Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 November 2014
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00901

Parallel processing of face and house stimuli by V1 and specialized visual areas: a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study

  • Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, London, UK

We used easily distinguishable stimuli of faces and houses constituted from straight lines, with the aim of learning whether they activate V1 on the one hand, and the specialized areas that are critical for the processing of faces and houses on the other, with similar latencies. Eighteen subjects took part in the experiment, which used magnetoencephalography (MEG) coupled to analytical methods to detect the time course of the earliest responses which these stimuli provoke in these cortical areas. Both categories of stimuli activated V1 and areas of the visual cortex outside it at around 40 ms after stimulus onset, and the amplitude elicited by face stimuli was significantly larger than that elicited by house stimuli. These results suggest that “low-level” and “high-level” features of form stimuli are processed in parallel by V1 and visual areas outside it. Taken together with our previous results on the processing of simple geometric forms (Shigihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014), the present ones reinforce the conclusion that parallel processing is an important component in the strategy used by the brain to process and construct forms.

Introduction

In this study, which is a continuation of our earlier studies on parallel processing in the brain's visual form systems (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014), we set out to learn whether faces and houses activate the primary visual cortex (V1) and areas outside it sequentially or within the same time frame. The simplest way of doing so was to use magnetoencephalography (MEG), which has a superior temporal resolution, and record the earliest cortical responses that result from viewing face and house stimuli constituted from straight lines. The use of straight lines to constitute readily distinguishable stimuli belonging to the two categories was important because it is generally supposed that the cortical processing of all forms, including faces and houses, has its source in the orientation selective (OS) cells of area V1 (Bruce and Young, 1986; Biederman and Kalosai, 1998; Riesenhuber and Poggio, 1999; Haxby et al., 2000). Yet recent results show that simple geometrical forms constituted from straight lines activate V1 and the surrounding prestriate visual areas (areas V2 and V3), which also have heavy concentrations of OS cells (Zeki, 1978; Tootell et al., 1988; Kourtzi et al., 2003; Yacoub et al., 2008; Tong et al., 2012), within the same time frame, suggesting that, in addition to the hierarchical strategy, the cortex may also employ a parallel one for processing not only simple geometric forms but more complex ones such as faces and houses as well.

We chose faces and houses as our “higher” level stimuli partly because they have a well-known temporal signature, resulting in a cortical activation at 170 ms (although that time interval has been revised downwards, see Discussion). This has been used to argue in favor of a hierarchical strategy, in which “low-level” elements are processed and analyzed first, beginning in V1, before being combined to constitute distinct and more complex forms in “higher” areas outside it. We also chose these stimuli because there are cortical areas outside V1 that are critical for processing of faces and houses, especially the occipital face area (OFA) (Peelen and Downing, 2005; Pitcher et al., 2012), the fusiform face area (FFA) (Sergent et al., 1992; Kanwisher et al., 1997; Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006) and the parahippocampal place area (PPA) (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998), although whether these areas are uniquely specialized for faces or houses has been debated (Haxby et al., 2001). Such specializations made it more plausible to suppose that the parallel inputs from subcortical stations to prestriate cortex would manifest themselves temporally in an early latency response, comparable to that in V1. In this time-based study, we were not however especially concerned with localizing these areas precisely, our sole concern being to learn whether the responses elicited could be traced to V1 and to areas lying outside it. Nor were we particularly concerned with the characteristic N 170 ms temporal signature of the reaction to face and house stimuli. Rather, our concern was solely with the earliest response provoked by these stimuli in striate and prestriate cortex. In this way, we hoped to supplement our earlier results (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014) and learn whether “low-level” features, generally considered to be processed in V1, and “high-level” features, generally thought to be processed in specialized visual areas outside it, are in fact processed in parallel by V1 and the areas lying in cortex outside it.

We also used faces and houses as stimuli because they constitute two very distinct categories of forms. Faces have a very privileged position in visual perception, one not shared by other categories of form such as houses and other man-made objects, which are known to activate specialized areas of the visual brain as well. Whether due to an inherited brain template or a privileged rapid onset plasticity (Gauthier and Nelson, 2001; Zeki and Ishizu, 2013 for a review), newborn infants orient to faces or face-like stimuli within hours after birth (Goren et al., 1975; Johnson et al., 1991). This made it plausible, intuitively at least, to suppose that faces are processed faster than stimuli such as houses, which belong to man-made categories, even when both sets of stimuli are constructed from the same elements (lines) which are also optimal for activating the OS cells of V1. Our approach thus offered the opportunity of learning whether (a) when constructed from lines, the early activity produced by these two categories of stimuli in V1 and the visual areas outside it occurs within the same time frame; (b) there is any difference in latency of activation between so privileged a stimulus as a face and stimuli depicting man-made artifacts such as houses and, most importantly, (c) whether the relationship between “low-level” (oriented lines) and “high-level” (faces and houses) form processing is chronologically hierarchical. This would be so if the early responses obtained from V1 and from cortex outside it differ temporally.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and Study Design

Eighteen right-handed healthy adult volunteers (8 female, mean age 28.3 ± 9.2 years) took part in the study. None had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder; written informed consent was obtained from all and the study, which conforms to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association, was approved by the Ethics Committee of University College London.

Stimuli and Task

Stimuli were generated using Cogent 2000 and Cogent Graphics (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php) toolboxes running in MATLAB (MathWorks, Na-tick, MA, USA) and were rear projected on a screen by a projector (RM-MSX21G, Victor Company of Japan, Kanagawa, Japan) which has a resolution of 800 × 600 pixels at 60 Hz. Trigger signals were recorded for the MEG system through an IEEE 1284 connection. The delay between the trigger signal and the projection of stimuli (33 ms) was confirmed using a photodiode on the screen before scanning subjects and was corrected during data processing.

