Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 21 August 2012
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 6 - 2012 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00232

Speed impairs attending on the left: comparing attentional asymmetries for neglect patients in speeded and unspeeded cueing tasks

Kristie R. Dukewich1 Gail A. Eskes1,2,3* Michael A. Lawrence1,2 Mary-Beth MacIsaac2 Stephen J. Phillips3 Raymond M. Klein2
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 3Department of Medicine, Neurology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada

Visuospatial neglect after stroke is often characterized by a disengage deficit on a cued orienting task, in which individuals are disproportionately slower to respond to targets presented on the contralesional side of space following an ispilesional cue as compared to the reverse. The purpose of this study was to investigate the generality of the finding of a disengage deficit on another measure of cued attention, the temporal order judgment (TOJ) task, that does not depend upon speeded manual responses. Individuals with right hemisphere stroke with and without spatial neglect and older healthy controls (OHC) were tested with both a speeded RT cueing task and an unspeeded TOJ-with-cuing task. All stroke patients evidenced a disengage deficit on the speeded RT cueing task, although the size and direction of the bias was not associated with the severity of neglect. In contrast, few neglect patients showed a disengage deficit on the TOJ task. This discrepancy suggests that the disengage deficit may be related to task demands, rather than solely due to impaired attentional mechanisms per se. Further, the results of our study show that the disengage deficit is neither necessary nor sufficient for neglect to manifest.

Introduction

Visuospatial neglect is a condition whereby people are deficient at attending to or noticing the contralesional side of space when the deficiency cannot be explained by primary sensory or motor deficits (Heilman and Valenstein, 1979). As such, neglect is often described as a cognitive disorder of attention (Bisiach et al., 1979; Posner and Petersen, 1990; Karnath et al., 2002b). The condition is more likely to manifest in people with right rather than left hemisphere lesions, and has been associated with lesions in a variety of brain regions and white matter networks, including anterior frontal lobe, the parietal lobe and tempo-parietal junction, superior temporal gyrus as well as subcortical areas (Mesulam, 1981; Vallar and Perani, 1986; Leibovitch et al., 1998; Vallar, 2001; Karnath et al., 2002a,b, 2004; Mort et al., 2003; Bartolomeo et al., 2007).

One of the most popular experimental paradigms for studying attention and neglect is the exogenous spatial cueing task developed by Posner and colleagues in the late 1970s (Posner, 1978, 1980; Posner et al., 1980). The task involves presenting participants with a central fixation flanked by two peripheral boxes. A cue is presented in one of the two peripheral locations, followed by a target presented in one of those locations; the cue may or may not predict the location of the target. When the time interval between the cue and the target is short (approximately 250 ms), participants are typically faster to respond to the target when it appears at the same location as the cue (a “valid” or “cued” trial) compared to when it appears at the opposite location (an “invalid” or “uncued” trial)—an effect referred to as facilitation (Posner and Cohen, 1984). The spatial cueing task has been exploited to examine various aspects of spatial attention and has allowed the description of several general attention-related effects (cf. Posner et al., 1985), as well as specific effects related to various neuropsychological and psychiatric disorders (cf. Maruff et al., 1995; Townsend et al., 1996).

Posner used the spatial cueing paradigm to help develop a model of orienting involving three distinct operations: attention first disengages from its current focus, it then shifts toward the new target location, and finally attention engages the new target (Posner et al., 1982, 1984). Posner and colleagues were the first to use the spatial cueing paradigm to investigate the effects of parietal lobe lesions and reported that after left or right parietal lobe damage, while individuals were able to benefit from cues provided on the same side as the target, they were disproportionately slower to respond to targets presented on the contralesional side of space following an ispilesional cue as compared to the reverse (Posner et al., 1982). Posner and others have characterized this pattern (increased cost for contralesional targets following ipsilesional cues) as a difficulty disengaging attention from the patient's “good” field in order to deal with a target presented to the “poor” field, and have thus christened the effect a disengage deficit (Posner et al., 1982; Rastelli et al., 2008).

Losier and Klein (2001)1 conducted a meta-analysis of the literature dealing with the disengage deficit to reveal several important characteristics of the effect. As with neglect itself, the disengage deficit is greater in patients with right compared to left hemisphere damage. In patients with right hemisphere damage, the disengage deficit is greater using shorter cue-target onset asynchronies (CTOAs; i.e., less than 550 ms) compared to longer CTOAs (Losier and Klein, 2001). However, patients with left hemisphere damage tend to have a relatively stable disengage deficit across cue-target intervals. There is some evidence that patients with damage to one hemisphere who fail to show clinical signs of neglect can exhibit a disengage deficit; however, the disengage deficit is significantly larger in patients with neglect (cf. Posner et al., 1984). Additionally, the size of the disengage deficit is related to neglect severity, such that patients with more severe neglect tend to have larger disengage deficits (e.g., Baynes et al., 1986; Morrow and Ratcliff, 1988; Farah et al., 1989; D'Erme et al., 1994; Egly et al., 1994; Losier and Klein, 2001; Snyder and Chatterjee, 2006; Bonato et al., 2009; Schindler et al., 2009; Olk et al., 2010), although this relationship has not always been found (Posner et al., 1984; Sacher et al., 2004; Sieroff et al., 2007). This effect is amplified in right-hemisphere neglect patients compared to left-hemisphere neglect patients. Recently, Rastelli et al. (2008) have shown that the disengage deficit is greater when the cue remains on screen for the entire trial compared to when the cue is removed before the target appears, suggesting the disengage deficit is stronger for objects than it is for locations.

