Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 November 2013
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards a psychological and neuroscientific account of the wandering mind View all 35 articles

A penny for your thoughts: dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing

\r\nJessica R. Andrews-Hanna*Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna1*Roselinde H. KaiserRoselinde H. Kaiser2Amy E. J. TurnerAmy E. J. Turner1Andrew E. ReinebergAndrew E. Reineberg2Detre GodinezDetre Godinez1Sona DimidjianSona Dimidjian2Marie T. Banich,Marie T. Banich1,2
  • 1Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
  • 2Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA

A core aspect of human cognition involves overcoming the constraints of the present environment by mentally simulating another time, place, or perspective. Although these self-generated processes confer many benefits, they can come at an important cost, and this cost is greater for some individuals than for others. Here we explore the possibility that the costs and benefits of self-generated thought depend, in part, upon its phenomenological content. To test these hypotheses, we first developed a novel thought sampling paradigm in which a large sample of young adults recalled several recurring thoughts and rated each thought on multiple content variables (i.e., valence, specificity, self-relevance, etc.). Next, we examined multi-level relationships among these content variables and used a hierarchical clustering approach to partition self-generated thought into distinct dimensions. Finally, we investigated whether these content dimensions predicted individual differences in the costs and benefits of the experience, assessed with questionnaires measuring emotional health and wellbeing. Individuals who characterized their thoughts as more negative and more personally significant scored higher on constructs associated with Depression and Trait Negative Affect, whereas those who characterized their thoughts as less specific scored higher on constructs linked to Rumination. In contrast, individuals who characterized their thoughts as more positive, less personally significant, and more specific scored higher on constructs linked to improved wellbeing (Mindfulness). Collectively, these findings suggest that the content of people’s inner thoughts can (1) be productively examined, (2) be distilled into several major dimensions, and (3) account for a large portion of variability in their functional outcomes.

Introduction

“You are today where your thoughts have brought you; you will be tomorrow where your thoughts take you.” – James Allen (1864–1912), author.

A wealth of recent cognitive and neuroscientific research suggests that humans spend a remarkable amount of time engaged in self-generated thought (Singer, 1966; Klinger, 1971; Klinger and Cox, 1987; Kane et al., 2007; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Song and Wang, 2012), an internal mode of cognition characterized by its capacity to overcome the constraints of the external environment (Smallwood, 2013). Supporting its frequent occurrence, self-generated thought is associated with a wide variety of benefits, enabling us to confront future challenges, solve problems, and navigate our social world (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006; Baars, 2010; Christoff et al., 2011; McMillan et al., 2013; Mooneyham and Schooler, 2013; Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). At the same time, numerous studies suggest the experience can be associated with significant costs, including disrupted task performance and poor psychological wellbeing (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006; Moberly and Watkins, 2008; Watkins, 2008; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; McVay and Kane, 2010; Mooneyham and Schooler, 2013; Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). In fact, a variety of mental health disorders are defined by impairments arising from self-generated thought (Harvey et al., 2004), and such findings suggest that the adaptive and maladaptive manifestation of the experience varies widely across individuals. Understanding the factors that give rise to this variability could help individuals harness the beneficial aspects of self-generated thought and, in doing so, improve their psychological wellbeing.

Here we tackle this research objective by testing recent hypotheses that the content of self-generated thought represents an important factor underlying its costs and benefits (Watkins, 2008; Mar et al., 2012; Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). The content regulation hypothesis proposes that self-generated thoughts are particularly beneficial for individuals who are able to regulate the content of the experience to positive or productive topics (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). In line with this hypothesis, we predicted that individuals who primarily engaged in positive or productive thoughts would exhibit improved emotional health and wellbeing, whereas negative or unproductive thought content would be indicative of poor emotional health and wellbeing. Whereas existing studies primarily focus on the frequency of self-generated thought or a single aspect of thought content (i.e. valence) – typically assessed in the context of cued autobiographical memory retrieval paradigms, mood induction paradigms, or ongoing experimental tasks – we examined several aspects of self-generated thoughts that occupy participants’ minds throughout their daily lives. By developing a novel thought sampling paradigm in which participants generate several recurring thoughts and rate each thought on numerous content-specific variables, we were able to (1) quantify normative estimates of thought content in a large group of adults, (2) examine multi-level relationships among content variables, and (3) explore which content variables predicted unique variance in the costs and benefits of the experience.

We focused on individual differences in depressive symptoms as an important marker of poor emotional wellbeing because (1) major depressive disorder (MDD) is the leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization, 2008), (2) depressive symptoms predict significant distress and impairment at subclinical levels of severity (Judd et al., 1998), and (3) individuals with a current or past history of depression often exhibit intrusive self-generated thoughts focused on negative self-schema (Beck, 1967; Watkins, 2008). Depressed individuals also “overgeneralize” based on single instances (Beck, 1967) and exhibit overgeneral autobiographical memories that tend to be associated with a broad range of unconstructive consequences (Williams et al., 2007; Watkins, 2008). However, it is neither clear whether overgeneral memory applies to the types of self-generated thoughts that occur in daily life, nor whether these characteristics are driven by the affective symptoms of depression or the repetitive styles of thinking associated with the disorder. To disentangle the effects of negative affect and repetitive thought on the content of self-generated thought, we also examined individual differences in rumination, a repetitive style of thinking focused on “one’s symptoms of distress and the circumstances surrounding these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1997, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). We hypothesized that individuals with higher levels of depressive symptoms would exhibit thought content characterized as more negative, more self-focused and – to the degree that they also ruminate – less specific.

