Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 August 2013
Sec. Neuropharmacology

Performance in Neurocognitive Tasks in Obese Patients. Does Somatic Comorbidity Matter?

  • 1Schoen Clinic Bad Bramstedt, Bad Bramstedt, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Friedrich-Alexander University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Erlangen, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany
  • 4Department of Surgery, Herzogin Elisabeth Hospital, Braunschweig, Germany
  • 5Department of Surgery, Municipal Hospital Schwabach, Schwabach, Germany

The aim of the present study was to examine if obese individuals with obesity-related somatic comorbidity (i.e., hypertension, diabetes, sleep apnea, dyslipidemia, pain disorder) perform worse in neurocognitive tasks compared to obese individuals without any somatic disorder. Neurocognitive functioning was measured by a computerized test battery that consisted of the following tasks: Corsi Block Tapping Test, Auditory Word Learning Task, Trail Making Test-Part B, Stroop Test, Labyrinth Test, and a four-disk version of the Tower of Hanoi. The total sample consisted of 146 patients, the majority (N = 113) suffered from obesity grade 3, 26 individuals had obesity grade 2, and only 7 individuals obesity grade 1. Ninety-eight participants (67.1%) reported at least one somatic disorder (Soma+-group). Hypertension was present in 75 individuals (51.4%), type 2 diabetes in 34 participants (23.3%), 38 individuals had sleep apnea (26.0%), 16 suffered from dyslipidemia (11.0%), and 14 individuals reported having a chronic pain disorder (9.6%). Participants without a coexisting somatic disorder were younger [MSoma− = 33.7, SD = 9.8 vs. MSoma+ = 42.7, SD = 11.0, F(1, 144) = 23.01, p < 0.001] and more often female [89.6 and 62.2%, χ2(1) = 11.751, p = 0.001] but did not differ with respect to education, regular binge eating, or depressive symptoms from those in the Soma+-group. The Soma-group performed better on cognitive tasks related to memory and mental flexibility. However, the group differences disappeared completely after controlling for age. The findings indicate that in some obese patients increasing age may not only be accompanied by an increase of obesity severity and by more obesity-related somatic disorders but also by poorer cognitive functioning.

Introduction

Research indicates an association between obesity and neurocognitive dysfunction (14). The causal pathways for this relationship, however, remain poorly understood. One potential explanation concerns the association between obesity and comorbid medical conditions that in turn may lead to cognitive impairment. It remains uncertain whether the proposed cognitive decline can be attributed to effects of a specific obesity-related somatic disorder or rather to additive effects of multiple conditions. Cognitive dysfunction seen in patients with obesity might be caused by the presence of several somatic disorders that are prevalent in obese individuals such as diabetes type 2, hypertension, sleep apnea, dyslipidemia, or chronic pain.

Type 2 diabetes that is characterized by reduced insulin sensitivity and relative insulin deficiency is related to microvascular and macrovascular complications that may affect the brain (57). The underlying mechanisms of diabetes-related cognitive impairment, however, are difficult to establish due to the presence of several confounding somatic comorbidities. There exists evidence that cerebrovascular disease contributes substantially to decreased cognitive abilities in patients with diabetes type 2 (6, 7). Similarly, chronic elevation in blood pressure is common in obese persons and linked to an increased risk for alterations in the cerebral artery structure, vascular dementia, and Alzheimer-type neuropathy (8). Hypertension affects cerebral circulation that may adversely influence performance in cognitive tasks, in particular those assessing executive functions (911). Another vascular risk factor for cognitive dysfunction is dyslipidemia that is strongly associated with diabetes type 2 and hypertension in obese individuals (12, 13). Furthermore, sleep apnea has been associated with a higher risk for cognitive impairment (14, 15). Sleep fragmentation and cessation or reduction of breathing during sleep resulting in recurrent hypoxemia can contribute to structural and functional brain abnormalities that are related to cognitive dysfunction (1618). In addition, there exists evidence for the negative influence of chronic pain on cognitive performance (19, 20). Possible mechanisms involved in pain-related cognitive impairment include the overlap in brain morphology, neurotransmitters, and other neural mediators that are involved in both pain processing and cognition, altered neuroplasticity, and dysregulated neurochemistry [for review see (20)].