The current interpretation of the disengage deficit implies something general about the way that neglect manifests; once attention is captured by a cue in the good field, targets presented to the poor field have particular difficulty generating disengagement from this cue. However, to our knowledge, the effect has only been studied using variants of Posner's spatial cueing paradigm. If the disengage deficit is actually about attention, then it should generalize to other paradigms sensitive to attentional cueing. One candidate task for testing this hypothesis is the temporal order judgment (TOJ) paradigm. In a conventional TOJ task two stimuli are presented in rapid succession, e.g., one on the left and one on the right side. The order of side of first presentation, left or right, varies across trials, along with the interval, or stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA), between the stimulus onsets. To avoid response biases, participants can be asked to report which item was presented first using a stimulus characteristic (e.g., color or orientation) rather than stimulus location (Spence et al., 2001). Typically, left-first trials are coded with negative SOAs while right-first trials are coded with positive SOAs; the likelihood of reporting the item on the right as appearing first is calculated and plotted per SOA. The SOA at which the likelihood for reporting right-first is 50% is considered the point at which participants subjectively experience the two events as occurring at the same time. This SOA is referred to as the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS). Normal observers are usually very accurate, and under neutral conditions without cueing the PSS averages around 0, indicating no spatial asymmetry. Judgments in this task are also influenced by spatial cueing (Stelmach and Herdman, 1991; Shore et al., 2001), such that presenting a cue on either the left or right side prior to presenting the test stimuli can impact which item participants perceive as occurring first. This shift in the PSS is presumed to reflect a perceptual change due to the drawing of attention to the cued side, with corresponding earlier arrival at a temporal comparison stage (Stelmach and Herdman, 1991; Shore et al., 2001). The TOJ task has been used to reveal visual spatial attention asymmetries in individuals with extinction and/or spatial neglect, and a number of investigators have reported a shift in the PSS under neutral conditions without cueing such that the stimulus on the contralesional side must temporally lead the stimulus on the ipsilesional side in order for the two to be perceived as simultaneous (Rorden et al., 1997; Robertson et al., 1998; Baylis et al., 2002; Berberovic et al., 2004; Sinnett et al., 2007; but see Dove et al., 2007).

While Posner's speeded RT cueing task and the unspeeded TOJ-with-cueing task share similar characteristics in spatial cuing (e.g., Eskes et al., 2007) that suggest neglect patients will show a disengage deficit in both tasks, there are several differences and reported dissociations between the tasks which may impact the results (e.g., Neumann et al., 1993; Miller and Schwarz, 2006). TOJ tasks can require participants to make a speeded response (cf. Heath, 1984; Shore et al., 2001), but because the primary measure is related to accuracy and SOA rather than speed, this requirement is unnecessary and the authors know of no published reports in patients using a speeded TOJ task. In the speeded RT cueing task, the actions of attention are inferred by RTs, and so speeded responding cannot be avoided. Table 1 summarizes the task characteristics for the TOJ and RT tasks.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary comparison of the characteristics of the two experimental tasks.

Some evidence that the disengage deficit may not transfer to a TOJ task was hinted at in Di Pellegrino et al. (1997). Di Pellegrino et al. described a case study of a 65-year-old patient with neglect and extinction following a right-hemisphere stroke. The patient was asked to report the identity of two target letters presented asynchronously, one on either side of a central fixation. If the patient's neglect and extinction were due to a disengage deficit, one would expect that contralesional targets would be less likely to be identified correctly when an ipsilesional target was presented first compared to when the ipsilesional target was presented second in the pair. However, the researchers found that the patient was significantly worse at reporting a contralesional target if it was presented within 600 ms of the letter presented on the ipsilesional side of space, and that the deficit was similar in duration and magnitude irrespective of whether the contralesional target was present first or second in the pair. Di Pellegrino et al. explain these results in terms of a competitive model of selective attention, rather than a model that assumes a difficulty in disengaging from ipsilesional objects. In the competitive model, ipsilesional targets are assumed to have a higher weight in terms of capturing selective attention, such that even if they arrive 300–400 ms after a contralesional target they still manage to capture attentional resources and interrupt contralesional processing before contralesional target identity is determined.

The current paper explores whether the disengage deficit is observable in a TOJ-with-cueing task as well as in a speeded RT cueing task that are matched for stimuli and task decision. The goal is to determine whether, when the stimuli (cues and targets) and decision demands of the two task are relatively similar, the disengage deficit is a general phenomenon of neglect, or whether it is specific to the speeded RT cueing task. The TOJ task in the current experiment was conducted using an unspeeded response, while the RT task was conducted using standard speeded responses. If both tasks produce a disengage deficit, this similarity would provide converging evidence that neglect is a general problem of attentional orienting. However, if the TOJ task fails to produce a disengage deficit, then this difference would suggest that the effect has more to do with speeded responding than attention.

While most cases of neglect involve changes in processing of the contralesional side of space associated with an ipsilesional disengage deficit, there have been some reports of ipsilesional neglect (Kim et al., 1999). Patients with ipsilesional neglect might be expected to show a contralesional disengage deficit. To differentiate these two patterns, we will refer to the ipsilesional disengage deficit predicted by Posner et al. (1984) model of attention as a standard disengage deficit, and a contralesional disengage deficit as a paradoxical disengage deficit. In the current study, only patients with right hemisphere damage were tested and so their attentional deficits, if any, should appear on the left side of space. Therefore a standard (ipsilesional) disengage deficit would represent a rightward bias in attention, while a paradoxical (contralateral) disengage deficit would represent a leftward bias in attention.

It should be noted that both tasks in this study required participants to make a similarly demanding 2-choice, non-spatial discrimination. In the RT tasks participants decided as quickly as possible whether the target was red or blue while in the TOJ task participants decided, with no speed pressure, whether the first of two successively presented targets was red or blue. While the disengage deficit is typically studied using simple detection in the RT cueing task, cueing effects are also obtained in non-spatial discrimination (e.g., color discrimination) tasks in studies of visual orienting with control populations, beginning with Jonides and Irwin (1981). The target discrimination task has been suggested by several investigators for use in the TOJ task, specifically (Spence and Driver, 1994; Spence et al., 2001) to be necessary to avoid the possibility that early facilitation is simply the result of a criterion shift for responding to targets at the cued location (i.e., accepting less evidence from that location than the uncued one). False alarms on catch trials (trials without a target) cannot be used in a simple detection task to distinguish speed-accuracy trade-offs because the false alarms cannot be attributed to the cued or uncued location. Therefore, we have two rationales for our decision to use a color choice judgment for the RT task; (1) we wanted to equate stimulus-response demands with the TOJ task, and (2) we wanted the ability to look at errors in order to create an analogous measure for the disengage deficit.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Individuals with a right hemisphere stroke were recruited for the stroke groups. Inclusion criteria included medically stable and normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Exclusion criteria included other current psychiatric or neurological disorders, severe aphasia or dementia and color blindness. Individuals were assigned to either the neglect group (NEG) or right hemisphere control group (RHC) based on their performance on the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT). The criterion for neglect was abnormal performance, as based on the standard clinical cut-off, on at least one subtest of the BIT (please see Table A1 for the scores for each of the BIT subtests for each patient). This criterion for neglect was similar to that used by Sieroff et al. (2007) and adopted in order to increase the sensitivity of the BIT to the presence of neglect, as paper and pencil tests are often less sensitive to the presence of neglect in the post-acute or chronic phase (Friedrich and Margolin, 1993; Mattingley et al., 1994; Deouell et al., 2005; List et al., 2008; van Kessel et al., 2010; Bonato et al., 2012; reviewed in Bonato, 2012). Patients' stroke location was determined by clinical CT report. A summary of clinical, demographic and baseline data for participants in the NEG and RHC groups is presented in Table 2. We also included a control group, referred to as older healthy controls (OHC). These participants had no history of stroke, no signs of dementia, and no visual deficits.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and baseline neuropsychological assessment.

