Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 04 March 2013
Sec. Neurotrauma

Functional MRI in the Investigation of Blast-Related Traumatic Brain Injury


      John Graner,* John Graner1,2*Terrence R. Oakes, Terrence R. Oakes1,2Louis M. French,, Louis M. French3,4,5Gerard Riedy,
   Gerard Riedy1,2
  • 1National Intrepid Center of Excellence, Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, Bethesda, MD, USA
  • 2National Capital Neuroimaging Consortium, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, Bethesda, MD, USA
  • 3Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, Bethesda, MD, USA
  • 4Center for Neuroscience and Regenerative Medicine, Bethesda, MD, USA
  • 5Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center, Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, Bethesda, MD, USA

This review focuses on the application of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to the investigation of blast-related traumatic brain injury (bTBI). Relatively little is known about the exact mechanisms of neurophysiological injury and pathological and functional sequelae of bTBI. Furthermore, in mild bTBI, standard anatomical imaging techniques (MRI and computed tomography) generally fail to show focal lesions and most of the symptoms present as subjective clinical functional deficits. Therefore, an objective test of brain functionality has great potential to aid in patient diagnosis and provide a sensitive measurement to monitor disease progression and treatment. The goal of this review is to highlight the relevant body of blast-related TBI literature and present suggestions and considerations in the development of fMRI studies for the investigation of bTBI. The review begins with a summary of recent bTBI publications followed by discussions of various elements of blast-related injury. Brief reviews of some fMRI techniques that focus on mental processes commonly disrupted by bTBI, including working memory, selective attention, and emotional processing, are presented in addition to a short review of resting state fMRI. Potential strengths and weaknesses of these approaches as regards bTBI are discussed. Finally, this review presents considerations that must be made when designing fMRI studies for bTBI populations, given the heterogeneous nature of bTBI and its high rate of comorbidity with other physical and psychological injuries.

Summary

This review focuses on the application of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate blast-related traumatic brain injury (bTBI). Relatively little is known about the exact mechanisms of neurophysiological injury and pathological and functional sequelae of bTBI. Furthermore, in mild bTBI, standard anatomical imaging techniques [MRI and computed tomography (CT)] generally fail to show focal lesions and most of the symptoms present as subjective clinical functional deficits. Therefore, an objective test of brain functionality has great potential to aid in patient diagnosis and provide a sensitive measurement to monitor disease progression and treatment. The heterogeneity of injury in the bTBI population and resulting lack of a specific anatomical target present a challenge when attempting to develop an fMRI protocol sensitive to TBI in blast victims. The goal of this review is to present the relevant body of blast-related TBI literature and, combined with a brief review of some currently used fMRI techniques, present suggestions and considerations in the development of bTBI fMRI studies.

bTBI Occurrence

Traumatic brain injury following blast exposure is quite prevalent in soldiers returning from the conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan. Although there is a range of estimates regarding the occurrence of TBI and bTBI in the recent military operations (Tanielian and Jaycox, 2008), the Joint Theater Trauma Registry has reported that, of the soldiers seen at Landstuhl Regional Medical Center, 22% had head, face, or neck injuries (Okie, 2005). The Armed Forces Health Surveillance Center reports that between January 2003 and January 2010, approximately 135,000 military service members were diagnosed with TBI, most of them mild1. One study (Murray et al., 2005) found that 78% of injuries seen in one combat field setting were due to explosions. The mechanism of injury for 87.9% of the service members with traumatic limb amputation was some form of explosive device (Stansbury et al., 2008). Another study (Hoge et al., 2008) reported that in those sustaining TBI with loss of consciousness (LOC), blast was involved in 79% of the injuries. While explosions were also a significant source of injury in World War I and World War II, accounting for 35 and 73% of the injuries, respectively, injuries sustained from explosions accounted for 78% of Operation Iraqi Freedom (OIF)/OEF injuries, the highest proportion seen in any large-scale conflict (in contrast to gunshot wounds which accounted for just 18% of the injuries from 2001 to 2005; Owens et al., 2008).

The relative numbers of TBI and bTBI cases seen in wounded military personnel returning from modern conflicts are larger than those seen in previous wars (Warden et al., 2009). This is most likely due to several factors, including advances in protective equipment (for example, thoracic armor reducing the potential for fatal pulmonary damage due to blast exposure) and in-the-field medical capabilities (Okie, 2005; Bass et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2012).

While the incidence of occurrence has risen sharply within the previous few years, there is a relatively limited body of research in blast-related TBI. A PubMed2 literature search using the keywords “traumatic brain injury” yielded 62,323 results, of which 25,616 (41%) were published after March 20, 2003, the beginning of OIF. By contrast, literature searches for articles containing the keywords “blast traumatic brain injury” or “explosive traumatic brain injury” or “explosion traumatic brain injury” yielded only 524 results, 387 of which were published after March 20, 2003. There may be several factors contributing to the relative scarcity of blast-related TBI literature. The relative levels of disparity and similarity between blast-related TBI and other forms of TBI (blunt trauma, etc.) are not well resolved. A lack of solid classification of blast-related TBI as a unique form of injury may have led to fewer blast-specific research studies. Finally, the relatively lower occurrence or observation of blast-related TBI seen prior to current military conflicts may also have contributed to a low focus on bTBI research.

Summary of Recent bTBI Publications

Several recent bTBI-related publications describe research-related and clinical challenges currently presented by bTBI. Recent blast-related animal studies have attempted to produce accurate exposure-injury models in several species (for example, Rafaels et al., 2011, 2012). One major challenge facing such models is the ability to scale exposure-injury relationships between species in order to provide accurate predictive information regarding human blast exposure (Bass et al., 2012). Ling et al. (2009) review a number of current clinical approaches for treating bTBI, highlighting many of the physical sequelae that accompany bTBI as well as methods to address them. These authors comment that a greater understanding of the physical and neurophysiological bases of bTBI is needed along with a reliable diagnostic tool for clinical evaluation.

Combat-Related TBI

For those with combat-related TBI, clinical issues can be significant. French and Parkinson (2008) describe the medical complexity in these cases, with three clinical military bTBI composite case studies providing examples of the variety and complexity of bTBI sequelae. In one case, a patient’s increased irritability led to significant behavioral changes, including involvement in an increased number of verbal arguments. All three of the individuals included in the paper reported problems with short-term memory and attention or concentration. Overall, the case reports show how polytrauma and high rates of medical comorbidity can affect the diagnostic and treatment process. In the VA polytrauma system of care, Service Members injured by blast had a wider range of physical injuries, higher levels of opioid analgesic use upon admission and discharge, reduced improvement in pain intensity after treatment, and much higher rates of psychiatric diagnoses than those injured in combat via motor vehicle collisions, falls, or other mechanisms (Clark et al., 2009).

In combat-related TBI, the emotional context in which injury occurs also matters. Service Members are routinely exposed to emotionally traumatic events at high rates (Hoge et al., 2004). For those individuals that do sustain a mild TBI with LOC, there are high rates of comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Hoge et al., 2008; Rosenfeld and Ford, 2010). It has been suggested (Kennedy et al., 2007) that damage to the pre-frontal cortex in TBI may result in disruption of neural networks involved in the regulation of anxiety, making the affected individual more vulnerable to the effects of an emotionally traumatic event. Whether or not this hypothesis may be relevant to blast-related TBI is unknown, however. Further, TBI has been shown to complicate or prolong recovery from preexisting or comorbid conditions such as PTSD (Vanderploeg et al., 2009). The complex problems facing care-givers of these TBI/PTSD patients are reviewed by Capehart and Bass (2012), who recommend an interdisciplinary approach to treatment. Previous studies (Lippa et al., 2010; Wilk et al., 2010; Belanger et al., 2011; Luethcke et al., 2011) have generally found no differences between blast and non-blast TBI on a range of symptoms such as depression, alcohol, happiness, vigor, fatigue, restlessness, anxiety, anger, and postconcussion symptoms. However, some studies have shown a relation between blast-related injury and PTSD symptom reporting (Sayer et al., 2008; Belanger et al., 2009, 2011; Kennedy et al., 2010; Lippa et al., 2010).

Application of Modern Techniques to bTBI Diagnosis

Several authors have reviewed the use of neuroimaging techniques for diagnosing bTBI. Benzinger et al. (2009) presented a discussion summary of a 2008 bTBI neuroimaging workshop in St. Louis. This report discusses how PET, CT, and MRI techniques may be applied to best address bTBI from a clinical standpoint as well as how these techniques may be incorporated into animal experiments. Van Boven et al. (2009) present a review article summarizing current and previous uses of various imaging techniques in the investigation of TBI and PTSD. In this work, the authors postulate that a wide range of MRI and PET imaging methods including diffusion tensor imaging, fMRI, MR spectroscopy, arterial spin labeling, susceptibility weighted imaging, FDG/metabolism imaging, and amyloid imaging may be of future use regarding both blast-related and non-blast-related TBI.

Initial attempts to diagnose and differentiate bTBI from non-blast TBI using individual imaging and clinical modalities have lead to disappointing results. When comparing blast to non-blast TBI, Belanger et al. (2010) saw no statistical difference in neurocognitive tests. A PET study on 12 mild bTBI demonstrated subtle hypometabolism in the cerebellum when compared to normals (Peskind et al., 2011). Mac Donald et al. (2011) and Hayes et al. (2012) show changes in DTI imaging but these reports suffer from the common theme of definition of a blast injury.

Common Themes

Despite the range of topics and views, several common threads run through current bTBI literature. First, the development of a more robust understanding of the mechanical effects primary blast-waves have on the human brain and how these effects then contribute to neurophysiological sequelae is necessary for an understanding of blast-related TBI. Secondly, there is a wide range of heterogeneity across bTBI victims, with respect to both characteristics of the causative incident as well as the course of disease progression and symptom manifestation. Thirdly, there is a need for reliable clinical tools to aid in the diagnosis and treatment planning of blast victims. The increasing availability and relative non-invasive nature of medical imaging techniques make them prime candidates for fulfilling this need. In addition, fMRI, which indirectly measures brain activation during functional tasks, may be more sensitive to damage to neural networks caused by TBI. One common theme seen in the clinics at Walter Reed is the increased cognitive effort reported by TBI patients. They often describe increased concentration and effort required to perform tasks that they could do “without thinking” prior to injury. While some of these symptoms could be related to comorbid states (as described by Capehart and Bass, 2012), sensitive diagnostic tests to evaluate and quantify the reported symptoms are needed. Unfortunately, the battery of neurocognitive testing performed is often normal in patients who describe this increased cognitive effort, suggesting that the tests might not be sensitive enough to detect the increased neural load requirements. Imaging of functional neural networks with fMRI has the potential to fill this gap. This review focuses on the challenges and considerations when applying one of these neuroimaging techniques, functional MRI, to a bTBI population.

Blast-Related Brain Injury Mechanisms

The multiple physical aspects of exposure to a blast can create a variety of specific injury mechanisms. Primary damage may be caused as the blast-wave propagates through the brain and interacts with the surrounding skull. The exact mechanisms of this type of damage are not fully understood. There is currently a widening body of literature focused on the development of computational and physical models for determining the damaging forces experienced by the brain as a blast-wave moves through it (Moore et al., 2009; Chafi et al., 2010; Nyein et al., 2010; Alley et al., 2011; Nakagawa et al., 2011; Ganpule et al., 2012; Jerusalem and Dao, 2012; Panzer et al., 2012). Results from such models may indicate regions of the brain that are more susceptible to blast-wave damage, providing guidance for which brain regions and related brain functions to investigate with neuroimaging techniques or as additional support for the imaging findings.

