dobel et al., 2010).

To our knowledge, the only other study that reported no consistent spatial bias investigated Korean participants that had either learned to read vertically from right-to-left, or horizontally from left-to-right (Barrett et al., 2002). These participants also drew images in response to sentences describing action events. An explanation for the null finding for participants with vertical right-to-left RWD might be that they were considerably older (> 60 years of age) than the control group. In fact, they were older than all other groups reported so far. There is indeed evidence that spatial biases, as measured with line-bisection tasks, disappear in older participants (Jewell and McCourt, 2000). Another explanation might be that the vertical reading direction conflicted with the horizontal drawing direction, and thus failed to induce a consistent transient. Barrett et al. (2002) assumed that the horizontal left-to-right RWD group was exposed to more than one scanning direction when learning to read, and that this failed to evoke a spatial bias. This explanation is supported by studies on participants subjected to variable amounts of experience with a specific RWD (Maass and Russo, 2003; Román et al., 2013). Both studies found that mixed experience with different RWDs leads to weaker spatial preferences.

From the perspective of innateness, one might predict a spatial bias for placing agents left to be present in preschoolers, who have far less exposure to a prominent RWD than adults. We addressed this question and found opposing biases in adult speakers of German and Hebrew, as already predicted by earlier studies. However, these spatial preferences were absent in illiterate preschoolers (Dobel et al., 2007). We also manipulated the order in which agents and patients (for transitives) or agents and recipients (for ditransitives) were presented by comparing active and passive voice. This manipulation did not influence Hebrew speakers, but German preschoolers and adults were affected by order-of-mention: first-mentioned event participants were more likely to be placed left.

Furthermore, although Hebrew is written from right to left, it might not be the best language to investigate directionality effects. Letters are written from left-to-right as is arithmetic and musical notation. As such a comparison of speakers of languages such as Urdu (right-to-left) and Hindi (left-to-right) might evidence even stronger directional biases than a comparison of speakers of Hebrew and some Germanic language (Vaid, 1995).

Using a different approach, we obtained further evidence against a universal predisposition with speakers of German Sign Language (DGS), who actually use space to convey meaning (Dobel et al., 2011). In signed language, a spatial preference might be more readily visible than in spoken language. As in the studies above, participants read event descriptions, and illustrated their interpretation either by drawing or arranging toy figures. We tested two types of verbs that differ in the way they are signed. Verbs with a horizontal transient are typically signed in a left-to-right manner, seen from the addressee's point of view (e.g., as in “giving” where the agent is typically signed to the right of the signer and the patient to the left). Verbs with sagittal transients, in contrast, are signed moving away from, or toward the speaker (e.g., as in “pushing,” where the direction of movement iconically represents onset and end points of a physical motion, assigning semantic, and syntactic roles to these points). For horizontal transients, signers placed agents at the same position in space as in the signed message. They thus showed a direct mapping preference for verbs with horizontal transients, by putting agents at the same position in space as in the signed message. This effect was not present for verbs following a sagittal transient. Taken together, spatial preferences in building conceptual-semantic representations are modulated by cultural and language-related factors, as well as by short-lived situational factors such as grammar.

Although these studies seem to deliver a clear-cut message, we wish to point out some caveats. First, as stressed by Román et al. (2013), all studies so far investigated speakers of languages with a dominant subject-verb-object (SVO) word order. We demonstrated that word order biases the spatial representation of actions (Dobel et al., 2007). A simple first-in-first-out strategy and representing events as they appeared follows pragmatic principles (Grice, 1975) and thus leads to a spatial arrangement of event participants. Second, the claim that learning to read and write induces spatial transients for action representations needs support from illiterate adults, not only from preschoolers. Even if consistent action transients are innate, they might reach their full manifestation only in adulthood and/or only after experience with reading and writing sets the necessary parameter for a spatial transient. To fill some of these gaps, we investigated monolingual speakers of Yucatec Maya, a language spoken by about 800.000–1.2 million people in the Yucatán peninsula, a south-eastern state in Mexico (Lewis, 2009). The most important characteristic for our purposes is that Yucatec Maya has a predominant verb-object-subject (VOS) word order (e.g., England, 1991; Bolles and Bolles, 2004). The passive voice is a common structure in this language (Verhoeven, 2007).