Apparatus and Stimuli

Each participant ran in both the RT task as well as the TOJ task. Stimuli were presented on an Apple iMac computer and were programmed using PsyScope Version 1.2.5. We used colored stimuli on a white background. A black fixation cross, measuring 1° × 1° of visual angle (VA), was presented in the center of the screen. Two box outlines in black, each measuring 4° × 4° VA, were positioned to the left and right of fixation offset from center by 4° to each box center. Cues consisted of a 45 ms change in box line-thickness (from one to four points) for one of the two boxes. A stimulus appeared in the center of each box and consisted of a red or blue pinwheel measuring 3° in diameter (see Figure 1). In the RT task, only one pinwheel was presented in one of the two boxes, while in the TOJ task one pinwheel was presented sequentially in each box.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic representation of the stimuli used in each task. (A) An example sequence of stimuli in a RT task trial, showing a sequence from the uncued condition. (B) An example sequence of stimuli in a TOJ task trial, showing a sequence from the uncued, left-target condition.

Procedure

The following tasks were carried out in accordance with the Tri-Council Policy Statement (Canada) and with the approval of the Capital Health Research Ethics Board (formerly the Queen Elizabeth II Health Sciences Center Research Ethics Board). Informed consent was obtained from all the participants.

Clinical tests

Standard neuropsychological tests for attention were administered to each participant in addition to the experimental tasks including Judgment of Line Orientation (Benton et al., 1983), the Elevator Counting Task (from the Test of Everyday Attention, Robertson et al., 1996), and the behavioral inattention task (BIT; Stone et al., 1991). The results of these tests are summarized with the patient data in Table 2. Three participants were found to have visual field deficits using the confrontation method; of those three, two showed detection of left-sided items on an in-house computerized perimetry testing task. The third participant with visual field deficits scored normal on the TOJ task. Thus, no individual had a visual field deficit that interfered with testing.

RT and TOJ tasks

Participants were seated 57 cm from the computer monitor and instructed to place their first two fingers of their right hand over the “2” and “8” keys of the number pad. The participants were instructed to fixate the central cross, which was on the screen for 1150 ms at the start of each trial. Fixation was visually monitored by the experimenter, and participants were reminded to remain fixated for the duration of the trial whenever they moved their eyes during a trial. A 45 ms cue was presented to one of the boxes (for left or right cue trials) or both boxes (for neutral cue trials) followed by the presentation of the pinwheel(s). Cues appeared at the same location as the target on 50% of trials, and at the opposite location on 50% of the trials.

In the RT task, only one target (a red or blue pinwheel) was presented in one of the peripheral boxes using a CTOA of 90 ms. The target remained visible until a response was made (see Figure 1). Participants ran in 120 trials. Participants were instructed to respond as fast as possible, without losing accuracy, pressing the “2” key if the target was red and the “8” key if the target was blue. Note that these keys were arranged one above the other on the number pad, and were therefore orthogonal to the dimension of spatial cueing (i.e., left vs. right).

In the TOJ task, the time interval between the onset of the cue and the onset of the first target pinwheel (CTOA) was fixed at 90 ms, while SOA between the first pinwheel and the second pinwheel varied, using the following intervals:−1920 ms, −960 ms, −480 ms, −240 ms, −120 ms, −60 ms, −30 ms, 30 ms, 60 ms, 120 ms, 240 ms, 480 ms, 960 ms, and 1920 ms, with negative SOAs indicating left-side first trials. Participants were asked to report the color of the first pinwheel (the target) presented following the cue without time pressure, and responses were recorded by pressing the “2” key if the target was red and the “8” key if the target was blue (a manual response was used in the TOJ task in order to better equate it with the RT task). Note that these keys were arranged one above the other on the number pad, and were therefore orthogonal to the dimension of spatial cueing (i.e., left vs. right). Both stimuli remained visible until a response was given. Participants ran in an average of 391 trials, and trials were distributed among the SOAs such that the smallest SOAs were sampled more often than the longest SOAs. The actual percentages used for the selection of SOA on each trial were 13.3% for +/−30 ms, 10% for +/−60 ms, 6.7% for +/−120 ms, +/−240 ms, and +/−480 ms, and 3.3% for +/−960 ms and +/−1920 ms. Although the number of trials per SOA condition varied slightly due to random sampling of SOA condition, on average the proportions achieved were very close to our intentions for both groups. Total trial number varied somewhat due to differences in fatigue level.

Methods of Analysis

In the RT task we used individual mean RTs to calculate two cueing effects (CE = uncued RT minus cued RT) for each participant, one for left-side targets and one for right-side targets. These CEs were then used to create a cueing asymmetry score for each participant. The formula for calculating cueing asymmetry scores is the same as that used to calculate the standard disengage deficit for patients with damage to the right hemisphere: CEleft-side targets minus CEright-side targets. Positive scores indicate a slower response to targets presented in the left side of space following a right side cue compared to the reverse. This pattern represents a rightward asymmetry, which is the same direction as the standard disengage deficit. Negative scores represent a leftward asymmetry (paradoxical disengage deficit).