An unfortunate element of bTBI is the almost universal co-occurrence of other injury mechanisms beyond the primary blast-wave. Current literature decomposes the effects of the blast TBI event on the brain into as many as four categories (DePalma et al., 2005; Benzinger et al., 2009; Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein, 2010; Ling et al., 2009). Primary effects are those due to the blast-wave itself as it travels through the brain tissue. Secondary blast effects are those inflicted by objects hurled or otherwise set in motion by the blast-wave which then strike the head, resulting in blunt or penetrating trauma. The tertiary effects of a blast are those caused by the violent movement of the victim due to the blast. These can be especially devastating in the case of an enclosed environment, such as the cabin of a vehicle (Leibovici et al., 1996; Taber et al., 2006; Ling et al., 2009). Finally, some authors have identified quaternary bTBI effects due to other damaging elements associated with the nature of the explosive device used, such as radiation, heat, or the release of chemicals (DeWitt and Prough, 2009). In selecting “blast-injured” TBI subjects for investigations, it is typical to include those with secondary and tertiary effects of blast (sometimes referred to as “blast-plus,” i.e., Moore and Jaffee, 2010; Lange et al., 2012). This is largely due to the rarity of documented isolated primary blast in this population. Given this, it is to be expected that individuals will have some injury characteristics related to the exposure to the primary blast, and some related to more traditional, mechanical injury.

Secondary Injury

Blunt impacts to the head from either secondary or tertiary effects can directly lead to contusions in the brain. Such impacts can produce violent, sudden movement of the brain within the skull, also causing diffuse axonal injury (DAI) or hemorrhage. In addition to these direct effects, the injury may lead to secondary injury, such as the creation of regions of edema or ischemia in the brain (Margulies and Hicks, 2009; Svetlov et al., 2009).

Effects of Systemic Injury

Along with direct damage to the brain from the elements of the blast, damage to other organs of the body may augment the potential for brain injury. For example, exposure of the torso, if unprotected, to a primary blast-wave or objects hurled by a blast can injure the lungs. This can lead to a state of hypoxemia that presents an additional challenge to the neural system beyond the direct blast effects (Ritenour and Baskin, 2008; DeWitt and Prough, 2009). Blast victims may also sustain injuries such as limb loss leading to hemorrhage, hypovolemia, decreased brain perfusion, and hypotension. Such situations present challenges to the brain as a whole, not only to those areas directly affected by the blast.

Even if the head itself is shielded from the blast directly, there is evidence that blast impact to the lungs can lead to damage and biochemical alterations in the brain (Cernak et al., 1996, 2001). One potential mechanism for this is the kinetic transfer of energy from the lungs to the brain and central nervous system. The blast-wave overpressure may be passed through the vasculature in the lungs to that in the brain, allowing pressure-related injury to occur. Another potential method through which damage to the lungs may impact the brain is the creation of air emboli at the alveoli (Ho, 2002). These emboli may then pass through the vasculature and lead to ischemic conditions in the brain (Mayorga, 1997; Wolf et al., 2009). Recent work by Wood et al. (2012) suggests that modern body armor may greatly decrease the risk of pulmonary injury due to primary blast-wave exposure, thus limiting the potential for related systemic insult. However, this method of injury remains a possibility for unarmored blast victims.

White Matter Damage

One of the consequences often seen in blast-related and non-blast-related TBI is the degeneration of white matter neurons (Bauman et al., 2009; Cernak and Noble-Haeusslein, 2010). This damage is most commonly seen in parasagittal white matter, the corpus callosum, and the brain stem (Meythaler et al., 2001; Povlishock and Katz, 2005). Hypotheses about the mechanism by which diffuse white matter damage occurs have changed as more research has been undertaken. The cell loss most likely involves both necrotic and apoptotic processes and may continue over a period of time following the injury (Povlishock and Katz, 2005). Such white matter injury has the potential to disrupt communication between parts of cognitive networks. This disruption may in part lead to some of the cognitive deficits seen in TBI victims (MacDonald et al., 2008; Strangman et al., 2009; Levin et al., 2010), even in cases where there are no regions of detectable gray matter damage (Scheid et al., 2006).

Elements of bTBI

Injury Classification

Subjects with closed-head TBI are usually broken down into three classifications of injury severity: mild, moderate, and severe. A fourth category of TBI, penetrating TBI, is used when an object penetrates the dura and enters the brain. In military TBI, this is most commonly a bullet or metallic fragment. Classification of closed-head TBI is generally made via a combination of Glasgow Coma Scale score (GCS; Teasdale and Jennett, 1974), estimated duration of post-traumatic amnesia (PTA), and estimated duration of LOC at the time of injury (Kay et al., 1993; Maas et al., 2008). An individual with mild TBI is defined by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (ACRM; Esselman and Uomoto, 1995) as a

“… person who has had a traumatically induced physiological disruption of brain function, as manifested by at least one of the following: any period of loss of consciousness, and loss of memory for events immediately before or after the accident, any alteration of mental state at the time of the accident, and/or focal neurological deficits that may or may not be transient, so long as the severity of the injury does not exceed 1) loss of consciousness for more than 30 minutes, 2) after 30 minutes, a Glasgow Coma Scale score of 13-15, and 3) a period of post-traumatic amnesia not to exceed 24 hours.”

Additional categorization has also been proposed via structural findings in clinical MRI and CT readings (Marshall et al., 1992; Firsching et al., 2001). The presence or absence of focal lesions on MRI or CT is often used in patient participant discrimination for research studies and has been suggested in multiple publications as a potential reason for conflicting functional study results (Levine et al., 2006; Sanchez-Carrion et al., 2008b). While the degree to which these various characteristics ultimately affect group heterogeneity has not been fully elucidated, there is a general consensus of their importance when considering patient populations. As TBI severity is based on injury characteristics, rather than extent of longer-term symptom expression (i.e., headache, balance problems, memory problems, poor concentration, etc.), some individuals may be classified as having mild TBI and have significant persisting symptoms while others with a more severe TBI classification may have fewer symptoms. In one recent military TBI sample (Belanger et al., 2010) those with mild TBI reported more post concussive symptoms than a group of those with moderate to severe TBI, although this appeared to reflect level of post-traumatic stress in the population. In general however, increased severity of TBI is more likely to result in more persisting symptoms.

Time Since Injury

An important factor for a TBI patient is the time since injury. While very few longitudinal TBI imaging studies have been reported, several groups have reported functional- or diffusion-related changes within TBI populations between initial and follow-up scan sessions, both with and without cognitive training in the interim (McAllister et al., 2006; Sanchez-Carrion et al., 2008a; Kim et al., 2009). Diffusion differences have also been noted between mild TBI subjects imaged less than 3 months post-injury and those imaged more than 3 months post-injury (Rutgers et al., 2008). These neuroimaging results and general rehabilitation outcomes for patients with TBI suggest a significant degree of change over time within individual subjects. Therefore, specific attention must be paid to differences in time from injury, even on the order of a few months, when creating TBI patient study populations. These differences in time since injury may correspond to significant differences in states of injury or recovery.

History of TBI

History of prior TBI can also have an effect on a patient’s ability to recover following a subsequent TBI event. While the exact mechanism of this compounding effect is not yet known, evidence suggests that patients with previous TBI are at greater risk for suffering another TBI incident, may have greater cognitive symptom severity following another TBI incident, and may have prolonged recovery periods following another TBI incident (Gronwall and Wrightson, 1975; Gaetz et al., 2000; Guskiewicz et al., 2003). This seems to be particularly the case when the second TBI is in close temporal proximity to an earlier incident (Vagnozzi et al., 2007, 2008; Fujita et al., 2012). While this evidence is from animal studies and patients who experienced non-blast-related TBI, this compounding injury effect should be taken into consideration when creating bTBI neuroimaging study populations as well, as previous TBI may be common (Ommaya et al., 1996; Ivins et al., 2003).

Behavioral Symptoms

Clinical symptoms of bTBI patients vary dramatically from coma to mild persistent headache. Some of the commonly reported symptoms associated with mild bTBI include problems with working memory and selective attention, and feelings of mental fatigue (Dikmen et al., 1986; Alexander, 1995; Bate et al., 2001; Vanderploeg et al., 2001). The majority of these symptoms are functional in nature, making them potential targets for fMRI investigation.

Relevance of Non-Blast TBI Findings to Blast-Related TBI

As the literature findings in the previous section show, much more research has been focused on the investigation of non-blast TBI than blast-related TBI. It remains unclear, however, how relevant non-blast TBI findings are to blast-related TBI (Elder et al., 2010). Both may share some aspects of injury causation, including blunt trauma and shearing strain on brain tissue. This may then in turn lead to shared forms of injury, such as hemorrhage and DAI. However, because the precise mechanics of neural injury due to a primary blast-wave are not fully understood, the degree to which the blast may affect or bolster injury caused by other methods of trauma is not known. In addition, systemic challenges due to the blast’s effects on other major organs of the body may offer further complications not typically seen in non-blast TBI. Blast victims may also suffer from increased probability of sensory impairment, post-injury pain, and polytrauma (French, 2010). These differences in acute injury may lead to differences in general underlying injury characteristics and recovery between non-blast TBI patients and bTBI patients. The potential for such differences should be taken into consideration when attempting to evaluate the degree to which blast- and non-blast TBI, and their corresponding bodies of literature, relate. Although differences in injury characteristics likely exist, there are also likely important areas of overlap between blast and non-blast injury mechanisms, injury sequelae, and associated research findings. Certainly from a neurocognitive standpoint, no differences have been found in neuropsychological evaluations between groups of service members sustaining TBI through a blast-related mechanism versus another mechanism (Belanger et al., 2009; Lange et al., 2012).

Similarly, brain injury associated with secondary and tertiary blast effects should be considered when designing and performing neuroimaging studies with patient populations who have suffered blast-related TBI. Depending on the question under investigation, it may be appropriate to exclude subjects with positive anatomical imaging findings consistent with blunt trauma (although limited population numbers may not make this feasible) in an attempt to reduce the relative effects of these injuries on study imaging results. With regard to fMRI, localized injury visible via anatomical imaging will obviously have a significant effect on a subject’s blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal in the injured region and, potentially, regions with which the injured tissue normally communicates. However, the goal of and motivation behind a given study will ultimately dictate how secondary and tertiary blast effects are handled when forming population groups.

Standard Clinical Imaging is Inadequate to Diagnose Mild TBI

CT and MRI in Moderate to Severe TBI

The variety of potential injury mechanisms associated with TBI creates a challenge for identifying the nature and extent of damage in an individual TBI patient. In cases of moderate to severe TBI, anatomical imaging techniques such as CT and standard MRI can provide details about the localization and extent of major injuries such as bone fracture, hematomas, or hemorrhage. Other anatomical sequela such as a shift in the midline of the brain or the presence of a mass effect can also be visible with CT or MRI (Broder, 2010). This structural information about the injury can then be used to assist reparative or neuro-protective procedures (Ling et al., 2009). Jacobs et al. (2011) also found correlations between the amount of midline shift and hematoma volume present and functional outcome 6 months post-injury in a large population of moderate and severe non-blast TBI patients.

CT and MRI in Mild TBI

In cases of mild TBI (blast- and non-blast-related), regions of injury are generally less observable. CT and MRI have been used to identify regions of mild injury to some extent, mostly identifying micro-hemorrhages and contusions (Mittl et al., 1994; McAllister et al., 2001; Benzinger et al., 2009). However, detection of injury-related lesions with these imaging methods has not necessarily led to predictable patient outcome (Scheid et al., 2003; Hughes et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2006). The full extent of DAI is also not always visible via anatomical MRI and CT (Povlishock and Katz, 2005; Levine et al., 2006). Thus, current anatomical imaging techniques may not be sufficient for clinical identification and prognostic evaluation of many mild and some moderate bTBI cases.

Summary of fMRI

Functional MRI takes advantage of the different magnetic susceptibilities of oxygenated and deoxygenated blood, where signal contrast for a given volume of blood depends on the ratio of deoxygenated to oxygenated blood. For this reason the signal is referred to as a BOLD response. With standard echo-planar-imaging (EPI) sequences, images of the whole brain can be obtained in 2–3 s. This rapid imaging rate, along with the BOLD signal, allows a measure related to blood oxygenation to be made at a relatively good temporal resolution (∼2 s) relative to blood flow changes, and forms the basis of fMRI.

