Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 22 February 2010
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 4 - 2010 | https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.001.2010

Personal familiarity influences the processing of upright and inverted faces in infants

1
Children’s Hospital Boston, Boston, MA, USA
2
Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
4
Universite Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
Infant face processing becomes more selective during the first year of life as a function of varying experience with distinct face categories defined by species, race, and age. Given that any individual face belongs to many such categories (e.g. A young Caucasian man’s face) we asked how the neural selectivity for one aspect of facial appearance was affected by category membership along another dimension of variability. 6-month-old infants were shown upright and inverted pictures of either their own mother or a stranger while event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. We found that the amplitude of the P400 (a face-sensitive ERP component) was only sensitive to the orientation of the mother’s face, suggesting that “tuning” of the neural response to faces is realized jointly across multiple dimensions of face appearance.

Introduction

Selectivity is a key feature of mature representations of facial appearance. As demonstrated by a substantial body of behavioral results obtained from adult observers, faces are processed and recognized by distinct processes that do not appear to be readily applied to control stimuli (including inverted, scrambled, or contrast-negated faces). Such features of face processing include high-fidelity encoding of so-called “2nd-order” features of the face (Rhodes et al., 1993 ) and holistic processing of face patterns as revealed by phenomena like the composite-face effect (Young et al., 1987 ) and the part-whole effect (Tanaka and Farah, 1993 ). That these features of adult face processing are generally evident in unaltered faces, but absent or reduced in faces that have been transformed by disorientation or other image-level manipulations indicates that the visual system has learned important distinctions between classes of face-like stimuli. That is, true faces are distinguished from non-face images that share many of the same low-level properties. Presently, we examine how such distinctions are acquired (and thus how selective representations are constructed) by investigating the selectivity of infants’ neural response to faces belonging to distinct categories.
The perceptual processes that contribute to face recognition in infancy become increasingly selective during the first year of life. Specifically, infants show poorer recognition of face exemplars within categories they are not exposed to frequently. This process has been termed “perceptual narrowing” (Nelson, 2001 ; Scott et al., 2007 ) and has been observed in the context of face categories defined by species (Pascalis et al., 2002 ; Sugita, 2008 ), gender (Quinn et al., 2002 ), and race (Kelly et al., 2005 , 2007 ). In each case, younger infants are able to successfully individuate faces in a broader set of categories than older infants, with older infants being worse at discriminating between faces belonging to infrequently encountered categories. Perceptual narrowing results demonstrate that experience shapes selectivity during infancy, leading to profound perceptual consequences. The first year of life (specifically the 6- to 9-month age range) is thus a critically important time period for studying how distinctions are made between faces and non-face control stimuli, as well as between distinct categories of faces defined at varying levels of granularity. Our aim in the present study was to characterize how distinct types of face selectivity may interact during this period of development. This is distinct from any effort to characterize true perceptual narrowing, but shares the same broad goal of perceptual narrowing research insofar as we wish to more fully understand the developmental timecourse of differential processing for various kinds of faces and face-like stimuli. Specifically, we ask how differential processing of faces at a sub-ordinate level (personally familiar vs. unfamiliar face) may lead to differential processing at a basic level (face vs. inverted face).
What do we mean when we say that faces may be distinguished at different levels of granularity or “scales?” Intuitively, we suggest that degrees of face selectivity can be considered hierarchically. A coarse face processing strategy (that is not very selective) may only differentiate between faces and non-faces (such as inverted, photo-negative or distorted faces (Figure 1 , top row). A more sophisticated and selective representation may distinguish between face categories on the basis of species, gender, race, or age (Figure 1 , middle row). Finally, a very sophisticated representation of facial appearance may be able to make distinctions between individual exemplars within a category. This naïve ontology is not motivated by any a priori understanding of how face perception is behaviorally or neurally organized. For example, we do not suggest that we can clearly state where category boundaries lie between the face and non-face stimuli we have included in Figure 1 . Nonetheless this framework serves as a useful starting point for asking important questions about how selectivity develops. How (if at all) do the representations used to make the basic distinction between upright faces and other stimuli depend on the representations used to make distinctions between individuals? Do increasingly specific representations of facial appearance develop hierarchically, so that increasingly “fine-grained” distinctions between faces can be made as development progresses and “coarser” distinctions are mastered? Alternatively, does experience with particular individuals drive selectivity across all scales? That is, does extensive experience with a particular face lead to an enhanced ability to make distinctions between that face and its counterparts at all levels of granularity? Do infants learn a neural representation of facial appearance solely by accumulating and applying general principles of selectivity (e.g. “young, white, female faces are processed differently than other faces”) or by applying the information if representations can also be learned from specific exemplars (e.g. “Faces that don’t look like Mom aren’t processed like her”).
