Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 30 September 2014
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Madness and Creativity: Yes, No or Maybe? View all 15 articles

Creative cognition and systems biology on the edge of chaos

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • 2Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA

Complexity theorists have suggested that production “on the edge of chaos” is important to self-organization and evolutionary change in thermodynamic systems, biology, and economics. We apply this heuristic to cognitive systems and neural network activation states, which can vary from an ordered (predictable) regime, to a chaotic (unpredictable) regime. Evolutionary cytoarchitectonic theory specifies complementary anatomical systems governing stability and flexibility. Psychopathology is associated with shifts in the regulation of stability and flexibility, and may yield both increased redundancy and increased entropy within the same individual. We suggest this fits existing literature showing: (a) examples of exceptional creativity in individuals with mental illness, without an overall increase in creative achievement associated with “madness”; (b) increases in creative achievement among relatives of people with mental illness, or people with milder syndromes, for whom increased flexibility or stability is less disabling; and (c) effects of pharmacological manipulations, suggesting an inverted-U function resembling the Yerkes-Dodson Law, possibly linked to tonic and phasic dopaminergic transmission.

Defining Creativity on the Edge of Chaos for Cognitive and Biological Research

Creative works may be defined as combining novelty (or originality) and utility (or effectiveness, or value) within the domain from which they emerge (Runco and Jaeger, 2012). We relate this to the ideas of Stuart Kauffman, who highlighted the importance of production “on the edge of chaos” to yield valuable change in self-organizing systems spanning thermodynamic, economic, and biochemical systems (such as those involved in the origins of life) (Kauffman, 1993, 1995). Kauffman argues that these systems can be characterized by the predictability of their components. Considering elements within complex systems, some are “orderly” (predictable, redundant) while others are more “chaotic” (unpredictable, entropic). Kauffman sees new and useful developments as emerging “on the edge of chaos,” the boundary between ordered and chaotic regimes.

The Edge of Chaos theory can be applied to cognitive processes and brain activation states important for creative cognition. Considering the diversity of possible cognitive states, we can differentiate the highly predictable and orderly from the unpredictable and chaotic. In more chaotic regimes, network states are more disconnected from those in the ordered regime. But “at the edge of chaos,” the states are maximally novel while still connected to states in the ordered regime, and thus are most likely to manifest the combination of novelty and utility that is the hallmark of creativity. A similar conceptual approach was used to distinguish “rigidity,” “chaos,” and “integration” to characterize semantic network states in people with Asperger's syndrome, schizophrenia, and healthy semantic processing, respectively (see Faust and Kenett, 2014 for good graphical models; see also Siegel, 2010; Kaufman, 2014 for additional examples).

The theory of evolutionary cytoarchitectonic trends may provide an anatomic and neuropsychopharmacologic substrate for these cognitive dimensions, with complementary systems that increase the stability or flexibility of cognitive states via the archicortical and paleocortical trends, respectively (Christensen and Bilder, 2000; Bilder, 2012). Local cortical networks employ the complementary actions of tonic and phasic dopamine signaling, which putatively mediate stability and flexibility, respectively (Bilder et al., 2004); similarly, D1- and D2-like dopamine transmission may mediate persistence or updating within cell assemblies (Durstewitz and Seamans, 2002). Others have emphasized noradrenergic mechanisms to achieve similar network dynamics (McClure et al., 2006). We believe this view advances conceptualization of the role of DA in creative cognition beyond models in which DA was suggested to have a unitary function by facilitating “working memory” or “mental associations” (Flaherty, 2011), or “flexibility” as manifest in higher-order personality traits (DeYoung, 2006).

This theory may help provide a neural systems basis for the theory that creativity results from Blind Variation and Selective Retention (BVSR) (Campbell, 1960; Simonton, 2011a,b). Assuming that blind variation relies on “flexibility” while selective retention relies on “stability,” then our theory has multiple implications: (a) the neural system basis for “sighted” vs. “blind” variations is synaptic facilitation governing the likelihood that a given network activation state will be stabilized and more likely to re-occur; (b) the “unit of analysis” upon which blind variation operates was first stated as a “thought trial” (Campbell, 1960); Simonton refined this to be the “ideational variant”; we suggest that BV operates on the “perception-action cycle,” with a frequency of about 3 Hz, approximately how long it takes to compare a stimulus to expectations; and (c) we distinguish systematic from stochastic variation, with the systematic approach engaging cognitive control (task positive networks), while the stochastic approach disengages cognitive control and engages default mode networks.