We wanted to generate comparable cueing asymmetry scores for both the RT and TOJ tasks. While we have used PSS scores to determine whether there was a cueing effect in the TOJ task (see Eskes et al., 2007), the PSS cannot be coded in terms of the visual field of the target nor in terms of the location of the cue relative to the target, because targets are presented to both fields on every trial in a TOJ study. In order to calculate an analogous cueing asymmetry score for the TOJ task we defined TOJ trials based on “target” side, with a target side referring to the visual field of the item that was presented first. For example, all trials when the left item came up first (i.e., negative SOA trials) were defined as left-target trials. This allowed us to examine error rate as both a function of cueing (uncued vs. cued) and target location, just as in the RT task. We used individual mean error rates to calculate a CE (uncued error rate minus cued error rate) separately for left-side targets and right-side targets. If TOJs are affected in the expected direction by the cues (PSS shift) then errors will necessarily be lower when the cue is presented on the side of the first target (cued) than on the side of the second target (uncued). We then performed the standard cueing asymmetry score subtraction for patients with damage to the right hemisphere (CEleft-side targets minus CEright-side targets). Positive scores represent a rightward cueing asymmetry, which is in the same direction as the standard disengage deficit, while negative scores represent a leftward cueing asymmetry (paradoxical disengage deficit). For clarity, the meaning ascribed to the direction of these cueing asymmetry scores is congruent with the meaning ascribed to the direction of the cueing asymmetry scores in the RT task.

Results

RT Task Results

We examined error rates and mean RTs for each participant and for the entire set of participants to confirm that each participant was competent at performing the task. One participant in the NEG group had an error rate more than 5 SD from the mean, so he was eliminated from further analysis. To ensure both tasks were easily compared, we also eliminated the same subject from the TOJ analysis described below. All other participants had mean error rates and mean RTs that were within 2 SD of the overall means. The average error rate was 4.2% (SD = 7.1, n = 16). A mixed effects ANOVA on errors including cue location and target location as the within-subject variables, and group as the between-subject variable revealed no significant main effects or interactions.

Trials on which participants made erroneous responses (4.2%) and trials on which the participant missed the target (1%) were eliminated from RT analyses. Trials on which RTs were greater than 2 SD above a participant's mean for each condition were considered outliers and were eliminated from subsequent analysis (1.5%). Trials on which RTs were less than 150 ms were also eliminated (0.02%). Mean RTs for the NEG group (n = 7), RHC group (n = 5), and OHC group (n = 3) were 1034 ms (SD = 2224), 1316 ms (SD = 2482), and 494 ms (SD = 194), respectively.

To ensure that the RT task was effective at producing a cueing effect, mean RTs were calculated by cueing condition collapsed over side. The overall mean for cued, neutral and uncued trials were 781 ms (SD = 510), 823 ms (SD = 587), and 876 ms (SD = 600), respectively (see left graph, Figure 2). A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of cueing condition [F(2, 28) = 5.91, p < 0.01, MSE = 5753].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean cueing effects for each of the two tasks across all participants. Error bars represent +/− the standard error of the mean for each condition.

Individual mean RTs were then used to calculate cueing effects (CEs) and cueing asymmetry scores for left- and right-side targets for each participant (see section “Methods of Analysis”, above). From these data, group averages were created. Table 3 includes the descriptive statistics by group for left- and right-side target CEs and cueing asymmetry scores. Figure 3 illustrates group means for the cueing asymmetry scores.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means and SDs (in brackets) for each condition and task; means and SDs for the CEs and cueing asymmetry scores have been derived from subtractions for each individual participant.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean cueing asymmetry score for each of the two tasks. Error bars represent +/− the standard error of the mean for each group. In both cases, positive scores represent a rightward bias (the direction of the standard disengage deficit) and negative scores represent a leftward bias.

Because of the small number of participants in each group, we used non-parametric statistics to evaluate group differences in cueing asymmetry scores. While it appeared that the NEG group overall showed a rightward bias that was much greater than the OHC group, and that the RHC group showed a leftward bias, a Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA revealed no significant difference in bias scores between the groups for the RT task (H = 0.08, df = 2, p > 0.9). To confirm that there was no difference between the stroke groups alone, we performed a Mann-Whitney U test on the mean bias scores for the NEG and RHC groups, also demonstrating no significant difference (W = 25, n = 12, p > 0.6).

Because the mean cueing asymmetry scores for the NEG group appeared different from the RHC and OHC in the hypothesized direction, and because of the large variability, we examined cueing asymmetry scores for each individual participant in both the NEG and RHC groups. Figure 4 illustrates these individual cueing asymmetry scores, presented in order of their BIT Star Cancellation scores (indicated in the figures in italics). We also used the mean cueing asymmetry score +/−2 SD from the OHC group (indicated with dotted lines; 52 and 29, respectively) to determine which individual patients showed a right cueing asymmetry, indicative of a standard disengage deficit, or a left cueing asymmetry, suggesting a paradoxical disengage deficit. The graph reveals three interesting patterns:

  1. All individuals in both stroke groups possessed cueing asymmetry scores that were +/−2 SD beyond the mean cueing asymmetry score of the OHC group, indicating that cuing effects for each patient were abnormal either on the left side or on the right side. That is, all patients in the RT task qualified for a disengage deficit label; however, they were almost equally likely to have a standard disengage deficit as they were to have a paradoxical disengage deficit.
  2. Patients in the NEG group were no more likely than patients in the RHC group to have cueing asymmetry scores that put them in the range of a standard disengage deficit. In fact, more participants with neglect showed a paradoxical disengage deficit than a standard disengage deficit, albeit the paradoxical disengage deficits were smaller in terms of the absolute scores.
  3. We found no relationship between neglect scores as derived from the BIT Star Cancellation task and the overall cueing asymmetry score in the RT task (r = 0.02, df = 10, p > 0.9). This was also true for the total BIT scores (r = 0.02, df = 10, p > 0.9) and the center of cancellation scores on the line cancellation subtest (see Rorden and Karnath, 2010; r = −0.02, df = 10, p > 0.9). That is, patients with clinical left neglect (indicated by low BIT Star Cancellation scores) did not necessarily have cueing asymmetry scores indicative of a standard disengage deficit. This was true for individuals in both the NEG and RHC groups (see Table 4 for the correlations and p-values comparing cueing asymmetry scores and scores from the BIT subtests and BIT subtest center of cancellation scores, using all of the stroke patients).
FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Cueing asymmetry scores for NEG (left graph) and RHC group (right graph) in the RT task. Patients are represented in order of the BIT Star Cancellation scores, from lowest to highest (individual BIT Star Cancellation scores are listed above or below the individual bars). Cueing asymmetry scores above zero represent a rightward bias. Note that the x-axis spaces patients equidistantly, and does not represent the listed BIT Star Cancellation scores on an ordinal scale. See text for details.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Pearson correlations for the subtests of the BIT with the cueing asymmetry scores for both the RT task and TOJ task, including patients from both the NEG and RHC groups.

