Skip to main content

FRONTIERS COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 13 July 2012
Sec. Neurogenomics

Putting Plasticity in Its Place


      Josef H. L. P. Sadowski* Josef H. L. P. Sadowski*Jack R. Mellor
   Jack R. Mellor
  • MRC Centre for Synaptic Plasticity, School of Physiological and Pharmacology, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK

A commentary on

Spike-timing dependent plasticity and the cognitive map
by Bush, D., Philippides, A., Husbands, P., and O’Shea, M. (2010). Front. Comput. Neurosci. 15:142. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00142

The dynamics of synaptic plasticity and the encoding of spatial information have been extensively studied in the hippocampus. Yet few attempts have been made to investigate how the two combine to produce the most striking emergent function of this system, the cognitive map (Toleman, 1948).

Here, Bush et al. present a neural network model describing how a cognitive map based on empirical findings from hippocampal electrophysiology could function in order to encode space and enable sequence learning. The model demonstrates how both functions could be accommodated within the same network architecture and using the same learning rule (Bush et al., 2010).

The network employed by the authors is based on the recurrently connected CA3 region of the hippocampus, where individual “place cells” fire maximally at specific locations within one, two, and three-dimensional space (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; O’Keefe, 1976; Hayman et al., 2011). Connection strengths are fully modifiable and are governed by spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) rules based on empirical findings from hippocampal cultures (Bi and Poo, 1998; Debanne et al., 1998; Wang et al., 2005). Here, the timing of pre- and post-synaptic activity is crucial in determining the magnitude and direction of synaptic plasticity, in contrast to purely rate-coded synaptic dynamics. In addition, changes in synaptic weights are dynamically modulated by ongoing theta-coded activity dictated by a phenomenological phase precession model (O’Keefe and Recce, 1993; Huxter et al., 2008).

When the network experiences neural activity corresponding to a series of shuttle runs across a square arena, heteroassociative connections between activated place cells become potentiated, cells with overlapping place fields develop strong bidirectional connections and asymmetric connections between cells against the direction of motion become depressed. The elevated connectivity of the activated ensemble resembles the broad development of a “cognitive map” with bidirectional connections providing detailed information about the relative proximity of individual place fields. The firing patterns of cells with overlapping place fields satisfy the STDP rule and enable connections between such cells to become potentiated, as demonstrated in slices (Isaac et al., 2009). The biasing of asymmetric connection weights toward the last direction of motion enables the encoding of recent trajectories or episodes. Thus the requirements for spatial encoding and sequence learning can be fulfilled simultaneously. The authors note that if there was only a single direction of motion, the connections between the place cells would become purely asymmetric and the cognitive map would thus remain undeveloped.

During simulated random exploration of an arena, a similar pattern of bidirectional connectivity develops between place cells encoding proximal place fields, with local asymmetric connections reflecting fragments of recent trajectories. Repeated traversals of a one dimensional route in a single direction through such a learned environment does not alter the gross structure of the map, but profoundly alters the connections of place cells that lie on the route. As before, connections following the direction of travel are significantly potentiated but those against it are depressed, suggesting information concerning recent trajectories can be embedded in the cognitive map.

The model goes some way to supporting in vivo and in vitro data demonstrating the role of synaptic plasticity within the of development of the cognitive map and the encoding of episodic memory; it also prompts a number of predictions as to when and where plasticity may be induced during these processes (Sadowski et al., 2011). However, these predictions are perhaps limited to the experimental context within which their STDP rule is rooted – that being data gleaned primarily from hippocampal cultures rather than acute brain slices (Buchanan and Mellor, 2010). The model is used to explore the consequences of dynamic plasticity modulation by theta-coded activity, yet other dynamic factors, most notably cholinergic tone, are also known to be powerful modulators of synaptic plasticity and may have a crucial role in tuning the development of the cognitive map.