Figure 1. A Hierarchical ontology for face categories. A schematic view of multiple levels of face specificity. At the broadest level on top of this diagram, faces may only be differentiated from non-faces. Continuing downwards, faces are grouped by categories like race and species, ultimately grouped within those categories by individual identity. The goal of the current study is to determine how these varying levels of specificity affect one another in terms of the neural response to faces.
In the present study, we asked if the magnitude of a neural “inversion effect” for faces was contingent on face familiarity. Face recognition performance suffers more than generic object recognition following a 180-degree planar rotation of the image (Yin, 1969 ), and ERP waveforms also exhibit a marked inversion effect. Specifically, the N170 component in adults, for example, tends to be delayed and of larger amplitude in response to inverted faces (Rossion et al., 1999 ). The distinction between upright and inverted faces is an example of differential processing at a “coarse” grain, while face familiarity obviously represents a comparatively much “finer” grain at which differential processing may be realized. Further, both face orientation and face familiarity have been studied behaviorally in infants, making their interaction an attractive starting point for investigating the neural basis of perceptual narrowing across multiple aspects of face variability. Newborn infants show a behavioral response suggestive of selectivity for upright vs. inverted faces (Farroni et al., 2005 ), an ability that improves dramatically over subsequent months (Turati et al., 2004 ). Further, within the first few months infants can visually discriminate the mother’s face from that of a stranger (Pascalis et al., 1995 ; de Haan et al., 2001 ). Given that infants of sufficient age can differentiate between faces along these dimensions, how do familiarity and orientation interact at the level of the neural response? We used event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine the extent to which face category membership defined at one “level” (as in Figure 1 ) modulated the selectivity of the neural response at another level.
We varied face orientation (upright vs. inverted) in a within-subjects design, varying face familiarity across participants. Our index of the neural response to faces was the P400 component, which is a face-sensitive response observed over occipital electrode sites (de Haan and Nelson, 1999 ; Halit et al., 2003 ). This component begins to emerge as early as 3 months of age (Halit et al., 2003 , 2004 ; Scott and Nelson, 2004 ; Macchi Cassia et al., 2006 ) and is thus a useful way to examine neural face processing during the first year of life. Our question is whether or not the P400 exhibits differential processing for upright and inverted faces as a function of face familiarity, or if there appears to be no influence of face familiarity on the magnitude of the inversion effect. The former case would suggest that face orientation selectivity for this component is initially driven by individual exemplars, while the latter would suggest that selectivity has been acquired and applied generally to wider subsets of faces by 6 months of age. We find evidence of an intriguing interaction between these levels of face categorization, suggesting that the exquisite selectivity exhibited by adult observers may be built up in an exemplar-driven fashion.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Infants were recruited from a metropolitan area. All infants were born full-term with no known pre- or perinatal complications. Informed consent was obtained from the parents and infants were assigned to one of two experimental groups. Infants belonging to the “Mother’s Face” group were presented with their mother’s face during the task, and infants belonging to the “Stranger’s Face” group were presented with a stranger’s face (see Stimuli for details). The final sample in the “Mother’s Face” group consisted of 20 participants (8 males) with a mean age of 183 days (range 177–191 days). An additional 40 infants were tested but not included in the final sample due to eye and/or body movements that resulted in excessive artifact (n = 29) or fussiness that resulted in too few trials being recorded (n = 11). The final sample in the “Stranger’s Face” group consisted of 14 participants (9 males) with a mean age of 184 days (range 177–188 days). An additional 13 infants were tested but not included in the final sample due to excessive eye and/or body movements that resulted in excessive artifact (n = 11) or fussiness that resulted in too few trials being recorded (n = 2). We emphasize that participants were only excluded for reasons relating to data quality (ERP artifacts) and not due to boredom or disinterest in the task. Further, our attrition rate (though high) is consistent with the 50–75% dropout rate typically reported for infant ERP studies (DeBoer et al., 2007 ).

