Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 20 January 2014
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00939

Distinct neural systems underlying reduced emotional enhancement for positive and negative stimuli in early Alzheimer's disease

  • 1Memory Clinic, University Center for Medicine of Aging Basel, Felix Platter Hospital, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 3University Center for Medicine of Aging Basel, Felix Platter Hospital, Basel, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Experimental Psychology, Centre for Speech, Language and the Brain, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 6Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany

Emotional information is typically better remembered than neutral content, and previous studies suggest that this effect is subserved particularly by the amygdala together with its interactions with the hippocampus. However, it is not known whether amygdala damage affects emotional memory performance at immediate and delayed recall, and whether its involvement is modulated by stimulus valence. Moreover, it is unclear to what extent more distributed neocortical regions involved in e.g., autobiographical memory, also contribute to emotional processing. We investigated these questions in a group of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), which affects the amygdala, hippocampus and neocortical regions. Healthy controls (n = 14), patients with AD (n = 15) and its putative prodrome amnestic mild cognitive impairment (n = 11) completed a memory task consisting of immediate and delayed free recall of a list of positive, negative and neutral words. Memory performance was related to brain integrity in region of interest and whole-brain voxel-based morphometry analyses. In the brain-behavioral analyses, the left amygdala volume predicted the immediate recall of both positive and negative material, whereas at delay, left and right amygdala volumes were associated with performance with positive and negative words, respectively. Whole-brain analyses revealed additional associations between left angular gyrus integrity and the immediate recall of positive words as well as between the orbitofrontal cortex and the delayed recall of negative words. These results indicate that emotional memory impairments in AD may be underpinned by damage to regions implicated in emotional processing as well as frontoparietal regions, which may exert their influence via autobiographical memories and organizational strategies.

Introduction

Events colored with positive or negative associations are better remembered than those that do not carry any emotional significance (Cahill and McGaugh, 1998; Kensinger and Corkin, 2003; Phelps, 2006). This emotional enhancement effect (Kensinger, 2006) has been suggested to be primarily driven by amygdala-modulated consolidation processes in the medial temporal lobe (Kensinger et al., 2002; Kensinger and Corkin, 2004; Ritchey et al., 2008). However, it is not known if the amygdala is also necessary for the short-term, i.e., immediate recall of emotional information, and whether its involvement in emotional memory is modulated by stimulus valence. Moreover, it is unclear to what extent damage to more distributed brain regions, which have been implicated in emotional processing (Dolcos et al., 2004a; Kensinger and Corkin, 2004; Smith et al., 2004; Kumfor et al., 2013) and autobiographical memory (Fink et al., 1996; Piefke et al., 2003) such as frontal lobe regions including the orbitofrontal cortex, are associated with emotional memory impairments at immediate and delayed recall. In the present study, we therefore examined whether amygdala volume is significantly related to immediate and delayed episodic memory performance with positive and negative emotional stimuli. We additionally performed whole-brain voxel-based analyses to determine the entire set of brain regions associated with emotional memory performance with positive and negative valences. We studied these questions in the context of Alzheimer's disease (AD) dementia, which is associated with neurofibrillary pathology in the amygdala and neocortical regions (Braak and Braak, 1991), as well as emotional memory impairments (Kensinger, 2006).

The importance of the amygdala for emotional long-term memory is well-established (Cahill and McGaugh, 1998; McGaugh, 2000; Phelps, 2006; Ritchey et al., 2008), and this region, together with its interactions with the hippocampus (LeDoux, 1993; Dolcos et al., 2004b; Ritchey et al., 2008), is claimed to enhance consolidation and strengthen emotional memory traces over time (McGaugh, 2000; Phelps, 2004). The relevance of the amygdala for the consolidation of emotionally arousing material is supported by classical aversive conditioning studies (Davis, 1992; LeDoux, 1993), functional imaging studies in humans (Hamann et al., 1999; Dolcos et al., 2004b; Ritchey et al., 2008) and clinical data (Cahill et al., 1995; Adolphs et al., 1997), where patients with amygdala damage fail to profit from stimulus emotionality at delayed recall (but see also Phelps et al., 1997).

In addition to these time-dependent effects, the amygdala purportedly facilitates the on-line processing of emotional material by enhancing and directing perception and attention toward emotional stimuli (Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Phelps et al., 2006; De Martino et al., 2009; Jacobs et al., 2012). This attentional facilitation for emotionally salient stimuli putatively increases the probability of these stimuli being stored (LaBar and Cabeza, 2006) and may partly account for the beneficial effects of emotionality on memory performance. Thus, this claim leads to the prediction that amygdala damage impairs not only the delayed, but also the immediate recall of emotional material. This prediction has to our knowledge not been explicitly tested and it is not known if the possible involvement of the amygdala at immediate recall is modulated by stimulus valence.

Imaging studies provide evidence for the involvement of the amygdala in the emotional enhancement of both positive and negative stimuli (Hamann et al., 1999; Garavan et al., 2001; Hamann and Mao, 2002). According to the hemispheric lateralization hypothesis, the left and right hemispheres process positive and negative material, respectively (Sackeim et al., 1982; Silberman and Weingartner, 1986). However, few studies supporting the hemispheric lateralization hypothesis directly compared psycholinguistically matched sets of positive and negative stimuli, such that it remains unclear whether or not such a lateralization exists in the amygdala. Some functional imaging studies which directly compared positive and negative material found the predicted lateralized pattern of amygdala activation (Canli et al., 1998; Zalla et al., 2000), while others reported bilateral (Garavan et al., 2001; Yang et al., 2002) or left-lateralized amygdala activation for both valences (Schneider et al., 1997; Hamann and Mao, 2002). Consistent with this latter finding, the majority of studies examining emotional processing (Morris et al., 1998; Canli et al., 2000; Zalla et al., 2000) and long-term memory (LaBar and Phelps, 1998; Adolphs et al., 2000; Buchanan et al., 2001; Frank and Tomaz, 2003) for either negative or positive material found primarily left-sided amygdala involvement for both valences (for an overview see Wager et al., 2003; Zald, 2003; Beraha et al., 2012). Thus, these results suggest that the right amygdala may play a weaker role in emotional memory enhancement compared to the left amygdala (LaBar and Phelps, 1998; Adolphs et al., 2000; Buchanan et al., 2001; Meletti et al., 2003, 2009) and that the processing of negative stimuli may be subserved by the bilateral amygdalae (Liberzon et al., 2000; Hamann et al., 2002; Ritchey et al., 2008; Baeken et al., 2009).

