Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 01 July 2013
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Visual Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: A View into the Mechanisms of Madness? View all 31 articles

“To see or not to see: that is the question.” The “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) model: evidence from congenital blindness and visuo-cognitive aberrations


Steffen Landgraf,* Steffen Landgraf1,2*Michael Osterheider Michael Osterheider1
  • 1Department for Forensic Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, District Hospital, University Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany

The causes of schizophrenia are still unknown. For the last 100 years, though, both “absent” and “perfect” vision have been associated with a lower risk for schizophrenia. Hence, vision itself and aberrations in visual functioning may be fundamental to the development and etiological explanations of the disorder. In this paper, we present the “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) model, which grades the risk for developing schizophrenia as a function of an individual's visual capacity. We review two vision perspectives: (1) “Absent” vision or how congenital blindness contributes to PaSZ and (2) “perfect” vision or how aberrations in visual functioning are associated with psychosis. First, we illustrate that, although congenitally blind and sighted individuals acquire similar world representations, blind individuals compensate for behavioral shortcomings through neurofunctional and multisensory reorganization. These reorganizations may indicate etiological explanations for their PaSZ. Second, we demonstrate that visuo-cognitive impairments are fundamental for the development of schizophrenia. Deteriorated visual information acquisition and processing contribute to higher-order cognitive dysfunctions and subsequently to schizophrenic symptoms. Finally, we provide different specific therapeutic recommendations for individuals who suffer from visual impairments (who never developed “normal” vision) and individuals who suffer from visual deterioration (who previously had “normal” visual skills). Rather than categorizing individuals as “normal” and “mentally disordered,” the PaSZ model uses a continuous scale to represent psychiatrically relevant human behavior. This not only provides a scientific basis for more fine-grained diagnostic assessments, earlier detection, and more appropriate therapeutic assignments, but it also outlines a trajectory for unraveling the causes of abnormal psychotic human self- and world-perception.

Introduction

The Protection-Against-Schizophrenia Model

“To see or not to see” may be the fundamental question by which we can elucidate the still unknown causes (Insel, 2010) of one of the most devastating human experiences – schizophrenia. For the last 100 years, both “absent” and “perfect” vision have been associated with a lower risk for the disorder (Landgraf et al., 2012; Silverstein et al., 2013). Therefore, we argue that vision itself and aberrations in visual functioning may be fundamental to the development of the disorder and, thus, may provide important information about its causes. In this article, we present the “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) model by reviewing human visual functioning from two perspectives: (1) “Absent” vision or how congenital blindness contributes to PaSZ and (2) “perfect” vision or how aberrations in visual functioning are associated with an increased risk for psychotic symptomatology. Grading the risk for developing schizophrenia as a function of an individual's visual capacity, we argue that (early) diagnostic and interventional approaches need to be specific to the patient's visual capacity. On the one hand, individuals who suffer from visual deterioration (who previously had “normal” visual skills) may reduce their risk for developing schizophrenia through an improvement in visual capacity. On the other hand, individuals who suffer from visual impairment (who have never developed “normal” vision) may, in fact, reduce their risk for schizophrenia through a decline in visual capacity (Figure 1). Hence, rather than categorizing human behavior into, e.g., “normal” and “mentally disordered,” as has traditionally been the practice of diagnostic manuals (APA, 2000; WHO, 2007), our approach may be the first to use a continuous scale to represent psychiatrically relevant human behavior. This not only provides a scientific basis for more fine-grained diagnostic assessments, earlier detection, and more appropriate therapeutic assignments, but this model also outlines a trajectory for unraveling the causes of the schizophrenia disorder.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) Model. The continuous PaSZ model depicts the relative risk for schizophrenia as a function of the continuous variable visual capacity. Whereas both “absent” vision (congenital blindness) and “perfect” vision (“supernormal” vision) may be associated with a decreased risk for schizophrenia, the model suggests that the risk for developing schizophrenia increases from both ends of the visual capacity continuum toward a “peak risk” (Landgraf et al., 2012; Silverstein et al., 2013). The location of this peak risk has yet to be determined experimentally. However, the peak has important implications for the understanding of the etiology, development, and therapy of the disorder: individuals suffering from visual impairment (located to the left of the peak), who never developed “normal” vision, may reduce their risk for developing schizophrenia through a decline in visual capacity. Individuals suffering from visual deterioration (located to the right of the peak), who previously had “normal” visual skills, may reduce their risk for developing schizophrenia through an improvement in visual capacity. Note that the model does not make a concrete assumption on the association between vision capacity and risk for schizophrenia (linear, exponential, etc.). Instead, we suggest that extensive longitudinal and epidemiological investigations, also controlling for age-related visual capacity decline (Ofan and Zohary, 2007; Cattaneo et al., 2008), are necessary to elaborate this issue. In this context, visual capacity may comprise but is not limited to measures of visual acuity (near/far), sensitivity to light, motion, and color, visual field size, and stereoscopic vision. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first model that uses a continuous (more vs. less psychotic) rather than a categorical (“normal” vs. “mentally disordered”) approach to represent psychiatrically relevant human behavior. Abbreviations: PaSZ = Protection-Against-Schizophrenia; SZ = schizophrenia; Prodromals = individuals identified at ultra-high risk for developing schizophrenia; 1st episode patients = patients with schizophrenia that have had one (identified) psychotic episode; Chronic patients = patients with schizophrenia that have had at least three (identified) psychotic episodes.

Current models of developmental trajectories of schizophrenia emphasize the prognostic value of subclinical disease expressions. In his inspirational Nature article, National Institute of Mental Health Director Insel (2010) describes

“psychosis as a late, potentially preventable stage of the illness” (p. 187).

Thus, current diagnostic criteria and etiological models of the disease may be indicative of only ultimate disease stages. Even more problematic, the prognostic or prodromal]1 criteria of schizophrenia are still being defined in terms of diagnostic symptomatology, i.e., sub-threshold phenotypic expressions of actual full-blown psychotic episodes (Klosterkotter et al., 2001; McGorry et al., 2003; Yung et al., 2005). Currently, to be identified as prodromal for an impending psychosis, individuals need to present one of the following three criteria: (1) attenuated (sub-threshold) psychotic symptoms, (2) BLIPS or brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms (lasting less than 1 week and disappearing spontaneously), and (3) functional deterioration in the presence of vulnerability (they must suffer from schizotypal personality disorder or have a first-degree relative who has psychosis) (McGorry et al., 2003; Miller et al., 2003; Yung et al., 2005). Other descriptions of prodromal individuals, such as basic symptoms (Moyer and Landauer, 1967; Gross, 1969, 1989; Klosterkotter et al., 1997, 2001) by which early and late prodromal phases are distinguished (Niendam et al., 2007; Schultze-Lutter et al., 2007; Simon et al., 2007),2 also presume symptom continuity along with the progression of the disease. The (scientifically) recognized stages of schizophrenia include, in fact, the prodrome, the first-episode, and the stabilized chronic syndrome. Problematically, current early detection and intervention are based on the theoretical yet unproven assumption that symptoms of schizophrenia extend continuously from the prodrome to the stabilized syndrome (Gross, 1969, 1989; Klosterkotter et al., 1997, 2001; Simon et al., 2006; Niendam et al., 2007; Schultze-Lutter et al., 2007; McGorry, 2010). Consequently, prodromal approaches have not yielded high predictive power in their ability to identify transitions to full-blown psychosis (Gottesman and Erlenmeyer-Kimling, 2001; Miller et al., 2002; McGorry et al., 2003; Yung et al., 2003, 2004, 2005; Haroun et al., 2006; Olsen and Rosenbaum, 2006a,b). In their recent review, Gee and Cannon (2011) found that only about one third of prodromal individuals eventually convert to psychosis. Two thirds remain symptomatic at a sub-threshold level or recover completely. Hence, we and other researchers argue that current diagnostic criteria (APA, 2000) systematically underestimate the individuality of symptoms (Andreasen, 2007; Nelson et al., 2008), neglecting, for example, the fact that psychoses are often experienced in phases that include self-disturbances (Parnas et al., 1998; Sass and Parnas, 2001, 2003) and visuo-cognitive impairments (Elvevag and Goldberg, 2000; Andreasen and Black, 2006; Keefe and Harvey, 2012; Landgraf et al., 2012). This means, first, that prodromal extrapolations of current diagnostic criteria become less valid or less useful the earlier the prognostic verdict is acquired. Second, there may be underlying factors that are not included in the diagnostic criteria of schizophrenia and treatment options; nevertheless, these factors may be crucial for understanding the ontogenetic development and etiology of the disorder. Thus, there is still an imperative need for stage-specific disease progression markers to precisely predict deterioration and the functional outcome. One promising factor for this kind of progression marker is visually mediated cognition.