To explore which aspects of thought content predict adaptive functional outcomes, we examined individual differences in mindfulness – a purposeful, present-minded, and non-judgmental mode of awareness linked to improved health and emotional wellbeing (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Bishop, 2004). When measured as a trait, mindfulness seems to buffer against symptoms of depression and rumination (Brown and Ryan, 2003), and elements of mindfulness have been incorporated into effective treatment therapies for a variety of mental health disorders, including depression and anxiety (Kabat-Zinn, 1990; Segal et al., 2002; Baer, 2003). Mindful individuals report lower frequencies of negative automatic thoughts and a heightened ability to let go of such thoughts (Frewen et al., 2008; see also Evans and Segerstrom, 2011), and mindfulness-based interventions have been shown to alter the nature of self-generated thoughts in similar ways (Frewen et al., 2008). When applied to chronic depression, these therapies have also shown promise in increasing the specificity and perceived likelihood of achieving individuals’ life goals (Crane et al., 2012). Based on these findings, we predicted that dispositional mindfulness would negatively correlate with depression and rumination, and would be associated with thought content characterized as more positive, less self-focused, and more specific.

Material and Methods

Participants

Seventy-six young adults (mean age: 21.6 years; 18–35 years; 38 male) participated in the study for paid compensation. To facilitate interdisciplinary analysis for future studies, participants were recruited from a larger database of individuals from the University of Colorado Boulder and the greater Boulder/Denver community who had previously completed a battery of self-report questionnaires, behavioral tasks, and functional neuroimaging measures in our laboratory. Though participants within this database were previously screened for an absence of a current or previous self-reported psychiatric illness, nearly 30% of participants exhibited scores on the Beck Depression Inventory indicative of mild to moderate depression (see below), consistent with prior findings that depressive symptoms often go undiagnosed and untreated (Eisenberg et al., 2007). All procedures were carried out in accordance with the University of Colorado Boulder’s Institutional Review Board.

Materials and Procedure

Thought sampling paradigm

In a single session, all participants completed an autobiographical thought sampling paradigm, developed for the present study to quantitatively assess the phenomenological content of a broad range of self-generated thoughts and enable computation of reliable within-subject correlations. Participants recalled 36 different thoughts that had recently been on their mind in daily life, generated a three-word description for each thought, and rated each thought on a variety of different content variables, including self-relevance, frequency of occurrence, importance/value, centrality to one’s sense of self-identity, valence, emotional intensity, goal orientation, social orientation, specificity, imagery, perspective taking, duration of the topic or event surrounding the thought, temporal orientation, and certainty that the event for which the thought pertains occurred or will occur (see Table 1 for precise questions). Both the nature of the instructions and the descriptions provided by participants (i.e., “My College GPA,” “Climb Pike’s Peak,” “Miss My Parents”) suggest that participants primarily generated self-generated thoughts that were independent of external stimuli (i.e., stimulus-independent) and similar in content to that observed during episodes of mind-wandering (Stawarczyk et al., 2011).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Content variables.

Questionnaires

To assess individual differences in constructs relevant to mental health, a subset of participants (N = 70) completed a variety of self-report questionnaires related to emotional health and wellbeing. Whereas the thought sampling paradigm requires that participants retrieve specific examples of self-generated thoughts and rate those thoughts on a variety of different content variables, the mental health questionnaires assess more general aspects of a participant’s wellbeing including his/her behaviors, feelings, actions, and general styles of thinking. We used confirmatory factor analyses (using maximum likelihood estimation) to isolate three latent constructs defined from a large body of clinical literature: Depression/Negative Affect, trait Rumination, and trait Mindfulness (Table 2). The overall fit of these models was estimated using a non-significant χ2 and Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI) greater than 0.95 suggestive of an adequate fit of the data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Constructs related to emotional health and wellbeing.

Questionnaires assessing depressive symptoms and trait negative affect included the Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd Edition (BDI-2; Beck et al., 1996), the “Low Positive Affect” and “Loss of Interest” subscales of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ; Watson and Clark, 1991; Watson et al., 1995a,b), and the “Negative” subscale of the Trait version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988). All questionnaires loaded significantly (p < 0.05) on the Depression/Negative Affect latent construct (Table 2) and provided a good fit to the data (χ2 = 3.45, df = 2, CFI = 0.96). Note that there was considerable variability in scores across participants. Using previously recommended cutoff scores for the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1996), 71% of participants qualified as minimally depressed (scores 0–11), 22% as mildly depressed (scores 12–18), and 7% as moderately depressed (scores 19–29).

Questionnaires assessing trait rumination included “Brooding,” “Depression-Related,” and “Reflection-Related” subscales of the Ruminative Responses Scale (Roberts et al., 1998; Treynor et al., 2003), and the “Rumination” subscale of the Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire (RRQ; Trapnell and Campbell, 1999). All questionnaires loaded significantly (p < 0.05) on the Rumination latent construct (Table 2) and provided an excellent fit (χ2 = 0.63, df = 2, CFI = 1).

Questionnaires assessing dispositional mindfulness included all five subscales of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (Baer et al., 2006). While the “Acting with Awareness,” “Describing,” “Non-judging of Experience,” and “Non-reactivity to Inner Experience” subscales loaded highly onto the Mindfulness construct (p < 0.05), the “Observe” subscale loaded weakly onto the construct (p = 0.27; Table 2), consistent with prior observations (Baer et al., 2006). This model also provided an excellent fit to the data (χ2 = 3.49, df = 5, CFI = 1).

Participants’ factor scores for the Depression/Negative Affect, Rumination, and Mindfulness latent constructs were designated as the dependent (outcome) variable in subsequent multiple regression analyses to explore relationships with the content of individuals’ typical mind-wandering episodes. To examine the specificity of the relationships between Depression, Rumination, and thought content, we included questionnaires assessing individual differences in anxious arousal (the “Anxious Arousal” subscale of the MASQ) and anxious apprehension (the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990). We also assessed state affect immediately prior to the thought sampling paradigm using the “Positive” and “Negative” subscales of the State version of the PANAS questionnaire. Note that while the majority of other questionnaires were acquired in the same session subsequent to the thought sampling paradigm, the MASQ and the PSWQ were acquired as part of the larger study an average of 205 days earlier. As both the MASQ and the PSWQ have high test-retest reliability indicative of stable constructs (Stöber, 1998; Keogh and Reidy, 2000), we decided to include these questionnaires in our analyses.