The emotional enhancement effect may also be driven by neuroanatomical systems outside medial temporal lobe structures, such as the prefrontal cortex including the orbitofrontal cortex (Kensinger and Corkin, 2004; Murty et al., 2010; Kumfor et al., 2013) and other neocortical regions (Fink et al., 1996; Piefke et al., 2003; Holland and Kensinger, 2010). Emotional items are thought to benefit from e.g., controlled (i.e., self-initiated and conscious) processes and encoding strategies such as rehearsal and elaborative semantic processing putatively supported by the prefrontal cortex (Kensinger, 2004; Kensinger and Corkin, 2004; Mather, 2007). Moreover, memory for emotional material may evoke recollections of personal past events by their close relationship to one's own autobiographical history. Thereby, emotional material may be more readily integrated with context-rich autobiographical memories than neutral material, since the former are particularly personally relevant to each individual (Holland and Kensinger, 2010). Autobiographical memory processing is claimed to engage a distributed network of brain regions including prefrontal and parietal regions and the cingulate cortex in addition to the medial temporal lobe (Fink et al., 1996; Piefke et al., 2003; Denkova et al., 2006; Steinvorth et al., 2006). Thus, regions implicated in self-controlled, organizational and autobiographical processes may be important for the emotional enhancement effect, and damage to these regions may indirectly impair emotional memory processing.

We examined these questions in the context of AD which is associated with neurofibrillary pathology in the medial temporal lobe, including the amygdala and hippocampus, and neocortical regions including the prefrontal cortex (Braak and Braak, 1991; Riley et al., 2002; Thompson et al., 2003) as well as with emotional memory impairments (Abrisqueta-Gomez et al., 2002; Kensinger et al., 2002, 2004; Borg et al., 2011). Specifically, we tested whether atrophy of the left and right amygdalae affects both immediate and delayed recall of emotional material, and whether valence modulates these relationships. Based on previous research, we predicted that performance with positive material is related to left amygdala volume, whereas negative material is related to left and right amygdala volumes, both at immediate and delayed recall. We directly tested these hypotheses using anatomical region of interest (ROI) analyses. To examine the entire set of brain regions associated with immediate and delayed recall of emotional material, we additionally related performance with positive and negative stimuli with the integrity of the whole brain and explored all regions that were uniquely associated with emotional processing of one valence (positive or negative) over and above that of the opposing valence and performance with neutral words. These results would contribute to a better understanding of the interactions between emotion and episodic memory and provide insights into the neuroanatomical correlates of emotional learning and memory processing in AD.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data from 40 German-speaking participants were included in this study [mean age = 73.05 years, standard deviation (SD) = 6.67 years; mean education = 11.75 years, SD = 2.81 years; 20 women, 20 men; mean Mini Mental Examination score (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) = 27.7, SD = 2.34]. Fourteen were neurologically and cognitively healthy individuals [normal control participants (NC)] selected from longitudinal studies on aging and dementia at the Memory Clinic Basel. All NC participants had undergone a comprehensive medical and neuropsychological examination to ensure their cognitive health. Eleven participants fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) according to Winblad et al. (2004). Fifteen participants were diagnosed with AD according to the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria (McKhann et al., 1984). The three groups were comparable with respect to educational attainment and gender distribution, but differed significantly with respect to the MMSE score, as expected (see Table 1). Since group differences approached significance with respect to mean age, age was covaried in all statistical analyses (see Table 1). This study was approved by the ethics committee of both Basels and informed consent was obtained from each participant. A summary of participants' overall neuropsychological test performance is provided in the Supplementary Methods, Table S1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and MMSE scores of the NC, aMCI, and AD groups [mean (standard deviation)].

Emotional Memory Task

Stimuli

Fifteen word stimuli were selected from a pool of 180 German nouns which had been rated by an independent sample of 45 student participants on the dimensions of valence and arousal (for details see Kissler et al., 2007). We selected a total of 15 words, as longer word lists would have overly taxed the memory impaired patients and as this length is comparable to other verbal memory tasks used in clinical neuropsychological assessment. One third of the words had a positive (n = 5, e.g., kiss), one third a negative (n = 5, e.g., pain), and one third a neutral valence (n = 5, e.g., pencil). The three valence categories differed significantly with respect to their mean valence ratings (positive > neutral > negative, all p-values < 0.001), while the mean arousal ratings of the positive and negative valence items were comparable [t(8) = −1.84, p = 0.10] and differed significantly from neutral words [t(8) = 8.10, p < 0.001; t(8) = 12.63, p < 0.001, respectively]. The words were matched such that they did not differ with respect to concreteness, word length, number of phonemes or written word frequency (CELEX; Baayen et al., 1995). The means, standard deviations and comparisons of the psycholinguistic variables across emotional valence categories are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Emotional and psycholinguistic characteristics of the stimuli [mean (standard deviation)] and the results of an analysis of variance testing for differences between valence group means.

Procedure

In the emotional memory task, the 15 words were presented to each participant in black text on white cards, one card every 2 s, in the same, pseudorandomized order. To protect the emotional words (positive and negative) from primacy and recency effects (Foldi et al., 2003), the word list started and ended with a neutral item. The participants were instructed to read each word out loud and to try to memorize it. Immediately after presenting all words of each trial, the participants were asked to recall as many of the 15 words as possible from memory. There were three learning trials in total. After a delay interval of approximately 20 min (mean = 20.44, SD = 4.90), during which participants performed non-verbal tasks, participants were instructed to again recall as many words as possible from memory (delayed recall). Verbal responses were scored as correct or incorrect. The present analyses used the total number of correctly recalled words at immediate recall (i.e., total number of correctly recalled words across the three learning trials) and delayed recall for each valence category separately. One participant (NC) did not complete the delayed recall task and was therefore excluded from the analyses of delayed recall performance. Thus, for all analyses on delayed recall the n was 39.

MRI Measures

MRI image acquisition

All participants received a high-resolution, T1-weighted three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) anatomical imaging scan (TI = 1000 ms, TR = 2150 ms, TE = 3.5 ms, flip angle = 7°, rectangular field of view = 87.5%, acquisition matrix = 256 × 224 mm, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm). All scans were obtained from the same 3 Tesla MRI scanner (MAGNETOM Allegra, Siemens at the University Hospital Basel). MRI scanning was conducted within 3 months of behavioral testing (mean interval = 2.4 months, SD = 2.4 months).