Hemodynamic Response Model

In order to determine which areas of the brain are activated by the presented stimuli or task, the time-dependent fMRI signal from each image voxel (3-dimensional “blocks” into which the image space is broken down) is compared to an estimate or model of the signal expected from an ideal activation. The signal model usually takes the form of a prescription for the task or stimuli presentation timing convolved with a hemodynamic response function (HRF). The HRF is the theoretical BOLD response to a single short stimulus. Convolution of this HRF with the time series of the stimuli thus creates a model of the theoretical response of a region of the brain activated by the stimuli. Those voxels with time-courses found to be statistically significantly similar to the modeled stimuli response are characterized as relevantly activated.

Task Design

The specific questions and hypothesis of an investigation play a large role in the design of the task or stimuli presented to the subject. fMRI tasks broadly fall into two categories: (i) block designs and (ii) event-related designs. Studies aimed at investigating a prolonged mental state associated with a given task typically use block designs. Event-related designs, on the other hand, attempt to investigate brain activation in response to individual short-duration events. Generally, a block paradigm is more efficient (i.e., shorter acquisition time), but is constrained by the need for a paradigm that maintains the same mental state for the duration of each block. An event-related paradigm permits additional measurements related to magnitude and shape of the HRF, allowing the estimation of hemodynamic responses associated with individual cognitive processes.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging has been used to investigate many categories of neural processes and responses such as working memory, emotional processing, attention, along with auditory and visual processing. Those with particular relevance to bTBI are discussed below.

fMRI in Working Memory

Working memory encompasses the mental processes of encoding, storing, manipulating, and retrieving information over relatively short time frames. These processes are essential for most day-to-day tasks and interactions. Difficulty with working memory is a commonly reported impairment for victims of mild to severe blast and non-blast TBI (Dikmen et al., 1986; Alexander, 1995; Vanderploeg et al., 2001; Rosenfeld and Ford, 2010). Results of imaging studies suggest that the neural network associated with working memory is made up of brain regions including areas of the pre-frontal cortex, temporal cortex, parietal cortex, and the cerebellum (Jonides et al., 1998; Smith and Jonides, 1998; D’Esposito et al., 1999a, 2000; Na et al., 2000; Owen, 2000; Ranganath and D’Esposito, 2005; Drobyshevsky et al., 2006).

Regions Associated with Aspects of Working Memory

Results also suggest correlations between certain brain regions and specific functional aspects of working memory. The dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) has been implicated in assisting with both the manipulation and maintenance of information while the vasolateral pre-frontal cortex (VLPFC) may be more involved with maintenance alone (Smith and Jonides, 1998; D’Esposito et al., 1999b, 2000; Na et al., 2000; Owen, 2000). Regions of the posterior parietal lobe also show evidence of helping to mediate information storage (Jonides et al., 1998; Smith and Jonides, 1998) while regions of the temporal cortex appear to be specifically associated with visual working memory processes (Ranganath and D’Esposito, 2005). There is also evidence to suggest that regions in the left hemisphere are more involved with verbal working memory tasks while the regions in the right hemisphere are more involved with spatial working memory tasks (Smith and Jonides, 1998; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Strangman et al., 2008).

The reliance on multiple regions in a wide range of locations in the brain may make working memory especially susceptible to diffuse TBI-related injuries. Working memory function may be impacted not only by gray matter damage due to hemorrhage or contusion, but also by damage to white matter tracts connecting the various brain regions involved. For instance, damage to the uncinate fasciculus or the superior longitudinal fasciculus could disrupt communication between the frontal lobe and the temporal lobe or the parietal lobe, respectively. Such disruption could slow or limit working memory processes.

N-Back Task

Of the various tasks that have been used in fMRI studies to investigate working memory, some of the most common are the N-back task and various forms of a delayed recall (DR) task. In the N-back task, the subject is presented with a rapid series of stimuli and asked to compare each stimulus to one of those previously presented. For each stimulus, the subject must respond whether it is the same or different than the stimulus shown some number (“N”) back of previous stimuli. Typically, the N-back task is run with N = 1, 2, or 3 stimuli back. A “0-back” version can also be used, in which the subject is given an initial target stimulus and simply responds based on whether each subsequent stimulus is the same or different than the single target stimulus. As the task progresses in the 1-, 2-, or 3-back cases, subjects must continually update and maintain information in working memory. The 3-back level is considered very difficult for most people, taxing the working memory networks to a high degree. There are many variations on the N-back task, including the type of stimuli used (letters, numbers, pictures, etc.) and the method of presentation (visual, auditory, etc.).

The nature of the N-back task allows several points of measurement to be made on the same subject. By increasing N the task can be made more difficult while maintaining the same overall task structure. This allows responses to multiple levels of working memory challenge to be compared within the same subject. The difficulty of the 3-back task should be taken into account when interpreting results, however, as some subjects may exhibit neural activation due to processes not directly related to working memory, such as stress or frustration (Qin et al., 2009).

The N-back task has typically shown activation associated with working memory in the pre-frontal cortex as well as the temporal and parietal cortices in healthy controls (Cohen et al., 1997; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Perlstein et al., 2003). It has also shown differences between controls and non-blast TBI patients (McAllister et al., 1999, 2006; Smits et al., 2009). While both groups were seen to activate similar general regions when carrying out the N-back task, the specific extent of activation corresponding to different levels of working memory challenge differed between healthy controls and TBI patients.

Activation of multiple regions across different sections of the brain provides a potential advantage when attempting to investigate the effects and extent of bTBI within an individual. Especially in cases of mild bTBI, where anatomical imaging may not provide a robust localization of injury, the ability to examine multiple areas with a single functional study may provide useful diagnostic information. This examination of multiple neural regions, along with the N-back task’s ability to target various degrees of working memory (Figure 1), makes it a potentially powerful tool for investigating a common functional sequela of bTBI.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Working memory tasks typically show activation in the bilateral and superior frontal cortex as well as in parts of the superior bilateral parietal cortex. The highlighted regions showed significantly different activation between an individual performing a 1-Back task versus a 2-Back task.

Delayed Recall Task

Another task often used to investigate working memory is the DR task. The general form of this task involves presenting the subject with a number of stimulus objects to encode, such as a row of letters. The subject maintains the objects in working memory for a short time (on the order of 5–10 s), and is then presented a probe asking the subject to recall one or multiple objects from the initial stimulus group. Alterations to this task can ask the subject to perform mental manipulation of the stimulus objects during the delay period, invoking executive control as well as memory maintenance and recall.

An advantage of the delayed recall task, as with the N-back, is the ability to present varying degrees of working memory challenge (for example, by changing the number of initial stimulus objects to encode). Inclusion of manipulation of the objects held in memory allows a direct within-subject comparison between the encoding-retrieval tasks and the encoding-manipulation-retrieval tasks.

Like the N-back task, DR tasks have been shown to activate various brain regions associated with working memory in healthy controls including the frontal cortex, DLPFC, VLPFC, and parietal regions (D’Esposito et al., 1999a, 2000; Chein and Fiez, 2001; Turner and Levine, 2008). Working memory-related activation has been seen in regions of the cerebellum using DR tasks as well (Desmond et al., 1997; Durisko and Fiez, 2010; Marvel and Desmond, 2010). The task has also been used to show differences in regional activation between healthy controls and subjects with DAI due to motor vehicle accidents (Turner and Levine, 2008). Although the number of subjects is limited (8 patients and 12 controls), DAI patients showed higher task-related activation in several PFC and posterior regions of the brain than did healthy subjects, suggesting that engagement of more of the brain was necessary for the DAI patients to perform the task.

Several characteristics of DR tasks are attractive for investigation of bTBI. Along with the ability to probe the working memory network, the potential to observe more process-specific (encoding, maintenance, manipulation, and retrieval) differences in activation patterns within a subject may help in the localization of mild bTBI and perhaps lead to a better understanding of the subject’s specific functional impairment.

fMRI in Selective Attention

Many individuals with bTBI also report problems with attention. In general, maintaining attention is the ability to consistently direct mental effort and vigilance toward a target task or goal. This requires the discrimination of related stimuli from unrelated or distracting stimuli as well as control of mental focus. Like working memory, attention is necessary for many day-to-day tasks, especially in a work environment. Impairment of attention due to bTBI can significantly impact quality of life.

Regions Associated with Selective Attention

The attention neural network consists of several regions of the brain including the DLPFC, ACC, striatum, VLPFC, parietal cortex, and possibly areas of the cerebellum (Bush, 2010). Although the precise role of each of these regions with regard to attention is not fully known, several general trends are suggested in the literature. Areas in the PFC and ACC are thought to be associated with directing cognitive control (Duncan and Owen, 2000; MacDonald et al., 2000; Milham et al., 2003; Haupt et al., 2009). Regions of the dorsal ACC have also been implicated in processes involving response selection and stimulus conflict management (Bush et al., 1998; Bush, 2010). Areas of the parietal cortex involved with attention have mostly been associated with control and direction of visual attention (Corbetta, 1998; Corbetta et al., 2000; Culham and Kanwisher, 2001; Culham, 2002; Bush, 2010). Finally, it has been suggested that the striatum is involved in carrying out behavior related to current situational goals (Haber, 2003).

As with working memory, attention may be affected by TBI through damage to gray matter components of the associated neural network or through damage to white matter tracts that facilitate communication between these components. Axonal injury in the anterior limb of the internal capsule or to the anterior corona radiata may inhibit communication between regions in the frontal cortex and the striata or anterior cingulate.

Stroop Task

A common functional MRI task used to engage and investigate selective attention is the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935; Bush et al., 1998; Carter et al., 2000; Leung et al., 2000; Gruber et al., 2002; Smits et al., 2009). While there is a variety of specific implementations of the Stroop task, the basic principle is to ask the subject to respond to a given characteristic of each presented stimulus while the stimuli themselves exhibit secondary characteristics that are either congruent or incongruent with the characteristic to which the subject is supposed to respond (Stroop, 1935). The classic form of this task is to ask subjects to respond to various words denoting color printed in congruent (for example, the word “red” written in red font) or incongruent (the word “blue” written in red font) font colors. Subjects are asked to either state, as quickly and accurately as they can, the color spelled out by the word or the color text in which the word is written. The incongruous combinations typically lead to longer response times and altered neuronal response patterns. Other implementations involve the subject responding to stimulus characteristics such as the number of words presented at the same time or the relative size of words presented while the words themselves spell out congruent or incongruent number or size characteristics (Bush et al., 1998; Melcher and Gruber, 2009).

Functional MRI studies of healthy adults have shown activations associated with the Stroop task and the response interference condition it presents in areas including the anterior cingulate cortex, medial and inferior frontal lobe, and inferior parietal lobe (Bush et al., 1998; Leung et al., 2000; Gruber et al., 2002; Haupt et al., 2009). The task has also been used to investigate differences between activation associated with selective attention in healthy controls and mild non-blast TBI patients (Smits et al., 2009). A positive correlation was found between increased levels of activation in brain regions involved with selective attention and patients’ reported post concussive symptoms. The Stroop task’s long history of use in a variety of contexts and its focus on a common functional sequela of bTBI make it an attractive diagnostic candidate.

fMRI in Emotional Response

Regions Associated with Emotional Response

Victims of bTBI may also show alterations in emotional state. Emotional processing and response have generally been associated with regions of the limbic system, including the amygdala, thalamus, and hippocampus, as well as areas of the medial and inferior PFC and basal ganglia (Van Eden and Buijs, 2000; Phan et al., 2002; Morgane et al., 2005; Drevets et al., 2008). Neuroimaging and other studies have produced results suggesting that some of the regions in this system display activity with emotion-specific correlation. For example, activation in the amygdala seems to be most strongly correlated with fear (Phan et al., 2002; Ohman, 2005), while activation in the basal ganglia seems to be correlated with happiness or disgust (see Phan et al., 2002 for review). However, it is also clear that these correlations do not represent exclusive relationships; activation in the amygdala is seen in emotional responses other than fear (Phan et al., 2002; Zald, 2003; Peper et al., 2006). As with other mental processes, development of a robust anatomy-function relationship model for emotional processing is an ongoing endeavor.