To avoid cancelation effects that can occur when both banks of the calcarine sulcus are stimulated (Portin et al., 1999), stimuli were displayed separately in either the lower left or lower right quadrants of the visual field and covered an area of 0° and 7.0° below the fixation cross and 0°–8.4° on either side (Figure 1). Three different categories of stimuli, all composed of the same 26 lines combined into similar forms (six squares and one triangle), were used: faces, houses and ones which could not easily be categorized into either face or house (“Neither”). The latter stimuli were used only to ensure that subjects maintained their attention during the experiment. Two different versions of “House” and “Face” stimuli were used, while the “Neither” category had four versions (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Stimulus image and projected hemifield. The fixation cross is located in the center of the screen. Each stimulus was projected in the lower left or right quadrant, and covered up to 7.0° below the fixation cross and up to 8.4° on either side.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Three categories of stimuli. “Face” and “House” stimuli were composed of the same lines but had configurations that made them easily recognizable as belonging to one category or the other. The “Neither” category consisted of four figures that were not easy to categorize as “Face” or “House.” All figures consisted of the same elements: 6 squares and 1 triangle, of which the maximum heights and width were 7.0° and 8.4° respectively. The width of each line was 0.1°. A white fixation cross (1.0° × 1.0°) was projected at the center of the screen.

Scanning Details

MEG data were recorded continuously using a 275-channel CTF Omega whole-head gradiometer (VSM MedTech, British Columbia, Canada). Data were sampled at 1200 Hz with a 300 Hz low-pass filter without using high-pass filters. Subjects were fitted with localizer coils at the nasion and at 1 cm anterior to the left and right traguses to monitor head movements during the recording sessions and co-register them to individual MRI structural images acquired in a 3T MRI scanner (Siemens Magnetron Allegra MRI scanner or Trio Tim 3T scanner, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Gaze position and blinking were monitored by an EyeLink 1000 eyetracker (SR Research Ltd., Ontario, Canada).

Subjects viewed the display screen at a distance of 60 cm. The experiment consisted of five 5 min runs; each comprised 140 stimuli, 63 of which were “Faces,” 63 “Houses” while 14 belonged to the “Neither” category. The order of stimulus presentation according to category, sub-type and hemifield stimulated (left or right) was randomized. Stimulus presentation lasted 283 ms, with a randomly varied inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 1500–2000 ms. Subjects focused on the fixation cross throughout the MEG scan and were asked, during the ISI, to indicate which category of stimulus had been presented, by pressing one of 3 buttons using their index, middle, and ring fingers; the ring finger was always used for the “Neither” category, whilst the index and middle fingers were counter-balanced across subjects.

Data Processing

Data were analyzed offline using SPM-8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). They were divided into 1000 ms epochs, each starting 517 ms before stimulus onset. Epochs affected by blink artifacts (detected using the eye-tracker and also by manual inspection of the raw signal data) were discarded and the remaining ones averaged in each condition and baseline corrected. About 140 responses were recorded for each subject, category, and quadrant, except for the “Neither” category (Face and House stimuli presented in left quadrant, 139.7 ± 14.8 ms and 141.0 ± 18.4 ms, respectively; Face and House stimuli presented in right quadrant, 148.2 ± 34.9 ms and 145.1 ± 16.3 ms, respectively; Neither presented in left and right quadrants 35.2 ± 7.6 and 30.4 ± 5.4, respectively). MEG data for the “Neither” category was ignored in the subsequent analyses due to lack of usable epochs. The signal during the 100 ms period preceding stimulus onset was used as a baseline. Software filters produce artifacts (Acunzo et al., 2012; Ramkumar et al., 2013) and are not recommended (VanRullen, 2011); like others before us (Noguchi et al., 2004; Inui et al., 2006; Acunzo et al., 2012), we therefore analyzed our results without filters.

Sensor-Level Analysis

We hypothesized that (1) there is an early component of event-related magnetic fields (ERFs), before 50 ms post-stimulus (ffytche et al., 1995; Inui et al., 2006; Shgihara and Zeki, 2013), and that (2) there would be differences in the amplitude of magnetic responses between Face and House stimuli at the early component of ERFs. To test these hypotheses, we defined a sensor of interest (SOI) which showed the largest root-mean-square (RMS) amplitude of ERF between 25 and 50 ms among the 37 occipital sensors selected by us based on the sensor names MLO 11–53 and MRO 11–53, defined by SPM-8 (SOI approach: Liu et al., 2002; Noguchi et al., 2004) since our previous work had shown an early component of ERF around this time window (27–44 ms) (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013). SOIs were defined for each subject and for each of the 4 conditions [2 forms (Face/House) × 2 quadrants (Left/Right)] separately; the locations of SOIs are shown in the Supplementary Data. We confirmed that there was no difference in the peak time across the four conditions using a Two-Way ANOVA (2 forms × 2 quadrants) with repeated measures (Main effect of form, P = 0.619; Main effect of quadrant, P = 0.107; Interaction, P = 0.690).

Averaged RMS amplitudes at SOIs across 18 subjects showed peaks around 40 ms (37.2–47.2 ms) for all 4 conditions (Figure 4). To confirm that these peaks were significantly larger than the baseline level, we performed an analysis which is schematically represented in Figure 3 (within form comparison): We divided our data into ten 5 ms time windows, covering the period between 0 and 50 ms after stimulus onset. This included the early response period described in previous studies (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013), namely 27–44 ms. We also defined a baseline period between −50 and 0 ms before stimulus onset.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Schematic figure, not using real data, to illustrate the two types of comparison in the sensor-level analysis employed in this study. (A) Within form comparison: Average RMS amplitudes at each time window (5 ms in width) between 0 and 50 ms after stimulus onset were compared against the average amplitude during baseline (−50 to 0 ms), using One-Way ANOVA for repeated measures followed by post-hoc t-test. This comparison was carried out for 4 conditions (2 forms × 2 quadrants) separately. (B) Between forms comparison: Averaged RMS amplitudes at each time window were compared between Face and House conditions for each quadrant (left/right) separately. White and shaded columns show the averaged RMS amplitude at each time window and baseline, respectively.