There are other variables that can affect cognitive functioning. It is well known that morbidly obese individuals suffer from psychiatric disorders [e.g., (2123)]. For example, depressive symptoms are prevalent in obese persons (24) and may account for their cognitive abilities (25). One of the most common psychiatric disorders reported in morbidly obese patients is binge eating disorder (BED) with prevalence rates up to 50% (23, 2629). BED is characterized by experiencing loss of control while eating an unusual large amount of food and is therefore linked to food-related high impulsivity (30). Few studies have examined cognitive functions, in particular food-unrelated decision-making, in obese individuals with BED reporting mixed results. While one study found decision-making deficits in overweight and obese women with BED compared to those without (31) others did not (32, 33). The inconsistency between the studies could be caused by different cognitive tasks and by sampling characteristics [e.g., different body mass index (BMI) ranges].

Furthermore, higher education is often related to better performance in cognitive tasks (32, 34). Last but not least, there have been many reports regarding the inverse association between age and performance in cognitive tasks. The question at which age the cognitive decline starts, however, remains unresolved (34, 35).

The aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between neurocognitive performance and somatic disorders in obese patients taking into account BMI, depressive and eating disorder symptoms, and education. Based on the literature, we expected a reduced neurocognitive performance in obese individuals with any obesity-related somatic disorder compared to those without.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Study Design

Participation in the study was completely voluntarily. Inclusion criteria were a BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2, age between 18 and 65 years, and sufficient German language skills. Exclusion criteria were any neurological disorder, psychosis, dementia, current substance abuse, developmental or learning disorders, sensory impairments, and intellectual disability.

Between January 2011 and May 2012, 84 patients considering bariatric surgery who were seen for a routine preoperative psychiatric evaluation at the University Hospital Erlangen or the Hannover Medical School and 62 subjects from a psychosomatic inpatient unit in Bad Bramstedt were recruited. The total sample consisted of 146 patients (71.2% women, 28.8% men) with an average age of 39.8 years (SD = 11.4, Range 18–65). The majority (N = 113) suffered from obesity grade 3 (BMI ≥ 40 kg/m2), 26 individuals had obesity grade 2 (BMI: 35–39.9 kg/m2), and only 7 individuals had obesity grade 1 (BMI: 30–34.9 kg/m2). All participants gave written informed consent according to procedures approved by the Institutional Ethics Committees of the three study sites.

Assessments

The participants provided information on education, height, and weight. Information on medication and the following somatic disorders was taken from patients’ charts of the surgical departments or the inpatient unit: hypertension, diabetes, sleep apnea, dyslipidemia, and pain disorder.

To measure eating disorder symptoms, the German version of the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire (EDE-Q) (36) was used. This questionnaire consists of specific items to assess objective binge eating episodes (OBE; i.e., eating an objectively large amount of food with a sense of loss of control). Regular binge eating was defined as eight or more OBEs during the past 28 days.

The German version of the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire depression scale (PHQ-9) (37) was administered in order to assess depressive symptoms. The PHQ-9 scores each of the nine DSM-IV criteria for depression from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day).

Neurocognitive functioning was measured by a computerized test battery that consisted of the following neurocognitive tasks.

The Corsi Block Tapping Test (CBT) was administered to assess visual attention and working memory (38). Participants had to reproduce several sequences of block tappings displayed only once by the computer. When the sequence was correctly copied, the number of cubes which had to be touched was increased step-wise. The outcome variable of this task was the total number of correct answers.

To assess verbal memory, we administered an auditory verbal learning task (wordlist) in which a list of 15 not associated words was presented five times. Participants were asked to reproduce as many words as possible after each of the five trials. The number of correctly recalled words per trial was registered and the total of all correct answers across the five trials was used as outcome variable.