TOJ Task Results

To ensure that the cues in the TOJ task were effective at producing cueing, we examined PSS. A positive PSS indicates that the right-target would have to be presented before the left-target in order for a participant to experience them as being presented simultaneously; with the numerical value indicating how much of a lead the right-target would need (in ms). Three PSS scores were calculated per participant; one for trials on which a left cue was presented (left PSS), trials on which a neutral cue was presented (neutral PSS) and trials on which a right cue was presented (right PSS). Due to severe left neglect, we were unable to calculate reliable PSS scores for one neglect patient (ID 1159), so his data were excluded from the PSS cueing analysis. For the remaining participants, the mean PSS for these conditions was 295 ms (SD = 566), −58 ms (SD = 364), and −203 ms (SD = 413), respectively (see right graph, Figure 2). A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of cue location [F(2, 26) = 5.96, p < 0.01, MSE = 154,041]. Table 3 includes the descriptive statistics for left- and right-side target CEs based on error rates as well as the cueing asymmetry scores (see section “Methods of Analysis”, above).

Just as in the RT task, we used non-parametric tests to evaluate group differences in cueing asymmetry scores for the TOJ task. A Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA revealed no significant difference in cueing asymmetry scores between the groups (H = 1.72, df = 2, p > 0.4). To confirm that there was no significant difference among just the stroke groups, we performed a Mann-Whitney U test on mean cueing asymmetry scores for NEG and RHC groups, also demonstrating no significant difference (W = 21, n = 12, p < 0.64). Figure 3 (right graph) illustrates group means for the cueing asymmetry scores for the TOJ task.

Because TOJ cueing asymmetry scores had a finite range (1 to −1), and because the mean cueing asymmetry scores for each group were relatively small in comparison to this range, we were not compelled to examine the individual cueing asymmetry scores. However, to be consistent with the analysis in the RT task, we plotted the individual scores for each patient group in Figure 5 in order of their BIT Star Cancellation scores (indicated in italics). We again used the mean cueing asymmetry +/−2 SD from the OHC group (indicated with dotted lines; 0.12 and −0.31, respectively) to define a standard disengage deficit or a paradoxical disengage deficit. The graph reveals two interesting patterns:

  1. Most individuals in both groups were within a normal range, as defined by the mean cueing asymmetry score of the OHC group +/−2 SD.
  2. The two individuals with neglect whose cueing asymmetry scores were outside the normal range (OHC) and in the range of a standard disengage deficit did not have the most severe clinical neglect as determined by their BIT Star Cancellation scores. Indeed, there was no overall relationship between cueing asymmetry scores for the TOJ task and any of the BIT subtests (correlations and p-values are listed in Table 4).
FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Cueing asymmetry scores in the TOJ task for Neglect patients (left graph) and RHC patients (right graph) plotted in order of each patient's BIT Star Cancellation score (see text for details). Dotted lines represent +/− 2SD of mean OHC cueing asymmetry score.

Post-Hoc Analyses

To ensure the results were not due to a lack of counterbalancing the keys, we looked at responses as a function of response keys (“8” vs. “2”). For RT in the RT task, there was no main effect of key across all participants using a within-subject one-way ANOVA. There was also no interaction between key and participant group in a mixed ANOVA. This was also true for the TOJ task.

The correlation between the cueing asymmetry scores in two tasks, including all participants in all groups, was small and not significant (r = 0.08, df = 13, p > 0.7). We wanted to confirm that there was no relationship between cueing asymmetry scores in the RT task and TOJ task. To this end, we created two post-hoc groups of participants from the stroke population in our study: a Standard Disengage Deficit Group and a Paradoxical Disengage Deficit Group. These groups were chosen based on each patient's cueing asymmetry score from the RT task only. As one would expect, a Mann-Whitney U test revealed a significant effect of the post-hoc grouping on cueing asymmetry scores (W = 36, n = 12, p < 0.01). We then kept the groups the same to examine whether individuals who showed a standard disengage deficit in the RT task also show a standard disengage deficit (or even a bias in that direction) in the TOJ task. A Mann-Whitney U test revealed no significant difference between the post-hoc groups on cueing asymmetry scores in the TOJ task (W = 20, n = 12, p > 0.8). Figure 6 illustrates the mean cueing asymmetry scores for these post-hoc groups for both tasks. The TOJ graph nicely illustrates that there appears to be no difference between groups. Indeed, neither the Standard Disengage Deficit Group nor the Paradoxical Disengage Deficit Group showed a cueing asymmetry score different from zero (x = −0.003, df = 5, p > 0.9, and x = −0.02, df = 5, p > 0.6, respectively). The OHC group is included in the figure for reference.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Mean cueing asymmetry scores in each task for the post-hoc groups. OHC group is included for reference. See text for details.

Discussion

In the current study we sought to examine whether the disengage deficit typically observed in an RT task in patients with unilateral neglect might also be observed in the same patients using a TOJ task with spatial cueing. To this end, we compared performance on a standard speeded RT cueing task to a TOJ-with-cueing task that were equated for stimuli and response choice discrimination. It should be noted that for both tasks the peripheral cues were uninformative. As noted by Losier and Klein (2001) when neglect patients are subjected to a typical spatial cuing paradigm, the disengage deficit in RT is robust when peripheral cues are used (whether or not these cues are informative) while the disengage deficit is small to absent following purely endogenous cuing (informative central arrow cues).