Statistical Method

Part 1: Normative estimates of thought content

The first set of analyses sought to quantify normative estimates of thought content across the full group of 76 participants. To extract mean estimates of each content variable, we first averaged participants’ ratings across the 36 different thoughts for each participant separately. For each of the 12 content variables, we then averaged mean ratings across participants. Participants whose mean rating was more than 2.5 standard deviations above or below the group mean were included in figures for visual purposes only and were not included in calculations of the group mean.

Part 2: Identifying major dimensions of thought content

The second set of analyses sought to examine relationships among content variables within participants, and in doing so, identify the major dimensions of thought content. For each participant, we computed the relationship between each pair of content variables across his/her 36 different thoughts (i.e., specificity × self-relevancy, specificity × valence, etc.) using Spearman’s rho. Unlike Pearson’s R, Spearman’s rho does not assume the within-participant data are normally-distributed. Next, we averaged the 12 × 12 correlation matrices across participants to generate a group mean correlation matrix. Correlations were assessed for statistical significance by transforming them to Fisher’s z using the formula 0.5 × ln[(1 + r)/(1 - r)] and conducting one-sample t-tests across the group to determine if each correlation was significantly different from zero, using an alpha of p < 0.05. Given the large number of statistical tests, we also corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni method and assessed significance against a corrected alpha of p < 0.0008.

To identify major content dimensions characterizing participants’ self-generated thoughts, we applied hierarchical clustering analysis on the 12 × 12 group mean correlation matrix by first generating a distance matrix using Euclidean distance. Content dimension scores for each participant were extracted by converting his/her mean rating for each content variable to a z-score using the group mean, and averaging the standardized content variable ratings pertaining to the same cluster. These scores were then used to explore relationships with constructs relevant to emotional health and wellbeing (see below). Note that questions assessing perspective taking and certainty were excluded from the hierarchical clustering analyses. Perspective taking contained no intrinsic order in its response options, while certainty often yielded bimodal distributions at the within-subject level, a strongly positively skewed distribution at the between-subject level (skewness = -0.97), and was only answered for a subset of questions that pertained to a particular event. These non-normal distributions arose because thoughts pertaining to past events were almost always answered with 100% certainty. All other content variables were normally distributed at the between-subject level, with a skewness between –0.5 and +0.5.

Part 3: Relationships with constructs relevant to mental health

Our third objective was to examine relationships between individual differences in thought content and dimensional constructs linked to poor and satisfactory emotional health and wellbeing. In the subset of 70 participants who completed all mental health questionnaires, we ran three multiple linear regression analyses in which the participants’ thought content scores extracted from the hierarchical clustering analysis were entered together as independent predictors to predict participants’ factor scores from each of the Depression/Negative Affect, Rumination, and Mindfulness factors, respectively.

Because rumination and depression are often related, and because both depression and rumination often co-occur with anxiety, we also explored the specificity of each construct’s relationship to self-generated thought by conducting part correlations. This procedure involved (1) removing from the dependent variable (i.e., Depression/Negative Affect) variance shared with the questionnaires for which we wished to control (i.e., Anxious Arousal, Anxious Apprehension, and Rumination), and (2) using these standardized residuals as new dependent variables in subsequent multiple regressions with thought content scores as predictor variables.

For each multiple regression, we computed Cook’s Distance, a multivariate measure of the statistical influence of an individual on the regression function (Cook, 1977). Outliers with a Cook’s D of > 4/N were removed from each analysis (Bollen and Jackman, 1987).

In our final set of analyses, we isolated those aspects of thought content that exhibited significant relationships with emotional health and examined whether these relationships persisted after additionally controlling for state affect. Partial correlations were conducted between trait factor scores and each significant content dimension separately, while participants’ “Positive” and “Negative” subscales of the State PANAS questionnaire were entered as covariates of non-interest.

All tests were conducted using two-tailed criteria (p < 0.05) and analyses were conducted using a combination of R statistical software (R Development Core Team, 2010), SPSS (v21, IBM), and MPlus (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2011).

Results

Part 1: Normative Estimates of Thought Content

Although the phenomenological characteristics of self-generated thoughts varied widely across participants, several common themes emerged (Figure 1). On average, participants characterized their thoughts as highly self-relevant, of moderate importance or value, and moderately central to their sense of self-identity, yet they also indicated their thoughts were moderately social in nature. Participants characterized their thoughts as moderately recurrent, somewhat goal-oriented, of moderate emotional intensity, and positive in valence. Females rated their thoughts as more negative than males; t(74) = 2.94, p < 0.01. A majority of participants’ thoughts pertained to a particular event (mean = 77.4%, SD = 20.0%), although approximately one-quarter did not (mean = 22.6%, SD = 20.0%). Of those thoughts that pertained to a particular event, 59.8% were oriented toward the future (SD = 25.4%) and 40.2% were oriented toward the past (SD = 25.4%). On average, the events for which the thoughts pertained were expected to take place in the near future (today or tomorrow).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Phenomenological characteristics of self-generated thought. Individual data points represent each participant’s mean across 36 self-generated thoughts. Open circles represent outliers > 2.5 SD above or below the group mean (vertical bar) which are not included in calculations. The scale for temporal orientation corresponds to: 1 = more than 3 years ago, 2 = within the past 3 years, 3 = within the past 365 days (a year’s time), 4 = within the past 30 days (a month’s time), 5 = within the past 7 days (a week’s time), 6 = yesterday, 7 = earlier today, 8 = later today, 9 = tomorrow, 10 = within the next 7 days (a week’s time), 11 = within the next 30 days (a month’s time), 12 = within the next 365 days (a year’s time), 13 = within the next 3 years, 14 = more than 3 years from now.