The Trail Making Test-Part B (TMT-B) (39) was used to examine mental flexibility and the ability to switch attention. Participants had to tap numbers (1–9) and digits (A–I) in an alternating sequence as quickly and as accurately as possible within three trials. The mean time to completion per trial (in seconds) was used as dependent variable in this task.

The Stroop Test is a well known diagnostic tool for assessing selective attention and response inhibition (40). In this study, the answers were given not verbally but by pressing the corresponding button on the touch screen. The number of correctly identified words in the interference condition was used as dependent variable.

The Labyrinth Test was used to assess spatial memory, planning, and error utilization (41). Participants had to find a hidden path through a spatial maze. The task was completed after the hidden path was found two times without errors. The total number of errors during this task was used as dependent variable.

Finally, a four-disk version of the Tower of Hanoi (ToH) was used to measure planning abilities (42). The ratio between the number of ideal disk moves and the number of actually needed disk moves (effectivity) was the dependent variable.

Data Analysis

Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics v.20. The total sample was divided in a group without any somatic disorder and a group with at least one somatic disorder. Univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA) and χ2-tests were used to compare the groups in terms of sociodemographic variables, BMI and depression, as appropriate. In a second step, the groups were compared with regard to their performance in the neurocognitive tasks. These analyses were controlled for potential confounding variables that were found to be different between the groups in the aforementioned analyses (ANCOVA). The group comparisons were then repeated subsequently for specific somatic disorders (i.e., hypertension yes/no, diabetes yes/no, sleep apnea yes/no).

To investigate the relationship between the number of somatic disorders and the performance in neurocognitive tasks, we conducted Pearson correlations. Finally, to examine the association between neurocognitive performance and the presence of any somatic disorder we performed a set of logistic regression analyses adjusting for variables that differed between the two groups (age, gender, BMI).

The group comparisons regarding cognitive functioning and the regressions were made on the basis of Bonferroni-corrected significance levels.

Results

Somatic Comorbidity, BMI, Medication, Sociodemographic Characteristic, Eating Disorder Symptoms, and Depression

Based on patients’ charts, 67.1% of the total sample (N = 98) suffered from at least one somatic disorder (Soma+-group). Hypertension was present in 75 individuals (51.4%), type 2 diabetes in 34 participants (23.3%), 38 individuals had sleep apnea (26.0%), 16 suffered from dyslipidemia (11.0%), and 14 individuals reported having a chronic pain disorder (9.6%). The mean number of disorders in the Soma+-group ranged from 1 to 5 (M = 1.81, SD = 0.94). Almost half of that group reported suffering from only one somatic disorder (49%), 26.5% had two, and 20.4% three somatic disorders. The number of somatic disorders in the Soma+-group was positively related to age (r = 0.293, p < 0.01).

The groups did not differ in terms of any psychopharmacological treatment including neuroleptics, mood stabilizer, antidepressants, anxiolytics, stimulants, or anticonvulsive drugs [Soma: 37.5% vs. Soma+: 45.4%, χ2(1) = 0.811, p = 0.368]. Many participants reported taking antidepressant medication [Soma: 33.3% vs. Soma+: 43.3%, χ2(1) = 1.329, p = 0.249]. While only two participants in the Soma-group took non-opiate pain medication, 81.4% of participants with somatic comorbidity had at least one of the following pharmacological treatments: insulin and/or non-insulin anti-diabetic medication, antihypertensive agents, opiate or non-opiate pain medication, or cholesterol lowering drugs.

As shown in Table 1, patients with any somatic disorder were substantially older and had a significantly higher BMI than those without somatic disorders. There were more male patients in the Soma+-group. The groups did not differ significantly from each other with regard to education.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of sociodemographic characteristics, BMI, eating disorder, and depressive symptoms between individuals without any somatic disorder (Soma) and those with any somatic disorder (Soma+).