Stimuli

The stimuli consisted of color images of female faces displaying neutral expressions. Each woman was photographed wearing a gray scarf in order to conceal any clothing while seated in front of a gray background. Infants in the “Mother’s Face” group viewed pictures of their own mother’s face presented multiple times in both upright and inverted orientations. Infants in the “Stranger’s Face” group viewed pictures of an unfamiliar female face (mothers of “Mother’s Face” infants) that was judged to be dissimilar-looking to their own mother, but matched for the presence of eyeglasses and/or race of the model. This face was also presented in upright and inverted orientations.

Procedure

Testing took place in a dimly lit, quiet room after application of the sensor net. Infants were tested while sitting on their parent’s lap. Stimuli were presented using E-Prime software v1.2 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The faces were presented on the center of the screen on a white background. The computer monitor was 48 cm wide and 31 cm high. When viewed from a distance of 60 cm, the faces on average subtended a horizontal angle of approximately 16° and a vertical angle of 15°. A video camera mounted above the monitor and centered on the infant’s face allowed for observation of the infant at all times during the testing session. On-line judgments were made to present the pictures only when the infant was attending to the monitor. Trials were immediately marked for deletion by the experimenter if the infant looked away during stimulus presentation.
Stimuli were presented for 500 ms followed by an experimenter controlled inter-stimulus interval of at least 1500 ms during which time the screen was white. The two orientations were randomly presented with equal probability, with the constraint that stimuli from the same orientation were not repeated more than three times in succession. Stimulus presentation continued until the infant became bored or too fussy to attend, with a maximum of 100 trials. The average number of total trials viewed by infants was 71 and 80, for the “Mother’s Face” and “Stranger’s Face” groups respectively.

ERP Recording and Analysis

ERPs were recorded using a 64-channel Geodesic Sensor Net v2.0 (Electrical Geodesics Inc., Eugene, OR). EEG was recorded continuously and referenced to a single vertex electrode (Cz). Signals were amplified using an EGI NetAmps 200 amplifier with a band-pass filter of 0.1–80 Hz and a sampling rate of 200 Hz. Impedances were checked on-line prior to beginning the session and were considered acceptable if lower than 50 KOhm.
Continuous EEG data were processed offline using NetStation v4.1.2 (Eugene, OR). A 30-Hz lowpass filter was applied and trials were constructed that consisted of a 100 ms baseline period and 1500 ms period following stimulus onset. Data were baseline corrected to the average voltage during the 100 ms prior to stimulus onset. Segmented data were edited for EOG and motion artifact. Data from individual sensors were rejected if there was artifact resulting from poor contact or movement. The entire trial was excluded if more than nine sensors had been rejected, or if an eye-blink or other significant artifact had occurred. Of the remaining trials, individual channels containing artifact were replaced using spherical spline interpolation. Individual subject averages were constructed separately for the upright and inverted faces (M = 22 trials per condition in both groups), and data were re-referenced to the average reference.
Inspection of the grand-averaged waveforms revealed a well-defined P400 component that was subsequently analyzed within a time window 345–599 ms. Electrode groupings and time windows were selected based on previous reports of this component and through visual inspection of the grand-averaged and individual waveforms. Ten occipital electrodes were identified for our analysis of the P400 (32, 33, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 44, 45) which were further partitioned into left, midline and right regions for analysis (Figure 2 A).
Figure 2. Responses to upright and inverted faces as a function of personal familiarity. (A) The P400 waveform depicted here represents averaged activity over 10 occipital electrodes included in the analysis. These 10 electrodes were grouped into three distinct regions for statistical analysis of regional effects. (B) Grand-averaged waveforms obtained for mother’s face upright and mother’s face inverted. (C) Grand-averaged waveforms obtained for a stranger’s face upright and inverted. Note the absence of a differential response to upright and inverted faces in the “unfamiliar face” group.