Performance on both tasks showed significant cueing effects as expected and validated the cueing manipulation. Across both tasks, however, there were three major surprising and noteworthy patterns of results:

  1. Few stroke patients showed a disengage deficit in the TOJ task and cueing asymmetry scores in the TOJ task were not consistent with the cueing asymmetry scores in the RT task, either in a correlational analysis, or by post-hoc grouping.
  2. In the RT task typically used to demonstrate a standard disengage deficit, stroke patients were almost equally likely to show a standard disengage deficit (indicative of a spatial bias in favor of the ipsilesional side of space) as they were to show a paradoxical disengage deficit (indicative of a spatial bias in favor of the contralesional size of space).
  3. There was no relationship between the size and/or direction (i.e., whether it was standard or paradoxical) of the speeded disengage deficit and neglect severity. Indeed, patients who did not meet the criteria for clinical neglect (i.e., RHC) were just as likely to have a disengage deficit as patients who did meet the criteria for clinical neglect (i.e., NEG).

The cueing asymmetry scores in the two tasks were not consistent with each other, and indeed patients seemed to have very low cueing asymmetry scores in the TOJ task. This inconsistency would indicate that the disengage deficits evident in the RT task are a manifestation of the task demands rather than a general attentional state in patients with or without neglect. One possibility is that the differences in task demands elicited different mental sets; in the RT task the action system needed to be rapidly recruited, while in the TOJ task it did not.

Goodale and Milner (1992); Milner and Goodale (2008) proposed an influential model of vision that divides visual processing into two functional streams. The dorsal stream is involved in the use of vision for action; this stream controls detailed programming of online movements using bottom-up inputs from the retina to determine the specific parameters for movement. The ventral stream is involved with vision for identification; this stream enables the perception and identification of objects and their spatial relations. Knowing in advance that they would be required to execute a speeded motoric response in the RT task involving a key choice could have set the participants up to engage their dorsal stream. The cue may have provided an exogenous trigger for the initiation of a visually-guided motor plan, which would have to be inhibited in favor of a new plan triggered by the target. However, neglect patients often show motor perseveration—that is, a continuation of a behavior after a change in task demands (Kim et al., 1999). For example, neglect patients frequently mark individual stars repeatedly in the BIT star cancellation task, even while ignoring all of the stars in their neglected field. Engagement of the dorsal stream coupled with motor perseveration would preferentially impact reaction times in the RT task, but not accuracy in the TOJ task.

Another surprising finding, however, was that a direction of the patient's deficit in the RT task was not necessarily predicted by the location of their lesion or the side of space they were prone to neglect in standard tests of neglect. About half of the patients with right hemisphere damage, irrespective of whether they met the criteria for contralesional neglect or not, showed a disengage deficit in the RT task that was in favor of their contralesional side of space (i.e., demonstrating a paradoxical disengage deficit). The presence of a paradoxical disengage deficit as seen in our study has also been reported by others (e.g., Sacher et al., 2004 in their individual analyses). One reason for this pattern may be due to compensatory effects (Robertson et al., 1994; Dove et al., 2007). That is, patients who have contralesional neglect and are aware of their deficit may overcompensate for their spatial bias by making an effort to direct their attention leftward. Compensatory strategies for a task involving a time-pressured response coupled with motor perseveration might explain the paradoxical disengage deficits that were observed on some of the patients tested, although this compensation was not evident in the TOJ task. The role of compensatory strategies in neglect recovery and interaction with task demands has been highlighted in recent papers and reviews (e.g., Manly et al., 2002; Bonato et al., 2010; Bonato, 2012).

The fact that the standard disengage deficit was not consistent in the NEG group, and was also present in some of the RHC group is perhaps not surprising. Previous studies have shown that patients who have right hemisphere damage but who do not meet the criteria for clinical neglect might also have a standard disengage deficit, suggesting that some experimental tasks, including the Posner spatial cueing RT task that we used, might be more sensitive at detecting behavioral neglect than the standard clinical tests of neglect (Rengachary et al., 2009). In their meta-analysis, Losier and Klein (2001) found that the standard disengage deficit was significantly more severe in patients with neglect and, among those with neglect there was a significant correlation between neglect severity and the size of the disengage deficit. Neither of these patterns held up in our group of patients; however, this pattern is itself not entirely consistent in the literature. For example, Sieroff et al. (2007) found that there was no relationship between the magnitude of the disengage deficit and neglect severity in a group of patients defined in a similar way as our study. The lack of a group correlation between the disengage deficit and neglect severity, or the dissociation between the presence of a disengage deficit and neglect symptoms on clinical tests in individual patients has also been reported in several studies (e.g., Sacher et al., 2004; Olk et al., 2010; Bonato et al., 2012). Thus, further examination of what other variables may underlie the disengage deficit pattern appears warranted.

One explanation for our deviation from this pattern could be the variability of the data itself. The mean cueing asymmetry score for the NEG group was not significantly different from zero, and the only significant effects were found in the post-hoc groups, which is no surprise. What is noteworthy is that even when we grouped the patients according to whether they showed a standard disengage deficit in the RT task, they did not show a corresponding disengage deficit on TOJ task.

The performance of our patient participants on the RT task is more variable between-subject than the representative literature (cf., Losier and Klein, 2001), but there are several reasons that may explain this difference. First, we used relatively unrestricted inclusion criteria for our neglect patients and our individual analyses may have identified more variability than is normally included in small group studies. Second, researchers often report data in aggregate form, which may obscure individual differences among patients, and hide patients who lack the standard disengage deficit. We specifically examined the individual patients to determine how variable the disengage deficit was. We were surprised by the variability; however, we also felt that the extent and direction of the variability itself was interesting and important to report. Third, patient or methodological differences in the current study may have led to these findings. We chose to use a discrimination task in the cuing paradigm, in order to directly match the task requirements from the TOJ task. Since most cuing tasks use a detection response, this difference may have created some of the variability. Further investigation into the impact of type of task processing on the spatial cuing paradigm may help to resolve this issue. In any event, the disengage deficit has been regarded as a better test of neglect than the standard clinical tests, but if patients only show the effect under a very narrow range of conditions, the disengage deficit may not be an effective explanation or marker of neglect. Finally, and possibly most critically, the variability of performance among neglect patients in a spatial cueing paradigm may be under-reported due to a publication bias. Authors may shelve studies in which the patients are highly variable because the data don't conform to the author's theoretical framework, or may be rejected by reviewers because the data don't conform to the literature. However, every patient is providing valuable data, irrespective of one's theoretical framework. It is important that a literature represents the population it is studying, and in this case that population is highly variable.