According to the EDE-Q mean total scores which were available from 134 patients, we did not find any significant group differences in terms of eating disorder symptoms including binge eating. The mean number of OBEs during the past 28 days did not differ between the two groups [MSoma+ = 4.27, SD = 6.34 and MSoma− = 4.78, SD = 7.18, F(1, 133) = 0.175, p = 0.676]. Regular binge eating was found in 21.3% of the Soma+ group and 28.9% of the Soma group [χ2(1) = 0.935, p = 0.334]. Information on depressive symptoms was available from 126 patients (86% of the total sample). The PHQ-9 data indicated a lack of significant group differences with respect to depression.

Comparison of Performance in Neurocognitive Tasks between Patients without and with Somatic Disorders

As can be seen in Table 2, the results of the univariate ANOVA suggest differences between the Soma and the Soma+ groups in three of six tasks, particularly in the CBT, the wordlist, and the TMT-B. After controlling for gender, only the difference in the CBT remained significant. After adjusting for age all group differences disappeared completely (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of performances in neurocognitive tasks between individuals without any somatic disorder (Soma, N = 48) and those with any somatic disorder (Soma+, N = 98).

Figures 13 present the unadjusted group differences of task performance in the CBT, the wordlist, and the TMT-B by the presence/absence of specific disorders, in particular hypertension, diabetes, and sleep apnea indicating the strongest effect on cognitive performance for hypertension. Similarly to the aforementioned findings, after controlling for age the differences were no longer significant (data not reported here but available upon request).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Number of correct answers in the Corsi Block Tapping Task, unadjusted comparisons.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Number of correctly recalled words in the Wordlist Task, unadjusted comparisons.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean time to completion per trial in the Trail Making Test-Part B, unadjusted comparisons.

Due to the relatively low prevalence of dyslipidemia and pain disorder in the present sample we did not conduct group comparisons based on the absence or presence of these conditions.

Relationship between the Number of Comorbid Disorders and Performance in Neurocognitive Tasks

Pearson correlations between the number of somatic disorders and neurocognitive performance yielded significant negative correlations with regard to the CBT (r = −0.273, p < 0.01), the wordlist (r = −0.309, p < 0.001), and the TMT-B (r = −0.315, p < 0.001) but not the other tasks. Since the number of somatic disorders was positively correlated with age (r = 0.452, p < 0.001), we conducted additional partial correlations controlling for age. After this, the correlations mentioned above were no longer significant (CBT: r = −0.111, ns; wordlist: r = −0.126, ns; TMT-B: r = 0.111, ns).

Association between Performance in Neurocognitive Tasks and Somatic Comorbidity

Table 3 summarizes the results of hierarchical regression analyses with the performance in the specific neurocognitive tasks as dependent variables. Age, gender, and BMI were entered into block 1 of the regressions as control variables. The variable “presence of any somatic disorder” was entered in a second step. Changes in predictive ability (R2) were examined to determine significance. As shown in Table 3, only lower age was associated with more correct answers in the CBT and with a higher number of correctly recalled words in the wordlist task. Furthermore, age was positively related to an increased time to completion in the TMT-B.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of linear regression models concerning the association between performance in neurocognitive tasks and the presence of any comorbid somatic disorder, controlled for age, gender, and BMI.

Discussion

In the present sample, individuals with any somatic disorder were about 10 years older and reported significantly higher BMIs than those without any somatic disorder. Moreover, age was positively related to the number of obesity-related somatic disorders in the Soma+-group.

At the first glance, our results concerning cognitive performance suggested working and verbal memory deficits (CBT, wordlist) and poorer mental flexibility (TMT-B) in obese individuals with obesity-related somatic comorbidity. While controlling for BMI did not impact the results substantially, the group differences disappeared after adjusting for age and gender. Given the results of the regression analyses with the performance in the specific neurocognitive tasks as dependent variables and age, gender, BMI, and somatic comorbidity as independent variables, the better task performance in the Soma-group seems mainly driven by younger age. The groups did not differ in terms of education, depressive symptoms, or eating pathology. Hence, we may assume that the findings were not confounded by these variables. Taken together, it appears that worse performance in three of the six cognitive tasks were rather a problem of older age than of somatic comorbidity. This is in line with previous research demonstrating age-related cognitive decline (34, 35).