Results

The peak amplitude (maximum value attained within the previously mentioned time window) and latency of the P400 was analyzed for each group separately using a 2 × 3 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Orientation (upright, inverted) and Region (left, mid, right electrode groupings) as within-subjects factors. Greenhouse-Geisser corrected degrees of freedom were used and post hoc paired t-tests were conducted using a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

“Mother’s Face” Group

Amplitude

There was a main effect of Orientation, F(1, 18) = 5.83, p = 0.027, such that the mother inverted (M = 15 μV, SD = 5.6) elicited a significantly larger P400 peak amplitude than mother upright (M = 12.6 μV, SD = 6; see Figure 2 B). We note that in particular that there were no interactions of Region (left, midline, or right sensor groups), which is why we have chosen to present an average waveform over all 10 electrodes in Figure 2 .

Latency

For latency to the P400 peak, there were no main effects or interactions of Region or Orientation.

“Stranger’s Face” Group

Amplitude

For the amplitude of the P400, there were no main effects or interactions of Orientation or Region. (see Figure 2 C).

Latency

For latency to the P400 peak, there was a main effect of Region, F(2,12) = 5.23, p = 0.019. Paired comparisons revealed that the midline electrode group (M = 478 ms, SD = 39) peaked marginally faster than the left electrode group only (M = 506 ms, SD = 34), t(13) = 2.67, p = 0.057.
In summary, we find evidence of a differential response to upright and inverted faces only in the “Mother’s Face” group. Specifically, the amplitude of the P400 is larger to inverted faces than to upright faces. In Figure 3 , we display the amplitude data across subjects as a function of stimulus type, sensor region, and subject group. There were no effects of face orientation on latency in either group, which may be due to fairly large variability across subjects (pooled SD across all conditions and groups = 36 ms).
Figure 3. Average P400 amplitudes by subject group and face orientation. At left, we present the between-subjects average of the P400 amplitude to the Mother’s face in upright and inverted orientations for each of our three sensor groups. At right, the same results are presented for the Stranger’s Face group. Error bars represent+/−1 sem. calculated over the subjects in each group.
Given the difference in the number of subjects included in the “Mother’s Face” and “Stranger’s Face” group, we also conducted an additional Monte Carlo analysis to rule out the possibility that a significant effect was observed in the larger group (“Mother’s Face”) simply due to the larger sample size. We carried out a “jackknife” procedure in which 14 participants were randomly sampled with replacement 10,000 times from the full set of 19 participants in the Mother’s Face group. At each iteration, the F-statistic for the main effect of face inversion was computed. This allows us to compare the distribution of F-ratios obtained from this procedure to the observed F-ratio obtained from the Stranger’s Face group. We find that this procedure yields an F-value for face inversion that is both significantly larger than one and also significantly larger than the F-ratio obtained from the Stranger’s Face group as determined by the 5th quantile of the empirical jackknife distribution. We therefore conclude that the observed difference between the groups is likely not due to the difference in sample size.