Whatever the reason, the results of our study show that the disengage deficit is neither necessary nor sufficient to produce neglect. Not necessary because patients with left neglect might show no disengage deficit or a paradoxical disengage deficit; not sufficient because some patients without classic neglect as seen on paper and pencil tasks (i.e., RHC patients) show the standard disengage deficit. Our finding that the disengage deficit is not consistent across well-matched tasks (RT cuing vs. TOJ) also suggests that the disengage deficit is perhaps not the unifying explanation of neglect that some researchers hoped it would be (Adair and Barrett, 2008). In addition, our findings suggest that a better understanding of contributory factors that can influence visuo-spatial responding (e.g., non-spatial attention deficits, compensation strategies, and the role of task demands and manual responses) appears necessary to further advance theories of the basic mechanisms underlying spatial neglect.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research operating grant (to Drs. Gail A. Eskes et al.). We thank Dr. David Shore for his helpful advice and support with the TOJ paradigm, Hilary Duncan for technical support and the Capital Health stroke teams for help with patient recruitment. Thanks also go to the patients and controls who volunteered their time for this project.

Footnotes

  1. ^Losier and Klein's (2001) meta-analysis included studies that used both predictive and non-predictive cues. In the current study, we used non-predictive cues in both tasks; however, Losier and Klein found that the disengage deficit is robust with peripheral cues whether or not they are informative.

References

Adair, J. C., and Barrett, A. M. (2008). Spatial neglect: clinical and neuroscience review. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1142, 21–43.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartolomeo, P., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., and Doricchi, F. (2007). Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2479–2490.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baylis, G. C., Simon, S. L., Baylis, L. L., and Rorden, C. (2002). Visual extinction with double simultaneous stimlulation: what is simultaneous? Neuropsychologia 40, 1027–1034.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baynes, K., Holtzman, J. D., and Volpe, B. T. (1986). Components of visual attention: alteration in response pattern to visual stimuli following parietal lobe infarction. Brain 109, 99–114.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. D., Varney, N. R., and Spreen, O. (1983). Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Berberovic, N., Pisella, L., Morris, A. P., and Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Prismatic adaptation reduces biased temporal order judgments in spatial neglect. Neuroreport 15, 1199–1204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bisiach, E., Luzzatti, C., and Perani, D. (1979). Unilateral neglect, representational schema and consciousness. Brain 102, 609–618.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonato, M. (2012). Nelgect and extinction depend greatly on task demands: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:195. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., Umilta, C., and Zorzi, M. (2010). Increased attentional demands impair contralesional space awareness following stroke. Neuropsychologia 48, 3934–3940.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., Umilta, C., and Zorzi, M. (2012). Deficits of contralesional awareness: a case study on what paper-and-pencil tests neglect. Neuropsychology 26, 20–36.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., and Zorzi, M. (2009). Normal and impaired reflexive orienting of attention after central nonpredictive cues. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 745–759.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D'Erme, P., Gainotti, G., Bartolomeo, P., and Robertson, I. H. (1994). “Early ipsilateral orienting of attention in patients with contralateral neglect,” in Cognitive Neuropsychology and Cognitive Rehabilitation, eds M. J. Riddoch and G. W. Humphreys (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 205–223.

Deouell, L., Sacher, Y., and Soroker, N. (2005). Assessment of spatial attention after brain damage with a dynamic reaction time test. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 697–707.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Di Pellegrino, G., Basso, G., and Frassinetti, F. (1997). Spatial extinction on a double asynchronous stimulation. Neuropsychologia 35, 1215–1223.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dove, M. E., Eskes, G., Klein, R. M., and Shore, D. (2007). A left attentional bias in chronic neglect: a case study using temporal order judgments. Neurocase 13, 37–49.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Egly, R., Driver, J., and Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 123, 161–177.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eskes, G. A., Klein, R. M., Dove, M. B., Coolican, J., and Shore, D. I. (2007). Comparing temporal order judgments and choice reaction time tasks as indices of exogenous spatial cuing. J. Neurosci. Methods 166, 259–265.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farah, M. J., Wong, A. B., Monheit, M. A., and Morrow, L. A. (1989). Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: modality-specific or supramodal. Neuropsychologia 27, 461–470.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friedrich, F. J., and Margolin, D. I. (1993). Response time measures of hemi-inattention: a longitudinal case report. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 6, 54–59.

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heath, R. A. (1984). Response-time and temporal-order judgment in vision. Aust. J. Psychol. 36, 21–34.

Heilman, K. M., and Valenstein, E. (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Ann. Neurol. 5, 166–170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jonides, J., and Irwin, D. E. (1981). Capturing attention. Cognition 10, 145–150.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Karnath, H.-O., Berger, M. F., Kuker, W., and Rorden, C. (2004). The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1164–1172.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karnath, H.-O., Himmelbach, M., and Rorden, C. (2002a). The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. Brain 125, 350–360.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karnath, H.-O., Milner, A. D., and Vallar, G. (Eds.). (2002b). The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kim, M., Na, D. L., Kim, G. M., Adair, J. C., Lee, K. H., and Heilman, K. M. (1999). Ipsilesional neglect: behavioural and anatomical features. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 67, 35–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leibovitch, F. S., Black, S. E., Caldwell, C. B., Ebert, P. L., Ehrlich, L. E., and Szalai, J. P. (1998). Brain-behavior correlations in hemispatial neglect using CT and SPECT: the Sunnybrook stroke study. Neurology 50, 901–908.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

List, A., Brooks, J. L., Esterman, M., Flevaris, A. V., Landau, A. N., Bowman, G., Stanton, V., Vanvleet, T. M., Robertson, L. C., and Schendel, K. (2008). Visual hemispatial neglect, re-assessed. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 243–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Losier, B. J. W., and Klein, R. M. (2001). A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 1–13.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Manly, T., Ward, S., and Robertson, I. (2002). “The rehabilitation of attention,” in Neuropsychological Interventions: Clinical Research and Practice. ed P. J. Eslinger (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 105–136.