The findings do not support the assumption that somatic comorbidity is related to a higher risk for cognitive impairment in obese individuals. This is surprising given earlier reports on the association between cognitive dysfunction and somatic comorbidity. Though, many of those did not focus on obese individuals [e.g., (10, 15, 19)]. The discrepancy between our and previous findings reporting cognitive decline in overweight or obese individuals with somatic comorbidity might be explained by differences in test selection. Moreover, differences in sampling may account for the discrepant findings. For example, other studies had included either substantially younger [e.g., (17)] or older [e.g., (9)] individuals or those with lower BMIs [e.g., (7)]. It is certainly possible that the lack of group differences in the present investigation was related to a ceiling effect in both the Soma+ and the Soma group given the high BMI range. According to previous reports, obese individuals suffer from impaired cognitive function compared to normal-weight controls [e.g., (3, 4, 43, 44)]. However, the present investigation did not include a normal-weight control group that is a shortcoming.

On the other hand, our results are consistent with other reports of lacking differences in cognitive function between individuals with and without somatic comorbidity. Recently, Singh-Manou et al. (45) examined the association between midlife obesity, metabolic abnormalities (i.e., dyslipidemia, hypertension, hyperglycemia), and cognitive decline in early old age in a large longitudinal study (N = 6401). The sample consisted of 582 obese adults. In this study, the metabolically healthy obese participants did not have a better cognitive profile than individuals with metabolic abnormalities. Also, no significant differences were found between the two groups with regard to cognitive decline over a 10 years period.

There are some other limitations that need to be acknowledged. First, the diagnosis of both depressive symptoms and binge eating based on questionnaires instead of a standardized interview is concerning. Second, medication for somatic disorders could have influenced the task performance. Also, the group with somatic comorbidity is twice as large as the group without any somatic disorder that might have biased the results.

In conclusion, our findings indicate that in some obese patients increasing age may not only be accompanied by an increase of obesity severity and by more obesity-related somatic disorders but also by poorer cognitive functioning. Further studies should include control groups matched for age, gender, and education to examine whether somatic comorbidity may add a negative impact on cognition above the effect of age.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We kindly thank Dr. Laszlo Gaal (Bernried) for his support during the course of the study and the two reviewers for their helpful comments.

References

1. Cournot M, Marquié JC, Ansiaun D, Martinaud C, Fonds H, Ferriere J, et al. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology (2006) 67:1208–14. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

2. Dahl AK, Hassing LB. Obesity and cognitive aging. Epidemiol Rev (2012). doi:10.1093/epirev/mxs002 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text

3. Fagundo AB, de laTorre R, Jiménez-Murcia S, Agüera Z, Granero R, Tárrega S, et al. Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: from anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLoS ONE (2012) 7:e43382. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043382

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Gordon E. Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged adults. Eat Weight Disord (2006) 11:15–9.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

5. Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB. Obesity, diabetes and cognitive deficit: the Framingham heart study. Neurobiol Aging (2005) 26S:S11–6. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.08.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

6. McCrimmon RJ, Ryan CM, Frier BM. Diabetes and cognitive dysfunction. Lancet (2012) 379:2291–9. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60360-2

CrossRef Full Text

7. Pedersen M, Pedersen KK, Bruunsgaard H, Krabbe KS, Thomsen C, Færch K, et al. Cognitive functions in middle aged individuals are related to metabolic disturbances and aerobic capacity: a cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE (2012) 7:e51132. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051132

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

8. Elias MF, Goodell AL, Dore GA. Hypertension and cognitive functioning. A perspective in historical context. Hypertension (2012) 60:260–8. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.186429

CrossRef Full Text

9. Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB. Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hypertension: the Framingham heart study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord (2003) 27:260–8. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.802225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

10. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Acker D. Hypertension and the brain: vulnerability of the prefrontal regions and executive functions. Behav Neurosci (2003) 117:1169–80. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1169