Goals of the Review

Since the introduction of the term “Schizophrenia” by Bleuler (1908), a plethora of scientific articles have stressed the importance of vision aberrations in schizophrenia. By contrast, “absent” and “perfect” vision have, to the best of our knowledge, been associated with a lower risk for developing schizophrenia. Hence, visual information acquisition and/or processing at some point in life appears to be necessary but not sufficient for the human brain to develop psychosis. Only disturbed visual processing may be sufficient. We therefore present the idea of the continuous PaSZ model, for which we review two vision-related indicators of the etiology and development of the disease.

In the first part of this paper – the blindness perspective – we review cognitive alterations with regard to real-world mental representations, neurofunctional reorganization, and multisensory integration in congenitally blind individuals. We assess how these altered functions provide insight into the developmental causes of schizophrenia and the protection against it.

In the second part – the vision perspective – we provide evidence for how abnormalities in the visual system can lead to schizophrenia. Specifically, we show that visual information acquisition and processing deficits progress along with the progression of the disease. We describe how perceptual abnormalities contribute to higher-order cognitive dysfunctions and subsequently lead to diagnostic phenotypic expressions (symptoms).

Finally, in the third part – the therapeutic perspective – we review interventional implications of the PaSZ model. Specifically, we show how both an increase in visual impairment and a decrease in visual deterioration may contribute to lowering a person's risk for schizophrenia.

The Blindness Perspective on Schizophrenia

A Whole World Without Vision

Understanding blindness may shed light on the nature and etiological causes of schizophrenia. Specifically, the ways in which blind individuals perceive and mentally represent the world may hold the key to identifying vision-specific mediations of schizophrenia. Some information can be perceived by only one modality; for example, vision is needed to perceive stars in the sky or hue, and audition is needed to perceive pitch. More importantly, amodal world representations have been proposed to have fundamental importance for the development and expression of schizophrenia (Andreasen, 2007; Fletcher and Frith, 2009; Insel, 2010). Interestingly, since the famous example of William Molyneux's letter to John Locke in 1688, some people have questioned whether or not blindness affects amodal world representations:

“Suppose a man born blind, and now adult, and then taught by his touch to distinguish between a cube and a sphere of the same metal, and the same bigness, so as to tell, when he felt one and the other, which is the cube, which is the sphere. Suppose then, the cube and the sphere placed on a table, and the blind man to be made to see. Query, whether by sight, before he touched them, he could distinguish, and tell, which is the globe, which is the cube?” (Degenaar, 1996)

Almost two and a half centuries later, Von Senden (1932) studied patients in whom blindness was surgically cured. These patients could localize but not discriminate between a cube and a sphere immediately after the operation. However, because other researchers have refuted Von Senden's results (Gregory and Wallace, 1963; Morgan, 1977; Hollins, 1989; Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011) and rapid cross-modal transfer indicates that the underlying mental representations may be rather similar in nature (Held et al., 2011), an ultimate conclusion to the question is still missing.

According to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) (WHO, 2007), visual impairment is defined as 6/18–3/60 of the visual acuity of an unimpaired sighted individual. Being blind entails less than 3/60 of a sighted person's visual acuity or a central visual field of less than 10°. Whereas 161–259 million individuals suffer from visual impairment worldwide, 37–42 million individuals of the world's population fulfill ICD-10's criteria for blindness (Resnikoff et al., 2004; Dandona and Dandona, 2006). About 1.4 million of them are younger than 15 years; 30 million are older than 50. In contrast to individuals with schizophrenia, only 12% of all blind individuals live in developed countries. The most common causes of blindness are cataracts, glaucoma, and age-related macular degeneration (Resnikoff et al., 2004).

Schizophrenia affects approximately 70–80 million individuals worldwide, independent of cultural background, ethnicity, or social status (Andreasen and Black, 2006). Men and women are similarly affected (Markowitch, 1997). Given the prevalence estimates of both diseases, at least 0.00605% of the world's population, or approximately 450,000 individuals should suffer from both blindness and schizophrenia. Whereas it is important to keep in mind that the absence of proof (of individuals suffering from both conditions simultaneously) is not proof of absence, the protection mechanism appears specific to blindness (e.g., there are congenitally deaf individuals who become psychotic) and to schizophrenia (e.g., individuals are not protected against other psychiatric diseases) (Silverstein et al., 2013). To account for the lack of individuals who meet the diagnostic criteria for both congenital blindness and schizophrenia, Sanders et al. (2002, 2003) have suggested that dynamic adaptations of NMDA (N-Methyl-D-Aspartic) receptor channels in the visual cortex may account for cognitive functioning that is insusceptible to psychosis in the blind. However, this point of view may be incomplete because functional reorganization in visually deprived individuals also occurs in brain areas other than the visual information processing sites of sighted individuals (Weeks et al., 2000; Burton et al., 2006; Silverstein and Keane, 2011b). Reorganization, thus, depends on brain maturation (Kujala et al., 2000) and the functional interaction of subsystems in the developing brain (Striem-Amit et al., 2012a).

Silverstein and colleagues have a different point of view. According to these authors, cognitive coordination is impaired in patients with schizophrenia due to impaired NMDA ion receptor flow (Phillips and Silverstein, 2003; Silverstein and Keane, 2011a). Because occipital (perceptual) and non-occipital (cognitive) activity is required for cognitive organization to occur, early AND late visual processing deficits are associated with the progression of the disease and altered cognitive performance. Psychotic symptoms are, therefore, closely related to disturbed visual information acquisition and processing, which may, in turn, result in brain networks and functionality that are susceptible to psychosis. Stated differently, experiencing the world without vision may be qualitatively similar to but functionally different from experiencing the world as a sighted individual. We consequently consider the following questions in this first part of the review: (i) What evidence is there that blind individuals cognitively represent and experience the world similarly to sighted individuals? (ii) What changes in neurofunctional organization and multisensory integration are necessary for blind individuals to experience the world similarly to sighted individuals? (iii) And most decisively for the present review, how do these changes contribute to PaSZ?

“Blind” Perception and Cognition

On the one hand, there is no doubt that non-visual perceptual advantages are present in blind individuals (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011). Congenitally blind individuals have shown superior performance compared to sighted controls, for example, in auditory sound localization and speech discrimination tasks (Muchnik et al., 1991; Lessard et al., 1998; Roder et al., 1999; Kujala et al., 2000) as well as in haptic two-point discrimination tasks (Roder and Neville, 2003). Interestingly, cognitive and behavioral development can be delayed to up to 2 years in congenitally blind children compared to sighted ones (Warren, 1994). This may be due to the fact that visual information is memorized, discriminated, and explored more quickly than auditory or haptic information. In fact, auditory and haptic memory is limited due to serial information channeling; auditory discrimination and tactile exploration is limited due to simultaneous information channeling. This means that specific attention-orienting and stimulus-awareness mechanisms are necessary for visual but not other perceptual domains (Posner et al., 1976; Phillips and Silverstein, 2003). These mechanisms, in turn, have been described as relevant for the development of schizophrenia (Butler et al., 2005, 2007; Johnson et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2005; Haenschel et al., 2007).

On the other hand, amodal mental representations, e.g., how the idea of a “tree” is represented in the brain, are independent of their perceptual source (Avraamides et al., 2004; Barsalou, 2008). In other words, no matter which modality this information is perceived through (e.g., audition, touch, vision), the ultimate mental representations are similar. And this is true for real-world mental representations of blind and sighted individuals as Cattaneo and Vecchi (2011) put it so eloquently:

“Our brain, indeed, doesn't need our eyes to ‘see’…” (p. 3).

In line with this argument, Pascual-Leone and Hamilton (2001) proposed that specialized neural functionality is formed as a consequence of receptive field input to brain structures. Hence, there may be no a priori functional segregation of the human brain. Instead, the existence of small receptive perceptual fields, which are required for quick categorization, and large receptive fields, which are necessary to coordinate thought processes, result in the specialization of cortical regions for processing “visual” or “auditory” information. The authors refer to the metamodal brain as a “mixture of expert architecture” (p. 15) (Pascual-Leone and Hamilton, 2001) and argue that sighted and congenitally blind individuals have an equivalence of amodal representations.