Averaged RMS amplitudes at each time window were compared against baseline. This comparison was carried out for 4 conditions (2 forms × 2 quadrants) separately, using One-Way ANOVA with repeated measures followed by post-hoc t-tests with Ryan correction (Ludbrook, 1991).

To address the second hypothesis (that Face and House stimuli produce responses with different amplitudes), RMS amplitudes at each time window were compared between Face and House stimuli using a paired t-test for each quadrant (left/right) separately (Figure 3: Between forms comparison).

Source-Level Analysis

Sensor-level analysis revealed that all four conditions produced early components of ERFs at around 40 ms (37.2–47.2 ms) after stimulus onset, and that Face stimuli produced larger responses than House stimuli for left quadrant stimulation. To learn which brain areas were responsible for producing these responses, we applied source level analyses for the early component of ERFs using SPM-8. Forward modeling was performed between 0 and 50 ms after stimulus onset using a single sphere model (fine mode), and source inversion (estimation) was performed for the peak time window for each condition (shown in Figure 4) using Multiple Sparse Priors (MSP, Greedy Search; Mattout et al., 2005; Friston et al., 2008) for each subject and condition (first level analysis). The source images produced by the first level analysis were smoothed using a Gaussian smoothing kernel of 9 × 9 × 9 mm and taken to the second (between subject) level analysis using t-tests within occipital areas defined by WFU_PickAtlas (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/wfu_pickatlas/). The existence of these ERFs was independently established at a statistically significant level for the sensor-level analysis (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013). Here, we report the source locations of peak level activations at a significance threshold of P(uncorrected) < 0.001 as well as P(FWE corrected) < 0.05. The same source localizations were performed for the other time bins as well. Figure 6 shows source locations at each time bin and for each condition. These sources were accumulated across time bins for each condition using Image Calculator in SPM-8 to locate the areas which were activated at any time during the early component of the response (Figure 7).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Time courses of averaged RMS amplitude across 18 subjects. Each condition shows a peak at around 40 ms after stimulus onset. For left quadrant presentation (A), RMS of amplitude for Face stimuli (bold lines) was larger than for House stimuli (broken lines) not only after 100 ms, but also before 50 ms after stimulus onset. For right quadrant presentation (B), the difference is only clear after 100 ms. Arrows indicate a peak at around 40 ms after stimulus onset. Although N170 responses are shown between 100 and 200 ms for left quadrant presentation (A) and 80 and 150 ms for right presentation (B), we do not discuss these components in this paper, because our interest was with the early responses alone.

Results

Behavioral Results

Accuracies and response times for subjects' answers were measured during MEG scans (Table 1) and a Two-Way repeated measures ANOVA (quadrant × form) was applied to each analysis. No significant main effect or interaction was found for accuracy [Main effect of Quadrant, F(1, 17) = 1.376, P = 0.257; Form, F(1, 17) = 0.014, P = 0.906; Interaction, F(1, 17) = 4.178, P = 0.057]. Accuracy for each stimulus condition was more than 95%, which shows that the two types of stimuli constituted from the same lines were perceptually accurately, and readily, distinguishable. There were no significant differences between conditions. Although both the main effect of quadrant stimulated and the interaction were not significant, a significant main effect of form was found for response time [Quadrant, F(1, 17) = 1.533, P = 0.233; Main effect of Form, F(1, 17) = 8.057, P = 0.011; Interaction, F(1, 17) = 0.094, P = 0.762]. Differences in form (Face and House) led to significant differences in terms of response time, which was 20 ms longer for House stimuli than Face stimuli.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Accuracy and response time for button pushing.

Results of Sensor-Level Analysis

To better define the early component of ERFs, we calculated a RMS of amplitudes at the SOI which showed the largest amplitude among all 37 occipital sensors, between 25 and 50 ms after stimulus onset. The averaged time course of the RMS amplitudes at the SOIs for the four conditions [2 forms (Face/House) × 2 quadrants (Left/Right)] and across 18 subjects is shown in Figure 4 and a representative RMS time course for a single subject (Subject #12) is shown in Figure S2 in the Supplementary Data. There is, on average, a peak at around 40 ms after stimulus onset for each condition: 37.2 and 39.7 ms for Face and House stimuli presented in the left quadrant, respectively; 47.2 and 44.7 ms for Face and House stimuli presented in the right quadrant, respectively (See Table 2), and the average peak for Face stimuli lasted longer than that for House stimuli for left quadrant presentation. Contour maps at the peak time bins are shown in Figure 5. They show the existence of magnetic sources in occipital areas for all four conditions, although the activation may extend beyond anteriorly.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of ANOVA, post-hoc t-test, paired t-test between two forms (Face vs. House), and Peak time of ERF.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Averaged contour maps across 18 subjects and statistical parametric maps of estimated source locations of ERFs for group-level analysis (between subjects), superimposed on a standard brain image, broken down by stimulus form and display quadrant. Source estimation was performed using MSP during peak time window. Contour maps: Red areas represent outflow of magnetic fields and blue areas their inflow, hence the proximity of the two signifies the presence of an electrical current between them. In these areas, at least one part of the red and the blue zones can be traced to occipital sensors; hence each contour map suggests the existence of magnetic sources at least in occipital areas. Statistical parametric maps: Sources were estimated not only around the mid line (striate cortex) but also in other areas for all conditions. The display threshold is at peak level P(unc.) < 0.001.

To confirm these observations, we performed two analyses on the data, divided into 5 ms time windows, as shown in Figure 3: (a) One-Way ANOVA with repeated measures for each of the four conditions (2 forms × 2 quadrants) to confirm that the peak was larger than baseline (within form comparison in Figure 3) and (b) paired t-tests to compare the RMS amplitudes produced by Face and House stimuli at each of the 10 time windows for each quadrant separately (between form comparison in Figure 3) with the following results:

(a) A One-Way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of time window for all four conditions (Table 2). Post-hoc t-tests showed that the RMS amplitude was significantly larger than baseline at 20–50 ms for Face and at 30–45 ms for House stimuli presented in the left quadrant, respectively, and at 20–50 ms for Face stimuli and 30–50 ms for House stimuli presented in the right quadrant, respectively in the right quadrant (Table 2).