Discussion

Infants in the current study were presented with digital images of either their mother’s face or a stranger’s face presented in an upright and inverted orientation. ERP data revealed that the amplitude of the P400 was greater to mother inverted vs. mother upright. However, no amplitude differences for this component were observed for the upright vs. inverted stranger manipulation. In terms of our initial question regarding how face selectivity is realized across different conceptual scales, even as early as 6 months, evidence for exemplar-driven influence on face processing can be observed. We take this as evidence that face selectivity may indeed be learned in an exemplar-driven fashion. That is, increasing selectivity for facial appearance emerges by repeated exposure to particular individuals, resulting in an enhanced capacity for differential processing at multiple scales of selectivity.
Our result is consistent with adult data from a number of studies (Bruce, 1986 ). Most directly, Balas et al. (2007) reported a familiarity effect for judging face orientation, suggesting that increased exposure to a face during adulthood facilitates orientation processing. Along other dimensions of face variation, including gender (Rossion, 2002 ), race (Bruyer et al., 2004 ), and expression (Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2004 ) similar familiarity effects are obtained. Narrowing may thus be driven by and constrained by the individual faces that are seen most often. It is also noteworthy that increased training with non-face categories including “Greeble” stimuli (Gauthier and Tarr, 1997 ) and texture patterns (Husk et al., 2007 ) leads to inversion effects in adults. This may mean that selectivity in any domain may be driven primarily by the individual items experienced which subsequently cut deeply across scales of stimulus categories.
How do our data relate to other studies of the face inversion effect in infancy? A key point of comparison is to consider our results alongside those reported by de Haan et al. (2002) , who presented 6-month-old infants and adults with upright and inverted images of human and monkey faces. While they report a species-specific inversion effect on the N170 component in adults, they also report the absence of any inversion effects on the “infant N170” (a negativity between 200–350 ms) and a non-specific inversion effect that is evident at the P400. This latter result is not consistent with the data we have reported here, insofar as monkey faces should certainly be unfamiliar to the infants in their task, and the models used in the human face trials were also not familiar to the infants. In neither case would our data suggest that an inversion effect should be evident at the P400. Moreover, there is an additional important discrepancy between our results and this previous study: The inversion effect reported at the P400 in this previous study goes in the opposite direction as our inversion effect. Inverted faces (both human and monkey) elicit a smaller positive peak at the P400, and effect which would appear broadly consistent with other results concerning the “narrowing” of face representations. Can we reconcile our results with this previous report? It is difficult to speak conclusively about the mutual relationship between such distinct results, but we briefly point out two important points that may be relevant to the differences we have described above. First, we note that while we did not observe any topographical differences between conditions, the de Haan paper reports that the inversion effect for Monkey faces was only evident over the left hemisphere, but obtained over both hemispheres for human faces. Second, it is perhaps important that the present study used only one model face per child (presented multiple times in each orientation) while the de Haan, Pascalis and Johnson study reports the use of 20 model faces per condition. The use of more model faces is laudable, and strengthens the generality of their results. In our case, obtaining a wide range of highly familiar faces was not practical, constraining the number of faces we could use in the “Stranger’s Face” condition. We suggest that it is possible that the repetition of the same model face repeatedly may have important consequences on the components we have examined here, an issue that has not yet been studied in great depth.