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

11. Saxby BK, Harrington F, McKeith IG, Wesnes K, Ford GA. Effects of hypertension on attention, memory, and executive function in older adults. Health Psychol (2003) 22:587–91. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.587

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

12. Rincon F, Wright CB. Vascular cognitive impairment. Curr Opin Neurol (2013) 26:29–36. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835c4f04

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. van den Berg E, Kloppenborg RP, Kessels RPC, Kappelle LJ, Biessels GJ. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, dyslipidemia and obesity: a systematic comparison of their impact on cognition. Biochim Biophys Acta (2009) 1792:470–81. doi:10.1016/j.bbadis.2008.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. Adams N, Strauss ME, Schluchter M, Redline S. Relation of measures of sleep-disordered breathing to neuropsychological functioning. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2001) 163:1626–31. doi:10.1164/ajrccm.163.7.2004014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Naëgelé B, Launois SH, Mazza S, Feuerstein C, Pépin JL, Lévy P. Which memory processes are affected in patients with obstructive sleep apnea? An evaluation of 3 types of memory. Sleep (2006) 29:533–44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

16. Canessa N, Castronovo V, Cappa S, Aloia M, Marelli S, Falini A, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea. Brain structural changes and neuro cognitive function before and after treatment. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2011) 183:1419–26. doi:10.1164/rccm.201005-0693OC

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

17. Hannon TS, Rofey DL, Ryan CM, Clapper DA, Chakravorty S, Arslanian SA. Relationships among obstructive sleep apnea, anthropometric measures, and neurocognitive functioning in adolescents with severe obesity. J Pediatr (2012) 160:732–5. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.10.029

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Lal C, Strange C, Bachman D. Neurocognitive impairment in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest (2012) 14:1601–10. doi:10.1378/chest.11-2214

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Landrø NI, Fors EA, Våpenstad LL, Holthe O, Stiles TC, Borchgrevink PC. The extent of neurocognitive dysfunction in a multidisciplinary pain centre population. Is there a relation between reported and tested neuropsychological functioning? Pain (2013) 154:972–7. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.01.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

20. Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP. The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog Neurobiol (2011) 93:385–404. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

21. Herpertz S, Burgmer R, Stang A, de Zwaan M, Wolf AM, Chen-Stute A, et al. Prevalence of mental disorders in normal-weight and obese individuals with and without weight loss treatment in a German urban population. J Psychosom Res (2006) 61:95–103. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Kalarchian MA, Marcus MD, Levine MD, Courcoulas AP, Pilkonis PA, Ringham RM, et al. Psychiatric disorders among bariatric surgery candidates: relationship to obesity and functional health status. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:328–34. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.328

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Mühlhans B, Horbach T, de Zwaan M. Psychiatric disorders in bariatric surgery candidates: a review of the literature and results of a German prebariatric surgery sample. Gen Hosp Psychiatry (2009) 31:414–21. doi:10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.05.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. de Zwaan M, Enderle J, Wagner S, Mühlhans B, Ditzen B, Gefeller O, et al. Anxiety and depression in bariatric surgery patients: a prospective, follow-up study using structured clinical interviews. J Affect Disord (2011) 133:61–8. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Cserjési R, Luminet O, Poncelet AS, Lénárd L. Altered executive function in obesity. Exploration of the role of affective states on cognitive abilities. Appetite (2009) 52:535–9. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.01.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Alger-Mayer S, Rosati C, Polimeni JM, Malone M. Preoperative binge eating status and gastric bypass surgery: a long-term outcome study. Obes Surg (2009) 19:139–45. doi:10.1007/s11695-008-9540-9

CrossRef Full Text

27. Burgmer R, Grigutsch K, Zipfel S, Wolf AM, de Zwaan M, Husemann B, et al. The influence of eating behavior and eating pathology on weight loss after gastric restriction operations. Obes Surg (2005) 15:684–91. doi:10.1381/0960892053923798

CrossRef Full Text

28. Hsu LK, Mulliken B, McDonagh B, Krupa Das S, Rand W, Fairburn CG, et al. Binge eating disorder in extreme obesity. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord (2002) 26:1398–403. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0802081