A plethora of investigations have, in fact, provided evidence of this equivalence. First, higher-order cognitive abilities, and spatial abilities in particular, have shown no differences between congenitally blind and sighted individuals, for example, in mental scanning and rotation (Craig, 1973; Marmor and Zaback, 1976; Carpenter and Eisenberg, 1978; Kerr, 1983; Zimler and Keenan, 1983; Vecchi et al., 2004) or allocentric referencing tasks (Tinti et al., 2006). Investigations of the mental number line (Moyer and Landauer, 1967; Dehaene et al., 1999; Krueger et al., 2008, 2011; Landgraf et al., 2010b) have shown similar spatial representations in congenitally blind and healthy sighted individuals (Castronovo and Seron, 2007). Hence, in congenitally blind individuals, equivalent amodal (spatial) representations can apparently be formed from perceptual cues other than vision. Interestingly, in this same experiment, congenitally blind individuals were able to classify the numbers one and two more quickly than sighted individuals. The authors attributed this effect to the serial processing of tactile and auditory information. Blind individuals may be more accustomed to the idea of distinguishing perceptual phenomena with regard to number counting, especially one and two (e.g., counting steps).

Second, whereas a lack of visual input is detrimental to some tasks, the superior performance of sighted individuals has been shown to disappear when visual task demands are increased. For example, in a 3-D working memory task, congenitally blind individuals performed as well as sighted individuals when information input exceeded visual information processing capacities (Cornoldi et al., 1991). Furthermore, in matching standard figures and line drawings, congenitally blind and late blind individuals did not differ in their reaction times and error rates (Heller and Kennedy, 1990). The authors interpreted their results as indicating that visual imagery and visual experience appear unnecessary for tactile perspective taking. Interestingly, Vecchi et al. (2004) reported that the memory of locations decreased with an increasing number of visuo-spatial (VS) representations that had to be held in working memory. This shows that higher task demands, such as speed, interactive images, and movement, may account for performance decreases in blind individuals. Remarkably, the performance of schizophrenia patients also declines when task demands are increased, indicating a coping mechanism for imprecise visual information acquisition (Landgraf et al., 2011a,b).

Third, it has been proposed that visual input deprivation may lead not only to perceptual enhancement in congenitally blind individuals (Rauschecker, 1995; Lessard et al., 1998; Roder et al., 1999) but also to improved attentional capacities (Cattaneo et al., 2008). Better performance in higher-order cognitive abilities has, in fact, been observed in various memory span (Tillman and Bashaw, 1968; Smits and Mommers, 1976; Pozar, 1982; Hull and Mason, 1995; Roder et al., 2001; Amedi et al., 2003; Roder and Rosler, 2003; Raz et al., 2007) and auditory attention tasks (Roder et al., 1996, 1999, 2001; Roder and Rosler, 2003). Van Velzen et al. (2006) investigated early and late attention indicators in congenitally blind and sighted individuals with electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings. The authors found that early but not late attention modulation could be elicited in blind individuals, indicating that late cognitive coordination mechanisms may be important for the development of psychosis. Late attention indicators also depended heavily on having the individual focus on task-relevant external spatial reference frames, an ability that has been compromised in patients with schizophrenia (Dreben et al., 1995; Parnas et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2005; Cavezian et al., 2007; Coleman et al., 2009; Landgraf et al., 2011b). In fact, patients with chronic schizophrenia have been found to display a deficit in the disengagement and reorientation of attention (Posner et al., 1988; Daban et al., 2004; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al., 2007; Kebir et al., 2008, 2010) even without medication (Amado et al., 2009). Hence, the interaction between bottom-up and top-down processes may provide insight into the immunity against psychosis that blind individuals appear to have (see the Protective Mechanism “Cognition” in Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Protection-against-schizophrenia (PaSZ) – contributions from congenital blindness.

Neurofunctional Reorganization and Compensation

The ability to behaviorally compensate for visual deprivation is another indication that there are equivalent amodal representations in sighted and congenitally blind individuals (Pascual-Leone and Hamilton, 2001; Knauff and May, 2006; Cattaneo et al., 2008). However, because up to 35% of neocortical functioning in sighted humans is devoted to visual information processing (Gilbert and Walsh, 2004), blind and sighted individuals may differ considerably in their neurofunctional processing. In sighted individuals, cortical activity has traditionally been ascribed to region-specific functionality (Kanwisher, 2010), such as visual information processing being ascribed to occipital activity. However, blindfolding sighted individuals for a 5-days period has been found to lead to behaviorally relevant neurofunctional changes (Kauffman et al., 2002) that mimick the supranormal auditory performance of blind individuals (Rauschecker, 1995; Lessard et al., 1998; Roder et al., 1999). This not only implies that neurofunctional reorganization occurs in the adult human brain (Kujala et al., 2000), but also indicates that cortical functionality may be determined by information processing necessities (Cohen et al., 1997) and innate pathways (Striem-Amit et al., 2012c) rather than brain regions.

Blind individuals have been found to employ compensatory neurofunctional strategies to overcome visual information deprivation. Using positron emission tomography (PET), Sadato et al. (1996, 1998) were the first to find a relation between activity in the occipital cortex and non-visual perception and cognition. Specifically, the authors found that during braille reading and non-braille haptic tasks, the primary, and medial occipital lobes were activated in individuals who became blind both early and late in life. In sighted individuals, on the other hand, these haptic tasks were associated with activity in non-occipital regions, such as the bilateral inferior parietal lobes, as well as the left primary sensorimotor area, insula, and prefrontal regions. In subsequent studies, congenitally blind individuals have shown recruitment of the occipital cortex during higher linguistic and (Cohen et al., 1997; Burton et al., 2003, 2006; Gilbert and Walsh, 2004; Raz et al., 2005; Amedi et al., 2007) auditory motion processing (Poirier et al., 2006), as well as during the localization of auditory signals (Weeks et al., 2000), tactile processing (Sadato et al., 2002), and tongue stimulation (Kupers et al., 2006). Finally, even sighted individuals have shown non-visual information processing in visual areas. In a sophisticated transcranial magnet stimulation (TMS) paradigm, Lewald et al. (2004) demonstrated that the temporary disruption of occipital activity can deteriorate auditory localization in sighted individuals.

These results led us to postulate the following two assumptions: first, neurofunctional plasticity in congenitally blind individuals includes reorganization in non-visual cortical areas, which has been confirmed, for example, in lingual and posterior fusiform gyri (Smith and Gasser, 2005; Striem-Amit et al., 2012a). Therefore, the reorganization of non-visual cortical areas in patients with schizophrenia may be important for the development of the disorder. Second, non-visual information processing in the occipital lobe of blind individuals resembles visual information processing in the occipital lobes of sighted individuals (Burton et al., 2010). Thus, whereas functional and most likely also structural processing mechanisms are similar between blind and sighted individuals, the contents of the information (visual vs. non-visual) are different.

This has direct consequences for the development of schizophrenia (see also the Protective Mechanism “Neurofunctioning” in Table 1). If brain structures are not utilized according to their functional and structural specificity, this could result in psychosis or at least in subclinical symptoms. For example, as mentioned above, depriving sighted individuals of their vision for 5 days can lead to neurofunctional and cognitive changes (Kauffman et al., 2002). This means that not only eye-related dysfunction but also functional changes in the primary and secondary occipital cortex can lead to hallucinations (Kazui et al., 2009; Schadlu et al., 2009). Further, individuals suffering from the so-called Charles Bonnet Syndrom (CBS) have reported visual hallucinations as a consequence of visual deterioration (ffytche and Howard, 1999; ffytche, 2009). Hallucinations due to the CBS can vary widely, comprising abstract geometric patterns, mosaic vision (tessellopsia), increased color vision (hyperchomatopsia), and miniturizations and magnifications of objects (micropsia and macropsia), and can occur for several minutes (70%), seconds (18%), or hours (12%) (Hughes, 2013). There are two main theories about how CBS develops. The release theory claims that a mixture of impaired and unimpaired neuronal signals from the visual cortex lead to hallucinatory interpretations in the higher-order association cortices. This is similar to our argument that schizophrenia patients can profit from giving more weight to visual information in multisensory integration tasks. Hence vision training may be supplementarily useful to cognitive remediation programs. On the other hand, the deprivation theory of CBS argues that reduced sensory input may result in spontaneous image production in the visual association cortex, thus leading to visual hallucinations. This implies, as indicated by the PaSZ model, that perceptual deprivation per se is not sufficient for psychosis protection to occur. Instead, functional and possibly structural cortical reorganization need to be taken into consideration to avoid psychosis. In fact, there is strong evidence that brain regions are not adequately utilized in schizophrenia because patients use more sequential information processing strategies (Fatemi and Folsom, 2009; Landgraf et al., 2011a,c) possibly due to impaired higher-order cognitive deficits, i.e., in single- or multisensory integration (Park and Holzman, 1992; Park et al., 2002; Tek et al., 2002; Landgraf et al., 2008; Fuller et al., 2009).