The Edge of Chaos, Creativity, and Mental Health

How do these ideas relate to the theme of this issue: creativity and madness? We suggest the link between creativity and mental illness can benefit from considering the stability and flexibility of cognitive states. For example, Martin Paulus and colleagues used a two-choice guessing paradigm to calculate the entropy of sequential responses made by people with schizophrenia (Paulus et al., 1999). In this paradigm, people “guess” which of two alternatives (e.g., “left” or “right”) will occur next, when in fact the order of outcomes is random. We can analyze the sequence of guesses and quantify their entropy (unpredictability). Healthy people tend to have a Gaussian distribution of sequential guesses, with most responses at intermediate levels of entropy, and fewer very redundant or very entropic responses. In contrast, people with schizophrenia tended to show both more redundant (predictable) and more entropic (unpredictable) responses; longer periods of predictable behavior were interrupted by very unpredictable behavior. We showed that the entropy of responses is linked to dopamine (DA) dynamics; specifically we found that DA “tone” (the balance of agonism to antagonism) had an “inverted-U” association with predictability of responses (Bilder et al., 1992). These examples highlight that the balance of stability and flexibility is critical to maintain optimal, healthy function.

The Edge of Chaos theory is also compatible with the neural network frameworks that Ralph Hoffman, Stephen Grossberg, and their colleagues have used to describe schizophrenia (Hoffman and Dobscha, 1989; Grossberg, 1999, 2000; McGlashan and Hoffman, 2000). Either widespread dysconnectivity or impairment of dopamine dynamics governing the stability and flexibility of neural network activation states can lead a network to get “stuck” in a local energy minimum (thus explaining abnormal predictability), but when the activation state does shift, the new state is likely to be more “distant” and less “connected” to the preceding state (thus explaining abnormal unpredictability).

We believe this model conforms with the inverted-U model relating creativity with psychopathology traits and genetic risk for psychosis (Richards et al., 1988) and with existing literature showing examples of exceptional creativity in individuals with mental illness, even though the aggregated results tend to show no overall increase in creative achievement for those with severe mental illness (Rothenberg, 1983; Eisenman, 1990; Abraham et al., 2007; Jaracz et al., 2012). Consistent reports suggest that healthy relatives of those with schizophrenia, and those with schizotypal traits, may have elevated creativity; we suggest that these individuals may have a tendency to greater network flexibility, but additional traits help protect them from developing schizophrenia (Schuldberg, 2000; Kinney et al., 2001; Karimi et al., 2007; Abraham and Windmann, 2008; Batey and Furnham, 2008; Nelson and Rawlings, 2010). Consistent with this are Kinney et al. (2001) results showing higher levels of creativity in people with intermediate levels of schizotypal or schizoid traits, relative to those with lower levels of these traits and relative to those with overt schizophrenia.

Evidence about bipolar disorder is less clear, but empirical studies show similar inverted-U distributions: a milder degree of mood disorders, bipolar temperaments, or genetic liability (without full-blown bipolar disorder) may be linked to increased creative achievement, but more severe illness is not. For example, children with pediatric bipolar disorder perform worse on tests of set-shifting or cognitive flexibility (Gorrindo et al., 2005; Dickstein et al., 2007), and while adults with bipolar disorder may have increased inventiveness, scores are lower during depressive episodes (Rybakowski and Klonowska, 2011). Richards et al. (1988) also reported that participants with cyclothymia, and healthy first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder, had higher lifetime creativity scores compared to patients with bipolar disorder and healthy controls with no family history of major affective disorder or schizophrenia. Similarly, researchers found that hypomanic (Furnham et al., 2008) or hyperthymic traits in healthy individuals (Shapiro and Weisberg, 1999) were significantly correlated with measures of creativity. Overall, the data suggest that hereditary risk (without severe impairment), and/or moderate subclinical variations of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia may be associated with enhanced creative achievement, while the more severe forms of these syndromes are associated with impairments of creativity, paralleling impairments in other cognitive processes.