Participants also characterized their thoughts as exhibiting a moderate degree of specificity and imagery. A first person (“self”) visual perspective was adopted for 64.3% of thoughts (SD = 24.8%), while participants adopted a different perspective for 18.7% of thoughts (SD = 16.0%), and did not adopt a particular perspective for 17.0% of thoughts (SD = 18.8%). On average, participants indicated that their thoughts pertained to topics or events that lasted (or will last) between multiple days and one month in duration.

Part 2: Identifying Major Dimensions of Thought Content

As shown in Figure 2, several significant relationships emerged between content variables at the within-subject level, and these relationships are reflected in the pattern of content dimensions identified from a hierarchical clustering analysis (Figure 3). One dimension, termed Level of Construal, refers to the style of abstract versus concrete processing adopted during self-generated thought, and includes questions pertaining to specificity, imagery and duration of the topic or event surrounding the thought. Social orientation also clustered with this dimension. Ratings for questions encompassed in the Level of Construal dimension were significantly correlated with one another, with the exception of social orientation (which was only related to specificity and imagery; Figure 2). Thoughts rated as more detailed and specific were more likely to pertain to topics characterized by a shorter duration (i.e., a specific episode as opposed to a general topic), to be experienced with more vividness/imagery, and to involve other people.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Relationships between content variables. Relationships between content variables were averaged across 76 participants. For scales corresponding to each content variable, see Figure 1 and Table 1. Note that higher scores for Duration of Topic correspond to thoughts concerning topics or events lasting shorter durations. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.0008 (alpha corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni method).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Hierarchical clustering reveals major dimensions of thought content. Higher scores for the Level of Construal cluster correspond to thoughts that are more temporally and perceptually specific, as well as more socially oriented. Higher scores for the Personal Significance cluster correspond to thoughts rated as more personally significant. Higher scores for the Outlook cluster correspond to thoughts rated as more positive and future-oriented. On the y-axis, height is a measure of similarity or distance between content variables such that two variables forming a single cluster at a small height are more similar than two variables linked at a greater height.

A second dimension, termed Personal Significance, included the following content ratings: self-relevance, centrality, frequency of occurrence, emotional intensity, value/importance, and goal orientation. While all of these questions were significantly positively correlated (all r’s > 0.2, p’s < 0.001), self-relevance exhibited the strongest relationships with centrality and subjective value (Figure 2). Additionally, thoughts that were attributed high value were rated as more central to one’s sense of self-identity, higher in emotional intensity, and more recurrent. Goal-oriented thoughts were also rated as more self-relevant, more central to one’s sense of self-identity, more recurrent, more intense, and more important/valuable.

A third dimension, termed Outlook, reflected significant positive relationships between temporal orientation and valence (Figures 2 and 3). Future-oriented thoughts were rated as more positive and past-oriented thoughts were rated as more negative.

In addition to relationships between content variables within each content dimension (i.e., self-relevancy, value, etc.), relationships also emerged across variables belonging to different content dimensions (Figure 2). More future-oriented thoughts were generally experienced with less specificity, while thoughts that were rated as more personally significant pertained to topics of longer durations, yet were not necessarily less specific or vivid. Furthermore, socially oriented thoughts were experienced with greater emotional intensity, and positive associations were observed between emotional intensity and both imagery and specificity.

Part 3: Relationships between thought Content and Emotional Wellbeing

The combined contribution of the 12 content variables explained a large amount of variance in individual differences in the three psychological constructs of interest. Specifically, thought content explained 45.8% of the variance in Depression/Negative Affect, 30.5% of the variance in Rumination, and 36.3% of the variance in Mindfulness. We next repeated the analyses after combining the content variables into the composite dimensions identified in Part 2, but splitting the dimension corresponding to Outlook into temporal orientation and valence because of the observed weak correlations between those content ratings and our a priori hypotheses linking valence to depression. Thus, four independent variables were entered together into each multiple regression (Level of Construal, Personal Significance, Valence, and Temporal Orientation).

Collectively, these four types of thought content explained 33.0% of the variance in Depression/Negative Affect, R = 0.58, F(4,58) = 7.15, p < 0.001, with Personal Significance and Valence contributing independent variance (Personal Significance, β = 0.22, t(57) = 1.99, p = 0.05; Valence, β = -0.57, t(57) = -4.65, p < 0.001). Individuals who rated their thoughts as more personally significant and more negative reported higher levels of Depression/Negative Affect (Figure 4A). To examine the specificity of these findings for Depression/Negative Affect, we next controlled for statistical effects of anxious arousal, anxious apprehension, and Rumination on Depression/Negative Affect using a part correlation, and the effects were numerically stronger, R = 0.57, F(4,56) = 6.20, p < 0.001; Personal Significance, β = 0.27, t(55) = 2.41, p < 0.05; Valence, β = -0.57, t(55) = -4.45, p < 0.001.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Independent content predictors of constructs relevant for mental health. Positive βs are characterized by thoughts that are more concrete, more personally significant, more future oriented and more positive (see scales in Figure 1). (A) Personal Significance and Valence contributed a significant amount of variance toward explaining Depression/Negative Affect. (B) Level of Construal was the only dimension that contributed a significant amount of variance toward predicting individual differences in Rumination. (C) Level of Construal and Personal Significance contributed a significant amount of variance toward predicting Mindfulness, with a considerable additional contribution of Valence (β = 0.25, p = 0.054). *p ≤ 0.05.