(b) A paired t-test showed that the RMS amplitude at 25–30 ms post-stimulus onset was significantly larger in response to Face than to House stimuli for left quadrant presentation (Table 2, Table S1 in Supplementary Data). This time bin (25–30 ms) precedes the time bin of the peaks (35–40 ms) produced by both stimuli, a finding that matches our observation that the response elicited by Face stimuli lasts longer than that elicited by House stimuli when presented in the left quadrant. No other differences were found.

In summary, all four conditions showed an early component of ERF at around 40 ms after stimulus onset and ERF amplitudes elicited by Face stimuli were different (and larger) than those elicited by House stimuli.

Results for Source-Level Analysis

We next wanted to determine the cortical source of the regions producing these ERFs, restricting ourselves to determining whether the source was in V1 as well as the (prestriate) areas outside it, without trying to localize the precise source to previously demarcated regions of the prestriate cortex, a difficult task because of the relatively poor spatial resolution of MEG. Source localization was performed using MSP to estimate the sources of the ERFs during the peak time windows (which corresponds in Figure 3, left panel, to the 35–40 ms bin). See also Table 2 and Figure 5). Figures 6, 7 show the sources at different time windows). At the peak time windows, sources were estimated in both striate (V1) and prestriate cortices for all conditions (Figure 5; Table 3 and Table S2); sources in both were significant at peak level P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) for Face and House stimuli presented in the left quadrant. For right quadrant Face stimuli presentation, however, only sources in prestriate cortex were significant while for right quadrant House presentation, only sources in striate cortex were significant.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Averaged contour maps across 18 subjects and statistical parametric maps of the estimated source locations of ERFs for group-level analysis along time bins, superimposed on a standard brain image and broken down by stimulus form and display quadrant. Source estimation was performed using MSP during time bins and significant activation confirmed by sensor-level analysis. The display threshold is at peak level P(unc.) < 0.001. Blue square, a set of conditions which showed significant difference in amplitudes using sensor-level analysis; Red square, peak time bin; Black square, time bin in which significant response was not confirmed by sensor-level analysis.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Brain areas in which sources were estimated at time bins between 20 and 50 ms after stimulus onset. The display threshold is at peak level P(unc.) < 0.001. Red circle, V1 (striate cortex); Green circle, Occipital Face Area (OFA); Blue circle, lateral occipital complex (LOC); Black circle, Fusiform Face Area (FFA), and Parahippocampal Place Area (PPA).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sources locations which were responsible for producing the early components of ERFs.

Sources for other time windows are shown in Figures 6, 7. All four conditions show sources in prestriate cortex which apparently include areas V3, OFA, FFA, PPA, and lateral occipital complex (LOC), although these source locations are not as accurate as one would wish, due to technical limitations (see Discussion).

Sensor-level analysis showed a difference in amplitudes between Face and House stimuli for left quadrant presentation at 25–30 ms after stimulus onset. Although House stimulation did not produce a significant response against the baseline, Face stimuli did so and the sources of the response were localized in both striate and prestriate cortex (Figure 6, Blue square). This shows that the difference in amplitude at the earliest time window is responsible for both striate and prestriate cortex responses.

In summary, Face and House stimuli presented in the left quadrant activated both V1 (striate cortex) and the prestriate cortex outside it with a peak at around 40 ms after stimulus onset, with the activation produced by Face stimuli being larger in amplitude than those produced by House stimuli. For stimuli presented in the right quadrant, activities were less evident at around 40 ms after stimulus onset, in both striate and prestriate cortex. During the whole period of the early component of ERFs, both striate and prestriate cortex were activated for all four conditions in both hemispheres.

Discussion

Our results show, in summary, that (a) Face and House stimuli constituted from straight lines produce an early response around 40 ms post-stimulus; (b) the amplitude of the response produced by Face stimuli is significantly larger than that produced by House stimuli when presented in the left quadrant; and (c) both sets of stimuli produce early activity in both striate and prestriate cortex when presented in the left quadrant but that activity produced by them following right quadrant stimulation was less easily localizable because the statistical power for locating them was less significant.

The view that, in the cerebral cortex, the processing of visual forms (of which faces and houses constitute two examples) begins in V1, coupled to the well documented late latency (N170) in response to both categories of stimuli (Bentin et al., 1996), has fortified the belief that a hierarchical strategy, whose source is in the OS cells of V1, is the ubiquitous one used for the processing of forms, including faces. Our present results, together with our previous ones (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014), suggest however that a parallel strategy, involving both V1 and specialized visual areas outside it, may also be used to process forms, in addition to the hierarchical strategy.

Hierarchical and Parallel Processing and the Temporal Order of Activation of Visual Areas

The temporal signature, at which a robust response is elicited from cortex in response to face and house stimulation is the N170, which is in the 130–170 time window post-stimulation (Rousselet and Husk, 2008). This means that activity in response to face or house stimulation cannot be distinguished earlier than this time frame, the time up to that period being considered to be taken up by processing of “low-level” features of form (e.g., those of orientation, spatial frequency and contrast, see Johnson and Olshausen (2003) and Rousselet and Husk (2008). 170 ms is considerably longer than the latency with which a response is obtained from V1 and longer than the latencies of the main component of visual perception (i.e., N75, P100, and N145) (see review by Tobimatsu and Celesia, 2006). The difference in latency between the early (N75) response to visual stimulation and the late (N170) component corresponding to the recognition of stimuli as distinct is consistent with a hierarchical strategy in which “low-level” features are processed first. More recently, the latency of activation produced by face stimuli, at least, has been revised downwards. In particular Seeck et al. (1997), using intra- and extra-cranial electrodes, obtained activity at 50 ms post-stimulus when subjects were asked to differentiate between familiar and unfamiliar faces. Other studies using MEG have also shown that face stimuli lead to activation in occipito-temporal cortex before 100 ms (Braeutigam et al., 2001; Meeren et al., 2008) As well, transcranial magnetic stimulation studies (Pitcher et al., 2007) have shown that face perception can be disrupted at 40–50 ms post-stimulus. Although these results might suggest that the cortical processing of faces (and abstract objects) occurs much earlier than previously thought, these evoked response studies used photographs of actual faces as stimuli to elicit responses, which raises the question of whether the early responses may not have been related to the processing of “low-level” features such as lines. Moreover, these studies leave open the question of whether the early responses can be traced to V1 or to prestriate cortex, or both.