Multisensory Integration

The integration of information from multiple modalities has been found to improve performance compared to using one single perceptual channel alone (Calvert et al., 2003). In fact, multisensory integration allows the brain to generate a coherent amodal view of the self and the world (Smith and Gasser, 2005). Supramodal information, such as spatial and temporal information, is coded by all human perceptual systems and allows the comparison of multisensory integration between, for example, blind and sighted individuals. For the scope of this review, we restrict our considerations of multisensory integration to temporal and spatial processes that involve haptic, auditory, and, for sighted individuals, visual capacities. For a review of olfactory and gustatory perception regarding visual and auditory multisensory integration, please refer to Walla (2008) or Zampini and Spence (2012).

Efficient multisensory integration remaps information into amodal representations. In the absence of visual input, this remapping may develop differently (see Protective Mechanism “Multisensory Integration” in Table 1) (Hotting et al., 2004; Roder et al., 2004; Wallace et al., 2004). For example, in congenitally blind individuals, judging the temporal order of tactile stimuli with the right or left hand is not affected by whether the hands are in normal or crossed-over positions. By contrast, sighted individuals show longer reaction times when their hands are crossed, implying interference between visual and tactile external frames of reference (Roder et al., 2004, 2007; Collignon et al., 2007). Hotting et al. (2004) reported that the influence of task-irrelevant auditory tones for tactile discrimination is stronger in sighted than in congenitally blind individuals. Interestingly, humans deprived of vision between 5 and 24 months after birth due to retinal cataracts showed less auditory-visual interference and integration later in life than normally developing sighted individuals (Putzar et al., 2007). In line with these observations, the brain regions responsible for visual imagery (e.g., the fusiform face area) may retain their functional specificity even decades after the onset of blindness in late blind individuals (Goyal et al., 2006). In fact, there may be a specific critical period during which neurofunctional plasticity to sensory loss is maximal (Sathian, 2005). Until now, the duration of that time window has been unclear, but it has been proposed to be between 10 and 14 years for visual information processing (Cohen et al., 1999; Ofan and Zohary, 2007), implying that this may be a critical period for cortical changes regarding the protection and development of schizophrenia as well. Although changes in cortical functioning might not occur until several years after sensory deprivation (Cattaneo et al., 2008), the overall evidence implies that ontogenetic development must occur to establish the multi-sensory integration interferences observed in sighted individuals. Therefore, it can be hypothesized that this development may be impaired in patients with schizophrenia, thus leading to impaired or unusual performances in multisensory integration tasks.

In sighted individuals, auditory information processing is heavily influenced by other modalities and prior knowledge. Alain and Arnott (2000) distinguished between auditory attention (allocation of attentional resources to perceptual objects), auditory object discrimination (perception of sound attributes across a certain time period), and auditory event perception (perception of sound at a particular time, in a certain place, and having specific characteristics). The authors argue that auditory information quality impacts cognitive performance in all of these stages. For example, degradation of auditory information influences comprehension and memory differently in younger and older listeners (Pichora-Fuller and Singh, 2006). Furthermore, the characteristics of speakers are processed in parallel with semantic information. Whereas female voices are associated with more extraversion and openness, male voices are associated with higher emotional stability and greater agreeableness (Imhof, 2010). This is in line with other studies that have shown that identical semantic information is interpreted differently depending on whether the person is perceived as male or female (Addington, 1968; Knapp and Hall, 2002). Hence, auditory processing is inextricably linked to non-auditory information, thus implying that the working memory demands of listeners include the stream of the sound, semantic cues, and perceptual voice cues. Patients with schizophrenia have been shown to struggle when asked to integrate these perceptual cues with other cues (Hardoy et al., 2004; Leitman et al., 2005; Butler et al., 2008a, 2009). Interestingly, integrating non-auditory and auditory information directly from perceptual cues can be tested in multi-sensory integration tasks. For example, clustering task-irrelevant stimuli should not affect the performance of blind individuals if multisensory integration occurs in a manner that is similar to what has been observed in healthy individuals (Alain and Arnott, 2000).

Another auditory phenomenon is the so-called “right ear advantage,” which refers to the observation that auditory information is processed with greater ease (more quickly, less erroneously) by the right ear compared to the left. In fact, it has been observed that left frontal lobe lesions diminish the advantage of the right ear (Hugdahl et al., 2003), indicating a functional preference for auditory information processing in the left frontal lobe. Congenitally blind individuals have shown a less pronounced right ear advantage, thus outperforming sighted individuals in, for example, dichotic listening tasks (Hugdahl et al., 2004; Castronovo and Seron, 2007). It may be interesting to investigate whether patients with schizophrenia would perform like sighted or blind individuals in this paradigm. If schizophrenia patients' performance more closely resembles the performance of sighted individuals, this would be an indication of their local processing preference. If, however, the patients' performance was more like that of blind individuals, this may be attributable to a lower reliance on multisensory integration.

Congenitally blind individuals have been found to rely less on multisensory integration, thus leading to strategic compensatory behavior. On the one hand, they recognize haptic stimuli more quickly than sighted individuals, due to, for example, more efficient sensory information processing during the first 100 ms after stimulus onset (Roder et al., 1996). On the other hand, however, if blind individuals were to employ an adequate multisensory integration strategy, they might perform equally as well as sighted individuals. In a haptic mental imagery memory task, Cornoldi et al. (2009) showed that congenitally blind individuals performed worse than healthy controls only when they adopted a spatial strategy but not when they adopted a verbal strategy. Mental representations of size are more erroneously influenced by multisensory information in sighted than in blind individuals (Bartley et al., 1955; Bolles and Bailey, 1956). Smith et al. (2005) showed that size estimations of real objects are less error-prone in congenitally blind individuals compared to sighted controls. Instead, blind individuals rely more on a haptic memory strategy to perform these tasks. This implies that multisensory integration involving touch depends on task characteristics, and a lack of visual input can be compensated for by adequate strategies (Postma et al., 2007). In fact, perceptual and long-term memory may be used to encounter multisensory integration deficits in blind individuals. Possibly patients with schizophrenia may profit from this strategy as well.

It is noteworthy that multimodal integration has been found to change with age. Warren and Pick (1970) found that auditory localization is strongly influenced by visual perceptual input in sighted adults but not in sighted children. Furthermore, auditory-proprioceptive integrations may be influenced by the increasing importance of vision with increasing age in sighted individuals. Pitch direction changes are better discriminated by blind individuals than by sighted controls even when the stimuli are presented 10 times more quickly but only when individuals became blind at an early age (Gougoux et al., 2004). Age at the onset of blindness has been shown to regulate the use of visual reference frames in haptic and auditory perception (Roder et al., 2004, 2007). However, these effects may also be attributable to the fact that blind individuals show better skills in processing unimodal perceptual inputs (Hotting and Roder, 2004; Hotting et al., 2004). More importantly, for congenitally blind individuals, if no change was observed across time in these functions, the weighing of intermodal information could be assumed to follow different developmental trajectories in blind and sighted individuals. Regarding schizophrenia, patients may start out with already impaired preconditions in their multi-sensory integration abilities. Hence, visual information in patients with schizophrenia may not be given enough multisensory integration weight. Instead, in their attempt to compensate perceptually, schizophrenia patients would integrate more perceptual information from other modalities than sighted controls, possibly leading to their preference for local information processing (Landgraf et al., 2011b).

The Vision Perspective on Schizophrenia

The Vision-Space-Body-and-Cognitive-Identity Model of Schizophrenia

Thus far, we have argued that the inability to perceive and process visual information prevents congenitally blind individuals from becoming psychotic. Specifically, we have shown that PaSZ in congenitally blind individuals may be associated with the acquisition of information and reorganizations of processing at cognitive, neurofunctional, and multisensory levels of integration. It appears that more than “zero” vision AND impaired visual processing must occur simultaneously in order to render the human brain susceptible to psychosis. This determines the visual impairment part of the PaSZ model (left side of the curve in Figure 1).

In our laboratory, we have developed a model that shows the processing levels at which the deteriorations in visual capacity contribute to the development of psychosis. The ViSBI, short for Vision-Space-Body-and-cognitive-Identity, model of schizophrenia (Landgraf et al., 2012) shows that deficits in the acquisition and processing of visuo-cognitive information increase along with the progression of the disorder and are, indeed, associated with higher-order cognitive dysfunctions and expression of symptoms. We now turn our attention to “Biomoehrchen schmecken gut” the corresponding visual deterioration part of the PaSZ model (right side of the curve in Figure 1).

