Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 18 July 2013
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Does Language Production Shape Language Form and Comprehension? View all 14 articles

Overview: does language production shape language form and comprehension?

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA, USA
  • 2Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, San Sebastián, Spain
  • 3IKERBASQUE. Basque Foundation for Science, San Sebastián, Spain

Analyzing how predictability, especially statistically-based predictability, affects language comprehension has become a major theme in psycholinguistics (e.g., Jurafsky, 1996; Hale, 2001; Lau et al., 2006; Staub and Clifton, 2006; Levy, 2008). This interest can be traced back to a series of papers (notably MacDonald et al., 1994) challenging the received claim that frequency is largely irrelevant to language, combined with observations that young language learners are very sensitive to statistical regularities (e.g., Saffran et al., 1996). It is now clear that adult language users comprehend frequent words, phrases, constructions, etc. more readily than less frequent ones. What is not so clear is the nature of the relation between frequency and comprehensibility. Is their relation superficial, perhaps reflecting the action of other causal factors? Is the relation causal, but mediated by processes other than a direct mapping of experience onto expectation? What gets counted in determining frequency? Is frequency just one of many factors that affect comprehension, or is it the dominant one? Whatever its nature, why does the relation between frequency and comprehension exist?

In the core target article of this Topic, Maryellen MacDonald assumes that there is a direct and immediate relation between frequency of experience and comprehensibility, and asks, what is the reason for the frequency differences? She advances the Production-Distribution-Comprehension (PDC) approach, which claims that a speaker/writer has biases that reduce production difficulty. These biases result in some linguistic forms being more common than others, and listener/readers learn to take advantage of the resulting regularities. She lays out three biases, which one commentator humorously terms “blurt, mimic, space,” and shows how they can account for some phenomena of sentence comprehension and language typology.

The Research Topic contains commentaries from a bakers dozen (and more) prominent psycholinguistic researchers. They uniformly applaud MacDonald's program, recognizing that an account of statistical regularities is needed and finding promise in basing the regularities on production constraints. But given that they are academic researchers, each commentator necessarily finds some shortcomings in MacDonald's proposal. Some note that the approach described in her paper is an approach, not an explicit, implemented, testable theory. Some suggest that there are grammatical phenomena, and grammatical differences among languages, that are not plausibly based in simple applications of the production biases MacDonald proposes. Several argue that considerations of the perceiver must play a larger role than MacDonald gives them, and some argue that an integrated account of production and comprehension demands is needed. In a related vein, some commentators argue that information structure and communication pressures play more of a role in statistical regularities than the PDC approach provides. Finally, some commentators argue for a broadening of the evidential base and even the underlying theoretical assumptions. They argue that language is not produced or comprehended in isolation, and suggest that we should consider communication not as the transmission of a message from source to receiver, but as the reduction of mutual uncertainty.

Frontiers' Research Topics format permitted this lively exchange of proposal and critiques. In her rejoinder, MacDonald acknowledges some of the current shortcomings of her approach, but also argues that some have been overcome in her other work and that others are well worth addressing. The availability of the discussion in this Research Topic will certainly stimulate researchers to evaluate the PDC proposal and to formulate alternatives, and thus advance our understanding of the nature of language.

References

Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. Proc. NAACL 2, 156–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jurafsky, D. (1996). A probabilistic model of lexical and syntactic access and disambiguation. Cogn. Sci. 20, 137–194. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2002_1

CrossRef Full Text

Lau, E., Straud, C., Plesch, S., and Phillips, C. (2006). The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis. Brain Lang. 98, 74–88. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition 106, 1126–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., and Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Pyschol. Rev. 101, 676–703. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.676

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., and Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learn-ing by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926

CrossRef Full Text

Staub, A., and Clifton, C. J. (2006). Syntactic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from either ….or. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 425–436. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Clifton C Jr. and Carreiras M (2013) Overview: does language production shape language form and comprehension? Front. Psychol. 4:458. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00458

Received: 26 June 2013; Accepted: 02 July 2013;
Published online: 18 July 2013.

Edited by:

Axel Cleeremans, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Copyright © 2013 Clifton and Carreiras. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: cec@psych.umass.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.