With respect to the evolutionary advantages, Kauffman wrote:

“If it proves true that selection tunes genomic systems to the edge of chaos, then evolution is persistently exploring networks constrained to this fascinating ensemble of dynamical systems (Kauffman, 1993, p. 522).”

We suggest that the underlying genomic systems, and their systems biology correlates at the level of neural network activation states, are tuned to the edge of chaos, helping explain both the observed associations and dissociations of creativity with mental illness.

These assumptions are consistent with the hypothesis that neural network dynamics associated with cognitive flexibility are linked to creative achievement because these dynamics generate activation states that are novel with respect to population averages, and favor performance on metrics that are tuned to divergent thinking (problem-solving processes that involve exploring multiple alternative solutions). But if the dynamics proceed too far in the direction of flexibility, entropy, and unpredictability, then cognitive products may be novel and unpredictable, but may be “over the edge of chaos,” and not be perceived as useful, valuable, or effective.

Implications for Mental Health and Promoting Creative Achievement

The Edge of Chaos hypothesis may help understand the pathophysiology and treatments of mental illness, and suggest paths to augment creativity. We can assess positive traits of cognitive stability and flexibility (or disadvantageous traits: rigidity and lability), and determine their relations with cellular and neuromodulatory factors. For example, widespread dysconnectivity and DA dysregulation remain candidates in the pathophysiology and treatment of schizophrenia. To the extent that interventions impact these states, monitoring stability/flexibility may be beneficial in determining when treatments are managing chaotic (overly flexible) states but not causing cognitive rigidity. For example, measures of network stability/flexibility could help titrate treatments in schizophrenia to maximize freedom from positive symptoms while minimizing cognitive impairment, or titrate the DA and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) to maximize attentional control.

Non-pharmacological treatments might someday be tuned to optimize flexibility and stability. For example, EEG neurofeedback strategies emphasizing cognitive stability or flexibility may enhance creativity (Gruzelier, 2014; Gruzelier et al., 2014a,b; but see also Schaller et al., 2013). Slow cortical potential neurofeedback training may enhance stability and benefit those with ADHD (Monastra, 2008; Studer et al., 2014). Transcranial magnetic stimulation or direct current stimulation may also moderate cortical stability and flexibility (Nitsche et al., 2009). TDCS studies already have shown enhancements of creative thinking (Chi and Snyder, 2011; Metuki et al., 2012; Chrysikou et al., 2013; but see also Ghacibeh et al., 2006).

There are further links of cognitive stability and flexibility to meditation practices referred to as focused attention and open monitoring, respectively (Lutz et al., 2008a,b; Slagter et al., 2011). We are not aware of studies showing enhanced creativity among Buddhist monks, but other studies already have shown a positive impact of meditation practices on divergent thinking (Horan, 2009; Colzato et al., 2012). In the future, assessment of individual differences in baseline stability and flexibility might lead to prescriptive contemplative practices. These approaches already have gained traction in the management of anxiety by broadening of attention using mobile phone applications (Enock et al., 2014), and we anticipate the future will deliver additional tools to manage brain activation states, perhaps combined with personal EEG devices.

In conclusion, we believe that understanding both mental illness and creative cognition from the perspective of neural network dynamics, and specifically the regulation of the stability and flexibility of cortical activation states, helps to clarify the relation between creativity and mental illness. We further believe understanding these cognitive dynamics may have profound implications for both understanding of pathophysiology of mental illness, the development of novel intervention strategies for those who are ill, and the enhancement of creative cognition more broadly across the population.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by the Michael E. Tennenbaum Family Center for the Biology of Creativity, a grant from the John Templeton Foundation, and a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH101478).

References

Abraham, A., and Windmann, S. (2008). Selective information processing advantages in creative cognition as a function of schizotypy. Creat. Res. J. 20, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/10400410701839819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abraham, A., Windmann, S., McKenna, P., and Gunturkun, O. (2007). Creative thinking in schizophrenia: the role of executive dysfunction and symptom severity. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 12, 235–258. doi: 10.1080/13546800601046714

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batey, M., and Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Pers. Individ. Dif. 45, 816–821. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilder, M., Volavka, J., Lachman, H. M., and Grace, A. A. (2004). The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1943–1961. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300542

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilder, R. M. (2012). Executive control: balancing stability and flexibility via the duality of evolutionary neuroanatomical trends. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 14, 39–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilder, R. M., Lieberman, J. A., Kim, Y., Alvir, J. M., and Reiter, G. (1992). Methylphenidate and neuroleptic effects on oral word production in schizophrenia. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 5, 262–271.