Finally, what are we to make of the differing directions of the face inversion effects reported here and in the de Haan study? If we are to begin by comparing the results of each study to the established pattern of inversion effects in the adult literature we must decide if the important feature of the adult face inversion effect at the N170 is the fact that it is of greater absolute amplitude, or the fact that it is more negative. The former would appear to be consistent with our result, while the latter would be more consistent with an inversion effect in the opposite direction (a more negative positive deflection implies a smaller peak). One way to potentially resolve this ambiguity is to consider data from other components used to study adult face processing. Both the P1 and the P2 exhibit larger amplitudes for inverted faces (Itier and Taylor, 2002 ) suggesting that perhaps the hallmark of inversion effects is an overall increase in the absolute amplitude of the ERP signal, rather than increased negativity. Though this provides a tentative suggestion that our data are broadly consistent with adult ERP results, future study of the consistency of inversion effects at the P400 during the first year of life will ultimately prove invaluable. Finally, we note that changes in the sign of the inversion effect have been observed in longitudinal studies of infant memory (Bauer et al., 2006 ), indicating that the same child may “flip” the inversion effect between test sessions. This suggests that it is possible to find inversion effects of differing sign within a relatively short timespan of development.
We close by suggesting potential extensions of this study that would further clarify the consequences of differential processing at multiple scales of face categorization. First, an important extension of this work (that is unfortunately difficult to achieve due to limitations on infant ERP study designs) is the inclusion of multiple stimulus categories representing a more comprehensive sampling of “face space”. (Valentine, 1991 ). Our comparison of familiar to unfamiliar female face processing across distinct subject groups is a fairly crude tool for examining the rich multi-dimensional code for facial appearance. As we have already noted, our desire to use personally familiar faces severely necessarily limited the number of distinct exemplars we could present to our infant subjects with, potentially limiting the generality of our results. Should exploring a larger set of stimuli within individual subjects prove practical (perhaps including father’s faces, or other siblings), further elaboration of interactions across “scales” of face specificity would be vital to understanding the development of face recognition. Specifically, further characterizing how categories defined by race, age, or gender all interact with personal familiarity or basic-level face/non-face distinctions would be a crucial step towards describing the acquisition of selective face responses. Second, a similar means of extending the present results would be to consider the impact of personal familiarity (or any other categorical distinction between faces) on the processing associated with other kinds of transformed face stimuli. Here, we have only examined the inversion effect as one example of a “coarse-scale” distinction between face and non-face control images. There are, of course, a wide range of similar transformations that can be carried out on face images that leave many low-level features of the face intact, including color or luminance negation (Galper, 1970 ; Kemp et al., 1990 ), or scrambling of face parts. Indeed, many other aspects of face recognition (such as the acquisition of view-invariant recognition) are developing during the first year of life, making them interesting candidates for a similar analysis. Examining how differential processing at the level of exemplars or other face categories impacts differential processing of these other kinds of transformation would represent an additional important contribution.
Finally, we also point out that in our current data we were unable to consistently identify the so-called “infant N170,” an additional face-sensitive component that is frequently reported at ∼290 ms (de Haan et al., 2002 ). Should it prove possible to obtain a sufficiently robust response that this component could also be analyzed in the same manner as we have examined the P400 here, it would be useful to determine the extent to which these two components index similar or distinct aspects of face selectivity behave similarly. Similarly, in terms of further understanding the neural substrate supporting our effects, limits in the spatial resolution of ERPs preclude us from drawing inferences as to where in the brain these processes are taking place. Extending the study presented here using Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS) would permit more robust localization and provide an important complement to this work (Otsuka et al., 2007 ).
To conclude, our findings suggest that by 6 months of age, the neural architecture underlying face processing reflects selectivity learned from experience with particular faces. Differential processing at coarse scales appears to be modulated by differential processing at comparatively finer scales. Overall, these data complement the extensive behavioral literature on the development of face processing by revealing that the ability to process information about familiarity and orientation are not fully independent.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The writing of this paper was supported by grants from the NIH (NS32976 and MH078829) to Charles A. Nelson. The authors thank Venessa Peña and Hannah Mandel for their assistance in data collection and coding; and the parents who participated.