Analyses

Statistical analyses of behavioral data

Group differences on behavioral indices from the emotional memory task were tested with a 3 (group) × 3 (valence) repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare the effects of valence and diagnostic group at (a) immediate and (b) delayed recall performance. Valence was treated as a within-subjects and diagnosis as a between-groups factor, and age was included in the model as a covariate. The first and last item on the list, which were used to buffer emotional words from primacy and recency effects, were excluded from the behavioral analyses (thus, n = three neutral words). The equality of variances at different levels of the repeated factor was tested using Mauchly's test. Post-hoc analyses comparing diagnostic groups were conducted using independent samples' t-tests. The t-tests were Bonferroni-corrected for the number of comparisons conducted {αcorrected = 0.05/[3(groups/valence) × 2(time points)] = 0.008}. If Levene's test for equality of variances was violated (p = 0.05), a t-statistic was conducted which did not assume homogeneity of variances. All statistical analyses of behavioral data were performed with SPSS version 21 (SPSS Inc. IBM company, 2012).

ROI analyses

We concentrated a priori on the left and right amygdalae as ROIs. Volumes of these structures were acquired with an automatic subcortical segmentation procedure (Fischl et al., 2002) in FreeSurfer v5.0 (Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). This procedure uses signal intensities and an a priori probabilistic atlas as well as information from neighboring voxels to establish an accurate subcortical segmentation (Fischl et al., 2002). This segmentation method has been shown to perform comparably to manual segmentations (Fischl et al., 2002) and has been used in previous studies in AD (Desikan et al., 2006, 2009; Morey et al., 2009). One-way ANOVAs were used to test for differences in the mean volumes of the left and right amygdalae and hippocampi in the three diagnostic groups using age as a covariate. Post-hoc comparisons were performed using one-tailed independent samples' t-tests.

We tested the eight predictions described in the introduction [i.e., immediate and delayed recall (2) × positive and negative stimuli (2) × left and right amygdalae (2)] using linear regression analyses. Whole brain total gray matter volume and age were included as covariates in the first step to control for the combined effect of head size and overall cortical atrophy and a potential confounding effect of age, respectively. As the hippocampus has a well-established role in episodic memory (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997; Aggleton and Brown, 1999) and is assumed to interact with the amygdala to give rise to the emotional enhancement effect, we report analogous regression analyses for left and right hippocampal volumes. We did not include the hippocampus and amygdala in the same regression analyses to avoid multicollinearity, as both structures are comparably affected in MCI and in mild AD (Poulin et al., 2011).

The standard alpha-level (α = 0.05) was Bonferroni-corrected for the number of the a priori ROIs (n = 2; left and right amygdalae) and the number of time points (immediate and delayed recall; n = 2). Thus, the adjusted conservative alpha-level was set at 0.01 {αcorrected = 0.05/[2(ROIs) × 2(time points)] = 0.01}. For completeness, the analogous linear regression models with the left and right amygdalae and hippocampi as predictors for the performance with all neutral word stimuli are provided in the supplementary results section (see Supplementary Results, Table S2). For simplicity, we used the same statistical threshold as in these analyses above. To maximize variability in brain integrity, we collapsed the diagnostic groups in all brain-behavioral analyses. Statistical analyses with ROI data were performed with SPSS version 21 (SPSS Inc. IBM company, 2012).

Whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analyses

Preprocessing of MPRAGE images was performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8, Wellcome Institute of Cognitive Neurology, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk) in Matlab 2010 (Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, MA; USA). The MRI images were first segmented into gray matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid volumes. Gray matter volume misclassifications around the medial temporal lobe were then manually identified by one of the authors (SLK) and the MPRAGEs were segmented again while masking the misclassifications (see Kivisaari et al., 2012 for details). A study-specific template was created with DARTEL (Ashburner, 2007) using the re-segmented gray matter images. The gray matter images were then normalized to the DARTEL template and MNI space, modulated and smoothed with 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. The subsequent whole brain analyses were conducted using the general linear model in SPM8. To determine the relationship between immediate recall and delayed recall memory performance in each valence category and regional brain volume, participants' preprocessed gray matter brain volumes were subjected to two regression analyses using total scores for positive and negative words, at (a) immediate recall and (b) delayed recall. As in the ROI analyses, the groups were collapsed for the VBM analyses. Performance with each valence category (positive and negative words separately) at a single time point was correlated with whole-brain signal intensities while controlling for the performance with the neutral and opposing emotional valence. Age, total gray matter volume (http://www.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/G.Ridgway/vbm/get_totals65.m) and performance with neutral words and the opposing valence were included as covariates to account for possible age-related effects, the combined effect of head size and global atrophy, and “baseline” episodic memory performance, respectively. Statistical parametric maps were thresholded at p < 0.01 (uncorrected) in all analyses at the voxel-level. Coordinates of peak voxels in clusters surviving a FWE-corrected p < 0.05 adjusted for the entire brain are reported in MNI space. Anatomical areas were determined using the AAL atlas in SPM 8 (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002).

Results

Behavioral Results

The mean proportions correct for each group and valence category at immediate and delayed recall are reported in Table 3. To compare NC, aMCI, and AD participants' memory performance in each valence category, repeated measures ANOVAs were performed: one for immediate recall and another for the delayed recall. Mauchly's test indicated that the variances at different levels of the repeated factor were spherical at immediate [χ2(2) = 4.97, p = 0.08] and delayed recall [χ2(2) = 4.96, p = 0.08]. Thus, uncorrected ANOVA results are reported. At immediate recall, there was a significant main effect of diagnostic group [F(2, 36) = 19.57, p < 0.001], but no main effect of valence [F(2, 72) = 0.24, p = 0.79]. The main effect of diagnostic group reflected better performance of NC participants relative to aMCI participants [t(58.56) = 3.64, p < 0.008] and AD participants [t(85) = 8.37, p < 0.008], as well as better performance of aMCI participants relative to AD participants [t(76) = 3.36, p < 0.008]. There was no significant interaction between valence and diagnostic group at immediate recall [F(4, 72) = 0.08, p = 0.99]. Age significantly predicted immediate recall performance [F(1, 36) = 9.20, p = 0.004], such that increasing age was associated with poorer performance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean proportions correct at immediate and delayed recall for each valence category [mean (standard deviation)] and for each diagnostic group.