Twelve Yucatec speakers (mean age = 57 years; 4 men) were compared to 12 monolingual speakers of Spanish (mean age = 57; 3 men), from the state of Veracruz (Eastern part of Mexico). All participants were right handed (Oldfield, 1971). Importantly, members of both language groups received no formal schooling and were illiterate. As in earlier studies, participants were asked to draw or to arrange toy figures in response to spoken descriptions of action events. Sentences were read in active or passive voice, and described actions toward or away from an agent, with eight sentences in each condition (see Examples). There were 64 trials for each participant consisting of 16 different actions (comprising transitive and ditransitive verbs) with the transient of movement either away or toward the agent (e.g., away: to kick, to push; toward: to pull, to take). Each sentence was presented in active and passive voice and the content of each sentence had to be drawn or little toy figures had to be arranged. Actors were “woman,” “man,” “boy” and “girl.” The order of sentences was balanced such that maximally two items from the same condition occurred in succession. A separate list of trials was created for each participant. Testing was performed individually at the participant's home in presence of two experimenters, one being a native speaker of Yucatec. Informing participants about the study, getting their approval and testing lasted about 60 min.

Examples (agent marked in italics, verb in bold, recipient/patient underlined):

1. Direction of movement from agent to recipient, active voice.

Spanish: Un niño regala flores a una niña

Yucatec: Ku síij lool ti‘jun túul chan x ch‘úupal jun túul chan xi‘ipal.

English: A boy gives flowers to a girl.

2. Direction of movement from agent to recipient, passive voice.

Spanish sentence: Un niño es empujado por una mujer.

Yucatec: Ku túulch'inta'al jun túul chan xi'ipal tumeen jun túul ko'olel.

English: A boy is pushed by a woman.

3. Direction of movement from patient to agent, active voice.

Spanish: Una mujer deja a una niña.

Yucatec: Ku p'atik jun túul chan x ch'úupal jun túul ko'olel.

English: A woman leaves a girl.

4. Direction of movement from patient to agent, passive voice.

Spanish: Un niño es jalado por un hombre.

Yucatec: Ku kóola'al jun túul chan xi'ipal tumeen jun túul wíinik.

English: A boy is pulled by a man.

Based on earlier results, we expected at least for the speakers of Spanish an influence of the factors transient (more agent-left placements if the action transient moves away from the agent; Chatterjee, 2001), voice (more agent-left placements in active voice; Dobel et al., 2007), and task (more agent-left placements in drawing than in arranging; cf. Dobel et al., 2011). If there is indeed an innate left-to-right predisposition, we should find evidence for this in speakers of Yucatec as well, possibly similarly mediated by transient, voice, and task. In contrast, if a spatial bias depends on learning to read and write, there should be no evidence for such a representational bias in either group.

The results from the repeated measurement ANOVAs (based on arcsine transformed ratios of agent-left placements) with the above-mentioned factors, however, showed only a significant effect of task [F(1, 22) = 9.095; p = 0.006]. More agent-left placements were observed in drawing than in arranging (a similar effect was reported in Dobel et al., 2011). None of the other main effects and interactions became significant. Taking a statistically very liberal approach, comparing each condition in each language against chance (see Figure 1 for an illustration of results), revealed that only one comparison reached significance (drawing of actions away from agent in active voice for Yucatec speakers [t(11) = 2.6; p = 0.025; r = 0.61; 63% agent-left placements]. In the seven studies investigating the bias for agents using similar tasks as here (Chatterjee et al., 1995; Barrett et al., 2002; Maass and Russo, 2003; Altmann et al., 2006; Dobel et al., 2007, 2011; Román et al., 2013), ten out of eleven comparisons were significant. In light of this, the effect seems to be rather strong and easy to find if present. Thus, even though the literature on event representation in adults made strong predictions about the presence of an agent-left bias, at least in speakers of Spanish, these hypotheses were not supported.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Percentage left-placements as a function of condition and language (t-bars represent one SE).