Depression/Negative Affect was marginally correlated with Rumination, r(63) = 0.22, p = 0.08, allowing us to examine relationships between self-generated thought content and each mental health construct specifically. Although a smaller percentage of variance in Rumination was explained by the four classes of thought content, R = 0.38, F(4,62) = 2.68, p = 0.04, R2 = 14.7, Level of Construal was the only content dimension that contributed significant independent variance to Rumination, β = -0.32, t(61) = -2.56, p < 0.05 (Figure 4B). As predicted, individuals who generated more abstract and less specific thoughts were characterized by higher levels of Rumination. Importantly, the relationship between Level of Construal and Rumination became numerically stronger after controlling for comorbid effects of anxiety and Depression/Negative Affect on Rumination, R = 0.41, F(4,63) = 3.24, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17; Level of Construal: β = -0.35, t(60) = -2.77, p < 0.01.

The four types of thought content explained 21.2% of the variance in mindfulness, F(4,59) = 3.97, p < 0.01, with Level of Construal, Personal Significance and Valence contributing independent variance (Level of Construal, β = 0.25, t(58) = 2.00, p = 0.05; Personal Significance, β = -0.24, t(58) = -2.1, p < 0.05; Valence, β = 0.27, t(58) = 2.09, p < 0.05). Individuals who rated their thoughts as more concrete and specific, less personally significant, and more positive scored higher on dispositional mindfulness questionnaires (Figure 4C). Both Depression/Negative Affect and Rumination were negatively correlated with Mindfulness, r(64) = -0.68, p < 0.001 and r(64) = -0.28, p < 0.05.

Relationships with state affect

Individual differences in Depression/Negative Affect were strongly predictive of state affect as assessed with the “positive” and “negative” subscales of the state PANAS, R = 0.59, F(2,60) = 15.60, p < 0.001. However, state affect was not related to individual differences in Rumination, R = 0.13, F(2,66) = 0.58, p = 0.56. State affect was strongly related to Valence, R = 0.49, F(2,69) = 10.73, p < 0.001, but not to Personal Significance, R = 0.12, F(2,68) = 0.49, p = 0.62. When controlling for state affect, the relationship between Depression/Negative Affect and Valence remained significant (r(62) = -0.51, p < 0.001; partial r(58) = -0.44, p < 0.01).

Discussion

The current study indicates that self-generated thoughts can be well-characterized along three major content dimensions regarding their level of construal, their personal significance, and the outlook they embody. Moreover, these dimensions explain a notable amount of variance in individual differences in emotional health and wellbeing. As such, our study speaks to a long-standing debate in psychology and neuroscience (e.g., Smallwood and Schooler, 2006; Watkins, 2008; McVay and Kane, 2010) by suggesting that the ability to regulate the content of self-generated thought is a key factor underlying its costs and benefits (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). Individuals who characterized their thoughts as more personally significant, more negative, and/or less detailed scored higher on constructs linked to poor psychological wellbeing (Depression and Rumination), whereas those who characterized their thoughts as less personally significant, more positive, and more specific scored higher on constructs linked to improved wellbeing (Mindfulness). Below, we synthesize our findings in the context of prior literature and conclude by discussing future directions for research on self-generated thought and mental health.

Positive and Productive Modes of thought Content Reflect the Norm in a Sample of Young Adults

Measuring the content of self-generated thought across numerous thought samples, content variables, and participants allowed us to extract at least three dimensions of content: Level of Construal, Personal Significance, and Outlook. On average, participants’ thoughts were moderately perceptually and temporally specific, consistent with prior studies (Klinger and Cox, 1987; Delamillieure et al., 2010; Stawarczyk et al., 2011, 2013). These concrete “low-level” construals have been linked to beneficial outcomes encompassing effective problem solving, successful attainment of future goals, and enhanced self-regulatory behavior (Pham and Taylor, 1999; Watkins and Baracaia, 2002; Watkins and Moulds, 2005; Leary et al., 2006). Interestingly, socially oriented thoughts were also experienced with greater perceptual detail. Although not the main topic of the present manuscript, the finding that participants spend a notable amount of time thinking about other people is in line with prior studies (Mar et al., 2012; see also Ruby et al., under review) and provides strong support for theories highlighting the importance of social information for human cognition (Dunbar, 1998; Baumeister and Masicampo, 2010; Corballis, 2013).

Furthermore, participants’ thoughts were highly personally significant, somewhat goal-oriented, and attributed strong value, resonating with theories that personal concerns form the basis of human thought content (Klinger, 1971, 2009; Fox et al., 2013; McMillan et al., 2013; Stawarczyk et al., 2013).

Consistent with prior studies, participants rated their thoughts as somewhat positive (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Stawarczyk et al., 2011, 2013; Fox et al., 2013) and future-oriented (Smallwood et al., 2009b; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010a; Baird et al., 2011; Stawarczyk et al., 2011, 2013; Song and Wang, 2012; Fox et al., 2013; Ruby et al., 2013), supporting a growing body of work regarding the adaptive significance of prospective thoughts (Gilbert and Wilson, 2007; Schacter et al., 2007; Suddendorf and Corballis, 2007). Future-oriented thoughts were perceived as more positive than past-oriented thoughts, supporting prior findings linking retrospective focus to negative mood (Smallwood and O’Connor, 2011).

Thought Content Relates to Emotional Health and Wellbeing

Providing strong support for the content regulation hypothesis (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013), our findings indicate that thought content is an important factor underlying the adaptive and maladaptive consequences of self-generated thought. According to content regulation hypothesis, self-generated thoughts are primarily beneficial when individuals are able to regulate the content of the experience to positive or productive topics (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013). Conversely, individuals who lack this capacity may experience maladaptive psychological consequences of self-generated thought.