At delayed recall, we found a significant main effect of diagnostic group [F(2, 35) = 14.81, p < 0.001], as expected, but not of valence [F(2, 70) = 0.12, p = 0.89]. The group effect reflected better performance of NC participants relative to aMCI participants [t(72) = 3.14, p < 0.008] and AD participants [t(59.43) = 8.10, p < 0.008], and better performance of aMCI participants relative to AD participants [t(43.11) = 3.20, p < 0.008]. The interaction between valence and diagnostic group approached significance [F(4, 70) = 2.20, p = 0.08] reflecting a weakened emotional enhancement effect in the AD as compared to the other diagnostic groups. There was no significant effect of age at delayed recall [F(1, 35) = 1.67, p = 0.21].

ROI Results

One-way ANOVAs revealed that the mean volumes of left and right amygdalae and hippocampi differed significantly over the groups: left amygdala [F(2, 36) = 10.79, p < 0.001; NC > aMCI, NC > AD, both p-values < 0.007, aMCI = AD]; right amygdala [F(2, 36) = 8.72, p = 0.001; NC > aMCI > AD, all p-values < 0.007]; left hippocampus [F(2, 36) = 18.66, p < 0.001; NC > aMCI > AD; all p-values < 0.003]; right hippocampus [F(2, 36) = 14.69, p < 0.001; NC > aMCI > AD, all p-values < 0.02]. Regression models were conducted in which the volume of each ROI was used to predict immediate and delayed recall performance with the positive and negative valence groups to determine whether the integrity of the amygdalae and hippocampi were related to the immediate and delayed recall of emotional information (Table 4). These analyses yielded a significant association between left amygdala volume and immediate recall performance of positive and negative words. Immediate recall of positive but not negative words was also significantly predicted by the left hippocampus. Left amygdala volume and bilateral hippocampal integrity significantly predicted the delayed recall of positive words, whereas only right amygdala volume was significantly associated with the delayed recall of negative words.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of linear regression analyses where gray matter volume in left and right amygdalae and hippocampi predicted immediate and delayed recall performance for positive and negative stimuli.

These results suggest that the integrity of the left amygdala is significantly associated with performance with positive and negative words at immediate recall, whereas the integrity of the right amygdala is not associated with immediate recall performance with either valence. At delay we found a lateralized pattern with the delayed recall performance with positive words associated with left and delayed recall performance with negative words with right amygdala volume. Left and right hippocampus volumes were significantly associated only with the delayed recall with positive, but not negative words. To confirm the robustness of these analyses and to ensure the analyses did not suffer e.g., from floor or ceiling effects, we provide scatterplots representing the relationship between the recalled words at immediate and delayed recall and the volume of left and right amygdalae for significant ROI results (see Supplementary Results, Figure S1).

Whole-Brain VBM Results

Immediate recall by valence

We conducted a whole-brain VBM analysis to examine the entire network of brain regions associated with the immediate recall of words from different valence categories. We tested for brain regions where immediate recall performance with positive and negative items significantly correlated with regional brain volume while controlling for the effects of the opposing valence and neutral words. Immediate recall performance with positive words was associated with one significant cluster centered in the left angular gyrus (−44, −69, 37), which extended into the middle temporal gyrus (Figure 1). There were no significant clusters for the contrast testing for brain regions where reduced volume was associated with poorer immediate recall performance with negative words.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neuroanatomical regions where poorer performance at (A) immediate recall and (B) delayed recall was associated with decreased volume by valence. There were no significant results for negative words at immediate recall. Results are thresholded at p < 0.01. MNI coordinates are reported and L = L.

Delayed recall by valence

Next, we examined brain regions where delayed recall of positive and negative items was associated with reduced gray matter integrity while controlling for the effects of the remaining valences. Poorer delayed recall of positive words was significantly associated with reduced gray matter volume in one cluster centered in the left hippocampus (−18, −35, −4) extending into the amygdala, perirhinal, entorhinal and parahippocampal cortices (Kivisaari et al., 2013) and the lingual gyrus (Figure 1). Impaired delayed recall of negative words resulted in one significant cluster in the bilateral medial orbitofrontal areas (−31, 62, −15), extending bilaterally into the ventrolateral and -medial prefrontal areas (Figure 1).

The present sample size (n = 40 at immediate recall; n = 39 at delayed recall) was relatively small compared to conventional VBM studies (cf. Pell et al., 2008), potentially rendering the results susceptible to violations of statistical assumptions, i.e., heteroscedasticity or non-normality of the residuals (Lumley et al., 2002). To test the robustness of these results, we extracted the mean signal intensities for each significant cluster in each VBM model (one cluster centered in the angular gyrus at immediate recall, two clusters at delayed recall centered in the left hippocampus and in the orbitofrontal area). We then replicated each VBM regression analysis in SPSS using the respective mean signal intensities as the dependent variables and examined the residuals of these models. Shapiro-Wilk tests indicated that the standardized residuals were normally distributed [at immediate recall: D(40) = 0.97, p > 0.3; both clusters at delayed recall: D(39) > 0.9, all p > 0.4]. Moreover, an examination of the standardized residuals plotted against the standardized predicted values confirmed that these data were homoscedastic. Thus, these analyses support the validity of the VBM multiple regression approach despite the relatively small sample size.

Discussion

Left amygdala integrity was significantly associated with immediate recall performance with both valences, but a lateralized pattern emerged at delayed recall, such that left and right amygdala were associated with performance with positive and negative words, respectively. The whole-brain analyses testing for regions uniquely associated with the immediate or delayed recall performance with either positive or negative valence revealed additional associations between the left angular gyrus volume and performance with positive words at immediate recall, as well as between the orbitofrontal, bilateral ventrolateral and -medial prefrontal cortex and performance with negative words at delayed recall. Thus, these analyses revealed a network of regions supporting emotional immediate and delayed memory performance and contribute to a better understanding of the neural underpinnings of emotional memory impairments in conditions such as AD that affect the functioning of this system.

At the behavioral level, the groups did not differ in their ability to benefit from stimulus emotionality. This finding is at odds with behavioral studies which found weakened emotional memory effects in AD patients (Ikeda et al., 1998; Moayeri et al., 2000; Boller et al., 2002; Nieuwenhuis-Mark et al., 2009; Nashiro et al., 2011; Perrin et al., 2012; for an overview see Broster et al., 2012; Klein-Koerkamp et al., 2012). It should be noted, however, that the interaction between valence and diagnosis approached significance at delay, reflecting a weakened emotional enhancement of positive vs. neutral items in the AD group as compared to the two other diagnostic groups. The lack of significant group differences in the behavioral analyses may partially be explained by the relatively short time interval between immediate and delayed recall (ca. 20 min), which may have attenuated the differences in emotional enhancement between groups (cf. McGaugh, 2000).