Emotional Response Tasks

Functional MRI tasks used to investigate emotion typically present the subject with emotionally charged stimuli and seek to observe elicited changes in brain activity. Stimuli can be in a variety of forms including emotionally charged images, sounds, or even tastes or smells (Small et al., 2003; Drobyshevsky et al., 2006; Koch et al., 2007; Vuilleumier and Pourtois, 2007; Gamer and Buchel, 2009). One of the most commonly used forms of stimuli is emotionally charged faces. Throughout the fMRI task, subjects can be presented with faces expressing a variety of emotions such as happiness, anger, fear, or concern. Using photo manipulation software, the relative degree to which the faces epitomize these emotions can also be changed (Lennox et al., 2004). Thus, the presentation of emotional faces allows the subject to be tasked with both a wide range of emotions as well as a range of the degree of these emotions. Such tests have been used in the investigation of various mood disorders, including bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder (MDD; Lennox et al., 2004; Surguladze et al., 2005; Leppanen, 2006).

The emotional Stroop task has also been used to investigate alterations in response to emotionally charged words. As with the classic Stroop task, in the emotional Stroop subjects are asked to respond to a characteristic of presented words, such as the number of words shown at once or the color in which the words are presented. In the emotional variant of the task, however, words of various emotional valences are used and subjects’ performances in response to various categories of words are compared; for example, performance in response to generally positive words (“gift,” “love,” etc.) may be compared to performance in response to generally negative words (“hate,” “murder,” etc.). Williams et al. (1996) provide a review of many applications of the emotional Stroop, showing behavioral differences in a range of anxiety, depressive, and other psychological conditions. Recent imaging studies using PET and fMRI have also shown functional activation differences related to emotional category of stimulus word presentation in patients with PTSD (Shin et al., 2001; Bremner et al., 2004; McNally, 2006). While the emotional Stroop task may provide information on brain regions affected by bTBI, its sensitivity to psychological disorders highlights the fact that these must be taken into consideration when designing and interpreting results from fMRI studies using this task.

Emotional Response and bTBI

The relationship between bTBI and the alteration of emotional processing is far from apparent. Conclusive findings have yet to be made tying region-specific injury to emotional consequences (Schwarzbold et al., 2008). Furthermore, the high degree of comorbidity between bTBI and PTSD and MDD presents an additional challenge to accurate diagnosis (Elder et al., 2010).

Resting State fMRI

The previous sections have described task-oriented fMRI techniques, which require the subject to observe and respond to various stimuli while in the scanner. In contrast to these study designs, resting state fMRI is performed as the subject simply rests in the scanner. Even when “resting,” the human brain is not inactive. When the subject is resting in the scanner, fluctuations are seen in the BOLD signal. For a time these fluctuations were considered to be noise by many researchers, but close inspection has revealed that they are not entirely random. Specifically, various regions of the brain may show very similar fluctuation patterns. Resting state fMRI takes advantage of these spontaneous fluctuations to investigate connectivity within the brain (for an in-depth review, the reader is referred to Fox and Raichle, 2007).

The link between shared spontaneous BOLD activity and functional connectivity is the idea that brain regions involved in the same mental process will exhibit similar activity patterns, even in a “resting” state. Thus, coherence in spontaneous BOLD fluctuations of different regions may provide an indirect measure of network connectivity. This idea is supported by results in the literature showing that spontaneous BOLD fluctuations can indeed be used to identify several well-known networks, including the somatomotor system and the visual system (Lowe et al., 1998; Cordes et al., 2000; De Luca et al., 2005; Fox and Raichle, 2007).

Default Mode Network

Raichle et al. (2001) described a “default mode network” (DMN) within the brain. The regions in this network exhibit decreased activation associated with many goal-oriented or attention-demanding tasks and were therefore proposed to facilitate a “default” functional state within the brain. This network includes the medial pre-frontal cortex, precuneus, posterior cingulate, and bilateral parietal lobe (Raichle et al., 2001; Figure 2). Various regions of the DMN may be responsible for processes such as introspective or self-referential thought, monitoring of the external environment, emotional processing (Gusnard et al., 2001; Broyd et al., 2009), spontaneous cognition, and predicting possible actions demanded by the environment (Raichle and Snyder, 2007). Dysfunction of the DMN may contribute to functional deficits (Broyd et al., 2009). Weissman et al. (2006) found that less task-induced deactivation of regions in the DMN correlated with increased response time in healthy subjects performing an attention task. Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos (2007) presented a “default mode interference hypothesis” which suggests that inappropriate activation of the DMN during attention-demanding tasks may detract from mental effort put toward the task, leading to distraction and poorer behavioral performance. It is possible that damage to the DMN due to bTBI could bolster such inappropriate spontaneous activation and lead to attention-deficits often seen in bTBI victims.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The default mode network (DMN) includes regions in the medial pre-frontal cortex, precuneus, and bilateral parietal cortex. Highlighted regions in the images were all correlated with the same component from an independent component analysis of an individual’s resting state fMRI data. The color scale reflects correlation z-scores, with a lower threshold of z = 3.

Applications of Resting State fMRI

Resting state fMRI connectivity analysis has been applied to several neurological disorders and diseases (Voss and Schiff, 2009) including Parkinson’s Disease (Wu et al., 2009; Helmich et al., 2010), schizophrenia (Ke et al., 2010; Skudlarski et al., 2010), MDD (Anand et al., 2005a,b; Greicius et al., 2007), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Tian et al., 2006; Kelly et al., 2007; Castellanos et al., 2009), Alzheimer’s Disease (Li et al., 2002; Supekar et al., 2008; Koch et al., 2012), and post-traumatic stress disorder (Bluhm et al., 2009b). In most cases, evidence was found of a relationship between disease state and network connectivity in brain regions believed to be associated with the disease under investigation. Nakamura et al. (2009) have analyzed resting network plasticity in a small group of TBI patients, showing change in network characteristics over a period of recovery. The capacity to correlate network connectivity with behavioral sequelae presents an important potential use of resting state fMRI in the investigation of bTBI.

Advantages for the Study of bTBI

Resting state fMRI studies also present several logistical advantages when investigating a patient population such as bTBI victims (Fox and Greicius, 2010). Resting state fMRI does not depend on the subject’s ability to perform a given task. This may be particularly relevant to blast-related TBI populations, where rates of comorbidity with other major injuries such as limb loss may be high. Such major injuries may preclude a subject from performing a task requiring a specific motor response. Resting state fMRI studies also reduce the number of possible complicating factors when attempting to replicate protocols across sites, reducing variability in multi-site studies and assisting in the accumulation of larger numbers of potentially comparable sets of patient data.

In addition to these logistical advantages, resting state connectivity analysis is not limited to investigating a single functional network but allows the identification of multiple brain networks (De Luca et al., 2006; Mantini et al., 2007; Voss and Schiff, 2009). This ability to interrogate the connectivity of multiple networks simultaneously could be very powerful in locating specific injuries in individuals within a bTBI population.

Oculomotor Tasks and TBI

Vestibular symptoms, such as dizziness, are also common following bTBI. Several recent studies have shown the potential for visual oculomotor tasks and testing to be sensitive to the presence of blast and non-blast-related TBI (Suh et al., 2006; Kraus et al., 2010; Scherer et al., 2011). In these studies, poor or altered performance on oculomotor tasks correlated with the presence of TBI or increased TBI symptom severity.

Oculomotor tasks used in combination with fMRI, EEG, and PET imaging studies have helped identify regions of the brain associated with performance of eye saccades (for in-depth review, see McDowell et al., 2008). As with the more complex mental processes mentioned above, the generation of eye saccades involves areas spread across the brain, including occipital cortex, lateral frontal cortex, and parietal cortex, as well as subcortical regions including the striatum, thalamus, and regions of the cerebellum. Furthermore, more complex eye movement, such as predictive saccades, may involve more frontal brain regions (Simo et al., 2005; McDowell et al., 2008). Thus, performance of eye saccades, especially those of greater complexity, may be hindered as a result DAI due to bTBI.

Two of the studies mentioned above (Suh et al., 2006; Kraus et al., 2010) specifically show alteration in predictive eye movement (where the visual stimuli are presented in a learnable pattern) in subjects with non-blast-related TBI, suggesting the presence of frontal lobe damage or white matter damage between regions of the brain associated with predictive saccade performance. Although these studies did not involve subjects with bTBI, predictive saccade tasks show promise for being sensitive to the presence of TBI.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows potential as a means of confirming the neurophysiological underpinnings of the sensitivity of predictive saccade tasks to the presence of bTBI. Previous work has already shown differences between fMRI activation patterns elicited by a predictive saccade task and a visually guided saccade task (in which the visual stimuli appear randomly, thus excluding any predictive learning element from the task) in healthy controls (Suh et al., 2006). Similar studies involving bTBI subjects as well as healthy controls may provide imaging results that support the current behavioral findings.

Comorbidity of PTSD and Major Depressive Disorder with mTBI and Considerations for fMRI

Post-traumatic stress disorder and MDD have been increasingly common in U.S. soldiers returning from current conflicts (Tanielian and Jaycox, 2008). While a causal relationship between mild TBI and PTSD or MDD has not been identified, several studies have shown correlations between mTBI and these psychological health issues in military populations returning from deployment (Hoge et al., 2008; Schneiderman et al., 2008; Vanderploeg et al., 2012; Bazarian et al., 2013). Correlations between TBI and MDD have also been reported in non-military populations (Jorge et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Bombardier et al., 2010). Vanderploeg et al. (2012) found that injury due to blast did not significantly increase risk of developing PTSD or MDD over injury due to other mechanisms in a National Guard population, but this finding has not been consistent in other populations. And, while concurrent diagnosis of bTBI and PTSD is difficult due to common symptoms and problems with self-report assessments (Capehart and Bass, 2012), there may still be relatively high rates of PTSD or MDD within a population of patients who have experienced bTBI.

Several previously published reviews highlight both functional and anatomical neuroimaging findings associated with PTSD and MDD (Leppanen, 2006; Bremner, 2007; Lorenzetti et al., 2009; Holzschneider and Mulert, 2011; Hughes and Shin, 2011; Robinson and Shergill, 2011; Stuhrmann et al., 2011; Delvecchio et al., 2012). The findings that most directly relate to the application of fMRI to individuals with comorbid bTBI and PTSD or MDD involve alterations in brain activation patterns attributable solely to these two disorders. Various emotionally charged or trauma-related tasks used in previous fMRI studies of PTSD have revealed activation alterations in regions including the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus, ACC, thalamus, medial frontal gyrus, amygdala, mPFC, and ventral frontoparietal region (Lanius et al., 2001, 2003; Shin et al., 2001, 2005; Yang et al., 2004; Sakamoto et al., 2005). Several previous fMRI studies of MDD using emotional stimuli have shown activation differences associated with MDD in the amygdala (Siegle et al., 2002; Surguladze et al., 2005; Leppanen, 2006; Stuhrmann et al., 2011). Activation alterations associated with MDD in response to emotional facial stimuli have also been reported in regions including the insula, parahippocampal gyrus, putamen, caudate, and orbitofrontal cortex (Stuhrmann et al., 2011; Delvecchio et al., 2012). Directly relevant to bTBI, Matthews et al. (2011) found differences in amygdala activation associated with the presence of MDD in a population consisting of subjects who had all experienced bTBI; differences in amygdala response were found when comparing subjects with bTBI and MDD to those who only had bTBI.

Differences in activation patterns between control subjects and subjects with PTSD have also been seen in response to emotionally neutral, non-trauma-related fMRI tasks. Using an auditory oddball paradigm (in which subjects were asked to provide a motor response to relatively infrequent target tone stimuli interspersed within more frequent standard tone stimuli), Bryant et al. (2005) saw increased ACC and left amygdala activation in PTSD subjects relative to controls. Moores et al. (2008) reported abnormal activation associated with PTSD in bilateral DLPFC and inferior parietal lobe during the memory maintenance period of a working memory task. During the memory updating periods of this task the PTSD group showed relatively less activation in hippocampus, anterior cingulate, and brainstem pons. Astur et al. (2006) also reported a negative correlation between PTSD severity and hippocampal activation when subjects performed an fMRI adaptation of a water maze task. Finally, Geuze et al. (2008) reported that, during performance of a neutral word-pair encoding and retrieval task subjects with PTSD showed less activation in the frontal cortex and greater activation in the temporal cortex relative to controls while encoding information, and less activation in the right frontal cortex, bilateral middle temporal gyri, and left posterior hippocampus relative to controls while retrieving information.