In our study, we tried to circumvent these problems by using stimuli constituted from straight lines, thus ensuring that the (early) “low-level” elements constituting the face or house stimuli were uniform throughout, differed in configuration alone and capable of stimulating strongly areas such as V1 and areas of prestriate visual cortex which have high concentrations of OS cells. We also tried to localize the source of the activity that we obtained, to establish whether both V1 and specialized areas of the prestriate cortex react with similar latencies. Our results show that, with both Face and House stimulation, not only is there a response from V1 but also from prestriate cortex, at around 40 ms after stimulus onset. This latency is similar to the earliest latency 28–32 ms in area V5 of prestriate visual cortex obtained after stimulation with fast motion (>22°s−1) (ffytche et al., 1995), 37 ms in V1 (Inui et al., 2006) and 27–44 ms in striate and prestriate cortex (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013), and it is even earlier than the ones reported by previous studies on face perception. Moreover, we have shown that there is a significant difference in the amplitude of the response to faces and houses, suggesting that the two stimuli are differentiated at this early time period. Our results therefore suggest that differentiation between face and house related activity in the visual brain is due to differences in amplitude of the response provoked by the two categories of stimuli rather than to differences in latency.

Parallelism in the Form Pathway?

Our results thus make it plausible to suppose that the brain uses a parallel strategy, in addition to the hierarchical one, to process forms. Although they show that V1 and areas of the prestriate cortex give an early ERF component at around 40 ms, they do not show conclusively the operation of a parallel strategy since it is still conceivable that a signal from V1 originating at, say, 40 ms after stimulus onset would be processed hierarchically by striate and prestriate cortex within the 40–45 ms peak time window. But there are three lines of evidence which suggest strongly that the hierarchical strategy in constructing forms may be supplemented by a parallel one, in which the OS cells of V1 are not the sole source for the construction of forms in the specialized visual areas of the prestriate cortex.

MEG evidence

The first comes from the results of MEG experiments, which show that lines and more complex geometrical forms (rhombuses) constituted from them activate V1 and the visual areas of the prestriate cortex with similar latencies (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013). This is supplemented by fMRI evidence, which shows that lines, angles and rhombuses activate V1 and areas of the prestriate cortex with similar strengths, with angles producing the strongest and rhombuses the weakest activation in all (Shigihara and Zeki, 2014). This is contrary to what one might expect from the hierarchical doctrine, which would point to rhombuses as producing the strongest activation or to lines activating area V1 more strongly and rhombuses activating prestriate areas more strongly (Hubel and Wiesel, 1965). The results showing that V1 and areas V2 and V3 of prestriate cortex are engaged in parallel in processing geometric forms of increasing complexity (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014) are consistent with the present results, which show that, likewise, V1 and specialized areas of prestriate cortex are also engaged in parallel in the processing of Face and House stimuli. One would conclude from this that there is no neat separation between the processing of “low-level” and “high-level” features and no parcellation of the former to V1 and the latter to “higher” areas in prestriate cortex.

Anatomical evidence

That there are parallel strategies within visual cortex, implied by the parallel anatomical connections from V1 and V2 (which are themselves interconnected) to say, V4 and V5, has long been acknowledged and its computational significance evaluated (e.g., Ballard et al., 1983; Grossberg, 1991). Much less attention has been given to the parallel inputs to V1 and areas of the prestriate visual cortex from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and the pulvinar, the latter of which may respond to visual (motion) stimuli before striate cortex does so (Ouellette and Casanova, 2006). This is surprising, since such pathways have been known to exist for a long time, from LGN (Cragg, 1969; Fries, 1981; Yukie and Iwai, 1981) and pulvinar (Cragg, 1969; Benevento and Rezak, 1976; Leventhal et al., 1980; Leh et al., 2008), both of which also receive input from the retina (Itaya and Van Hoesen, 1983; Nakagawa and Tanaka, 1984; Ouellette and Casanova, 2006; Baldwin et al., 2012). As well, the capacity of this “V1-bypassing” pathway to mediate a crude but conscious experience of at least visual motion, has been acknowledged (Barbur et al., 1993; Zeki and ffytche, 1998; Weiskrantz, 2004). The direct input to the motion sensitive area V5, from the LGN or the pulvinar (or both), leads in fact to a shorter latency activation of V5 (at between 28 and 32 ms) than does the input from the LGN to V1 (at about 75 ms), for fast moving stimuli (>22°s−1) (ffytche et al., 1995; Gaglianese et al., 2012), leading to the concept of dynamic parallelism. Hence, it becomes plausible to suppose that direct inputs from LGN and pulvinar to visual areas of the prestriate cortex with large concentrations of OS cells may deliver signals related to form vision directly to them (to areas such as V2, V3, and V3A) without passing through V1 (Schmid et al., 2009), just as they deliver fast motion-related signals directly to V5 (Beckers and Zeki, 1995; ffytche et al., 1995; Sincich et al., 2004). In light of our present results, they may also deliver signals that are critical for the perception of faces and houses directly to the relevant, specialized areas of the visual brain, especially since the pulvinar projections to the cortex are extensive and include the inferior temporal and the posterior parietal cortex, in addition to the occipital lobe (Leh et al., 2008). Here, it is interesting to note that the activation we observed, though it must remain tentative with respect to the precise subdivisions of the prestriate cortex because of the relatively poor spatial resolution of MEG, nevertheless suggests that V3, which contains high concentrations of OS cells (Zeki, 1978), and the areas which have been thought to play critical roles in face and house perception, were active at the early time windows. In such a scenario, the direct input to the latter areas, as well as the input through V1, would play different roles in processing stimuli but what role each input plays has yet to be determined.