Visual Information Acquisition

Visual information acquisition accounts for more than 80% of the perceptual input in humans and is therefore considered to provide the foundation for amodal world representations (Lüer et al., 1988; Becker, 1991; Leigh and Zee, 1999). Deficits in visual information acquisition have been described extensively in patients with schizophrenia (see “Vision” complex in Figure 2). Deficits deteriorate along the continuum from the prodrome to the full-blown syndrome, i.e., with the progression of the disease. One of the longest and best-studied types of deteriorations is that of oculomotor deficits. They not only jeopardize the availability of visual information but also impair the spatial and temporal accuracy of individuals. Hence, they are crucial for the formation of concepts of the self and the surrounding world.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The Progressive Vision-Space-Body-and-Cognitive-Identity (ViSBI) model of schizophrenia. Source: Reprinted with kind permission from Bentham Science Publishers. Note. The progressive ViSBI model stresses vision-related deteriorations from prodrome to chronic syndrome that may lead to schizophrenia. The model comprises four complexes: “Vision,” “Space,” “Body,” and “cognitive Identity. ”The “Vision,” “Space,” and “Body” complexes are reviewed in the sections on “The Vision Perspective on Schizophrenia“ in the current paper. The “Cognitive Identity” complex (shadowed in gray) is hypothesized as a part of the model (see Landgraf et al., 2012). Complexes are sub-divided into specific research areas (e.g., in the “Vision” complex: oculomotricity and scanning) and disease progression status (prodromal, first episode, and chronic schizophrenia patients). Tasks listed in each row indicate deficient performance of the corresponding patient group. Some areas have not been investigated in schizophrenia patients (“suggestions for further research”). The “Visual Input” triangle in the middle indicates that (congenital) blindness may prevent these mechanisms from taking place because visual perceptual input is required for this pattern of aberrations to occur. Abbreviations: prodromal = individuals identified at ultra-high risk for developing schizophrenia; first episode = patients with schizophrenia that have had one (identified) psychotic episode; chronic = patients with schizophrenia that have had at least three (identified) psychotic episodes; SPEM = smooth pursuit eye movements; n-back = spatial n-back task.

Oculomotor deficits are least pronounced in prodromal individuals. Only a few studies have demonstrated that prodromal individuals show an increased rate of errors in the antisaccade paradigm (Nieman et al., 2007). In first-episode schizophrenia patients, deficits have been described with regard to antisaccade errors, reaction time, and accuracy (Broerse et al., 2001; Ettinger et al., 2004; Hutton et al., 2004). Furthermore, deficits such as a higher error rate in memory-guided saccades and lower acuity in predictive saccades have been reported (Krebs et al., 2001; Hutton et al., 2004; Keedy et al., 2006). In chronic patients, all these deficits have been observed (e.g., Karoumi et al., 2001; Brownstein et al., 2003; Reuter et al., 2006; Radant et al., 2007; Amado et al., 2008; Landgraf et al., 2008). In addition, chronic patients have shown lower acuity in memory-guided saccades (Crawford et al., 1995a; Park et al., 1995; McDowell and Clementz, 1996; Radant et al., 1997; Karoumi et al., 1998; McDowell et al., 2001). Although there is some debate about the usefulness of oculomotor deficits in schizophrenia research (e.g., Calkins and Iacono, 2000; Brownstein et al., 2003; Calkins et al., 2004, 2008; Levy et al., 2004, 2008; Boudet et al., 2005; Heydebrand, 2006), deficits have been identified as schizophrenia spectrum markers (Amador et al., 1991; Sweeney et al., 1994; Faraone et al., 1995; McDowell and Clementz, 1997; Rosenberg et al., 1997; Avila et al., 2002; Kathmann et al., 2003), independent of clinical state (Calkins et al., 2003; Kallimani et al., 2009), or medication (Crawford et al., 1995b; Muller et al., 1999). Furthermore, oculomotor aberrations have been found to fulfill some endophenotypic characteristics (Gottesman and Gould, 2003; Calkins et al., 2008).

Another interesting phenomenon regarding the acquisition of visual information is eye-movement strategies. Eye-movement strategies allow for a meaningful and useful succession of visual information input with regard to, for example, goal attainment or planning behavior (Land and Furneaux, 1997). Deficits in eye-movement strategies may lead to inefficient visual input and may thus alter an individual's perception of the world. As seen in oculomotor deficits, the severity of eye-movement-strategy deficits increases with the increasing progression and severity of schizophrenia. Prodromal individuals have rarely been studied in visual scanning paradigms. Koethe et al. (2006, 2009) showed that abnormal binocular depth inversion was specific to prodromal individuals. Furthermore, first-episode schizophrenia patients have shown an abnormal clustering of fixations and shorter visual scanpaths when scanning faces, landscapes, and abstract patterns (Benson et al., 2007). The largest deteriorations have been reported for stabilized chronic patients with schizophrenia, with fewer fixations, shorter scanpaths, narrower clustering of fixations, and avoidance of predefined features in different visual scanning tasks (e.g., Gaebel et al., 1987; Gordon et al., 1992; Kurachi et al., 1994; Phillips and David, 1997; Williams et al., 1999; Loughland et al., 2002, 2004; Minassian et al., 2005; Koethe et al., 2006).

In our own laboratory, we found that, in contrast to healthy controls, chronic schizophrenia patients do not adapt their eye-movement strategies to task demands but instead use similar strategies regardless of task difficulty (Landgraf et al., 2011b). In other words, patients employ the same visual scanning strategy no matter whether the task is easy or difficult. In another study, we even found that chronic schizophrenia patients use a less efficient information acquisition strategy more often than healthy controls (Landgraf et al., 2011a). This implies that inefficient visual information acquisition impedes patients from obtaining (task-) relevant information. Patients have probably noticed that inflexible visual scanning suits most of the demands they encounter in their daily lives. One intriguing question concerning the discussion of blindness and schizophrenia is whether patients are unable to develop other strategies from birth on or whether patients develop the use of only one strategy despite being able to develop others.

These findings imply the following. First, patients with schizophrenia attribute less weight to visual information and are not able to integrate visual information as well as healthy sighted individuals. This means that patients' deficient performances on higher-order cognitive tasks, therefore, can be accounted for by lower-level visual aberrations. Second, patients' visual channels are still “open.” Hence, they are unable to obtain cognitive and neurofunctional reorganization in the same way as blind individuals. This suggests that PaSZ may be available in two directions. On the one hand, if patients learn to put more weight on visual information in the same way that healthy sighted individuals do, they may lower their risk for a psychotic episode. On the other hand, if patients are taught to put less weight on visual information and reorganize their neurofunctional information processing, they might be able to stabilize their self and world perceptions in the same way that blind individuals do (See “Vision Training: Decline vs. Improvement” for further details on vision training).

Visual Information Processing

Aberrant visual information processing (see “Space” complex in Figure 2) deteriorates thought processes, contributing to cognitive dysfunctions. It has been acknowledged that cognitive deficits may be one of the best predictors of the development of psychosis (Elvevag and Goldberg, 2000; Insel, 2010; Keefe and Harvey, 2012), and this link could provide the basis for quantitative stage markers of the disease. Most of the paradigms used in schizophrenia cognition research are visual tasks. Moreover, fundamental associations between visuo-spatial (VS) abilities and oculomotor capacities (Leigh and Zee, 1999; Hutton et al., 2004; Lawrence et al., 2004; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2005; Milea et al., 2007) indicate that disturbances in the VS domain may stem from oculomotor dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia. Some of the cognitive domains deficient in patients with schizophrenia include but are not limited to VS memory (Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Green et al., 2000; Piskulic et al., 2007; Landgraf et al., 2011c), VS attention (Posner et al., 1988; Danckert et al., 2004; Granholm and Verney, 2004; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al., 2007), and VS executive functions (Laws, 1999; Eisenberg and Berman, 2010; Landgraf et al., 2011a,b,c). An impressive number of studies have actually shown that the development of schizophrenia may be associated with these cognitive deficits and that these cognitive deficits could be stage-specific (Heaton et al., 1994; Niendam et al., 2006; Fusar-Poli et al., 2007; Lysaker et al., 2007; Langdon and Ward, 2008; Picard et al., 2009; Barlati et al., 2012).

Visuo-spatial cognitive deficits in prodromal individuals are controversial. Some studies have shown a greater number of errors and inferior performance in prodromal individuals compared to healthy controls on spatial delayed-response tasks (Wood et al., 2003; Bartok et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2006; Kimhy et al., 2007; Nieman et al., 2007). Other investigations have not confirmed this observation (Brewer et al., 2005; Lencz et al., 2006; Niendam et al., 2006; Pukrop et al., 2007). The reasons for this controversy have not yet been resolved. Possibly, heterogeneous results are due to imprecise diagnoses of the schizophrenia prodrome (high rate of false negatives), demographic differences between participating groups, or task simplicity (Wood et al., 2003; Conklin et al., 2005; Longevialle-Henin et al., 2005; Fusar-Poli et al., 2007; Pukrop and Klosterkotter, 2010). Nevertheless, prodromal individuals have been consistently reported to display deficient performances on the Trail Making Tests (TMT) A and B and the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-R) visual reproduction task parts I and II (Hawkins et al., 2004; Brewer et al., 2005).