In the ROI regression analyses, the volumes of the left and right amygdalae as well as those of the hippocampi were associated with emotional memory performance at delay. These results are consistent with the well-established finding that the amygdala is critical for the consolidation of emotional material together with its interactions with the hippocampus (Cahill and McGaugh, 1998; McGaugh, 2000; Dolcos et al., 2005). Importantly, also the immediate recall of positive words significantly correlated with left amygdala volume. This result supports the claim that the amygdala enhances emotional memory performance already at immediate recall. The amygdala is thought to serve a facilitatory role at encoding by influencing the direction of attentional focus and increasing the likelihood of perceiving and attending to emotionally salient stimuli, resulting in enhanced memory for these items (Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Phelps et al., 2006; De Martino et al., 2009; Jacobs et al., 2012; but see also Piech et al., 2011; Edmiston et al., 2013). Thus, by facilitating the perception and attention of emotional stimuli, the amygdala promotes hippocampal consolidation of these items (Richardson et al., 2004). The association between the volumes of the left and right amygdalae and the immediate recall of neutral words (see Supplementary Results, Table S2) suggests that this region may also facilitate the learning of neutral material, possibly by embedding it in an emotional context (cf. Maratos et al., 2001). Taken together, these findings from the brain-behavioral analyses suggest that the amygdala, together with its interactions with the hippocampus, may underpin the emotional memory impairments in part by facilitating the on-line processing of emotional material (Phelps, 2004; Richardson et al., 2004).

In line with the hemispheric lateralization hypothesis, using a matched set of positive and negative stimuli, we found that the left and right amygdalae were involved in emotional enhancement at delay (cf. Hamann and Mao, 2002). Specifically, the left amygdala significantly predicted delayed recall of positive but not negative items, whereas right amygdala damage was associated with delayed recall of negative but not positive items. This finding is in line with functional imaging studies (Cahill et al., 1996; Canli et al., 1998; Zalla et al., 2000; Tabert et al., 2001; Kensinger and Schacter, 2006) and patient studies (LaBar and Phelps, 1998; Meletti et al., 2003, 2009) and suggests that stimulus valence may indeed modulate the laterality of amygdala involvement. Interestingly, however, we found that performance with both positive and negative words at immediate recall was associated with the left amygdala volume. This result corroborates previous findings suggesting that the left amygdala facilitates the on-line processing of especially verbal emotional stimuli (Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Hamann and Mao, 2002) and may help to explain why the left amygdala is more often associated with the processing of both valences compared to the right amygdala in the previous literature on emotional long-term memory processing (cf. Adolphs et al., 2000; Buchanan et al., 2001; Frank and Tomaz, 2003). That is, while the left amygdala may support the encoding of both positive and negative items as well as the consolidation of positive material, the right amygdala may primarily support the consolidation of negative material. In other words, processing of negative material may be supported by bilateral amygdala as suggested by others (cf. Liberzon et al., 2000; Hamann et al., 2002; Baeken et al., 2009), with the left and right amygdala possibly recruited at different stages of memory processing.

It should be noted that we cannot dissociate the effect of the amygdala shrinkage from that of the hippocampus by linear regression analyses, since both regions are comparably affected in AD (Poulin et al., 2011) and contribute to episodic memory processing. However, a complete dissociation of the two structures in emotional memory processing would not necessarily be expected in a patient study, since the emotional memory enhancement is claimed to be underpinned by the robust interactions between the two regions (Cahill and McGaugh, 1998; Dolcos et al., 2004b; Ritchey et al., 2008). Thus, the present findings support the idea that the amygdala, together with the hippocampal formation, underpins the emotional enhancement effect during episodic memory and processing.

The whole-brain analyses indicated that performance with negative words at delay was significantly associated with the integrity of the bilateral prefrontal cortex, including its ventrolateral and -medial aspects and the orbitofrontal cortex (see also Kumfor et al., 2013). These regions may serve to strengthen the consolidation of such information via controlled processes such as elaboration, focused attention, and associative processing of emotional information (Craik and Lockhart, 1972; Bechara et al., 2000a; Kensinger, 2004). For example, patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex, including the orbitofrontal and ventromedial cortices, have been shown to have deficits in regulating their behavior with respect to expected negative consequences (Bechara et al., 1994, 2000b) and exhibit an attenuated emotional response, such as regret, to unrewarding decisions in a gambling task with a monetary reward (Camille et al., 2004). Thus, the importance of the ventromedial and -lateral and orbitofrontal prefrontal regions for the processing of negative emotions may explain why damage to this region would particularly impair the memory for unpleasant material.

The whole-brain analyses revealed an additional association between the immediate recall of positive words and volume of the left angular gyrus in the parieto-temporal junction. This region has been linked with semantic processing and working memory (Price, 2000; Devereux et al., 2013) and has been shown to be activated during encoding and recognition of emotional (positive and negative) pictures (Smith et al., 2004) and during the viewing of happy facial expressions (Habel et al., 2005). Therefore, this region may support the learning of positive words by means of deeper elaboration and integration of the information into a broader semantic network. The results presented here extend the previous literature by suggesting that this region may also be necessary for such processing and for the immediate recall of information that carries positive content.

The involvement of prefrontal structures and the angular gyrus in emotional memory may reflect the importance of these areas in autobiographical processing. Autobiographical memory, in contrast to classical episodic memory tasks, is rich in emotional content (Cabeza and Jacques, 2007; Holland and Kensinger, 2010) and, therefore, may enhance emotional processing by e.g., providing contextual links with high personal relevance. Specifically, the ventral prefrontal region has been implicated in e.g., integrating new stimuli into a specific context of already existing information of past events and binding different aspects of an episode together (Thompson-Schill et al., 1997; Fernández and Tendolkar, 2001), and processing and re-experiencing emotions that were present during learning (Gilboa, 2004; Daselaar et al., 2008). The angular gyrus has been suggested to support autobiographical processing by contributing to the richness and vividness of remembered detailed information of personal events (Berryhill et al., 2007). Thus, damage to these systems associated with autobiographical processing may impair emotional memory by hindering the putative privileged access of emotional information into existing semantic and episodic information.