One unexplored aspect of the paper relates to the “offline” dynamics of a network when such patterns of connectivity have been established. The phenomenon of place cell replay has been observed during in vivo hippocampal recordings, where sequences of place cell activity are recapitulated during sleep or rest following behavior (Skaggs and McNaughton, 1996). It is postulated that replay may facilitate the long term consolidation of episodic memory by driving correlated activity in other brain regions such the neocortex (Walker and Stickgold, 2010). In theory the potentiated asymmetric connections between place cells involved in recent route traversals would make it more likely for them to be reactivated in the same sequence during the post-encoding phase. Forward and reverse replay of place cell firing patterns have been observed in behaving rats (Foster and Wilson, 2006; Diba and Buzsaki, 2007), yet the model by Bush et al. does not provide as obvious an explanation for reverse replay than forward replay. The involvement of replay would allow learned sequences to become consolidated in the cortex, but it suggests that the consolidation of the cognitive map may be more piecemeal, if indeed it occurs at all.

Overall, the paper demonstrates how computational modeling can elegantly combine parallel strands of hippocampal electrophysiology and make defined predictions about how behavior might shape connectivity and therefore the function of neural networks. An integrated in vivo and in vitro approach is necessary to test such predictions; recording from place cells during behavior and investigating the consequences of such activity on connectivity. In addition, robust empirical data regarding the influence of neuromodulators on STDP must be acquired in order to improve the physiological relevance of this promising model of plasticity and the cognitive map.

References

Bi, G. Q., and Poo, M. M. (1998). Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type. J. Neurosci. 18, 10464–10472.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Buchanan, K. A., and Mellor, J. R. (2010). The activity requirements for spike timing dependant plasticty in the hippocampus. Front. Syn. Neurosci. 2:11. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00011

CrossRef Full Text

Bush, D., Philippides, A., Husbands, P., and O’Shea, M. (2010). Spike-timing dependent plasticity and the cognitive map. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 15:142. doi:10.3389/fncom.2010.00142

CrossRef Full Text

Debanne, D., Gahwiler, B. H., and Thompson, S. M. (1998). Long-term synaptic plasticity between pairs of individual CA3 pyramidal cells in rat hippocampal slice cultures. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 507 (Pt 1), 237–247.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diba, K., and Buzsaki, G. (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1241–1242.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foster, D. J., and Wilson, M. A. (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature 440, 680–683.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayman, R., Verriotis, M. A., Jovalekic, A., Fenton, A. A., and Jeffery, K. J. (2011). Anisotropic encoding of three-dimensional space by place cells and grid cells. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1182–1188.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huxter, J. R., Senior, T. J., Allen, K., and Csicsvari, J. (2008). Theta phase-specific codes for two-dimensional position, trajectory and heading in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 587–594.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Isaac, J. T., Buchanan, K. A., Muller, R. U., and Mellor, J. R. (2009). Hippocampal place cell firing patterns can induce long-term synaptic plasticity in vitro. J. Neurosci. 29, 6840–6850.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Keefe, J. (1976). Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Exp. Neurol. 51, 78–109.

CrossRef Full Text

O’Keefe, J., and Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175.

CrossRef Full Text

O’Keefe, J., and Recce, M. L. (1993). Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus 3, 317–330.

CrossRef Full Text

Sadowski, J. H., Jones, M. W., and Mellor, J. R. (2011). Ripples make waves: binding structured activity and plasticity in hippocampal networks. Neural Plast. 2011, 960389.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Skaggs, W. E., and McNaughton, B. L. (1996). Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science 271, 1870–1873.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Toleman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 189–208.

CrossRef Full Text

Walker, M. P., and Stickgold, R. (2010). Overnight alchemy: sleep-dependent memory evolution. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 218; author reply 218.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, H. X., Gerkin, R. C., Nauen, D. W., and Bi, G. Q. (2005). Coactivation and timing-dependent integration of synaptic potentiation and depression. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 187–193.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Sadowski JHLP and Mellor JR (2012) Putting plasticity in its place. Front. Neurosci. 6:110. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00110

Received: 19 June 2012; Accepted: 27 June 2012;
Published online: 13 July 2012.

Edited by:

Misha Tsodyks, Weizmann Institute of Science, Israel

Copyright: © 2012 Sadowski and Mellor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: josef.sadowski@bristol.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.