Clinical evidence

The dominant role played by the classical visual pathway extending from the retina to V1 through the LGN undoubtedly also played an important role in emphasizing the hierarchical doctrine of form processing. This is especially so since lesions along this pathway, and particularly in V1, lead to blindnesses commensurate with the size and position of the lesions. Yet there is also evidence that lesions restricted to V2 and V3 lead to a comparable blindness (Horton and Hoyt, 1991), although such evidence is sparse because much more difficult to obtain, owing to the disposition of V2 and V3 in relation to V1, which means that damage to the latter usually also involves damage to the former. Hence V1 lesions do not have a monopoly in producing hemianopias.

Asymmetric Results with Quadrant Stimulation

It is well established that the right hemisphere is dominant for face perception (Kanwisher et al., 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997; Pitcher et al., 2007; Yovel et al., 2008). This is probably why sensor level analysis showed a larger RMS amplitude for Face than House in left quadrant presentation, since left quadrant presentations are mainly processed in the dominant (right) hemisphere for face perception. Right hemisphere dominance for face perception in the early response indicates that the cortical processing of face stimuli starts at the earliest stage of visual perception, around 40 ms post-stimulus.

Problems Arising from the Present Results

There are five possible problems worth mentioning here:

(1) Response time: We note that the observed precedence in behavioral response times to Faces is not mirrored by an earlier cortical activation by Faces compared to Houses. However, behavioral response times may depend on later cortical activity rather than early ones (Johnson and Olshausen, 2003).

(2) Low-level feature differences in stimuli: Although both Face and House stimuli consisted of the same elements, there are some unavoidable differences. Face stimuli had ears, which would activate a larger part of the field of view. By contrast, House stimuli had a window, which adds to the complexity of the inner small space; these differences might modulate the MEG amplitude differently (Tanskanen et al., 2005). Furthermore, each Face and House stimulus had only two versions and this limitation in the range of stimuli might have affected our results. However, these objections cannot account for the laterality which we observed: the difference in amplitude in the early components of the ERFs was significant only for stimuli presented in the left quadrant. The dominance of the right hemisphere for face perception can account for this laterality and leads us to discount the possibility that the responses which we have observed are due to low-level features. Moreover, Area V3, which contains OS cells and which has been linked to low-level feature selectivity, was not active with right quadrant presentation of face and house stimuli.

(3) N170 response: N170 is the most prominent and well-known electromagnetic response for face perception. Although we detected the response (see, for example, Figure 4), we do not discuss it in this paper because it is not the main or even secondary aim of this work. The main finding here is that differences related to the processing of higher forms (faces and houses) were found to occur much earlier than N170 and were localizable to V1 and cortex outside it.

(4) Electrical activity in the retina: Retinal activity can produce electromagnetic fields which can contaminate the MEG signals. The second wave (b-wave) produced by ON bipolar cells and the Muller cells could appear at around 25–50 ms (Crick and Khaw, 2003), which is the time window we are interested in. However, our responses were evidently not attributable to a retinal origin alone since the source was localized to occipital rather than frontal cortex.

(5) Source localization: Estimated source locations at the peak time window for the four conditions are similar to each other (Figure 5 and Table 3), although there are differences in contour maps. There are two interpretations of this result: that the same brain areas were activated at the peak of the response but with different amplitudes or that the difference in source distributions was too small to be detected using our method (MSP). MSP uses source priors which are part of SPM-8. These priors restrict the variability of source distributions, a limitation which might have prevented us from detecting differences in source distribution between the four conditions in Figure 5. Although estimated sources at other time windows (Figures 6, 7) seem to include V3, OFA, FFA, and PPA, we have to exercise considerable caution in assigning the activity to specific cortical areas, given the limitations imposed by the use of MSP. We repeat, however, that our main aim was to learn whether sources could be localized to striate and prestriate cortex at the earliest time windows. The exact location of activity in prestriate cortex was of secondary interest, given that we had to sacrifice spatial resolution to obtain good temporal resolution.

Unexpected Results

There are two unexpected results. (1) In the source level analysis, sources were estimated in both left and right striate cortices (Figure 5). This result seems to conflict with the primary rule that striate cortex processes visual information originating from the contralateral visual field. There are two possible explanations for this. Firstly, the estimated source can be distributed to the opposite side of striate cortex, due to the limited spatial resolution of MEG and MSP source estimation (Cicmil et al., 2014). Secondly, our stimuli extended from 8.4 degrees on either side to the vertical meridian. A previous study has shown that there is a strip of central retina which projects to both hemispheres. The width of this strip is 1 to 6 degrees (Marzi et al., 2009), which may account for our results. (2) Sources in striate cortex for right hemifield presentation were not significant at P(FWE) < 0.05, although they were significant at P(unc.) < 0.001. This lack of significant activation (or weak activation) in striate cortex is surprising given that we do have significant prestriate activation. If anything, this result makes the case for parallel processing more emphatic for it suggests the possibility of a more potent input to prestriate than striate cortex with the face and house stimuli that we used. A parallel result may be found in the fact that signals from appropriate fast moving stimuli (>22°s−1) signal reach V5 directly and before they reach V1 (ffytche et al., 1995).

Conclusion

Against this background, and in light of our earlier experiments (Shgihara and Zeki, 2013, 2014), we suggest that a strong case can now be made for allocating to parallel strategies an important role in the cortical visual processing of forms as they relate to simple geometric forms as well as to higher level forms such as faces and houses.