References

Balas, B., Cox, D., and Conwell, E. (2007). The effect of real-world personal familiarity on the speed of face information processing. PLoS ONE 2, e1223. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001223.
Bauer, P. J. (2006). Constructing a past in infancy: a neuro-developmental account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 175–181.
Bruce, V. (1986). Influences of familiarity on the processing of faces. Perception 15, 387–397.
Bruyer, R., Leclere, S., and Quinet, P. (2004). Ethnic categorisation of faces is not independent of face identity. Perception 33, 169–179.
de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., Maurer, D., and Perrett, D. (2001). Recognition of individual faces and average face prototypes by 1- and 3-month-old infants. Cogn. Dev. 16, 659–678.
de Haan, M., and Nelson, C. A. (1999). Brain activity differentiates face and object processing in 6-month-old infants. Dev. Psychol. 35, 1113–1121.
de Haan, M., Pascalis, O., and Johnson, M. H. (2002). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 199–209.
DeBoer, T., Scott, L. S., and Nelson, C. A. (2007). Methods for acquiring and analyzing infant event-related potentials. In Infant EEG and Event-Related Potentials. M. de Haan, ed. (New York, Psychology Press), pp. 5–37.
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., and Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 17245–17250.
Galper, R. E. (1970). Recognition of faces in photographic negative. Psychon. Sci. 19, 207–208.
Gauthier, I., and Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a ‘‘Greeble’’ expert: exploring the face recognition mechanism. Vision Res. 37, 1673–1682.
Halit, H., Csibra, G., Volein, A., and Johnson, M. H. (2004). Face-sensitive cortical processing in early infancy. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1228–1234.
Halit, H., de Haan, M., and Johnson, M. H. (2003). Cortical specialisation for face processing: face-sensitive event-related potential components in 3- and 12-month-old infants. Neuroimage 19, 1180–1193.
Husk, J. S., Bennett, P. J., and Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Inverting houses and textures: investigating the characteristics of learned inversion effects. Vision Res. 47, 3350–3359.
Itier, R. J., and Taylor, M. J. (2002). Inversion and contrast polarity reversal affect both encoding and recognition processes of unfamiliar faces: a repetition study using ERPs. Neuroimage 15, 353–372.
Kaufmann, J. M., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2004). Expression influences the recognition of familiar faces. Perception 33, 399–408.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge., L., and Pascalis, O. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy: evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1084–1089.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., Ge, L., and Pascalis, O. (2005). Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces. Dev. Sci. 8, F31–F36.
Kemp, R., McManus, C., and Pigott, T. (1990). Sensitivity to the displacement of facial features in negative and inverted images. Perception 19, 531–543.
Macchi Cassia, V., Kuefner, D., Westerlund, A., and Nelson, C. A. (2006). A behavioural and ERP investigation of 3-month-olds’ face preferences. Neuropsychologia 44, 2113–2125.
Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development and neural bases of face recognition. Infant Child Dev. 10, 3–18.
Otsuka, Y., Nakato, E., Kanazawa, S., Yamaguchi, M. K., Watanabe, S., and Kakigi, R. (2007). Neural activation to upright and inverted faces in infants measured by near infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 34, 399–406.
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., and Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life. Science 296, 1321–1323.
Pascalis, O., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., and Fabre-Grent, M. (1995). Mother’s face recognition by neonates: a replication and an extension. Infant Behav. Dev. 18, 79–95.
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., and Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: a preference for female. Perception 31, 1109–1121.
Rhodes, G., Brake, S., and Atkinson, A. P. (1993). What’s lost in inverted faces? Cognition 47, 25–57.
Rossion, B. (2002). Is sex categorisation from faces really parallel to face recognition? Vis. cogn. 9, 1003–1020.
Rossion, B., Delvenne, J. F., Debatisse, D., Goffaux, V., Bruyer, R., Crommelinck, M., and Guerit, J. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal localization of the face inversion effect: an event-related potentials study. Biol. Psychol. 50, 173–189.
Scott, L., and Nelson, C. A. (2004). The developmental neurobiology of face processing. In Review of Psychiatry Series, Vol. 23, B. J. Casey, ed. (Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Publishing), pp. 29–68.
Scott, L. S., Pascalis, O., and Nelson, C. A. (2007). A domain-general theory of the development of perceptual discrimination. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 197–201.
Sugita, Y. (2008). Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 394–398.
Tanaka, J. W., and Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2, 225–245.
Turati, C., Sangrigoli, S., Ruel, J., and de Schonen, S. (2004). Evidence of the face inversion effect in 4-month-old infants. Infancy 6, 275–297.
Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 161–204.
Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. J. Exp. Psychol. 81, 141–145.
Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., and Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception 16, 747–759.
Keywords:
face recognition, familiarity, ERPs, perceptual learning
Citation:
Balas BJ, Nelson III CA, Westerlund A, Vogel-Farley V, Riggins T and Kuefner D (2010). Personal familiarity influences the processing of upright and inverted faces in infants. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.001.2010
Received:
14 July 2009;
 Paper pending published:
08 September 2009;
Accepted:
07 January 2010;
 Published online:
22 February 2010.

Edited by:

Silvia A. Bunge, University of California at Berkeley, USA

Reviewed by:

Leslie J. Carver, University of California, USA
Kalanit Grill-Spector, Stanford University, USA
Copyright:
© 2010 Balas, Nelson III, Westerlund, Vogel-Farley, Riggins and Kuefner. This is an open-access article subject to an exclusive license agreement between the authors and the Frontiers Research Foundation, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original authors and source are credited.
*Correspondence:
Charles A. Nelson III, Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Children’s Hospital Boston, 1 Autumn St., AU601, Boston, MA 02115, USA. e-mail: charles.nelson@childrens.harvard.edu
Download