Google Scholar

Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychol. Rev. 67, 380–400.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, R. P., and Snyder, A. W. (2011). Facilitate insight by non-invasive brain stimulation. PLoS ONE 6:e16655. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016655

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, B. K., and Bilder, R. M. (2000). Dual cytoarchitectonic trends: an evolutionary model of frontal lobe functioning and its application to psychopathology. Can. J. Psychiatry 45, 247–256.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 81–89. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2013.768221

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., and Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: the impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Front. Psychol. 3:116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00116

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 1138–1151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1138

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickstein, D. P., Nelson, E. E., McClure, E. B., Grimley, M. E., Knopf, L., Brotman, M. A., et al. (2007). Cognitive flexibility in phenotypes of pediatric bipolar disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46, 341–355. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31802d0b3d

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durstewitz, D., and Seamans, J. K. (2002). The computational role of dopamine D1 receptors in working memory. Neural Netw. 15, 561–572. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00049-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenman, R. (1990). Creativity, preference for complexity, and physical and mental illness. Creat. Res. J. 3, 231–236. doi: 10.1080/10400419009534355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enock, P. M., Hofmann, S. G., and McNally, R. J. (2014). Attention bias modification training via smartphone to reduce social anxiety: a randomized, controlled multi-session experiment. Cogn. Ther. Res. 38, 200–216. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9606-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faust, M., and Kenett, Y. N. (2014). Rigidity, chaos and integration: hemispheric interaction and individual differences in metaphor comprehension. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:511. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00511

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W. (2011). Brain illness and creativity: mechanisms and treatment risks. Can. J. Psychiatry 56, 132–143.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K., and Manfield, J. (2008). Personality, hypomania, intelligence and creativity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 44, 1060–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghacibeh, G. A., Shenker, J. I., Shenal, B., Uthman, B. M., and Heilman, K. M. (2006). Effect of vagus nerve stimulation on creativity and cognitive flexibility. Epilepsy Behav. 8, 720–725. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2006.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorrindo, T., Blair, R. J. R., Budhani, S., Dickstein, D. P., Pine, D. S., and Leibenluft, E. (2005). Deficits on a probabilistic response-reversal task in patients with pediatric bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1975–1977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1975

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossberg, S. (1999). Neural models of normal and abnormal behavior: what do schizophrenia, parkinsonism, attention deficit disorder, and depression have in common? Prog. Brain Res. 121, 375–406. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63084-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossberg, S. (2000). How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning, attention, and volition. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 6, 583–592. doi: 10.1017/S135561770065508X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H. (2014). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. II: creativity, the performing arts and ecological validity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 142–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H., Foks, M., Steffert, T., Chen, M. J., and Ros, T. (2014a). Beneficial outcome from EEG-neurofeedback on creative music performance, attention and well-being in school children. Biol. Psychol. 95, 86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H., Holmes, P., Hirst, L., Bulpin, K., Rahman, S., van Run, C., et al. (2014b). Replication of elite music performance enhancement following alpha/theta neurofeedback and application to novice performance and improvisation with SMR benefits. Biol. Psychol. 95, 96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, R. E., and Dobscha, S. K. (1989). Cortical pruning and the development of schizophrenia: a computer model. Schizophr. Bull. 15, 477–490.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Horan, R. (2009). The neuropsychological connection between creativity and meditation. Creat. Res. J. 21, 199–222. doi: 10.1080/10400410902858691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaracz, J., Patrzala, A., and Rybakowski, J. K. (2012). Creative thinking deficits in patients with schizophrenia: neurocognitive correlates. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 200, 588–593. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31825bfc49

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi, Z., Windmann, S., Gunturkun, O., and Abraham, A. (2007). Insight problem solving in individuals with high versus low schizotypy. J. Res. Pers. 41, 473–480. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauffman, S. (1995). At Home in the Universe: the Search for Laws of Self-Organization and Complexity. New York, NY: Oxford.

Google Scholar

Kauffman, S. A. (1993). The Origins of Order: Self Organization and Selection in Evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Kaufman, S. B. (2014). The Controlled Chaos of Creativity in Beautiful Minds. New York, NY: Scientific American.