Although we believe that our results suggest strongly that, in addition to the parallel connections between visual cortical areas, parallel anatomical inputs to V1 and to the specialized visual areas of the prestriate cortex from the LGN and the pulvinar, confers on the visual brain the capacity to process signals in parallel by V1 and areas of the prestriate cortex, we do not mean to imply that parallel processing in these different stations of the visual brain is also synchronous. There is good reason from psychophysical experiments (Moutoussis and Zeki, 1997; Arnold et al., 2001; Holcombe and Cavanagh, 2001; Viviani and Aymoz, 2001) to show that some attributes of the visual world are perceived before others, and hence that the “read-out” from the parallel processing systems may be asynchronous, in addition to being in parallel. Thus, parallel processing coupled to asynchronous “parallel read-outs,” introduces an interesting element into future hypotheses about the strategies used by the visual brain to construct an image of our world.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust. We were given invaluable assistance at various stages of this work by our colleagues Gareth Barnes and Vladimir Litvak. We are especially grateful to Karl Friston, Tomohiro Ishizu, John Romaya, and Joe Sylvester for their critical comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00901/abstract

References

Acunzo, D. J., MacKenzie, G., and van Rossum, M. C. W. (2012). Systematic biases in early ERP and ERF components as a result of high-pass filtering. J. Neurosci. Methods 211, 309. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.08.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arnold, D. H., Clifford, C. W., and Wenderoth, P. (2001). Asynchronous processing in vision: color leads motion. Curr. Biol. 11, 596–600. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00156-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, M. K. L., Kaskan, P. M., Zhang, B., Chino, Y. M., and Kaas, J. H. (2012). Cortical and subcortical connections of V1 and V2 in early postnatal macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 544–569. doi: 10.1002/cne.22732

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, D., Hinton, G., and Sejnowski, T. (1983). Parallel visual computation. Nature 306, 21–26. doi: 10.1038/306021a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbur, J. L., Watson, J. D., Frackowiak, R. S., and Zeki, S. (1993). Conscious visual perception without V1. Brain 116, 1293–1302.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckers, G., and Zeki, S. (1995). The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception. Brain 118, 49–60.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Benevento, L., and Rezak, M. (1976). The cortical projections of the inferior pulvinar and adjacent lateral pulvinar in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta): an autoradiographic study. Brain Res. 108, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90160-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, I., and Kalosai, P. (1998). “Neural and psychophysical analysis of object and face recognition,” in Face Recognition, eds H. Wechsler, P. Phillips, V. Bruce, F. Sulie, and T. Huang (New York, NY: Springer), 3–25.

Google Scholar

Braeutigam, S., Bailey, A. J., and Swithenby, S. J. (2001). Task-dependent early latency (30-60 ms) visual processing of human faces and other objects. Neuroreport 12, 1531–1536. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105250-00046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, V., and Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 77, 305–327. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicmil, N., Bridge, H., Parker, A. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Krug, K. (2014). Localization of MEG human brain responses to retinotopic visual stimuli with contrasting source reconstruction approaches. Front. Neurosci. 8:127. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00127

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cragg, B. G. (1969). The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the Nauta method. Vision Res. 9, 733–747.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, R. P., and Khaw, P. T. (2003). A Textbook of Clinical Ophthalmology: A Practical Guide to Disorders of the Eyes and Their Management, 3rd Edn. Singapore: World Scientific Pub Co Inc.

Epstein, R., and Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ffytche, D. H., Guy, C. N., and Zeki, S. (1995). The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex. Brain 118, 1375–1394.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, W. (1981). The projection from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex of the macaque monkey. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 213, 73–86. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1981.0054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Harrison, L., Daunizeau, J., Kiebel, S., Phillips, C., Trujillo-Barreto, N., et al. (2008). Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem. Neuroimage 39, 1104–1120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaglianese, A., Costagli, M., Bernardi, G., Ricciardi, E., and Pietrini, P. (2012). Evidence of a direct influence between the thalamus and hMT+ independent of V1 in the human brain as measured by fMRI. Neuroimage 60, 1440–1447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.093

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, I., and Nelson, C. (2001). The development of face expertise. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00200-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goren, C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics 56, 544–549.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossberg, S. (1991). Why do parallel cortical systems exist for the perception of static form and moving form? Percept. Psychophys. 49, 117–141. doi: 10.3758/BF03205033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J., Hoffman, E., and Gobbini, M. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., and Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293, 2425–2430. doi: 10.1126/science.1063736

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holcombe, A. O., and Cavanagh, P. (2001). Early binding of feature pairs for visual perception. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 127–128. doi: 10.1038/83945

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horton, J., and Hoyt, W. (1991). Quadrantic visual field defects. Brain 114, 1703–1718. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.4.1703

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1965). Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 28, 229–289.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Inui, K., Sannan, H., Miki, K., Kaneoke, Y., and Kakigi, R. (2006). Timing of early activity in the visual cortex as revealed by simultaneous MEG and ERG recordings. Neuroimage 30, 239–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itaya, S. K., and Van Hoesen, G. W. (1983). Retinal projections to the inferior and medial pulvinar nuclei in the Old-World monkey. Brain Res. 269, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90131-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. S., and Olshausen, B. A. (2003). Time course of neural signatures of object recognition. J. Vis. 3, 499–512. doi: 10.1167/3.7.4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., and Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourtzi, Z., Tolias, A. S., Altmann, C. F., Augath, M., and Logothetis, N. K. (2003). Integration of local features into global shapes: monkey and human FMRI studies. Neuron 37, 333–346. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01174-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leh, S. E., Chakravarty, M. M., and Ptito, A. (2008). The connectivity of the human pulvinar: a diffusion tensor imaging tractography study. Int. J. Biomed. Imaging 2008:789539. doi: 10.1155/2008/789539

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leventhal, A. G., Keens, J., and Törk, I. (1980). The afferent ganglion cells and cortical projections of the retinal recipient zone (RRZ) of the cat's pulvinar complex. J. Comp. Neurol. 194, 535–554. doi: 10.1002/cne.901940305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2002). Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 910–916. doi: 10.1038/nn909