In conclusion, quite a few studies argued for spatial preferences in event and scene representation. Their extent depend on one's native language, short-lived contextual and situational factors (such as task, or word order, based on language-specific syntactic factors) and, most importantly, reading and writing direction. It was still an open question how these factors would be mediated by a language using VOS structure. Here, we found no evidence for a spatial preference in persons speaking such a language. We also observed no bias in illiterate speakers of Spanish, an SVO language. Thus, a mere first-in first-out strategy imposed by SVO languages, as argued above, is not sufficient to induce a spatial transient of action representation. As literacy distinguishes our groups from those of earlier studies, we conclude that a spatial bias in action representations is brought forth only if one learns to read and write. Note that there was also no spatial bias in preschoolers who had not yet learned to read and write (Dobel et al., 2007). If literacy happens, it has a very strong impact on a wide range of mental processes, some of which were listed above. In recent years, these finding from the cognitive sciences were supported by neuroscientific evidence demonstrating that learning to read and write has a profound impact on brain function (e.g., Dehaene et al., 2010) and even on the neural architecture (Carreiras et al., 2009). Thus, while it is still under debate whether language shapes thought (for opposing views see e.g., Pinker, 1994; Levinson, 2003a,b), there is now firm evidence that literacy does. Given the recent interest in non-verbal cognitive processes in bilinguals (e.g., De Groot, 2010; Cook and Bassetti, 2011), we consider the investigation of bilingual speakers that possess corresponding reading and writing skills with opposing directions as highly interesting. If the spatial bias is as context and task dependent as was shown in several studies, then the language in which the stimulus material is presented will exert an effect that is the product of the RWD associated with that language. Such a finding would provide strong evidence that RWD mediates between language and thought even if reading and writing is not at play.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Research was supported by the DFG (DFG DO 711/4-3) and the DAAD. We are very grateful to Jean and David Legters, Minneth Medina, Nicolas Aguilar Gil, and Miriam Uitz May (INDEMAYA) for their help and support. Christian Dobel and Stefanie Enriquez-Geppert contributed equally to producing the manuscript.

References

Altmann, L. P., Saleem, A., Kendall, D., Heilman, K. M., and Rothi, L. (2006). Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic. Brain Lang. 97, 306–316. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barrett, A. M., Kim, M., Crucian, G. P., and Heilman, K. M. (2002). Spatial bias: effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects. Neuropsychologia 40, 1003–1012. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00147-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bolles, D., and Bolles, A. (2004). A grammar of the Yucatecan Mayan language (revised online edition, 1996 Lee, New Hampshire). Los Angeles, CA: Foundation for the Advancement of Mesoamerican Studies, Inc.

Carreiras, M., Seghier, M. L., Baquero, S., Estévez, A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T., and Price, C. J. (2009). An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 461, 983–986. doi: 10.1038/nature08461

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: some interactions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 55–61. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01598-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chatterjee, A. (2002). Portrait profiles and the notion of agency. Empir. Stud. Arts 20, 33–41. doi: 10.2190/3WLF-AGTV-0AW7-R2CN

CrossRef Full Text

Chatterjee, A., Maher, L. M., Rothi, L., and Heilman, K. M. (1995). Asyntactic thematic role assignment: the use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain Lang. 49, 125–139. doi: 10.1006/brln.1995.1024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chatterjee, A., Southwood, M., and Basilico, D. (1999). Verbs, events and spatial representations. Neuropsychologia 37, 395–402. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00108-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chokron, S., and De Agostini, M. (2000). Reading habits influence aesthetic preference. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 45–49. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00021-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chokron, S., and Imbert, M. (1993). Influence of reading habits on line bisection. Cogn. Brain Res. 1, 219–222. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(93)90005-P

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cook, V. J., and Bassetti, B. (2011). Language and Bilingual Cognition. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

De Groot, A. (2010). Language and Cognition in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Hove: Psychology Press.

Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122, 371–396.

Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., et al. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330, 1359–1364. doi: 10.1126/science.1194140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobel, C., Diesendruck, G., and Bölte, J. (2007). How writing system and age influence spatial representations of actions: a developmental, cross-linguistic study. Psychol. Sci. 18, 487–491. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01926.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobel, C., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Hummert, M., Zwitserlood, P., and Bölte, J. (2011). Conceptual representation of actions in sign language. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 16, 392–400. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enq070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobel, C., Glanemann, R., Kreysa, H., Zwitserlood, P., and Eisenbeiss, S. (2010). “Visual encoding of meaningful and meaningless scenes,” in Event Representation in Language and Cognition, eds E. Pedersen and J. Bohnemeyer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 189–215.

England, N. C. (1991). Changes in basic word order in Mayan languages. Int. J. Am. Linguist. 57, 446–486.

Fausey, C. M., and Boroditsky, L. (2011). Who dunnit? Cross-linguistic differences in eye-witness memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 150–157. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0021-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fodor, A. J. (1975). The Language of Thought. New York, NY: Crowell.

Gentner, D., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). “Whither Whorf,” in Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, eds D. Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 3–14.

Grice, H. P. (1975). “Logic and conversation (from the William James lectures, Harvard University, 1967),” in Syntax and Semantics. Vol. 3, Speech Acts, eds P. Cole and J. Morgan (New York, NY: Academic Press), 4158.

Jahn, G., Knauff, M., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2007). Preferred mental models in reasoning about spatial relations. Mem. Cogn. 35, 2075–2087. doi: 10.3758/BF03192939

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jewell, G., and McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 93–110. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kranjec, A., Lehet, M., Bromberger, B., and Chatterjee, A. (2010). A sinister bias for calling fouls in soccer. PLoS ONE 5:e11667. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011667

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levinson, S. C. (2003a). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Levinson, S. C. (2003b). “Language and mind: let's get the issues straight,” in Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, eds D. Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 25–46.

Lewis, M. P. (2009). Ethnologue: Languages of the World, 16th Edn. Dallas, TX: SIL International.

Li, P., and Gleitman, L. (2002). Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning. Cognition 83, 265–294. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00009-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Front. Psychol. 3:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maass, A., and Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: nature or culture? Psychol. Sci. 14, 296–301. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14421

CrossRef Full Text

Maher, L. M., Chatterjee, A., Gonzales Rothi, L. J., and Heilman, K. M. (1993). Thematic roles in aphasia: conceptual knowledge and linguistic assignment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 15, 45–45.

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York, NY: William Morrow and Co.

Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D., and Rayner, K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain Lang. 14, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(81)90073-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Regier, T., and Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 439–446. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Román, A., Fathi, A., and Santiago, J. (2013). Spatial biases in understanding descriptions of static scenes: the role of reading and writing direction. Mem. Cogn. 41, 588–599. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0285-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., and Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 515–557.

Vaid, J. (1995). “Script directionality affects nonlinguistic performance: evidence from Hindi and Urdu,” in Scripts and Literacy: Reading and Learning to Read Alphabets, Syllabaries and Characters eds I. Taylor and D. R. Olson (Dordrecht: Kluwer), 295–310.

Vaid, J. (2010). “Asymmetries in representational drawing: alternatives to a laterality account,” in, Spatial Dimensions of Social Thought, eds T. Schubert and A. Maass (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 231–255

Verhoeven, E. (2007). Experiential Constructions in Yucatec Maya: A Typologically Based Analysis of a Functional Domain in a Mayan Language. Amsterdam; Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins.

Whorf, B. (1956). “Language, thought, and reality,” in Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf, ed J. B. Carroll (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Keywords: action representation, conceptual processes, Yucatec Maya

Citation: Dobel C, Enriquez-Geppert S, Zwitserlood P and Bölte J (2014) Literacy shapes thought: the case of event representation in different cultures. Front. Psychol. 5:290. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00290

Received: 30 April 2013; Paper pending published: 30 May 2013;
Accepted: 20 March 2014; Published online: 16 April 2014.

Edited by:

Guillaume Thierry, Bangor University, UK

Reviewed by:

Olaf Hauk, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, UK
Vivian Cook, Newcastle University, UK

Copyright © 2014 Dobel, Enriquez-Geppert, Zwitserlood and Bölte. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian Dobel, Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Malmedyweg 15, 48149 Münster, Germany e-mail: cdobel@uni-muenster.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.