Kinney, D. K., Richards, R., Lowing, P. A., LeBlanc, D., Zimbalist, M. E., and Harlan, P. (2001). Creativity in offspring of schizophrenic and control parents: an adoption study. Creat. Res. J. 13, 17–25. doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1301_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., and Davidson, R. J. (2008a). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3:e1897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008b). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S., Gilzenrat, M. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2006). An exploration-exploitation model based on norepinepherine and dopamine activity. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 18, 867. doi: 10.1080/026999399379069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGlashan, T. H., and Hoffman, R. E. (2000). Schizophrenia as a disorder of developmentally reduced synaptic connectivity. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 637–648. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metuki, N., Sela, T., and Lavidor, M. (2012). Enhancing cognitive control components of insight problems solving by anodal tDCS of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Stimul. 5, 110–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monastra, V. (2008). Quantitative electroencephalography and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: implications for clinical practice. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 10, 432–438. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0069-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, B., and Rawlings, D. (2010). Relating schizotypy and personality to the phenomenology of creativity. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 388–399. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Kuo, M.-F., Grosch, J., Bergner, C., Monte-Silva, K., and Paulus, W. (2009). D1-receptor impact on neuroplasticity in humans. J. Neurosci. 29, 2648–2653. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5366-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., Bilder, R. M., Lieberman, J. A., Prahdan, N., Rapp, P. E., and Sreenivasan, R. (1999). “Complex dysregulation in sequential organization and dysregulation in dopaminergic modulation in first episode schizophrenia patients,” in Nonlinear Dynamics and Brain Functioning, eds N. Pradhan, P. E. Rapp, and R. Sreenivasan (Huntington, NY: Nova), 309–326. doi: 10.1080/026999399379069

CrossRef Full Text

Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., and Merzel, A. P. (1988). Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 281–288. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.281

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothenberg, A. (1983). Psychopathology and creative cognition. A comparison of hospitalized patients, Nobel laureates, and controls. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 40, 937–942.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 24, 92–96. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.650092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rybakowski, J. K., and Klonowska, P. (2011). Bipolar mood disorder, creativity and schizotypy: an experimental study. Psychopathology 44, 296–302. doi: 10.1159/000322814

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, G., Lenz, B., Friedrich, K., Dygon, D., Richter-Schmidinger, T., Sperling, W., et al. (2013). No evidence for effects of a high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation series on verbal and figural fluency and TAP task performance in healthy male volunteers. Neuropsychobiology 67, 69–73. doi: 10.1159/000343502

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuldberg, D. (2000). Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal creativity. Creat. Res. J. 13, 5–16. doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1301_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, P. J., and Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and bipolar diathesis: common behavioural and cognitive components. Cogn. Emot. 13, 741–762. doi: 10.1080/026999399379069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, D. J. (2010). Mindsight: The New Science of Personal Transformation. New York, NY: Random House LLC.

Simonton, D. K. (2011a). Creativity and discovery as blind variation and selective retention: multiple-variant definition and blind-sighted integration. Psychol. Aesthetics Creat. Arts 5, 222. doi: 10.1037/a0023144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonton, D. K. (2011b). Creativity and discovery as blind variation: Campbell's 1960. BVSR model after the half-century mark. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 15, 158. doi: 10.1037/a0022912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., and Lutz, A. (2011). Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:17. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Studer, P., Kratz, O., Gevensleben, H., Rothenberger, A., Moll, G. H., Hautzinger, M., et al. (2014). Slow cortical potential and theta/beta neurofeedback training in adults: effects on attentional processes and motor system excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:555. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00555

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: creativity, cognition, systems biology, mental illness, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, dopamine, norepinephrine

Citation: Bilder RM and Knudsen KS (2014) Creative cognition and systems biology on the edge of chaos. Front. Psychol. 5:1104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01104

Received: 02 August 2014; Accepted: 11 September 2014;
Published online: 30 September 2014.

Edited by:

Anna Abraham, Kuwait University, Kuwait

Reviewed by:

Rex Eugene Jung, University of New Mexico, USA
Scott Barry Kaufman, New York University, USA

Copyright © 2014 Bilder and Knudsen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: rbilder@mednet.ucla.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.