In line with these predictions, individuals who rated their thoughts as more negative in valence as well as more personally significant scored higher on questionnaires assessing Depression/Negative Affect. These findings are consistent with long-held theories that depressed individuals hold negative schemas about themselves, their future, and the environment (Beck, 1967), and that a dispositional style of self-focused thinking exacerbates the duration and symptoms of depression (Smith and Greenberg, 1981; Larsen and Cowan, 1988). Depressed individuals also exhibit attentional and mnemonic biases toward negative and self-referential information, difficulties clearing this information from working memory, and impaired performance particularly when the material is task-irrelevant (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010; Joormann, 2010). Further support for associations between self-focus and negative affect comes from studies linking self-focused attention to state and trait negative affect, particularly when the content of the self-focused thought is negative in nature (Smith and Greenberg, 1981; Mor and Winquist, 2002). Consequently, experimental inductions that amplify external focus and dampen internal focus help depressed individuals adopt a less pessimistic view of their future (Pyszczynski et al., 1987).

Although depressed individuals commonly exhibit overgeneral autobiographical memories (Williams et al., 2007; Watkins, 2008), the present results demonstrate that overgeneral styles of thinking were more strongly related to individual differences in Rumination than to Depression/Negative Affect. These findings suggest that the repetitive nature of self-generated thought rather than negative affect per se, may be most strongly linked to abstract levels of construal. However, since many of the questions contributing to the rumination questionnaires pertain to negative content, it remains yet to be determined whether overgeneral patterns of thought also characterize other types of repetitive thought, independent of affective content. Rumination is strongly related to, yet distinct from, depression, and our observation that overgeneral thinking is particularly related to Rumination agrees well with studies reporting elevated overgeneral memory following rumination induction (Watkins and Teasdale, 2001; Sutherland and Bryant, 2007). The relationship between overgeneral memory, rumination, and other types of repetitive thought marks an interesting area for future research.

Mindful Aspects of thought Content are Reciprocally Related to Depression and Rumination

In contrast to depression and rumination, dispositional mindfulness and mindfulness meditation have been linked to improved health, cognitive ability, and psychological wellbeing (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Sedlmeier et al., 2012). Paralleling these findings, Mindfulness exhibited negative relationships with Depression and Rumination, and was partly explained by an opposing pattern of thought content, being characterized as more positive, less personally significant, and more specific. These results are consistent with prior research demonstrating that mindful individuals report higher levels of positive affect (Brown and Ryan, 2003), positive or less negative thought content (Frewen et al., 2008; Evans and Segerstrom, 2011), and better emotion regulation ability (Farb et al., 2012).

Of relevance, brief mindfulness inductions have been shown to dampen negative affect (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Huffziger et al., 2013), and many of the content variables that predicted increases in dispositional mindfulness in our study are central to mindfulness-based therapies for a variety of mental health disorders. For example, a key element of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy involves teaching people to become aware of the nature of their internal thoughts, view their thoughts from a more distant or “de-centered” perspective, and accept them for what they are rather than the literal truth (Segal et al., 2002; Mennin and Fresco, 2013). When applied to recurrent depression, MBCT reduces relapse rates by nearly 50% (Teasdale et al., 2000; Watkins, 2008) and increases the specificity of individuals’ autobiographical memory (Williams et al., 2000, 2007).

Conclusions and Future Directions

In summary, these findings provide strong evidence that the content of people’s inner thoughts in daily life can be productively examined and distilled into several major dimensions. Moreover, individual differences in thought content explain a notable amount of variance in constructs relevant to emotional health and wellbeing. As such, the present manuscript extends previous studies that focus solely on the frequency with which self-generated thoughts occur, particularly because increases in frequency have been linked to both adaptive and maladaptive outcomes (i.e., McVay and Kane, 2009; Levinson et al., 2012). In contrast, the content characterizing self-generated thought may more precisely constrain the functional consequences of this human experience. Quantifying an individual’s full profile of thought content – perhaps in combination with the frequency with which self-generated thoughts occur, and/or the difficulty of the task during which unrelated thoughts occurs (Smallwood and Andrews-Hanna, 2013) – might ultimately help researchers or clinicians distinguish between different trait dimensions and/or mental health disorders.

Unfortunately, we were unable to fully address this last question using our sample of participants because none of our participants were diagnosed with a mental health disorder. While we anticipate similar yet stronger findings in individuals with MDD, clinical diagnosis with depression may be associated with patterns of thought content distinct from those highlighted in our non-clinical sample. Although none of our participants reported current or previous diagnosis of depression, they exhibited a broad range of subclinical depressive symptoms, consistent with recent mental health initiatives highlighting the importance of dimensional classification systems (Kupfer and Regier, 2011; Adam, 2013). Furthermore, the finding that specific patterns of thought content predict symptoms of distress even in this non-clinical sample could provide important insight regarding risk factors for the development of more severe symptoms. Biases in thought content may emerge prior to the onset of a major depressive episode, perhaps providing a marker of clinical risk and a target for preventive therapies (Muñoz et al., 2010; Beshai et al., 2011). Future longitudinal research could address these possibilities by assessing changes in thought content over onset and recovery from MDD.

Another open question involves understanding the directional nature of the relationship between thought content and psychological wellbeing. Does dispositional unhappiness exert a causal influence on negative and personally significant thoughts, or does the act of entertaining such thoughts exacerbate poor emotional wellbeing? Might mental health and thought content dynamically influence each other, creating positive feedback loops? While some studies have found initial evidence for causal relationships between the frequency and/or content of self-generated thoughts and wellbeing (Moberly and Watkins, 2008; Smallwood et al., 2009a; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Smallwood and O’Connor, 2011; Marchetti et al., 2012a; Ruby et al., 2013), a complete understanding of the nature of these relationships will require overcoming several methodological and conceptual challenges (Mason et al., 2013).