These brain-behavioral effects reported above cannot be accounted for by overall disease severity as we controlled for the confounding effect of total gray matter volume. Pathology in AD typically affects the amygdala and hippocampus earlier than it affects the frontal cortices (Braak and Braak, 1991). However, a proportion of clinically diagnosed aMCI patients may in fact be in an advanced stage of AD neurofibrillary pathology (Petersen et al., 2006) with tangle deposition in the frontal regions (Braak stages III-V; Braak and Braak, 1991). Therefore, it is possible that subtle emotional memory impairments due to pathology in the frontal cortices are apparent even in clinically early stages of AD such as the patients in this sample. Finally, it should be noted that since we combined the three diagnostic groups for both the ROI regression and whole-brain analyses, the results demonstrate a general brain-behavioral relationship across a broad spectrum of variability, from healthy brain tissue to different stages of disease pathology. We suggest that emotional memory impairments can be accounted for by the degree and location of pathology rather than diagnostic status, such that similar patterns of behavioral impairment would be expected in any neuropathological condition that affects these brain networks.

The present results indicate that emotional memory in mature people and in the context of AD depends on the integrity of a network of brain regions. Specifically, we demonstrate that a distributed network of brain regions comprising the amygdala, the hippocampus, the prefrontal cortex and the angular gyrus, is necessary for the processing of emotional items. Moreover, this study provides evidence for the hemispheric lateralization model. However, the present results, taken together with the previous literature, suggest that the lateralization is relative rather than absolute and may vary at different stages of mnemonic processing. This study accounts for a better understanding of neuroanatomical networks involved in emotional memory deficits in AD and suggests that emotional memory may rely on brain regions implicated in emotional processing as well as frontoparietal regions which may support emotional memory via controlled organizational strategies and autobiographical processing.

Author Contributions

Panagiota Mistridis: data analyses, interpretation, drafting of the manuscript, critical reviews. Kirsten I. Taylor: study design, data acquisition, drafting of the manuscript, interpretation, critical reviews. Johanna M. Kissler: interpretation, critical reviews. Andreas U. Monsch: interpretation, critical reviews. Reto W. Kressig: interpretation, critical reviews. Sasa L. Kivisaari: data analyses, interpretation, drafting of the manuscript, critical reviews. The authors agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work and ensure that the work has been conducted accurately. All authors approved the manuscript before submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Swiss National Science Foundation Ambizione fellowship PZ00P1_126493 (Kirsten I. Taylor) and a postdoctoral fellowship for prospective researchers PBBSP1_146854 (Sasa L. Kivisaari), Swiss Foundation for Aging Research financed by the Loterie Romande (Kirsten I. Taylor), the Novartis Foundation, Basel, Switzerland (Andreas U. Monsch), and the Finnish Cultural Foundation (Sasa L. Kivisaari). The MRI scans were financed by grants from the Novartis Foundation and GlaxoSmithKline (Andreas U. Monsch). The funding sources were not involved in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing of the report or decision to submit the article for publication. The authors thank Irene Winkler for her contribution to designing the experiment and also the patients and healthy control participants who took part in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00939/abstract

Table S1. Summary of neuropsychological test performance (presented as age, gender and education adjusted z-scores) of the participants.

Table S2. Results of linear regression analyses where gray matter volume in left and right amygdalae and hippocampi predicted immediate and delayed recall performance for neutral stimuli.

Figure S1. Scatterplots representing the relationship between the recalled words at immediate and delayed recall and the volume of left and right amygdalae.

References

Abrisqueta-Gomez, J., Bueno, O. F. A., Oliveira, M. G. M., and Bertolucci, P. H. F. (2002). Recognition memory for emotional pictures in Alzheimer's patients. Acta Neurol. Scand. 105, 51–54. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.00035.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adolphs, R., Cahill, L., Schul, R., and Babinsky, R. (1997). Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learn. Mem. 4, 291–300. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.3.291

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., and Denburg, N. (2000). Impaired emotional declarative memory following unilateral amygdala damage. Learn. Mem. 7, 180–186. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.3.180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 425–444. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press

Anderson, A. K., and Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature 411, 305–309. doi: 10.1038/35077083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., and Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (Release 2) [CD ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Lingustic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.

Baeken, C., De Raedt, R., Ramsey, N., Van Schuerbeek, P., Hermes, D., Bossuyt, A., et al. (2009). Amygdala responses to positively and negatively valenced baby faces in healthy female volunteers: influences of individual differences in harm avoidance. Brain Res. 1296, 94–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (2000a). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 10, 295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bechara, A., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (2000b). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123, 2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beraha, E., Eggers, J., Attar, C. H., Gutwinski, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Stoy, M., et al. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetry for affective stimulus processing in healthy subjects – a fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e46931. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046931

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R., and Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci. 27, 14415–14423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-07.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boller, F., El Massioui, F., Devouche, E., Traykov, L., Pomati, S., and Starkstein, S. E. (2002). Processing emotional information in Alzheimer's disease: effects on memory performance and neurophysiological correlates. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 14, 104–112. doi: 10.1159/000064932

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borg, C., Leroy, N., Favre, E., Laurent, B., and Thomas-Antérion, C. (2011). How emotional pictures influence visuospatial binding in short-term memory in ageing and Alzheimer's disease? Brain Cogn. 76, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Broster, L. S., Blonder, L. X., and Jiang, Y. (2012). Does emotional memory enhancement assist the memory-impaired? Front. Aging Neurosci. 4:2. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buchanan, T. W., Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., and Adolphs, R. (2001). Verbal and nonverbal emotional memory following unilateral amygdala damage. Learn. Mem. 8, 326–335. doi: 10.1101/lm.40101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cabeza, R., and St. Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cahill, L., Babinsky, R., Markowitsch, H. J., and McGaugh, J. L. (1995). The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature 377, 295–296. doi: 10.1038/377295a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cahill, L., Haier, R. J., Fallon, J., Alkire, M. T., Tang, C., Keator, D., et al. (1996). Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 8016–8021. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.15.8016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cahill, L., and McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 21, 294–299. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01214-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Camille, N., Coricelli, G., Sallet, J., Pradat-Diehl, P., Duhamel, J.-R., and Sirigu, A. (2004). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science 304, 1167–1170. doi: 10.1126/science.1094550

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canli, T., Desmond, J. E., Zhao, Z., Glover, G., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Hemispheric asymmetry for emotional stimuli detected with fMRI. Neuroreport 9, 3233–3239. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Brewer, J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., and Cahill, L. (2000). Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. J. Neurosci. 20:RC99.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Craik, F. I. M., and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 11, 671–684. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X