The presence of PTSD or MDD has also been shown to correspond to alterations in resting state fMRI data. Studies have reported PTSD-related changes in resting state connectivity between regions of the limbic system and cortical regions or between the PCC and cortical or subcortical regions. A seed-based analysis performed by Yin et al. (2011) showed group differences in connectivity measures between the thalamus and cortical regions including frontal gyri and ACC when comparing a PTSD patient group with healthy controls. Two studies comparing combat veterans with and without PTSD reported increased connectivity between the amygdala and the insula associated with PTSD (Rabinak et al., 2011; Sripada et al., 2012). Sripada et al. (2012) also found decreased connectivity measures between the amygdala and the hippocampus in the PTSD group. Studies looking specifically at connectivity with the PCC have found PTSD-related connectivity alterations between this region and a number of other regions throughout the brain, including parts of the occipital, temporal, and frontal lobes, the insula, the ACC, the amygdala, and the hippocampus (Bluhm et al., 2009a; Qin et al., 2012).

Resting state studies of MDD have reported a variety of changes in connectivity associated with MDD. While, in general, these studies do not seem to reach a consensus about functional connectivity changes introduced by MDD, several show changes in the DMN. A recent study by Zhu et al. (2012) that investigated changes in the functional connectivity of the DMN showed increased connectivity in the mPFC and ACC and decreased connectivity in the PCC with the rest of the DMN in patients with MDD. Using seed regions based on MDD-related changes in gray matter volume, Ma et al. (2012) found alterations in resting state functional connectivity between the middle temporal gyrus and portions of the DMN and between the right caudate and portions of the frontal cortex associated with MDD, while Guo et al. (2012) saw decreased cross-hemispheric signal correlation in the mPFC and PCC in patients with MDD. Other resting state studies investigating MDD have shown altered connectivity between other cortical and subcortical regions (Anand et al., 2005a; Cao et al., 2012; Tang et al., 2012; Ma et al., 2013). Finally, Horn et al. (2010) found increased functional connectivity between the pregenual ACC and the anterior insula in a subset of severely depressed patients.

Given the rates of comorbidity of PTSD and MDD with mTBI and the effects these disorders can have on brain activation patterns in response to a variety of fMRI tasks, measurements of PTSD and MDD presence or severity [such as the PTSD checklist (Weathers et al., 1993) and the Beck Depression Inventory 2 (Beck et al., 1996)] should be taken on subjects involved in bTBI fMRI studies. In a military population, the PTSD checklist (PCL-C or PCL-M; Weathers et al., 1993) is frequently used, as is the Beck Depression Inventory 2 (Beck et al., 1996). In some cases, a more definitive and careful assessment of PTSD, such as the CAPS (Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale) should be considered. These measures should then be included in the analysis model as possible explanations of variance in activation or connectivity measures.

Statistical Considerations for fMRI Relevant to bTBI Populations

Over the past decade fMRI has become increasingly sophisticated and has been used to investigate increasingly complex questions about the nature of human neural function. Proper interpretation of fMRI data, however, relies upon statistically robust data acquisition and analysis. This dependency stems from the relatively small BOLD signal (changes on the order of 1–5%) which is invariably accompanied by several sources of noise. Since an fMRI experiment is typically composed of multiple measures compared to an ideal model, the signal as well as the noise is difficult to estimate. Furthermore, most fMRI results are not a simple magnitude or effect size, but rather are cast in terms of statistical reliability. Care must be taken when interpreting statistical results, as this will obviously affect any neuro-functional conclusions.

Group Analysis Considerations

Given the inherent heterogeneity of bTBI and traumatic brain injury in general, there are several important considerations to be made when interpreting group analysis results. Several papers have addressed the effects of inhomogeneous activation within populations consisting of healthy subjects These effects may be even greater in TBI populations.

Thirion et al. (2007) present an analysis of the reliability of group-wise activation maps for an event-related study design. They define reliability in two ways: stability of each voxel with regard to its identity as active or inactive, and how frequently significant clusters appear within the same locations across the subjects in a population. Working with data sets from 78 subjects they break the population into smaller groups of various sizes and compare the results from each sub-group for each sub-group size. The stability of voxel identity and agreement between cluster locations within a sub-group increased as the size of the sub-groups was increased. Their results suggest the need for between 20 and 25 subjects within a defined homogenous group for reliable statistical analyses of this event-related study design (Thirion et al., 2007). Along the same lines, Friston et al. (1999) have shown that, for an appropriately sensitive test, close to 20 conjunctional subject results are needed for a statistically reliable determination that 85–90% of a population will exhibit that result.

Murphy and Garavan (2004) also presented empirical work investigating the relative effect of group analysis size on statistically significant results from a GO/NO-GO event-related study design. In this work, they compared the statistical activation results from an analysis of a full group of 58 healthy volunteers to sub-groups of various sizes. Their data showed a need for about 50 subjects in the sub-group to obtain 80% power (defined by the percent of voxels seen in the full group activation map also seen in the sub-group activation map) for p-value thresholds ranging from 0.000001 to 0.001. However, even at smaller sub-group levels the majority of voxels reported as activated in the sub-groups were also seen as activated in the full group activation map. With a sub-group size of 20 subjects, 80% of the activated voxels seen in the sub-group activation map were also seen in the full group activation map for p-value thresholds from 0.01 to 0.0001. This behavior led to the conclusion that the sub-group activation maps contained regions of false negative voxels (i.e., loss of sensitivity) but not necessarily a large number of false-positive voxels (Murphy and Garavan, 2004; i.e., specificity is not greatly hampered).

Desmond and Glover (2002) present a statistical power analysis of a simulated block study design. They take into consideration the variability in the fMRI signal due to both inter- and intra-subject sources, the size of the experimental effect seen in the fMRI data, the number of time points per block condition, and the number of subjects. With an experimental effect size of 0.5%, intra- and inter-subject variability sizes of 0.75 and 0.5%, respectively, and a desired alpha value of 0.05, it was found that 12 subjects were needed to maintain 80% statistical power (Desmond and Glover, 2002). Keeping all other variables the same, changing the desired alpha to a more conservative 0.002 necessitated 21–22 subjects to maintain 80% power. Changes in inter-subject variability (which may be particularly relevant to bTBI) also obviously affected the number of subjects required to maintain statistical power. With an increase of this variability from 0.5 to 0.7%, the number of subjects needed for 80% power at an alpha of 0.05 increased from 12 to approximately 20.

These papers highlight statistical issues that are especially pertinent to group analysis in bTBI studies. First, group size will have a large impact on the reliability of group-wise statistical maps. One of the common potential weaknesses in current fMRI TBI study literature is the relatively low number of patients in each study. The arguments presented above show that extreme caution must be taken when interpreting group analysis results from small groups, especially when comparing patient group results with results from a control population of a different size. The papers discussed above that deal with event-related study design suggest that groups should consist of at least 20 subjects for reliable group parametric maps. Under optimal conditions fewer subjects may be needed to maintain statistical power using block study designs, but characteristics of the study including the relative size of the contrast being observed and sources of possible variability within the subject population should be taken into consideration when interpreting group analysis results.

Second, the major underlying factor contributing to the various group activation map disparities analyzed in the papers focused on event-related designs (Murphy and Garavan, 2004; Thirion et al., 2007) is differences in the individual subject activation maps, leading to heterogeneity in the group pools. Spatial heterogeneities were found in the activation results even in populations of healthy subjects performing relatively simple fMRI tasks: visual checkerboard, cued left-right button presses, computation, and sentence processing (Thirion et al., 2007); Go/No-Go (Murphy and Garavan, 2004). The level of inter-subject variability also had a large effect on the number of subjects needed to maintain statistical power in the block design study simulations performed by Desmond and Glover (2002). When considering a patient population with a potentially heterogeneous injury mechanism such as bTBI, and performing more complicated tasks investigating more subtle activation, these effects will most likely be exacerbated, leading to fairly unreliable group activation maps, maps that have regions of false negatives, or low statistical power. In these cases, the classic approach of using a voxel-wise group map for summary statistics will yield suboptimal results.

Interpretation of Results in the Presence of Comorbidities

The high rate of comorbid conditions with bTBI must be considered when interpreting results from fMRI studies of bTBI populations. Factors that may affect individuals’ performance of fMRI tasks or have a reasonable chance of affecting activity or resting state connectivity estimates in regions of interest should be taken into account in group data analysis models to avoid false-positive results or incorrect conclusions about what is driving observed group differences. Carefully designed exclusion/inclusion criteria may also help avoid these issues.

Conclusion

Given the nature of current military conflicts, bTBI has become an increasing occurrence in returning soldiers. At the same time, little is known about the precise neurobiological effects of primary blast-waves on the brain, and current clinical imaging methods cannot adequately diagnose the injury, especially in mild cases. Both task-based and resting state fMRI have the potential to provide useful objective diagnostic information associated with functional sequelae, including problems with attention, memory, and emotional processing, commonly reported in bTBI victims. Given the generally heterogeneous nature of bTBI and high rates of comorbidity with PTSD and MDD, additional considerations must be kept in mind when composing and interpreting group analysis results. Future studies should include sufficiently large (>20) patient populations and matched numbers of controls. Measures of group data heterogeneity and susceptibility to outliers would also support interpretations of group results.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by CDMRP grant to USU number PT074437.

Author Note

The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Army, Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government.

We certify that all individuals who qualify as authors have been listed; each has participated in the conception and design of this work, the writing of the document, and the approval of the submission of this version; that the document represents valid work; that if we used information derived from another source, we obtained all necessary approvals to use it and made appropriate acknowledgments in the document; and that each takes public responsibility for it. Nothing in the presentation implies any Federal/DOD/DON endorsement.

Footnotes

References

Alexander, M. P. (1995). Mild traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical management. Neurology 45, 1253–1260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alley, M. D., Schimizze, B. R., and Son, S. F. (2011). Experimental modeling of explosive blast-related traumatic brain injuries. Neuroimage 54(Suppl. 1), S45–S54.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anand, A., Li, Y., Wang, Y., Wu, J., Gao, S., Bukhari, L., et al. (2005a). Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: a functional magnetic resonance study. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1079–1088.

CrossRef Full Text

Anand, A., Li, Y., Wang, Y., Wu, J., Gao, S., Bukhari, L., et al. (2005b). Antidepressant effect on connectivity of the mood-regulating circuit: an FMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1334–1344.

Astur, R. S., St Germain, S. A., Tolin, D., Ford, J., Russell, D., and Stevens, M. (2006). Hippocampus function predicts severity of post-traumatic stress disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 234–240.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bass, C. R., Panzer, M. B., Rafaels, K. A., Wood, G., Shridharani, J., and Capehart, B. (2012). Brain injuries from blast. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 40, 185–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bate, A. J., Mathias, J. L., and Crawford, J. R. (2001). Performance on the test of everyday attention and standard tests of attention following severe traumatic brain injury. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 405–422.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bauman, R. A., Ling, G., Tong, L., Januszkiewicz, A., Agoston, D., Delanerolle, N., et al. (2009). An introductory characterization of a combat-casualty-care relevant swine model of closed head injury resulting from exposure to explosive blast. J. Neurotrauma 26, 841–860.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bazarian, J. J., Donnelly, K., Peterson, D. R., Warner, G. C., Zhu, T., and Zhong, J. (2013). The relation between posttraumatic stress disorder and mild traumatic brain injury acquired during operations enduring freedom and Iraqi freedom: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 28, 1–12.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beck, A. T., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory II Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Belanger, H. G., Kretzmer, T., Vanderploeg, R. D., and French, L. M. (2010). Symptom complaints following combat-related traumatic brain injury: relationship to traumatic brain injury severity and posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 194–199.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Belanger, H. G., Kretzmer, T., Yoash-Gantz, R., Pickett, T., and Tupler, L. A. (2009). Cognitive sequelae of blast-related versus other mechanisms of brain trauma. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 1–8.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Belanger, H. G., Proctor-Weber, Z., Kretzmer, T., Kim, M., French, L. M., and Vanderploeg, R. D. (2011). Symptom complaints following reports of blast versus non-blast mild TBI: does mechanism of injury matter? Clin. Neuropsychol. 25, 702–715.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benzinger, T. L., Brody, D., Cardin, S., Curley, K. C., Mintun, M. A., Mun, S. K., et al. (2009). Blast-related brain injury: imaging for clinical and research applications: report of the 2008 st. Louis workshop. J. Neurotrauma 26, 2127–2144.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bluhm, R., Williamson, P., Lanius, R., Theberge, J., Densmore, M., Bartha, R., et al. (2009a). Resting state default-mode network connectivity in early depression using a seed region-of-interest analysis: decreased connectivity with caudate nucleus. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 63, 754–761.