First-episode patients have shown deficient performances on a number of VS cognitive tasks. For example, they have shown aberrant memory on VS delayed-response tasks (Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Park et al., 1995; Rybakowski and Borkowska, 2002; Simon et al., 2007), Gestalt perception (Parnas et al., 2001), and the TMT (Rybakowski and Borkowska, 2002; Simon et al., 2007). This implies moderate deficits in these patients.

Chronic patients have shown the strongest aberrations on VS tasks. Their deficits include abnormal performance on delayed-response tasks (Park and Holzman, 1992; Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Glahn et al., 2003; Park et al., 2003; Saperstein et al., 2006; Genderson et al., 2007), figure search (Longevialle-Henin et al., 2005), mental rotation (de Vignemont et al., 2006; Halari et al., 2006), Gestalt perception (O'Donnell et al., 1996; Parnas et al., 2001; Cavezian et al., 2007; Kimhy et al., 2007), spatial span (Cannon et al., 2000; Perry et al., 2001; Manoach et al., 2005; Genderson et al., 2007; Thoma et al., 2007), 3-D real-world navigation Daniel et al. (2007), and referencing (Landgraf et al., 2010a; Mazhari et al., 2010). According to a meta-analysis, there is an overall effect size of −1.00 regarding VS working memory deficits in chronic schizophrenia patients (Piskulic et al., 2007), and these deficits are independent of gender differences (Albus et al., 1997; Reichenberg et al., 2002; Voglmaier et al., 2005; Halari et al., 2006; Wolitzky et al., 2006). Hence, visual information processing appears to be deteriorated in schizophrenia, increases with the progression of the disease, and is related to basic visual acquisition. It may be interesting to compare patients' performance to the performance of the blind in non-visual versions of these paradigms. Even though they have never been able to integrate visual information, blind individuals should outperform patients with schizophrenia on spatial tasks. This would indicate that schizophrenia patients' severely altered visual information processing is strongly associated with their deficits in visual information acquisition. We hypothesize that patients with schizophrenia could especially profit from the neurofunctional and cognitive reorganization observed in blind individuals. The most important question, however, for the consideration of PaSZ is whether or not VS deficits are associated with higher-order cognitive dysfunctions and the symptoms of schizophrenia.

From Visual Deterioration to Symptoms

Basic visual acquisition and processing dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia suggest a relation to phenotypic symptomatology. In other words, someone who is not able to obtain accurate and precise (visual) information cannot process this information adequately and thus may suffer from deficient amodal representations of the world. Scientifically, visual scanning deficits and oculomotor deteriorations are related to higher-order cognitive dysfunctions (e.g., theory of mind, perspective taking) and social cognition (Adolphs, 2003; Amodio and Frith, 2006; Kluwe-Schiavon et al., 2013), as well as to functional outcomes in schizophrenia (Green et al., 2000; Benson et al., 2007). In fact, the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), involved in perspective taking and theory of mind, and the insular cortex, involved in body-related multisensory integrations (Arzy et al., 2006; Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Danckert and Ferber, 2006; Schwabe and Blanke, 2007; Mitchell, 2008), have been associated with psychotic states (Penfield, 1955; Blanke et al., 2005; Vercammen et al., 2010), and impaired whole body and body-part processing in patients with schizophrenia (Tan et al., 2006; Butler et al., 2008b; Suchan, 2008). It has been hypothesized that the failure to predict the sensory consequences of motor commands (Frith et al., 1992, 2000a,b; Friston and Frith, 1995; Blakemore et al., 2002; Frith, 2005) and the improper planning of motor sequences (Delevoye-Turrell et al., 2003; Coello and Delevoye-Turrell, 2007; Voss et al., 2010; Waters and Badcock, 2010) are both essential to schizophrenia symptoms. Furthermore, motor-related deficits in chronic and first-episode patients with schizophrenia include an altered pattern of self-recognition, for example, in the rubber hand illusion (Franck et al., 2001; Versmissen et al., 2007), the inability to distinguish between the self and others (Schwabe and Blanke, 2007; Ebisch et al., 2012), and the identification of the source of self- or externally generated movements (de Vignemont et al., 2006). This implies, on the one hand, that symptom-related deficits in schizophrenia encompass a cognitive component (Bowins, 2011). On the other hand, these results point toward the critical role of multisensory integration deficits in patients (Friston and Frith, 1995; Fourneret et al., 2002) (see “Body” complex in Figure 2).

Because multisensoriality and amodality, as well as self and world representations have already been discussed for blind individuals, there are two things to note. First, the neural sites for multisensory integration may differ between patients with schizophrenia and blind individuals. This would mean that dissimilar neurofunctional processes are conducted. These processes and sites may represent good candidates for early detection markers and possible interventions for schizophrenia. Second, blind individuals and patients with schizophrenia have something in common regarding multisensory integration: they assign less weight to visual information. Blind individuals do so due to the absence of visual input; patients with schizophrenia may do so because they have deficits in visual information acquisition and processing. However, the underlying neurofunctional processes differ and may be an indication of the protective mechanisms of blindness. Interestingly, there is a strong association between oculomotor function and multisensory integration (for reviews, see, e.g., Previc, 1998; Milner and Goodale, 2006). Dysfunctions of visual information acquisition and processing have actually been found to be correlated with multisensory integration deficits (Park and Holzman, 1993; Ross et al., 1998; Jansen et al., 2002; Nieman et al., 2007; Picard et al., 2009) and symptomatology (Gaebel et al., 1987; Lencz et al., 2003; Semerari et al., 2003; Varga et al., 2007) in schizophrenia patients. Moreover, whereas cross-modal influences are dominated by visual information in patients (de Gelder et al., 2005), there is strong evidence that multisensory integration is compromised in chronic patients (Vrtunski et al., 1993; Marvel et al., 2004; Picard et al., 2008; Van den Stock et al., 2011; Castagna et al., 2012). Multisensory facilitation that is established potentially to compensate for deficient unisensory processing in patients with schizophrenia (Javitt, 2009; Williams et al., 2010; Stone et al., 2011) may need to be altered with regard to its reliance on visual information processing (de Gelder et al., 2005). Subsequently, patients might benefit from similar cognitive and perceptual protection against the disease as observed in blind individuals. The degree to which the neglect of visual information integration overlaps between blind and schizophrenic patients in multisensory integration may be an indicator of the severity of schizophrenia. We hypothesize that a greater degree of overlap would indicate a less severe schizophrenia outcome.

The Therapeutic Perspective on Schizophrenia

Continuous Diagnostic Criteria

In the previous section, we demonstrated that deteriorations in the acquisition and processing of visual information increase the risk for developing schizophrenia. We established a critical relation between lower- as well as higher-order visual deteriorations and symptom expression in schizophrenia. Whereas fundamental oculomotor and strategic eye-movement deficits may impact the acquisition of visual information, information processing deficits point toward VS cognitive aberrations. Both visual information acquisition and processing dysfunctions have been found to increase in severity with the progression of the disorder and are correlated with symptomatic expressions of the disease.

The PaSZ model postulates a continuous relation between visual capacity and the risk of developing schizophrenia. This means that looking at disturbances in vision from a disease progression perspective obviates the need for symptom-based prodromal criteria. Instead, because cognitive deficits can be depicted on much more fine-grained continua than psychotic symptoms (Saperstein et al., 2006; Uhlhaas and Mishara, 2007), we argue that a graded stage model of schizophrenia based on visual functioning will contribute to more reliable diagnostic and especially prodromal criteria for schizophrenia. More importantly, however, are the therapeutic implications of the model, which we will discuss in this final section of the review.

Vision Training: Decline VS. Improvement

So far, we have presented evidence for why the relative risk of developing schizophrenia is allegedly zero for congenitally blind individuals and for individuals with supernormal visual capacities. According to the PaSZ model, the risk of developing schizophrenia increases from both ends of the visual capacity continuum (congenital blindness and “supernormal” vision) toward a “peak risk” (Figure 1). Thus, depending on the person's initial visual capacity, a decrease in visual impairment or an increase in visual deterioration may similarly elevate the risk of developing schizophrenia. Consequently, therapeutic efforts may be differentially effective: a decline in a person's visual capacity (increase of visual impairment) may be more beneficial for an individual with visual impairment who never had “normal” visual skills in the first place. By contrast, an improvement in a person's visual capacity (decrease of visual deterioration) may be more beneficial for an individual with visual deterioration, that is, who had at some point developed “normal” vision. However, the difference between those two therapeutic approaches may not be clear cut as there is, until now, no clear agreement about (1) how much aberrations of visual functioning corresponds to the highest risk for schizophrenia and (2) whether or not an increase in visual impairment (e.g., via sensory substitution) actually corresponds to a decline in visual functioning.