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludbrook, J. (1991). On making multiple comparisons in clinical and experimental pharmacology and physiology. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 18, 379–392. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.1991.tb01468.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzi, C. A., Mancini, F., Sperandio, I., and Savazzi, S. (2009). Evidence of midline retinal nasotemporal overlap in healthy humans: a model for foveal sparing in hemianopia? Neuropsychologia 47, 3007–3011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattout, J., Pélégrini-Issac, M., Garnero, L., and Benali, H. (2005). Multivariate source prelocalization (MSP): use of functionally informed basis functions for better conditioning the MEG inverse problem. Neuroimage 26, 356–373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J., and Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 605–610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meeren, H. K. M., Hadjikhani, N., Ahlfors, S. P., Hämäläinen, M. S., and de Gelder, B. (2008). Early category-specific cortical activation revealed by visual stimulus inversion. PLoS ONE 3:e3503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moutoussis, K., and Zeki, S. (1997). A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision. Proc. Biol. Sci. 264, 393–399. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagawa, S., and Tanaka, S. (1984). Retinal projections to the pulvinar nucleus of the macaque monkey: a re-investigation using autoradiography. Exp. Brain Res. 5, 151–157.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Noguchi, Y., Inui, K., and Kakigi, R. (2004). Temporal dynamics of neural adaptation effect in the human visual ventral stream. J. Neurosci. 24, 6283–6290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0655-04.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouellette, B. G., and Casanova, C. (2006). Overlapping visual response latency distributions in visual cortices and LP-pulvinar complex of the cat. Exp. Brain Res. 175, 332–341. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0555-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelen, M. V., and Downing, P. E. (2005). Within-subject reproducibility of category-specific visual activation with functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 402–408. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20116

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Goldhaber, T., Duchaine, B., Walsh, V., and Kanwisher, N. (2012). Two critical and functionally distinct stages of face and body perception. J. Neurosci. 32, 15877–15885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2624-12.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., Yovel, G., and Duchaine, B. (2007). TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing. Curr. Biol. 17, 1568–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.063

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portin, K., Vanni, S., Virsu, V., and Hari, R. (1999). Stronger occipital cortical activation to lower than upper visual field stimuli. Neuromagnetic recordings. Exp. Brain Res. 124, 287–294. doi: 10.1007/s002210050625

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramkumar, P., Jas, M., Pannasch, S., Hari, R., and Parkkonen, L. (2013). Feature-specific information processing precedes concerted activation in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 7691–7699. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3905-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riesenhuber, M., and Poggio, T. (1999). Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1019–1025. doi: 10.1038/14819

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousselet, G., and Husk, J. (2008). Time course and robustness of ERP object and face differences. J. Vis. 8, 1–18. doi: 10.1167/8.12.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, M. C., Panagiotaropoulos, T., Augath, M. A., Logothetis, N. K., and Smirnakis, S. M. (2009). Visually driven activation in macaque areas V2 and V3 without input from the primary visual cortex. PLoS ONE 4:e5527. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005527

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeck, M., Michel, C. M., Mainwaring, N., Cosgrove, R., Blume, H., Ives, J., et al. (1997). Evidence for rapid face recognition from human scalp and intracranial electrodes. Neuroreport 8, 2749–2754. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199708180-00021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergent, J., Ohta, S., and Macdonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. Brain 115, 15–36. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shgihara, Y., and Zeki, S. (2013). Parallelism in the brain's visual form system. Eur. J. Neurosci. 38, 3712–3720. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12371

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shigihara, Y., and Zeki, S. (2014). Parallel processing in the brain's visual form system: an fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:506. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sincich, L. C., Park, K. F., Wohlgemuth, M. J., and Horton, J. C. (2004). Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1123–1128. doi: 10.1038/nn1318

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanskanen, T., Näsänen, R., Montez, T., Päällysaho, J., and Hari, R. (2005). Face recognition and cortical responses show similar sensitivity to noise spatial frequency. Cereb. Cortex 15, 526–534. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobimatsu, S., and Celesia, G. G. (2006). Studies of human visual pathophysiology with visual evoked potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1414–1433. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, F., Harrison, S. A., Dewey, J. A., and Kamitani, Y. (2012). Relationship between BOLD amplitude and pattern classification of orientation-selective activity in the human visual cortex. Neuroimage 63, 1212–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tootell, R., Hamilton, S., and Switkes, E. (1988). Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. IV. Contrast and magno-parvo streams. J. Neurosci. 8, 1594–1609.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

VanRullen, R. (2011). Four common conceptual fallacies in mapping the time course of recognition. Front. Psychol. 2:365. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00365

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, P., and Aymoz, C. (2001). Colour, form, and movement are not perceived simultaneously. Vis. Res. 41, 2909–2918. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00160-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiskrantz, L. (2004). Roots of blindsight. Prog. Brain Res. 144, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14416-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yacoub, E., Harel, N., and Ugurbil, K. (2008). High-field fMRI unveils orientation columns in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 10607–10612. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804110105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yovel, G., Tambini, A., and Brandman, T. (2008). The asymmetry of the fusiform face area is a stable individual characteristic that underlies the left-visual-field superiority for faces. Neuropsychologia 46, 3061–3068. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yukie, M., and Iwai, E. (1981). Direct projection from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 201, 81–97. doi: 10.1002/cne.902010107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeki, S. (1978). Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in rhesus monkey prestriate visual cortex. J. Physiol. 277, 273–290.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeki, S., and ffytche, D. H. (1998). The Riddoch Syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. Brain 121, 25–45. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.1.25

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeki, S., and Ishizu, T. (2013). The “Visual Shock” of Francis Bacon: an essay in neuroesthetics. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:850. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00850

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: form perception, hierarchical model of form processing, orientation selective cells, parallel model of form processing

Citation: Shigihara Y and Zeki S (2014) Parallel processing of face and house stimuli by V1 and specialized visual areas: a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:901. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00901

Received: 14 July 2014; Accepted: 21 October 2014;
Published online: 07 November 2014.

Edited by:

Aron K. Barbey, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA

Reviewed by:

Shozo Tobimatsu, Kyushu University, Japan
Seppo P. Ahlfors, Massachusetts General Hospital, USA

Copyright © 2014 Shigihara and Zeki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Semir Zeki, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, UK e-mail: s.zeki@ucl.ac.uk

Download