CrossRef Full Text

Bluhm, R. L., Williamson, P. C., Osuch, E. A., Frewen, P. A., Stevens, T. K., Boksman, K., et al. (2009b). Alterations in default network connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder related to early-life trauma. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 34, 187–194.

Bombardier, C. H., Fann, J. R., Temkin, N. R., Esselman, P. C., Barber, J., and Dikmen, S. S. (2010). Rates of major depressive disorder and clinical outcomes following traumatic brain injury. JAMA 303, 1938–1945.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bremner, J. D. (2007). Neuroimaging in posttraumatic stress disorder and other stress-related disorders. Neuroimaging Clin. N. Am. 17, 523–538.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bremner, J. D., Vermetten, E., Vythilingam, M., Afzal, N., Schmahl, C., Elzinga, B., et al. (2004). Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 612–620.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Broder, J. S. (2010). Head computed tomography interpretation in trauma: a primer. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 33, 821–854.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Broyd, S. J., Demanuele, C., Debener, S., Helps, S. K., James, C. J., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 279–296.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bryant, R. A., Felmingham, K. L., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M., Peduto, A. S., Rennie, C., et al. (2005). Neural networks of information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 111–118.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bush, G. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and attention networks. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 278–300.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., and Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging–validation study with functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 270–282.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cabeza, R., and Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 1–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cao, X., Liu, Z., Xu, C., Li, J., Gao, Q., Sun, N., et al. (2012). Disrupted resting-state functional connectivity of the hippocampus in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 141, 194–203.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Capehart, B., and Bass, D. (2012). Review: managing posttraumatic stress disorder in combat veterans with comorbid traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 49, 789–812.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carter, C. S., MacDonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., et al. (2000). Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944–1948.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castellanos, F. X., Kelly, C., and Milham, M. P. (2009). The restless brain: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, resting-state functional connectivity, and intrasubject variability. Can. J. Psychiatry 54, 665–672.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cernak, I., and Noble-Haeusslein, L. J. (2010). Traumatic brain injury: an overview of pathobiology with emphasis on military populations. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 30, 255–266.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cernak, I., Savic, J., Malicevic, Z., Zunic, G., Radosevic, P., Ivanovic, I., et al. (1996). Involvement of the central nervous system in the general response to pulmonary blast injury. J. Trauma 40, S100–S104.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cernak, I., Wang, Z., Jiang, J., Bian, X., and Savic, J. (2001). Ultrastructural and functional characteristics of blast injury-induced neurotrauma. J. Trauma 50, 695–706.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chafi, M. S., Karami, G., and Ziejewski, M. (2010). Biomechanical assessment of brain dynamic responses due to blast pressure waves. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 38, 490–504.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chein, J. M., and Fiez, J. A. (2001). Dissociation of verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cereb. Cortex 11, 1003–1014.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clark, M. E., Walker, R. L., Gironda, R. J., and Scholten, J. D. (2009). Comparison of pain and emotional symptoms in soldiers with polytrauma: unique aspects of blast exposure. Pain Med. 10, 447–455.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, J. D., Perlstein, W. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Noll, D. C., Jonides, J., et al. (1997). Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386, 604–608.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 831–838.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., and Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 292–297.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cordes, D., Haughton, V. M., Arfanakis, K., Wendt, G. J., Turski, P. A., Moritz, C. H., et al. (2000). Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional connectivity MR imaging. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 21, 1636–1644.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Culham, J. (2002). Dissociations within association cortex. Neuron 33, 318–320.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Culham, J. C., and Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 157–163.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Luca, M., Beckmann, C. F., De Stefano, N., Matthews, P. M., and Smith, S. M. (2006). fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29, 1359–1367.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Luca, M., Smith, S., De Stefano, N., Federico, A., and Matthews, P. M. (2005). Blood oxygenation level dependent contrast resting state networks are relevant to functional activity in the neocortical sensorimotor system. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 587–594.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delvecchio, G., Fossati, P., Boyer, P., Brambilla, P., Falkai, P., Gruber, O., et al. (2012). Common and distinct neural correlates of emotional processing in bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder: a voxel-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 22, 100–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DePalma, R. G., Burris, D. G., Champion, H. R., and Hodgson, M. J. (2005). Blast injuries. N. Engl. J. Med. 352, 1335–1342.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desmond, J. E., Gabrieli, J. D., Wagner, A. D., Ginier, B. L., and Glover, G. H. (1997). Lobular patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working-memory and finger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI. J. Neurosci. 17, 9675–9685.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Desmond, J. E., and Glover, G. H. (2002). Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: statistical power analyses. J. Neurosci. Methods 118, 115–128.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D’Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., Ballard, D., and Lease, J. (1999a). Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Cogn. 41, 66–86.

CrossRef Full Text

D’Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., Jonides, J., and Smith, E. E. (1999b). The neural substrate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 7514–7519.

CrossRef Full Text

D’Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., and Rypma, B. (2000). Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 3–11.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeWitt, D. S., and Prough, D. S. (2009). Blast-induced brain injury and posttraumatic hypotension and hypoxemia. J. Neurotrauma 26, 877–887.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dikmen, S., McLean, A., and Temkin, N. (1986). Neuropsychological and psychosocial consequences of minor head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 49, 1227–1232.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drevets, W. C., Price, J. L., and Furey, M. L. (2008). Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 93–118.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drobyshevsky, A., Baumann, S. B., and Schneider, W. (2006). A rapid fMRI task battery for mapping of visual, motor, cognitive, and emotional function. Neuroimage 31, 732–744.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duncan, J., and Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Durisko, C., and Fiez, J. A. (2010). Functional activation in the cerebellum during working memory and simple speech tasks. Cortex 46, 896–906.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elder, G. A., Mitsis, E. M., Ahlers, S. T., and Cristian, A. (2010). Blast-induced mild traumatic brain injury. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 33, 757–781.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Esselman, P. C., and Uomoto, J. M. (1995). Classification of the spectrum of mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 9, 417–424.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Firsching, R., Woischneck, D., Klein, S., Reissberg, S., Dohring, W., and Peters, B. (2001). Classification of severe head injury based on magnetic resonance imaging. Acta Neurochir. (Wien) 143, 263–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fox, M. D., and Greicius, M. (2010). Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:19. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019

CrossRef Full Text

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

French, L. M. (2010). Military traumatic brain injury: an examination of important differences. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1208, 38–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

French, L. M., and Parkinson, G. W. (2008). Assessing and treating veterans with traumatic brain injury. J. Clin. Psychol. 64, 1004–1013.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., and Worsley, K. J. (1999). How many subjects constitute a study? Neuroimage 10, 1–5.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fujita, M., Wei, E. P., and Povlishock, J. T. (2012). Intensity- and interval-specific repetitive traumatic brain injury can evoke both axonal and microvascular damage. J. Neurotrauma 29, 2172–2180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaetz, M., Goodman, D., and Weinberg, H. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence for the cumulative effects of concussion. Brain Inj. 14, 1077–1088.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gamer, M., and Buchel, C. (2009). Amygdala activation predicts gaze toward fearful eyes. J. Neurosci. 29, 9123–9126.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ganpule, S., Alai, A., Plougonven, E., and Chandra, N. (2012). Mechanics of blast loading on the head models in the study of traumatic brain injury using experimental and computational approaches. Biomech. Model. Mechanobiol. PMID:22832705. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Geuze, E., Vermetten, E., Ruf, M., De Kloet, C. S., and Westenberg, H. G. (2008). Neural correlates of associative learning and memory in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 659–669.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greicius, M. D., Flores, B. H., Menon, V., Glover, G. H., Solvason, H. B., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 429–437.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gronwall, D., and Wrightson, P. (1975). Cumulative effect of concussion. Lancet 2, 995–997.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gruber, S. A., Rogowska, J., Holcomb, P., Soraci, S., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2002). Stroop performance in normal control subjects: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 16, 349–360.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guo, W., Liu, F., Dai, Y., Jiang, M., Zhang, J., Yu, L., et al. (2012). Decreased interhemispheric resting-state functional connectivity in first-episode, drug-naive major depressive disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 41C, 24–29.

Guskiewicz, K. M., McCrea, M., Marshall, S. W., Cantu, R. C., Randolph, C., Barr, W., et al. (2003). Cumulative effects associated with recurrent concussion in collegiate football players: the NCAA Concussion Study. JAMA 290, 2549–2555.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 4259–4264.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haber, S. N. (2003). The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 26, 317–330.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haupt, S., Axmacher, N., Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2009). Activation of the caudal anterior cingulate cortex due to task-related interference in an auditory Stroop paradigm. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3043–3056.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayes, J. P., Morey, R. A., and Tupler, L. A. (2012). A case of frontal neuropsychological and neuroimaging signs following multiple primary-blast exposure. Neurocase 18, 258–269.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helmich, R. C., Derikx, L. C., Bakker, M., Scheeringa, R., Bloem, B. R., and Toni, I. (2010). Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in Parkinson’s disease. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1175–1186.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ho, A. M. (2002). A simple conceptual model of primary pulmonary blast injury. Med. Hypotheses 59, 611–613.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoge, C. W., Castro, C. A., Messer, S. C., McGurk, D., Cotting, D. I., and Koffman, R. L. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 13–22.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A. L., Engel, C. C., and Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 453–463.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holzschneider, K., and Mulert, C. (2011). Neuroimaging in anxiety disorders. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 13, 453–461.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Horn, D. I., Yu, C., Steiner, J., Buchmann, J., Kaufmann, J., Osoba, A., et al. (2010). Glutamatergic and resting-state functional connectivity correlates of severity in major depression – the role of pregenual anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:33. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00033

CrossRef Full Text

Hughes, D. G., Jackson, A., Mason, D. L., Berry, E., Hollis, S., and Yates, D. W. (2004). Abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging seen acutely following mild traumatic brain injury: correlation with neuropsychological tests and delayed recovery. Neuroradiology 46, 550–558.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hughes, K. C., and Shin, L. M. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Expert Rev. Neurother. 11, 275–285.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ivins, B. J., Schwab, K. A., Warden, D., Harvey, L. T., Hoilien, M. A., Powell, C. O., et al. (2003). Traumatic brain injury in U.S. army paratroopers: prevalence and character. J. Trauma 55, 617–621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobs, B., Beems, T., Van Der Vliet, T. M., Diaz-Arrastia, R. R., Borm, G. F., and Vos, P. E. (2011). Computed tomography and outcome in moderate and severe traumatic brain injury: hematoma volume and midline shift revisited. J. Neurotrauma 28, 203–215.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jerusalem, A., and Dao, M. (2012). Continuum modeling of a neuronal cell under blast loading. Acta Biomater. 8, 3360–3371.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., et al. (1998). The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory. J. Neurosci. 18, 5026–5034.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jorge, R. E., Robinson, R. G., Moser, D., Tateno, A., Crespo-Facorro, B., and Arndt, S. (2004). Major depression following traumatic brain injury. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 42–50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kay, T., Harrington, D. E., Adams, R., Anderson, T., Berrol, S., Cicerone, K., et al. (1993). Definition of mild traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 3, 86–87.