Nevertheless, vision improvement training is indispensable for PaSZ when the affected individual suffers from visual deterioration. Regarding information acquisition (see “Acquisition of visual information” in Table 2), patients should be visually trained to obtain the necessary task-relevant visual information and should be given the tools needed to interpret this information. This means that it may be beneficial to train individuals to utilize task-specific and successful eye-movement strategies. Patients should be taught to direct their attention (eye movements) and cognitive resources (pupil dilation, neurofunctionality) to task-relevant visual information: to look where the information is, to avoid information overload, and to access the “big picture” instead of focusing on attention-captivating details (Johnson et al., 2005; Longevialle-Henin et al., 2005; Cavezian et al., 2007; Coleman et al., 2009; Landgraf et al., 2011b). In addition, training should establish a link between how much weight is given to visual and non-visual information in multisensory integration tasks. Patients should learn when it is advantageous to rely on visual information (fast, parallel processing) and when it is advantageous to rely on non-visual information (slow, sequential processing). This may help them to build more reliable amodal representations of the world, to orient themselves better, and to avoid confusion.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. “Protection-against-schizophrenia” (PaSZ) – contributions from visual information acquisition and processing.

Interestingly, visual deterioration has been associated with the risk for criminal behavior (Bachara and Zaba, 1978; Zinkus and Gottlieb, 1978; Lane, 1980; McKay and Brumback, 1980; Broder et al., 1981; Clack, 1990), one of the strongest indicators of a severe course (Steinert, 1998; Nedopil, 2007; Hutton et al., 2012), outcome (Leygraf, 1988; Haller et al., 2001; Soyka and Morhart-Klute, 2002; Soyka et al., 2002; Fazel et al., 2009; Nitschke et al., 2011; Kooyman et al., 2012), and relapse (Soyka et al., 2004; Witt et al., 2013) in schizophrenia. Specifically, it was argued that in very young children and juveniles, perceptual deficits lead to a higher rate of learning disabilities, specifically reading problems. These problems, in turn, may exclude children from further participating in social interactions and, subsequently, could lead to frustration and feelings of exclusion. It was concluded that visual deterioration in children facilitates the development of delinquent and criminal behavior (Slaton and Jorgensen, 1958; Dzik, 1966, 1975). Vision training was established to counteract this problem and, in fact, it has been found to be effective for reducing criminal behavior and recidivism (Berman, 1989). Because (i) schizophrenia is strongly associated with criminal behavior AND with visual deterioration and (ii) vision training may reduce criminal behavior, the implementation of vision training may be effective for reducing outcome severity in schizophrenia.

Vision training regarding information processing (see “Processing of visual information” in Table 2) may be achieved by functional reorganization. Patients can be taught to utilize different neurofunctional pathways via neurofeedback. Neurofeedback from functional real-time MRI can be used to regulate one's own brain activity, as has been shown predominantly for affective disorders (Linden et al., 2012; Micoulaud-Franchi et al., 2012) but also for the dopamine system (Sulzer et al., 2013) and in Tourette's syndrome (Messerotti Benvenuti et al., 2011). Patients with schizophrenia may learn to avoid certain brain structures associated with hallucinative experiences, such as the Insula and the TPJ. Instead, activation patterns from blind individuals could be mimicked, that is, patients could be taught to activate the occipital lobe for non-visual information processing. Moreover, lower- and also higher-order visual information processing may be a target for neurofeedback training in patients with schizophrenia.

Producing declines in a person's visual capacity may be a more radical but also a more effective intervention method especially when the affected individual is suffering from visual impairment. An interesting approach here entails sensory substitution devices for patients with schizophrenia. Intriguingly, vision-deprived individuals can learn to behave in a manner similar to sighted individuals, that is, they may perceive depth, localization, and distance information in real-time from non-visual cues. In a series of inspiring reports, Amedi and colleagues as well as other research groups have shown that visual information can be transmitted via non-visual cues, thus allowing blind individuals to perceive and construct a 3-D image of their environment. The vision-deprived individual obtains VS information auditorily, that is, s/he hears visual information. Studies have shown that this type of sensory substitution is easily learned by blindfolded and blind individuals (Amedi et al., 2007; Collignon et al., 2007; Reich et al., 2012). With regard to the etiological considerations of schizophrenia, one interesting research question would be whether or not teaching blind individuals “to see” with sensory substitution devices could circumvent the protective mechanisms of congenital blindness against schizophrenia. This means that if individuals who have acquired the ability to decode VS information from non-visual (e.g., auditory) cues did not develop schizophrenia, cortical reorganization would contribute significantly to blind individuals' immunity against psychosis. However, if this was not the case, modality-unspecific world representations would be more crucial. Sensory substitution devices could then be used for patients with schizophrenia to induce neurofunctional compensation and reorganization for impaired (and possibly deteriorated) visual capacities, accordingly.

The fact that functional reorganization due to sensory deprivation is age-specific is important for all vision trainings (Cohen et al., 1999; Sathian, 2005; Ofan and Zohary, 2007; Cattaneo et al., 2008). For example, independent of sensory input or visual experience, certain brain areas maintain stimulus selectivity (Striem-Amit et al., 2012b), indicating that the human brain has intrinsic constraints with regard to functional plasticity (Striem-Amit et al., 2012d). These constraints must be taken into account when implementing vision training programs.

Finally, visual capacity and schizophrenia show gender effects. Interestingly, disease-related cognitive deficits are not different for men and women, implying that VS capacities do not affect patients' cognitive deficits (Albus et al., 1997; Halari et al., 2006; Wolitzky et al., 2006; Landgraf et al., 2010a). Nevertheless, there are some distinctions regarding the clinical trajectories of men and women who suffer from schizophrenia. Women are diagnosed at an average age of 26.5 years, whereas men are diagnosed earlier at around 21.4 years (Markowitch, 1997). Furthermore, comorbid substance abuse is more frequent in men than in women (Ochoa et al., 2012). Only 7% of female patients with schizophrenia compared to 22% of male patients are convicted of a crime after being discharged from the forensic facility (Soyka et al., 2004). Demo-graphically, 55% of all female forensic patients are single, 15% are married, and 25% are divorced (Melzer, 2001). By contrast, the vast majority of male forensic patients are single (85%), and only 15% are married or divorced (Leygraf, 1988; Nowara, 1993). Suicide rates differ between male and female patients, and men more often successfully commit suicide than women (Markowitch, 1997). When researchers have examined the continuity perspective of schizophrenia, they have rarely taken these gender differences into consideration.

Conclusion

The ability to see appears necessary but not sufficient for schizophrenia symptoms to develop. The PaSZ model provides a continuous measure for assessing the risk of schizophrenia. It suggests that, first, “absent” and “perfect” vision are associated with a lower risk of developing the disorder. Second, there is a peak in schizophrenia risk where visual capacity disturbances are “ideal” for the development of psychosis. Third, both declines AND improvements in visual functioning may improve PaSZ depending on the visual capacities (impaired, deteriorated) of the affected individuals. We argue that the understanding of the causes of schizophrenia and its development can be derived from a continuous vision-based model. And in this review, we presented evidence for this point of view from the “blindness” and the “vision” perspective, ultimately deriving interventional recommendations.

In the “blindness” part, we provided clues about what protects visually impaired, that is, congenitally blind individuals from psychosis. While blind individuals have cognitive experiences that are similar to sighted individuals, they show alterations in cognition (attentional capacities, inhibition of task-irrelevant stimuli, serial processing, strategic adaptation), neurofunctioning (amodal representations, information processing reorganization), and multisensory integration (interference, lateralization, temporal integration, imagery). In fact, these considerations raise the question of how much visual information processing is actually necessary for a person to become vulnerable to psychosis. Or, in other words, how little visual impairment is still protective against manifesting psychotic episodes. Future studies should investigate this and other questions regarding candidates for schizophrenia-specific developmental trajectories. In line with these assumptions, we already hypothesized in a former work that tasks that tap into multisensory integration and full-body motor and navigational control may improve discrimination rates between different disease stages (Landgraf et al., 2012). Nevertheless, individual differences may also contribute to qualitative and quantitative alterations in cognitive functioning regarding blind, vision impaired, and sighted schizophrenic patients (Heller and Kennedy, 1990; Cornoldi et al., 1991; Andreasen and Black, 2006; Andreasen, 2007; Cattaneo et al., 2008).