Another avenue for future research involves delineating the neural underpinnings of different dimensions of thought content. The brain’s “default network” is a large-scale brain system hypothesized to support spontaneous and goal-directed forms of self-generated thought (Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna, 2012). Though our results agree well with findings that depression and rumination are linked to hyperactivity of the default network and increased connectivity with limbic regions (Marchetti et al., 2012b; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012), recent studies suggest that the DMN is organized into multiple subsystems with dissociable functions (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010b; Andrews-Hanna, 2012). How the content dimensions of self-generated thought map onto these unique DMN components remains an open question.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge David Howett, Joseph Orr, Harry Smolker, and Kathy Pearson for assistance with data collection and analysis, and Joanna Arch, Tor Wager, Marina Lopez-Sola, and Tal Yarkoni for helpful discussion. This work was supported by the following grants from the National Institutes of Mental Health: F32 MH093985 (Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna) and P50 MH079485 (Marie T. Banich).

References

Adam, D. (2013). On the spectrum. Nature 496, 416–418. doi: 10.1038/496416a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist 18, 251–270. doi: 10.1177/1073858411403316

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., and Buckner, R. L. (2010a). Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 322–335. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010b). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baars, B. J. (2010). Spontaneous repetitive thoughts can be adaptive: postscript on “mind wandering.” Psychol. Bull. 136, 208–210. doi: 10.1037/a0018726

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention. Psychol. Sci. 10, 125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg015

CrossRef Full Text

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baird, B., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1604–1611. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baumeister, R. F., and Masicampo, E. J. (2010). Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: how mental simulations serve the animal-culture interface. Psychol. Rev. 117, 945–971. doi: 10.1037/a0019393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beck, A. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., and Ranieri, W. F. (1996). Comparison of Beck depression inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beshai, S., Dobson, K. S., Bockting, C. L. H., and Quigley, L. (2011). Relapse and recurrence prevention in depression: current research and future prospects. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 1349–1360. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bishop, S. R. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text

Bollen, K. A., and Jackman, R. W. (1987). “Regression diagnostics: an expository treatment of outliers and influential cases,” in Modern Methods of Data Analysis, Vol. 13, eds J. Fox and J. S. Long (Newbury Park, CA: Sage), 257–291.

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological wellbeing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christoff, K., Gordon, A., and Smith, R. (2011). “The role of spontaneous thought in human cognition,” in Neuroscience of Decision Making, eds O. Vartanian and D. Mandel (New York City: Psychology Press), 259–284.

Cook, R. D. (1977). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics 19, 15–18. doi: 10.2307/1268249

CrossRef Full Text

Corballis, M. C. (2013). Wandering tales: evolutionary origins of mental time travel and language. Front. Psychol. 4:1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00485

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crane, C., Winder, R., Hargus, E., Amarasinghe, M., and Barnhofer, T. (2012). Effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on specificity of life goals. Cognit. Ther. Res. 36, 182–189. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9349-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delamillieure, P., Doucet, G., Mazoyer, B., Turbelin, M.-R., Delcroix, N., Mellet, E., et al. (2010). The resting state questionnaire: an introspective questionnaire for evaluation of inner experience during the conscious resting state. Brain Res. Bull. 81, 565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.11.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolut. Anthropol. 6, 178–190. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8

CrossRef Full Text

Eisenberg, D., Golberstein, E., and Gollust, S. E. (2007). Help-seeking and access to mental health care in a university student population. Med. Care 45, 594–601. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31803bb4c1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, D. R., and Segerstrom, S. C. (2011). Why do mindful people worry less? Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 505–510. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9340-0

CrossRef Full Text

Farb, N. A., Anderson, A. K., and Segal, Z. V. (2012). The mindful brain and emotion regulation in mood disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 57, 70–77.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., and Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as mind wandering: evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:1–18. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00412

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frewen, P. A., Evans, E. M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D. J. A., and Partridge, K. (2008). Letting go: mindfulness and negative automatic thinking. Cogn. Ther. Res. 32, 758–774. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9142-1

CrossRef Full Text

Gilbert, D. T., and Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: experiencing the future. Science 317, 1351–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1144161

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gotlib, I. H., and Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harvey, A., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., and Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive Behavioural Processes across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment. New York City: Oxford University Press.

Huffziger, S., Ebner-Priemer, U., Eisenbach, C., Koudela, S., Reinhard, I., Zamoscik, V., et al. (2013). Induced ruminative and mindful attention in everyday life: an experimental ambulatory assessment study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 44, 322–328. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.01.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joormann, J. (2010). Cognitive inhibition and emotion regulation in depression. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 161–166. doi:10.1177/0963721410370293

CrossRef Full Text

Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Maser, J. D., Zeller, P. J., Endicott, J., Coryell, W., et al. (1998). Major depressive disorder: a prospective study of residual subthreshold depressive symptoms as predictor of rapid relapse. J. Affect. Disord. 50, 97–108. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00138-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full-Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York: Bantam Dell.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., and Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 18, 614–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keogh, E., and Reidy, J. (2000). Exploring the factor structure of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ). J. Pers. Assess. 74, 106–125. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA740108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Killingsworth, M. A., and Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330, 932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klinger, E. (1971). Structure and Functions of Fantasy. New York: John Wiley.

Klinger, E. (2009). “Daydreaming and fantasizing: thought flow and motivation,” in Handbook of Imagination and Mental Stimulation, eds K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, and J. A. Suhr (New York City: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis Group), 225–239.