Ke, M., Zou, R., Shen, H., Huang, X., Zhou, Z., Liu, Z., et al. (2010). Bilateral functional asymmetry disparity in positive and negative schizophrenia revealed by resting-state fMRI. Psychiatry Res. 182, 30–39.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, A. M., Margulies, D. S., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Recent advances in structural and functional brain imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 9, 401–407.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kennedy, J. E., Jaffee, M. S., Leskin, G. A., Stokes, J. W., Leal, F. O., and Fitzpatrick, P. J. (2007). Posttraumatic stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder-like symptoms and mild traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 44, 895–920.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kennedy, J. E., Leal, F. O., Lewis, J. D., Cullen, M. A., and Amador, R. R. (2010). Posttraumatic stress symptoms in OIF/OEF service members with blast-related and non-blast-related mild TBI. NeuroRehabilitation 26, 223–231.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kim, E., Lauterbach, E. C., Reeve, A., Arciniegas, D. B., Coburn, K. L., Mendez, M. F., et al. (2007). Neuropsychiatric complications of traumatic brain injury: a critical review of the literature (a report by the ANPA Committee on Research). J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 19, 106–127.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, Y. H., Yoo, W. K., Ko, M. H., Park, C. H., Kim, S. T., and Na, D. L. (2009). Plasticity of the attentional network after brain injury and cognitive rehabilitation. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair 23, 468–477.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koch, K., Pauly, K., Kellermann, T., Seiferth, N. Y., Reske, M., Backes, V., et al. (2007). Gender differences in the cognitive control of emotion: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 45, 2744–2754.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koch, W., Teipel, S., Mueller, S., Benninghoff, J., Wagner, M., Bokde, A. L., et al. (2012). Diagnostic power of default mode network resting state fMRI in the detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 466–478.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kraus, M. F., Little, D. M., Wojtowicz, S. M., and Sweeney, J. A. (2010). Procedural learning impairments identified via predictive saccades in chronic traumatic brain injury. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 23, 210–217.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lange, R. T., Pancholi, S., Brickell, T. A., Sakura, S., Bhagwat, A., Merritt, V., et al. (2012). Neuropsychological outcome from blast versus non-blast: mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. military service members. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 595–605.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., Neufeld, R. W., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1920–1922.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Hopper, J., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., et al. (2003). Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 204–210.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leibovici, D., Gofrit, O. N., Stein, M., Shapira, S. C., Noga, Y., Heruti, R. J., et al. (1996). Blast injuries: bus versus open-air bombings–a comparative study of injuries in survivors of open-air versus confined-space explosions. J. Trauma 41, 1030–1035.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lennox, B. R., Jacob, R., Calder, A. J., Lupson, V., and Bullmore, E. T. (2004). Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania. Psychol. Med. 34, 795–802.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leppanen, J. M. (2006). Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 34–39.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leung, H. C., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., Peterson, B. S., and Gore, J. C. (2000). An event-related functional MRI study of the stroop color word interference task. Cereb. Cortex 10, 552–560.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levin, H., Wilde, E. A., Troyanskaya, M., Petersen, N., Scheibel, R., Newsome, M., et al. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging of mild to moderate blast related TBI and its sequelae. J. Neurotrauma 27, 689–694.

CrossRef Full Text

Levine, B., Fujiwara, E., O’connor, C., Richard, N., Kovacevic, N., Mandic, M., et al. (2006). In vivo characterization of traumatic brain injury neuropathology with structural and functional neuroimaging. J. Neurotrauma 23, 1396–1411.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Li, S. J., Li, Z., Wu, G., Zhang, M. J., Franczak, M., and Antuono, P. G. (2002). Alzheimer disease: evaluation of a functional MR imaging index as a marker. Radiology 225, 253–259.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ling, G., Bandak, F., Armonda, R., Grant, G., and Ecklund, J. (2009). Explosive blast neurotrauma. J. Neurotrauma 26, 815–825.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lippa, S. M., Pastorek, N. J., Benge, J. F., and Thornton, G. M. (2010). Postconcussive symptoms after blast and nonblast-related mild traumatic brain injuries in Afghanistan and Iraq war veterans. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 856–866.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lorenzetti, V., Allen, N. B., Fornito, A., and Yucel, M. (2009). Structural brain abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a selective review of recent MRI studies. J. Affect. Disord. 117, 1–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lowe, M. J., Mock, B. J., and Sorenson, J. A. (1998). Functional connectivity in single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. Neuroimage 7, 119–132.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luethcke, C. A., Bryan, C. J., Morrow, C. E., and Isler, W. C. (2011). Comparison of concussive symptoms, cognitive performance, and psychological symptoms between acute blast-versus nonblast-induced mild traumatic brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 36–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ma, C., Ding, J., Li, J., Guo, W., Long, Z., Liu, F., et al. (2012). Resting-state functional connectivity bias of middle temporal gyrus and caudate with altered gray matter volume in major depression. PLoS ONE 7:e45263. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045263

CrossRef Full Text

Ma, Q., Zeng, L. L., Shen, H., Liu, L., and Hu, D. (2013). Altered cerebellar-cerebral resting-state functional connectivity reliably identifies major depressive disorder. Brain Res. 1495, 86–94.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maas, A. I., Stocchetti, N., and Bullock, R. (2008). Moderate and severe traumatic brain injury in adults. Lancet Neurol. 7, 728–741.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mac Donald, C. L., Johnson, A. M., Cooper, D., Nelson, E. C., Werner, N. J., Shimony, J. S., et al. (2011). Detection of blast-related traumatic brain injury in U.S. military personnel. N. Engl. J. Med. 364, 2091–2100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacDonald, A. W. III, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288, 1835–1838.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacDonald, C. L., Schwarze, N., Vaishnavi, S. N., Epstein, A. A., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2008). Verbal memory deficit following traumatic brain injury: assessment using advanced MRI methods. Neurology 71, 1199–1201.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mantini, D., Perrucci, M. G., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170–13175.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Margulies, S., and Hicks, R. (2009). Combination therapies for traumatic brain injury: prospective considerations. J. Neurotrauma 26, 925–939.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marshall, L. F., Marshall, S. B., Klauber, M. R., Van Berkum Clark, M., Eisenberg, H., Jane, J. A., et al. (1992). The diagnosis of head injury requires a classification based on computed axial tomography. J. Neurotrauma 9(Suppl. 1), S287–S292.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marvel, C. L., and Desmond, J. E. (2010). The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex 46, 880–895.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., O’connell, R. M., Reinhardt, L. E., and Moseley, S. A. (2011). A multimodal imaging study in U.S. veterans of operations Iraqi and enduring freedom with and without major depression after blast-related concussion. Neuroimage 54(Suppl. 1), S69–S75.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mayorga, M. A. (1997). The pathology of primary blast overpressure injury. Toxicology 121, 17–28.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McAllister, T. W., Flashman, L. A., McDonald, B. C., and Saykin, A. J. (2006). Mechanisms of working memory dysfunction after mild and moderate TBI: evidence from functional MRI and neurogenetics. J. Neurotrauma 23, 1450–1467.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McAllister, T. W., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A., Sparling, M. B., Johnson, S. C., Guerin, S. J., et al. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain injury: a functional MRI study. Neurology 53, 1300–1308.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McAllister, T. W., Sparling, M. B., Flashman, L. A., and Saykin, A. J. (2001). Neuroimaging findings in mild traumatic brain injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 775–791.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDowell, J. E., Dyckman, K. A., Austin, B. P., and Clementz, B. A. (2008). Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn. 68, 255–270.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNally, R. J. (2006). Cognitive abnormalities in post-traumatic stress disorder. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 10, 271–277.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Melcher, T., and Gruber, O. (2009). Decomposing interference during Stroop performance into different conflict factors: an event-related fMRI study. Cortex 45, 189–200.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meythaler, J. M., Peduzzi, J. D., Eleftheriou, E., and Novack, T. A. (2001). Current concepts: diffuse axonal injury-associated traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 1461–1471.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., and Barad, V. (2003). Competition for priority in processing increases prefrontal cortex’s involvement in top-down control: an event-related fMRI study of the stroop task. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 212–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mittl, R. L., Grossman, R. I., Hiehle, J. F., Hurst, R. W., Kauder, D. R., Gennarelli, T. A., et al. (1994). Prevalence of MR evidence of diffuse axonal injury in patients with mild head injury and normal head CT findings. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 15, 1583–1589.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Moore, D. F., and Jaffee, M. S. (2010). Military traumatic brain injury and blast. NeuroRehabilitation 26, 179–181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Moore, D. F., Jerusalem, A., Nyein, M., Noels, L., Jaffee, M. S., and Radovitzky, R. A. (2009). Computational biology – modeling of primary blast effects on the central nervous system. Neuroimage 47(Suppl. 2), T10–T20.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moores, K. A., Clark, C. R., McFarlane, A. C., Brown, G. C., Puce, A., and Taylor, D. J. (2008). Abnormal recruitment of working memory updating networks during maintenance of trauma-neutral information in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 163, 156–170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morgane, P. J., Galler, J. R., and Mokler, D. J. (2005). A review of systems and networks of the limbic forebrain/limbic midbrain. Prog. Neurobiol. 75, 143–160.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murphy, K., and Garavan, H. (2004). An empirical investigation into the number of subjects required for an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 22, 879–885.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murray, C. K., Reynolds, J. C., Schroeder, J. M., Harrison, M. B., Evans, O. M., and Hospenthal, D. R. (2005). Spectrum of care provided at an echelon II medical unit during Operation Iraqi Freedom. Mil. Med. 170, 516–520.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Na, D. G., Ryu, J. W., Byun, H. S., Choi, D. S., Lee, E. J., Chung, W. I., et al. (2000). Functional MR imaging of working memory in the human brain. Korean J. Radiol. 1, 19–24.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nakagawa, A., Manley, G. T., Gean, A., Ohtani, K., Armonda, R., Tsukamoto, A., et al. (2011). Mechanisms of primary blast-induced traumatic brain injury: insights from shock wave research. J. Neurotrauma 28, 1101–1119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nakamura, T., Hillary, F. G., and Biswal, B. B. (2009). Resting network plasticity following brain injury. PLoS ONE 4:e8220. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008220

CrossRef Full Text

Nyein, M. K., Jason, A. M., Yu, L., Pita, C. M., Joannopoulos, J. D., Moore, D. F., et al. (2010). In silico investigation of intracranial blast mitigation with relevance to military traumatic brain injury. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 20703–20708.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ohman, A. (2005). The role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 953–958.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Okie, S. (2005). Traumatic brain injury in the war zone. N. Engl. J. Med. 352, 2043–2047.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ommaya, A. K., Ommaya, A. K., Dannenberg, A. L., and Salazar, A. M. (1996). Causation, incidence, and costs of traumatic brain injury in the U.S. military medical system. J. Trauma 40, 211–217.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Owen, A. M. (2000). The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 33–43.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Owens, B. D., Kragh, J. F. Jr., Wenke, J. C., Macaitis, J., Wade, C. E., and Holcomb, J. B. (2008). Combat wounds in operation Iraqi Freedom and operation Enduring Freedom. J. Trauma 64, 295–299.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Panzer, M. B., Myers, B. S., Capehart, B. P., and Bass, C. R. (2012). Development of a finite element model for blast brain injury and the effects of CSF cavitation. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 40, 1530–1544.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peper, M., Herpers, M., Spreer, J., Hennig, J., and Zentner, J. (2006). Functional neuroimaging of emotional learning and autonomic reactions. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 342–354.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perlstein, W. M., Dixit, N. K., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). Prefrontal cortex dysfunction mediates deficits in working memory and prepotent responding in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 25–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peskind, E. R., Petrie, E. C., Cross, D. J., Pagulayan, K., McCraw, K., Hoff, D., et al. (2011). Cerebrocerebellar hypometabolism associated with repetitive blast exposure mild traumatic brain injury in 12 Iraq war veterans with persistent post-concussive symptoms. Neuroimage 54(Suppl. 1), S76–S82.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., and Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 16, 331–348.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Povlishock, J. T., and Katz, D. I. (2005). Update of neuropathology and neurological recovery after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 20, 76–94.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Qin, L. D., Wang, Z., Sun, Y. W., Wan, J. Q., Su, S. S., Zhou, Y., et al. (2012). A preliminary study of alterations in default network connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder patients following recent trauma. Brain Res. 1484, 50–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., Van Marle, H. J., Luo, J., and Fernandez, G. (2009). Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 25–32.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rabinak, C. A., Angstadt, M., Welsh, R. C., Kenndy, A. E., Lyubkin, M., Martis, B., et al. (2011). Altered amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2:62. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00062