CrossRef Full Text

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daselaar, S. M., Rice, H. J., Greenberg, D. L., Cabeza, R., LaBar, K. S., and Rubin, D. C. (2008). The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. Cereb. Cortex 18, 217–229. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 353–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Martino, B., Kalisch, R., Aguirre, G. K., and Dolan, R. J. (2009). Enhanced processing of threat stimuli under limited attentional resources. Cereb. Cortex 19, 127–133. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Denkova, E., Botzung, A., Scheiber, C., and Manning, L. (2006). Implicit emotion during recollection of past events: a nonverbal fMRI study. Brain Res. 1078, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.061

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desikan, R. S., Cabral, H. J., Hess, C. P., Dillon, W. P., Glastonbury, C. M., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2009). Automated MRI measures identify individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Brain 132, 2048–2057. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp123

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Devereux, B. J., Clarke, A., Marouchos, A., and Tyler, L. K. (2013). Representational similarity analysis reveals commonalities and differences in the semantic processing of words and objects. J. Neurosci. 33, 18906–18916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3809-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2004a). Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2004b). Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron 42, 855–863. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00289-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one year later: role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 2626–2631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409848102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edmiston, E. K., McHugo, M., Dukic, M. S., Smith, S. D., Abou-Khalil, B., Eggers, E., et al. (2013). Enhanced visual cortical activation for emotional stimuli is preserved in patients with unilateral amygdala resection. J. Neurosci. 33, 11023–11031. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0401-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fernández, G., and Tendolkar, I. (2001). Integrated brain activity in medial temporal and prefrontal areas predicts subsequent memory performance: human declarative memory formation at the system level. Brain Res. Bull. 55, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00494-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M., Bruckbauer, T., Kessler, J., and Heiss, W.-D. (1996). Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci. 16, 4275–4282.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: II. Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foldi, N. S., Brickman, A. M., Schaefer, L. A., and Knutelska, M. E. (2003). Distinct serial position profiles and neuropsychological measures differentiate late life depression from normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Res. 120, 71–84. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00163-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frank, J. E., and Tomaz, C. (2003). Lateralized impairment of the emotional enhancement of verbal memory in patients with amygdala–hippocampus lesion. Brain Cogn. 52, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00075-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garavan, H., Pendergrass, J. C., Ross, T. J., Stein, E. A., and Risinger, A. (2001). Amygdala reponse to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Neuroreport 12, 2779–2783. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-00036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gilboa, A. (2004). Autobiographical and episodic memory - one and the same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 42, 1336–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Habel, U., Klein, M., Kellermann, T., Shah, N. J., and Schneider, F. (2005). Same or different? Neural correlates of happy and sad mood in healthy males. Neuroimage 26, 206–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.014

CrossRef Full Text

Hamann, S., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., and Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 289–293. doi: 10.1038/6404

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamann, S., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., and Kilts, C. D. (2002). Ecstasy and agony: activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative emotion. Psychol. Sci. 13, 135–141. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamann, S., and Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. Neuroreport 13, 15. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holland, A. C., and Kensinger, E. A. (2010). Emotion and autobiographical memory. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 88–131. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.01.006

CrossRef Full Text

Ikeda, M., Mori, E., Hirono, N., Imamura, T., Shimomura, T., Ikejiri, Y., et al. (1998). Amnestic people with Alzheimer's disease who remembered the Kobe earthquake. Br. J. Psychiatry 172, 425–428. doi: 10.1192/bjp.172.5.425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobs, R. H. A. H., Renken, R., Aleman, A., and Cornelissen, F. W. (2012). The amygdala, top-down effects, and selective attention to features. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2069–2084. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: the contribution of valence and arousal. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 241–251. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.4.241

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A. (2006). “Remembering emotional information: effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease,” in Frontiers in Alzheimer's Disease. Research, ed E. M. Welsh (Hauppage, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.), 213–226.

Kensinger, E. A., Anderson, A., Growdon, J. H., and Corkin, S. (2004). Effects of Alzheimer disease on memory for verbal emotional information. Neuropsychologia 42, 791–800. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., and Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer's disease on emotional memory. Emotion 2, 118–134. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.118

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A., and Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Mem. Cogn. 31, 1169–1180. doi: 10.3758/BF03195800

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A., and Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 3310–3315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0306408101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2006). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J. Neurosci. 26, 2564–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Peyk, P., and Junghofer, M. (2007). Buzzwords: early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychol. Sci. 18, 475–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01924.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kivisaari, S. L., Probst, A., and Taylor, K. I. (2013). “The perirhinal, entorhinal and parahippocampal cortices and hippocampus: an overview of functional anatomy and protocol for their segmentation in MR images,” in fMRI - Basics and Clinical Applications, eds S. Ulmer and O. Jansen (Berlin: Springer), 239–267.

Kivisaari, S. L., Tyler, L. K., Monsch, A. U., and Taylor, K. I. (2012). Medial perirhinal cortex disambiguates confusable objects. Brain 135, 3757–3769. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws277

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klein-Koerkamp, Y., Baciu, M., and Hot, P. (2012). Preserved and impaired emotional memory in Alzheimer's disease. Front. Psychol. 3:331. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00331

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kumfor, F., Irish, M., Hodges, J. R., and Piguet, O. (2013). The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in emotional enhancement of memory: evidence from the dementias. Brain 136, 2992–3003. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt185

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

LaBar, K. S., and Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psychol. Sci. 9, 490–493. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00090

CrossRef Full Text

LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behav. Brain Res. 58, 69–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90091-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liberzon, I., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., Decker, L. R., Koeppe, R. A., and Minoshima, S. (2000). Limbic activation and psychophysiologic responses to aversive visual stimuli: interaction with cognitive task. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 508–516. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00157-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lumley, T., Diehr, P., Emerson, S., and Chen, L. (2002). The importance of the normality assumption in large public health data sets. Annu. Rev. Public Health 23, 151–169. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140546

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maratos, E. J., Dolan, R. J., Morris, J. S., Henson, R. N. A., and Rugg, M. D. (2001). Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context. Neuropsychologia 39, 910–920. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00025-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: an object-based framework. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 33–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x