CrossRef Full Text

29. Jones-Corneille LR, Wadden TA, Sarwer DB, Faulconbridge LF, Fabricatore AN, Stack RM. Axis I psychopathology in bariatric surgery candidates with and without binge eating disorder: results of structured clinical interviews. Obes Surg (2012) 22:389–97. doi:10.1007/s11695-010-0322-9

CrossRef Full Text

30. Schag K, Schönleber J, Teufel M, Zipfel S, Giel K. Food-related impulsivity in obesity and binge eating disorder – a systematic review. Obes Rev (2013) 14:477–95. doi:10.1111/obr.12017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Svaldi J, Brand M, Tuschen-Caffier B. Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder. Appetite (2010) 54:84–92. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Davis C, Patte K, Curtis C, Reid C. Immediate pleasure and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite (2010) 54:208–13. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Danner UN, Ouwehand C, van Haastert NL, Hornsveld H, de Ridder DT. Decision making impairments in women with binge eating disorder in comparison with obese and normal weight women. Eur Eat Disord Rev (2012) 20:e56–62. doi:10.1002/erv.1098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Elias MF, Elias PK, D’Agostino RB, Silbershatz H, Wolf PA. Role of age, education, and gender on cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study: community-based norms. Exp Aging Res (1997) 23:201–35. doi:10.1080/03610739708254281

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging (2009) 30:525–7. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.11.012

CrossRef Full Text

36. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B. Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire. Deutsche Übersetzung. Münster: Verlag für Psychotherapie (2006).

37. Löwe B, Spitzer RL, Zipfel S, Herzog W. PHQ-D: Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten: Manual – Komplettversion und Kurzform, from Pfizer. (2012). Available from: http://infektionskrankheiten.klinikum.uni-heidelberg.de/fileadmin/Psychosomatische_Klinik/download/PHQ_Manual1.pdf

38. Kessels RPC, van Zandvoort MJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EHF. The corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol (2000) 7:252–8. doi:10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

39. Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration and Scoring. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory (1992).

40. Golden CJ. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company (1978).

41. Bowden SC, Smith LC. What does the Austin maze measure? Aust Psychol (1994) 29:34–7. doi:10.1080/00050069408257317

CrossRef Full Text

42. Welsh MC, Huizinga M. Tower of Hanoi disk transfer task: influences of strategy knowledge and learning on performance. Learn Individ Differ (2005) 15:283–98. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.05.002

CrossRef Full Text

43. Galioto R, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, Paul R, et al. Cognitive function in morbidly obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder. Compr Psychiatry (2012) 53:490–5. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.002

CrossRef Full Text

44. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB, Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr Psychiatry (2007) 48:57–61. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

45. Singh-Manou A, Czernichow S, Elbaz A, Dugravot A, Sabia S, Hagger-Johnson G, et al. Obesity phenotypes in midlife and cognition in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Neurology (2012) 79:755–62. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182661f63

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: obesity, neurocognitive functioning, type 2 diabetes, hypertension, sleep apnea

Citation: Kiunke W, Brandl C, Georgiadou E, Gruner-Labitzke K, Horbach T, Köhler H, de Zwaan M and Müller A (2013) Performance in neurocognitive tasks in obese patients. Does somatic comorbidity matter? Front. Psychiatry 4:84. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00084

Received: 27 May 2013; Accepted: 26 July 2013;
Published online: 13 August 2013.

Edited by:

Kelly Costello Allison, University of Pennsylvania, USA

Reviewed by:

Unna N. Danner, Altrecht Eating Disorders Rintveld, Netherlands
Amanda Schurle Bruce, University of Missouri-Kansas City, USA

Copyright: © 2013 Kiunke, Brandl, Georgiadou, Gruner-Labitzke, Horbach, Köhler, de Zwaan and Müller. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Astrid Müller, Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Hannover Medical School, Carl-Neuberg-Str. 1, D-30625 Hannover, Germany e-mail: mueller.astrid@mh-hannover.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.