CrossRef Full Text

Rafaels, K., Bass, C. R., Salzar, R. S., Panzer, M. B., Woods, W., Feldman, S., et al. (2011). Survival risk assessment for primary blast exposures to the head. J. Neurotrauma 28, 2319–2328.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rafaels, K. A., Bass, C. R., Panzer, M. B., Salzar, R. S., Woods, W. A., Feldman, S. H., et al. (2012). Brain injury risk from primary blast. J. Trauma Acute Care Surg. 73, 895–901.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., and Snyder, A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37, 1083–1090; discussion 1097–1089.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ranganath, C., and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Directing the mind’s eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 175–182.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ritenour, A. E., and Baskin, T. W. (2008). Primary blast injury: update on diagnosis and treatment. Crit. Care Med. 36, S311–S317.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robinson, B. L., and Shergill, S. S. (2011). Imaging in posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 24, 29–33.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosenfeld, J. V., and Ford, N. L. (2010). Bomb blast, mild traumatic brain injury and psychiatric morbidity: a review. Injury 41, 437–443.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rutgers, D. R., Fillard, P., Paradot, G., Tadie, M., Lasjaunias, P., and Ducreux, D. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging characteristics of the corpus callosum in mild, moderate, and severe traumatic brain injury. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 29, 1730–1735.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sakamoto, H., Fukuda, R., Okuaki, T., Rogers, M., Kasai, K., Machida, T., et al. (2005). Parahippocampal activation evoked by masked traumatic images in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 26, 813–821.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanchez-Carrion, R., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., Falcon, C., Bargallo, N., Roig, T., et al. (2008a). A longitudinal fMRI study of working memory in severe TBI patients with diffuse axonal injury. Neuroimage 43, 421–429.

CrossRef Full Text

Sanchez-Carrion, R., Gomez, P. V., Junque, C., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Falcon, C., Bargallo, N., et al. (2008b). Frontal hypoactivation on functional magnetic resonance imaging in working memory after severe diffuse traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 25, 479–494.

CrossRef Full Text

Sayer, N. A., Chiros, C. E., Sigford, B., Scott, S., Clothier, B., Pickett, T., et al. (2008). Characteristics and rehabilitation outcomes among patients with blast and other injuries sustained during the global war on terror. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 163–170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scheid, R., Preul, C., Gruber, O., Wiggins, C., and Von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). Diffuse axonal injury associated with chronic traumatic brain injury: evidence from T2*-weighted gradient-echo imaging at 3 T. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 24, 1049–1056.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Scheid, R., Walther, K., Guthke, T., Preul, C., and Von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Cognitive sequelae of diffuse axonal injury. Arch. Neurol. 63, 418–424.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scherer, M. R., Burrows, H., Pinto, R., Littlefield, P., French, L. M., Tarbett, A. K., et al. (2011). Evidence of central and peripheral vestibular pathology in blast-related traumatic brain injury. Otol. Neurotol. 32, 571–580.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneiderman, A. I., Braver, E. R., and Kang, H. K. (2008). Understanding sequelae of injury mechanisms and mild traumatic brain injury incurred during the conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan: persistent postconcussive symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Epidemiol. 167, 1446–1452.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schwarzbold, M., Diaz, A., Martins, E. T., Rufino, A., Amante, L. N., Thais, M. E., et al. (2008). Psychiatric disorders and traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 4, 797–816.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Shin, L. M., Whalen, P. J., Pitman, R. K., Bush, G., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2001). An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 932–942.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shin, L. M., Wright, C. I., Cannistraro, P. A., Wedig, M. M., McMullin, K., Martis, B., et al. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 273–281.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2002). Can’t shake that feeling: event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 693–707.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simo, L. S., Krisky, C. M., and Sweeney, J. A. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of anticipatory behavior: dissociation between sensory-driven and memory-driven systems. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1982–1991.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Skudlarski, P., Jagannathan, K., Anderson, K., Stevens, M. C., Calhoun, V. D., Skudlarska, B. A., et al. (2010). Brain connectivity is not only lower but different in schizophrenia: a combined anatomical and functional approach. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 61–69.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., and Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron 39, 701–711.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 12061–12068.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smits, M., Dippel, D. W., Houston, G. C., Wielopolski, P. A., Koudstaal, P. J., Hunink, M. G., et al. (2009). Post concussion syndrome after minor head injury: brain activation of working memory and attention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2789–2803.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 977–986.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sripada, R. K., King, A. P., Garfinkel, S. N., Wang, X., Sripada, C. S., Welsh, R. C., et al. (2012). Altered resting-state amygdala functional connectivity in men with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 241–249.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stansbury, L. G., Lalliss, S. J., Branstetter, J. G., Bagg, M. R., and Holcomb, J. B. (2008). Amputations in U.S. military personnel in the current conflicts in Afghanistan and Iraq. J. Orthop. Trauma 22, 43–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strangman, G. E., Goldstein, R., O’neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Kelkar, K., Supelana, C., Burke, D., et al. (2009). Neurophysiological alterations during strategy-based verbal learning in traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair 23, 226–236.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Strangman, G. E., O’neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Goldstein, R., Kelkar, K., Katz, D. I., Burke, D., et al. (2008). Prediction of memory rehabilitation outcomes in traumatic brain injury by using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 974–981.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J. Exp. Psychol. 643–662.

CrossRef Full Text

Stuhrmann, A., Suslow, T., and Dannlowski, U. (2011). Facial emotion processing in major depression: a systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 1, 10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suh, M., Kolster, R., Sarkar, R., McCandliss, B., and Ghajar, J. (2006). Deficits in predictive smooth pursuit after mild traumatic brain injury. Neurosci. Lett. 401, 108–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Supekar, K., Menon, V., Rubin, D., Musen, M., and Greicius, M. D. (2008). Network analysis of intrinsic functional brain connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000100. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000100

CrossRef Full Text

Surguladze, S., Brammer, M. J., Keedwell, P., Giampietro, V., Young, A. W., Travis, M. J., et al. (2005). A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 201–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Svetlov, S. I., Larner, S. F., Kirk, D. R., Atkinson, J., Hayes, R. L., and Wang, K. K. (2009). Biomarkers of blast-induced neurotrauma: profiling molecular and cellular mechanisms of blast brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 26, 913–921.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taber, K. H., Warden, D. L., and Hurley, R. A. (2006). Blast-related traumatic brain injury: what is known? J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 18, 141–145.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tang, Y., Kong, L., Wu, F., Womer, F., Jiang, W., Cao, Y., et al. (2012). Decreased functional connectivity between the amygdala and the left ventral prefrontal cortex in treatment-naive patients with major depressive disorder: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychol. Med. 1–7. doi:10.1017/S0033291712002759

CrossRef Full Text

Tanielian, T., and Jaycox, L. (2008). Invisible Wounds of War: Psychological and Cognitive Injuries, Their Consequences, and Services to Assist Recovery. Available at: http://www.rand.org/pubs/monographs/MG720

Teasdale, G., and Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet 2, 81–84.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thirion, B., Pinel, P., Meriaux, S., Roche, A., Dehaene, S., and Poline, J. B. (2007). Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. Neuroimage 35, 105–120.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, L., Jiang, T., Wang, Y., Zang, Y., He, Y., Liang, M., et al. (2006). Altered resting-state functional connectivity patterns of anterior cingulate cortex in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 400, 39–43.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, G. R., and Levine, B. (2008). Augmented neural activity during executive control processing following diffuse axonal injury. Neurology 71, 812–818.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vagnozzi, R., Signoretti, S., Tavazzi, B., Floris, R., Ludovici, A., Marziali, S., et al. (2008). Temporal window of metabolic brain vulnerability to concussion: a pilot 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopic study in concussed athletes–part III. Neurosurgery 62, 1286–1295; discussion 1295–1286.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vagnozzi, R., Tavazzi, B., Signoretti, S., Amorini, A. M., Belli, A., Cimatti, M., et al. (2007). Temporal window of metabolic brain vulnerability to concussions: mitochondrial-related impairment–part I. Neurosurgery 61, 379–388; discussion 388–379.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Boven, R. W., Harrington, G. S., Hackney, D. B., Ebel, A., Gauger, G., Bremner, J. D., et al. (2009). Advances in neuroimaging of traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 46, 717–757.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Eden, C. G., and Buijs, R. M. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of the prefrontal cortex: autonomic interactions. Prog. Brain Res. 126, 49–62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vanderploeg, R. D., Belanger, H. G., and Curtiss, G. (2009). Mild traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder and their associations with health symptoms. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 90, 1084–1093.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vanderploeg, R. D., Belanger, H. G., Horner, R. D., Spehar, A. M., Powell-Cope, G., Luther, S. L., et al. (2012). Health outcomes associated with military deployment: mild traumatic brain injury, blast, trauma, and combat associations in the Florida National Guard. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 1887–1895.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vanderploeg, R. D., Crowell, T. A., and Curtiss, G. (2001). Verbal learning and memory deficits in traumatic brain injury: encoding, consolidation, and retrieval. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 185–195.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voss, H. U., and Schiff, N. D. (2009). MRI of neuronal network structure, function, and plasticity. Prog. Brain Res. 175, 483–496.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vuilleumier, P., and Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia 45, 174–194.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Warden, D. L., French, L. M., Shupenko, L., Fargus, J., Riedy, G., Erickson, M. E., et al. (2009). Case report of a soldier with primary blast brain injury. Neuroimage 47(Suppl. 2), T152–T153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J., and Keane, T. (1993). “The PTSD checklist (PCL): reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility,” in Annual Convention of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, San Antonio, TX.

Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., and Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilk, J. E., Thomas, J. L., McGurk, D. M., Riviere, L. A., Castro, C. A., and Hoge, C. W. (2010). Mild traumatic brain injury (concussion) during combat: lack of association of blast mechanism with persistent postconcussive symptoms. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 25, 9–14.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, J. M., Mathews, A., and MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol. Bull. 120, 3–24.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolf, S. J., Bebarta, V. S., Bonnett, C. J., Pons, P. T., and Cantrill, S. V. (2009). Blast injuries. Lancet 374, 405–415.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wood, G. W., Panzer, M. B., Shridharani, J. K., Matthews, K. A., Capehart, B. P., Myers, B. S., et al. (2012). Attenuation of blast pressure behind ballistic protective vests. Inj. Prev.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wu, T., Wang, L., Chen, Y., Zhao, C., Li, K., and Chan, P. (2009). Changes of functional connectivity of the motor network in the resting state in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci. Lett. 460, 6–10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, P., Wu, M. T., Hsu, C. C., and Ker, J. H. (2004). Evidence of early neurobiological alternations in adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 370, 13–18.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yin, Y., Jin, C., Hu, X., Duan, L., Li, Z., Song, M., et al. (2011). Altered resting-state functional connectivity of thalamus in earthquake-induced posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Res. 1411, 98–107.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 41, 88–123.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhu, X., Wang, X., Xiao, J., Liao, J., Zhong, M., Wang, W., et al. (2012). Evidence of a dissociation pattern in resting-state default mode network connectivity in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 611–617.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: blast injuries, traumatic brain injury, review, military injury, functional magnetic resonance imaging

Citation: Graner J, Oakes TR, French LM and Riedy G (2013) Functional MRI in the investigation of blast-related traumatic brain injury. Front. Neurol. 4:16. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00016

Received: 22 November 2012; Paper pending published: 05 December 2012;
Accepted: 09 February 2013; Published online: 04 March 2013.

Edited by:

Mattias Sköld, Uppsala University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Roger Wood, Swansea University, UK
Fredrik Clausen, Uppsala University, Sweden
Bruce P. Capehart, Duke University, USA

Copyright: © 2013 Graner, Oakes, French and Riedy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: John Graner, Neuroimaging Department, National Intrepid Center of Excellence, Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, 8901 Wisconsin Avenue, Bethesda, MD 20889, USA. e-mail: john.l.graner.civ@health.mil

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.