CrossRef Full Text

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory-a century of consolidation. Science 287, 248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., and Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 34, 939–939. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meletti, S., Benuzzi, F., Cantalupo, G., Rubboli, G., Tassinari, C. A., and Nichelli, P. (2009). Facial emotion recognition impairment in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 50, 1547–1559. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01978.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meletti, S., Benuzzi, F., Rubboli, G., Cantalupo, G., Maserati, M., Stanzani, N. P., et al. (2003). Impaired facial emotion recognition in early-onset right mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 60, 426–431. doi: 10.1212/WNL.60.3.426

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moayeri, S. H., Cahill, L., Jin, Y., and Potkin, S. G. (2000). Relative sparing of emotionally influenced memory in Alzheimer's disease. Neuroreport 11, 653–655. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200003200-00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Xu, Y., Pannu Hayes, J., Wagner, II. H. R. Lewis, D. V., et al. (2009). A comparison of automated segmentation and manual tracing for quantifying hippocampal and amygdala volumes. Neuroimage 45, 855–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Büchel, C., Frith, C. D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., et al. (1998). A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain 121, 47–57. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.1.47

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murty, V. P., Ritchey, M., Adcock, R. A., and LaBar, K. S. (2010). fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 48, 3459–3469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nashiro, K., Sakaki, M., and Mather, M. (2011). Age differences in brain activity during emotion processing: reflections of age-related decline or increased emotion regulation? Gerontology 58, 156–163. doi: 10.1159/000328465

CrossRef Full Text

Nieuwenhuis-Mark, R. E., Schalk, K., and de Graaf, N. (2009). Free recall and learning of emotional word lists in very elderly people with and without dementia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 24, 155–162. doi: 10.1177/1533317508330561

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pell, G. S., Briellmann, R. S. S., Chan, C. H., Pardoe, H., Abbott, D. F., and Jackson, G. D. (2008). Selection of the control group for VBM analysis: influence of covariates, matching and sample size. Neuroimage 41, 1324–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.050

CrossRef Full Text

Perrin, M., Henaff, M.-A., Padovan, C., Faillenot, I., Merville, A., and Krolak-Salmon, P. (2012). Influence of emotional content and context on memory in mild Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 29, 817–826. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-111490

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petersen, R. C., Parisi, J. E., Dickson, D. W., Johnson, K. A., Knopman, D. S., Boeve, B. F., et al. (2006). Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 63, 665–672. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.5.665

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 27–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070234

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phelps, E. A., LaBar, K. S., and Spencer, D. D. (1997). Memory for emotional words following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Brain Cogn. 35, 85–109. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1997.0929

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., and Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychol. Sci. 17, 292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piech, R. M., McHugo, M., Smith, S. D., Dukic, M. S., Van Der Meer, J., Abou-Khalil, B., et al. (2011). Attentional capture by emotional stimuli is preserved in patients with amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 49, 3314–3319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piefke, M., Weiss, P. H., Zilles, K., Markowitsch, H. J., and Fink, G. R. (2003). Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. Brain 126, 650–668. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg064

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Poulin, S. P., Dautoff, R., Morris, J. C., Barrett, L. F., and Dickerson, B. C. (2011). Amygdala atrophy is prominent in early Alzheimer's disease and relates to symptom severity. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 194, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Price, C. J. (2000). The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging. J. Anat. 197, 335–359. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19730335.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 278–285. doi: 10.1038/nn1190

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Riley, K. P., Snowdon, D. A., and Markesbery, W. R. (2002). Alzheimer's neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: findings from the nun study. Ann. Neurol. 51, 567–577. doi: 10.1002/ana.10161

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ritchey, M., Dolcos, F., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Role of amygdala connectivity in the persistence of emotional memories over time: an event-related fMRI investigation. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2494–2504. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm262

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sackeim, H. A., Greenberg, M. S., Weiman, A. L., Gur, R. C., Hungerbuhler, J. P., and Geschwind, N. (1982). Hemispheric asymmetry in the expression of positive and negative emotions: neurologic evidence. Arch. Neurol. 39, 210–218. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1982.00510160016003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneider, F., Grodd, W., Weiss, U., Klose, U., Mayer, K. R., Nägele, T., et al. (1997). Functional MRI reveals left amygdala activation during emotion. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 76, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(97)00063-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silberman, E. K., and Weingartner, H. (1986). Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain Cogn. 5, 322–353. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(86)90035-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, A. P. R., Henson, R. N. A., Dolan, R. J., and Rugg, M. D. (2004). fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts. Neuroimage 22, 868–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steinvorth, S., Corkin, S., and Halgren, E. (2006). Ecphory of autobiographical memories: an fMRI study of recent and remote memory retrieval. Neuroimage 30, 285–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tabert, M. H., Borod, J. C., Tang, C. Y., Lange, G., Wei, T. C., Johnson, R., et al. (2001). Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 39, 556–573. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00157-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, P. M., Hayashi, K. M., de Zubicaray, G., Janke, A. L., Rose, S. E., Semple, J., et al. (2003). Dynamics of gray matter loss in Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurosci. 23, 994–1005.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., and Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 14792–14797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Paesschen, W. V., and Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277, 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5324.376

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wager, T. D., Phan, K. L., Liberzon, I., and Taylor, S. F. (2003). Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: a meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging. Neuroimage 19, 513–531. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00078-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L.-O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Intern. Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, T. T., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Blasey, C., White, C., Reid, A. J., et al. (2002). Amygdalar activation associated with positive and negative facial expressions. Neuroreport 13, 1737–1741. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210070-00009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res. Rev. 41, 88–123. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00248-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zalla, T., Koechlin, E., Pietrini, P., Basso, G., Aquino, P., Sirigu, A., et al. (2000). Differential amygdala responses to winning and losing: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 1764–1770. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00064.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: emotional memory, lateralization, Alzheimer's disease, amygdala, prefrontal cortex, angular gyrus

Citation: Mistridis P, Taylor KI, Kissler JM, Monsch AU, Kressig RW and Kivisaari SL (2014) Distinct neural systems underlying reduced emotional enhancement for positive and negative stimuli in early Alzheimer's disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:939. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00939

Received: 11 November 2013; Accepted: 28 December 2013;
Published online: 20 January 2014.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Pierfilippo De Sanctis, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2014 Mistridis, Taylor, Kissler, Monsch, Kressig and Kivisaari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Panagiota Mistridis, Memory Clinic, University Center for Medicine of Aging Basel, Felix Platter Hospital, Schanzenstrasse 55, 4031 Basel, Switzerland e-mail: panagiota.mistridis@fps-basel.ch

Download