Maruff, P., Hay, D., Malone, V., and Currie, J. (1995). Asymmetries in the covert orienting of visual-spatial attention in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 33, 1205–1223.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., Bradshaw, J. A., and Nettleton, N. C. (1994). Residual rightward attentional bias after apparent recovery from right hemisphere damage: implications for a multicomponent model of neglect. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 597–604.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mesulam, M. M. (1981). A corical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann. Neurol. 10, 309–325.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, J., and Schwarz, W. (2006). Dissociations between reaction times and temporal order judgments: a diffusion model approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32, 394–412.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milner, A. D., and Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46, 774–785.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morrow, L. A., and Ratcliff, G. (1988). The disengagement of covert attention and the neglect syndrome. Psychobiology 16, 261–269.

Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., and Husain, M. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain 126, 1–12.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neumann, O., Esselmann, U., and Klotz, W. (1993). Differential effects of visual-spatial attention on response latency and temporal-order judgment. Psychol. Res. 56, 26–34.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Olk, B., Hildebrandt, H., and Kingstone, A. (2010). Involuntary but not voluntary orienting contributes to a disengage deficit in visual neglect. Cortex 46, 1149–1164.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric Explorations of Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Posner, M. I., and Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Atten. Perform. 10, 531–556.

Posner, M. I., Cohen, Y., and Rafal, R. D. (1982). Neural systems control of spatial orienting. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 298, 187–198.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., and Vaughan, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2, 228.

Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., and Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 109, 160–174.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., and Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J. Neurosci. 4, 1863–1874.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rastelli, F., Funes, M., Lupianez, J., Duret, C., and Bartolomeo, P. (2008). Left visual neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based? Exp. Brain Res. 187, 439–446.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rengachary, J., d'Avossa, G., Sapir, A., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2009). Is the Posner reaction time test more accurate than clinical tests in detecting left neglect in acute and chronic stroke? Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 90, 2081–2088.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robertson, I. H., Halligan, P. W., Bergego, C., Homberg, V., Pizzamiglio, L., Weber, E., and Wilson, B. A. (1994). Right neglect following right-hemisphere damage. Cortex 30, 199–213.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Robertson, I. H., Mattingley, J. B., Rorden, C., and Driver, J. (1998). Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature, 395, 169–172.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1996). The structure of normal human attention: the test of everyday attention. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2, 525–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rorden, C., and Karnath, H.-O. (2010). A simple measure of neglect severity. Brain 134, 903–912.

Rorden, C., Mattingley, J. B., Karnath, H.-O., and Driver, J. (1997). Visual extinction and prior entry: impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage. Neuropsychologia 35, 421–433.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sacher, Y., Serfaty, C., Deouell, L., Sapir, A., Hanik, A., and Soroker, N. (2004). Role of disengagement failure and attentional gradient in unilateral spatial neglect—a longitudinal study. Disabil. Rehabil. 26, 746–755.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schindler, I., McIntosh, R. D., Cassidy, T. P., Birchall, D., Benson, V., Ietswaart, M., and Milner, A. D. (2009). The disengage deficit in hemispatial neglect is restricted to between-object shifts and is abolished by prism adaptation. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 499–510.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shore, D. I., Spence, C., and Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. Psychol. Sci. 12, 205–212.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sieroff, E., Decaix, C., Chokron, S., and Bartolomeo, P. (2007). Impaired orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect: a componential analysis. Neuropsychology 21, 94–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sinnett, S., Juncadella, M., Rafal, R., Azanon, E., and Soto-Faraco, S. (2007). A dissociation between visual and auditory hemi-inattention: evidence from temporal order judgements. Neuropsychologia 45, 552–560.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Snyder, J. J., and Chatterjee, A. (2006). The frontal cortex and exogenous attentional orienting. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1913–1923.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spence, C. J., and Driver, J. (1994). Covert spatial orienting in audition - exogenous and endogenous mechanisms. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 555–574.

Spence, C., Shore, D. I., and Klein, R. M. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 799–832.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stelmach, L. B., and Herdman, C. M. (1991). Directed attention and perception of temporal order. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 17, 539–550.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stone, S. P., Halligan, P. W., Wilson, B., Greenwood, R. J., and Marshall, J. C. (1991). Performance of age-matched controls on a battery of visuo-spatial neglect tests. J. Neurol, Neurosurg. Psychiatry 54, 341–344.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., and Egaas, B. (1996). Slowed orienting of covert visual-spatial attention in autism: specific deficits associated with cerebellar and parietal abnormality. Dev. Psychopathol. 8, 563–584.

Vallar, G. (2001). Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. NeuroImage 14, S52–S58.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vallar, G., and Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions—a clinical ct-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia 24, 609–622.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Kessel, M. E., van Nes, I. J. W., Brouwer, W. H., Geurts, A. C. H., and Fasotti, L. (2010). Visuospatial asymmetry and non-spatial attention in subacute stroke patients with and without neglect. Cortex 46, 602–612.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. Behavioral inattention task subtest scores.

Keywords: attention, spatial cueing, temporal order judgments, unilateral neglect

Citation: Dukewich KR, Eskes GA, Lawrence MA, MacIsaac M-B, Phillips SJ and Klein RM (2012) Speed impairs attending on the left: comparing attentional asymmetries for neglect patients in speeded and unspeeded cueing tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:232.doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00232

Received: 26 January 2012; Accepted: 21 July 2012;
Published online: 21 August 2012.

Edited by:

Mario Bonato, University of Padova, Italy

Reviewed by:

Mario Bonato, University of Padova, Italy
Igor C. Schindler, University of Hull, UK

Copyright © 2012 Dukewich, Eskes, Lawrence, MacIsaac, Phillips and Klein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Gail A. Eskes, Department of Psychiatry, Brain Repair Centre, Life Sciences Research Institute, Dalhousie University, 1348 Summer Street, PO Box 15000, Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada. e-mail: gail.eskes@dal.ca

Download