Klinger, E., and Cox, W. (1987). Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 7, 105–128. doi: 10.2190/7K24-G343-MTQW-115V

CrossRef Full Text

Kupfer, D. J., and Regier, D. A. (2011). Neuroscience, clinical evidence, and the future of psychiatric classification in DSM-5. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 672–674. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Larsen, R. J., and Cowan, G. S. (1988). Internal focus of attention and depression: a study of daily experience. Motiv. Emot. 12, 237–249. doi: 10.1007/BF00993113

CrossRef Full Text

Leary, M. R., Adams, C. E., and Tate, E. B. (2006). Hypo-egoic self-regulation: exercising self-control by diminishing the influence of the self. J. Pers. 74, 1803–1831. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00429.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., and Davidson, R. J. (2012). The persistence of thought: evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychol. Sci. 23, 375–380. doi: 10.1177/0956797611431465

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mar, R. A., Mason, M. F., and Litvack, A. (2012). How daydreaming relates to life satisfaction, loneliness, and social support: the importance of gender and daydream content. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 401–407. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., and De Raedt, R. (2012a). Mindwandering heightens the accessibility of negative relative to positive thought. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1517–1525. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and De Raedt, R. (2012b). The default mode network and recurrent depression: a neurobiological model of cognitive risk factors. Neuropsychol. Rev. 22, 229–251. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9199-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mason, M. F., Brown, K., Mar, R. A., and Smallwood, J. (2013). Driver of discontent or escape vehicle: the affective consequences of mindwandering. Front. Psychol. 4:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00477

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., and Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Front. Psychol. 4: 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00626

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McVay, J. C., and Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35, 196–204. doi: 10.1037/a0014104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McVay, J. C., and Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychol. Bull. 136, 188–197; discussion 198–207. doi: 10.1037/a0018298

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mennin, D. S., and Fresco, D. (2013). What, me worry and ruminate about DSM-V and RDoC?: the importance of targeting negative self-referential processing. Clin. Psychol. 1–22. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12038

CrossRef Full Text

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moberly, N. J., and Watkins, E. R. (2008). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: an experience sampling study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 314–323. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.314

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mor, N., and Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 128, 638–662. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mooneyham, B. W., and Schooler, J. W. (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 11–18. doi: 10.1037/a0031569

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muñoz, R. F., Cuijpers, P., Smit, F., Barrera, A. Z., and Leykin, Y. (2010). Prevention of major depression. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 181–212. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-033109-132040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998–2011). Mplus User’s Guide, 6th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 504–511. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Mcbride, A., and Larsen, J. (1997). Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved partners. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 855–862. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.855

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

CrossRef Full Text

Pham, L. B., and Taylor, S. E. (1999). From thought to action: effects of process- versus outcome-based mental simulations on performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 250–260. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002010

CrossRef Full Text

Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., and Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 994–1001. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.994

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

R Development Core Team. (2010). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna. Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at: http://www.R-project.org

Roberts, E., Gilboa, E., and Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to episodes of dysphoria: gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Therapy 22, 401–423.

Ruby, F. J. M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., and Singer, T. (2013). How self-generated thought shapes mood – the relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PLoS ONE 8:e77554. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077554

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature 8, 657–661. doi:10.1038/nrn2213

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., et al. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138, 1139–1171. doi: 10.1037/a0028168

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., and Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York City: Gilford Press.

Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An Introduction to the Experimental Study of Inner Experience. New York: Random House, Inc.

Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process-occurence framework for self generated thought. Psychol. Bull. 139, 519–535. doi: 10.1037/a0030010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smallwood, J., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Front. Psychol. 4:441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., and Phillips, L. H. (2009a). Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion 9, 271–276. doi: 10.1037/a0014855

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smallwood, J., Nind, L., and O’Connor, R. C. (2009b). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smallwood, J., and O’Connor, R. C. (2011). Imprisoned by the past: unhappy moods lead to a retrospective bias to mind wandering. Cogn. Emot. 25, 1481–1490. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.545263

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, 946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, T. W., and Greenberg, J. (1981). Depression and self-focused attention. Motiv. Emot. 5, 323–331. doi: 10.1007/BF00992551

CrossRef Full Text

Song, X., and Wang, X. (2012). Mind wandering in Chinese daily lives–an experience sampling study. PLoS ONE 7:e44423. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044423

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stawarczyk, D., Cassol, H., and D’Argembeau, A. (2013). Phenomenology of future-oriented mind-wandering episodes. Front. Psychol. 4:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., Van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-wandering: phenomenology and function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method. Acta Psychol. 136, 370–381. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stöber, J. (1998). Reliability and validity of two widely-used worry questionnaires: self-report and self-peer convergence. Pers. Individ. Dif. 24, 887–890. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00232-8

CrossRef Full Text

Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 299–313; discussion 313–351. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001975

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sutherland, K., and Bryant, R. A. (2007). Rumination and overgeneral autobiographical memory. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2407–2416. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., and Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 615–623. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Trapnell, P. D., and Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 284–304. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Therapy 27, 247–259.

Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychol. Bull. 134, 163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watkins, E., and Baracaia, S. (2002). Rumination and social problem solving in depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 1179–1189. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00098-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watkins, E., and Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self- focus: impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion 5, 319–328. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watkins, E., and Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 110, 353–357. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.333

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M. E., and McCormick, R. A. (1995a). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104, 3–14. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss, M. E., and McCormick, R. A. (1995b). Testing a tripartite model: II. Exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression in student, adult, and patient samples. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104, 15–25. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1991). The Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Ford, J. M. (2012). Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8, 49–76. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., et al. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol. Bull. 133, 122–148. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, J. M. G., Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., and Soulsby, J. (2000). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory in formerly depressed patients. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 150–155. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.1.150

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

World Health Organization. (2008). The Global Burden of Disease 2004 Update. Available at: http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GBD_report_2004update_full.pdf

Keywords: mind-wandering, autobiographical, rumination, depression, mindfulness

Citation: Andrews-Hanna JR, Kaiser RH, Turner AEJ, Reineberg AE, Godinez D, Dimidjian S and Banich MT (2013) A penny for your thoughts: dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 4:900. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00900

Received: 31 July 2013; Accepted: 14 November 2013;
Published online: 29 November 2013.

Edited by:

Arnaud Delorme, Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, France

Reviewed by:

Arnaud Delorme, Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, France
Julia W. Y. Kam, University of British Columbia, Canada
Jonathan Jackson, Brandeis University, USA

Copyright © 2013 Andrews-Hanna, Kaiser, Turner, Reineberg, Godinez, Dimidjian and Banich. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, 594 UCB, Boulder, CO 80309 USA e-mail: jandrewshanna@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.