In the “vision” part, we demonstrated that deteriorated vision, in the form of disturbed visual information acquisition and processing, can lead to the development of schizophrenia symptoms. Specifically, we showed that basic functions, such as oculomotor control and strategic eye movements, are disturbed. Moreover, visuo-cognitive aberrations appear to be based on these deficits, thus resulting in a pattern that leads to motor and self-perception disturbances. These considerations allowed us to pinpoint the etiological underpinnings of schizophrenia because they show that deteriorated visual information acquisition and processing may contribute to the establishment of higher-order cognitive dysfunctions and subsequent symptoms. From a developmental point of view it could be argued that visual capacities may never develop normally in individuals with schizophrenia. If this was the case, the PaSZ model predicts that the majority (if not all) of patients with schizophrenia would be found on the “visual impairment” rather than the “visual deterioration” side. In addition, future research should identify vision- and cognition-related disease stages and determine the time point at which (or the time period in which) interventions may be most important and most effective.

In the “therapeutic” part, we proposed interventional strategies that resulted from the “blindness” and the “vision” perspectives. Decisively, declines in visual capacity (impairment) and improvements in visual capacity (deterioration) may increase PaSZ. We formulated treatment options, including vision training, with regard to visual information acquisition (scanning for task-relevant information, weighing information) and processing (neurofunctional sites, sensory substitution). In fact, cortical functional reorganization appears to be most crucial for successful interventions and may be induced by neurofeedback or sensory substitution. Hence, whereas patients would undoubtedly profit from being trained to improve and put more weight on visual functioning, we argue that learning how to decline and put less weight on visual functioning – the way (congenitally) blind individuals do – may lead to an even stronger protection against psychosis.

In this context, it has to be kept in mind that not all patients suffering from schizophrenia show visual dysfunctions and not all individuals with visual dysfunctions develop schizophrenia. On the one hand, this could be due to the fact that the PaSZ model may only apply to a specific subgroup of patients/individuals. For example, the influence of monocular vision, neurofunctional compensatory mechanisms, and genetic effects need to be investigated in the future. Further, it would be interesting to characterize specific subgroups along etiological dimensions. On the other hand, however, this may also indicate that the current diagnostic (and prodromal) criteria for schizophrenia are too coarse in order to dissociate between different etiological factors. Hence the PaSZ model may provide a fine-grained tool that assesses psychotic symptomatology on the basis of developmental trajectories in much greater detail than current diagnostic procedures. This would result in greater diagnostic precision and, in turn, better therapeutic assignments for affected individuals.

Overall, this review stresses that although the ability to see makes us human (Hegel, 1807; Darwin, 1859; Plato, 380 BC; Wittgenstein, 1921), it also precludes our “PaSZ.” In his “Metaphor of the Sun,” the great Greek philosopher Plato lets his teacher, Socrates, argue that the most important of all senses, vision, determines our ways of thinking and how we experience the world (Halfwassen, 2006). Hence, our task of understanding the contribution of the visual system to psychosis will not only eventually lead us to be better able to predict, diagnose, and heal one of the most devastating mental disorders. It will also increase our understanding of how visual functioning influences our ways of thinking and, therefore, our mere existence as human beings.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Christine M. Urbanski inspires this work. The paper profited from conversations with Elke van der Meer and the SCHAM group. The authors are deeply indebted to the members of the Department of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychotherapy in Regensburg, as well as to the three reviewers for their constructive comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. Inka Bauer, Jonathan Moreno, and Jane Zagorski helped with language editing. Finally, the authors would like to openly thank the journal's guest editor, Steven Silverstein, for providing us with this fruitful opportunity of inviting and accepting our paper.

Footnotes

1 ^For intelligibility, we use the term “prodromal” to refer to prodromal, ultra-high risk (UHR), or other patients assumed to experience signs of an impending psychotic episode.

2 ^Substantial overlap exists between the different approaches (Simon et al., 2006).

References

Addington, D. W. (1968). Voice and Perception of Personality: An Experimental Study. Stillwater: Oklahoma State University.

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178. doi:10.1038/nrn1056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alain, C., and Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Front. Biosci. 5:D202–D212. doi:10.2741/Alain

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Albus, M., Hubmann, W., Mohr, F., Scherer, J., Sobizack, N., Franz, U., et al. (1997). Are there gender differences in neuropsychological performance in patients with first-episode schizophrenia? Schizophr. Res. 28, 39–50. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00091-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amado, I., Bourdel, M. C., Daban, C., Poirier, M. F., Loo, H., Bouhours, P., et al. (2009). Preattentional processes and disorganization in schizophrenia: influence of a 6-week risperidone treatment. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 1107–1112. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amado, I., Landgraf, S., Bourdel, M. C., Leonardi, S., and Krebs, M. O. (2008). Predictive saccades are impaired in biological nonpsychotic siblings of schizophrenia patients. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 33, 17–22.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Amador, X. F., Sackeim, H. A., Mukherjee, S., Halperin, R., Neeley, P., Maclin, E., et al. (1991). Specificity of smooth pursuit eye movement and visual fixation abnormalities in schizophrenia. Comparison to mania and normal controls. Schizophr. Res. 5, 135–144. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(91)90040-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, R., and Zohary, E. (2003). Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 758–766. doi:10.1038/nn1072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amedi, A., Stern, W. M., Camprodon, J. A., Bermpohl, F., Merabet, L., Rotman, S., et al. (2007). Shape conveyed by visual-to-auditory sen-sory substitution activates the lateral occipital complex. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 687–689. doi:10.1038/nn1912

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amodio, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277. doi:10.1038/nrn1884

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andreasen, N. C. (2007). DSM and the death of phenomenology in America: an example of unintended consequences. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 108–112. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andreasen, N. C., and Black, D. W. (2006). Introductory Textbook of Psychiatry, 4th Edn. New York: American Psychiatric Publishing.

APA. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Arzy, S., Thut, G., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., and Blanke, O. (2006). Neural basis of embodiment: distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area. J. Neurosci. 26, 8074–8081. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0745-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avila, M. T., McMahon, R. P., Elliott, A. R., and Thaker, G. K. (2002). Neurophysiological markers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: sensitivity and specificity of specific quantitative eye movement measures. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111, 259–267. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avraamides, M. N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., and Golledge, R. G. (2004). Functional equivalence of spatial representations derived from vision and language: evidence from allocentric judgments. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 30, 804–814.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bachara, G. H., and Zaba, J. N. (1978). Learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency. J. Learn Disabil. 11, 58–62. doi:10.1177/002221947801100409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barlati, S., De Peri, L., Deste, G., Fusar-Poli, P., and Vita, A. (2012). Cognitive remediation in the early course of schizophrenia: a critical review. Curr. Pharm. Des. 18, 534–541. doi:10.2174/138161212799316091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartley, S. H., Clifford, L. T., and Calvin, A. D. (1955). Effect of visual imagery on tactual and kinesthetic space perception. Percept. Mot. Skills 5, 177–184. doi:10.2466/pms.1955.5.3.177

CrossRef Full Text

Bartok, E., Berecz, R., Glaub, T., and Degrell, I. (2005). Cognitive functions in prepsychotic patients. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 29, 621–625. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Becker, W. (1991). “Saccades,” in Eye Movements, ed. R. H. S. Carpenter (Basingstoke: McMillan Press), 95–137.

Benson, P. J., Leonards, U., Lothian, R. M., St Clair, D. M., and Merlo, M. C. (2007). Visual scan paths in first-episode schizophrenia and cannabis-induced psychosis. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 32, 267–274.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Berman, M. S. (1989). Vision care in a juvenile detention facility. Optom. Vis. Sci. 66, 23–25. doi:10.1097/00006324-198901000-00009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 6, 237–242. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. J. Neurosci. 25, 550–557. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bleuler, E. (1908). Die prognose der dementia praecox (Schizophre-niegruppe). Allg. Zeitschrift Psychiatrie. 65, 436–464.

Bolles, R. C., and Bailey, D. E. (1956). Importance of object recognition in size constancy. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 222–225. doi:10.1037/h0048080

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boudet, C., Bocca, M. L., Chabot, B., Delamillieure, P., Brazo, P., Denise, P., et al. (2005). Are eye movement abnormalities indicators of genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry 20, 339–345. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2004.12.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bowins, B. (2011). A cognitive regulatory control model of schizophrenia. Brain Res. Bull. 85, 36–41. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brewer, W. J., Francey, S. M., Wood, S. J., Jackson, H. J., Pantelis, C., Phillips, L. J., et al. (2005). Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 71–78. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.71

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Broder, P. K., Dunivant, N., Smith, E. C., and Sutton, L. P. (1981). Further observations on the link between learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency. J. Educ. Psychol. 73, 838–850. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.73.6.838

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Broerse, A., Crawford, T. J., and den Boer, J. A. (2001). Parsing cognition in schizophrenia using saccadic eye movements: a selective overview. Neuropsychologia 39, 742–756. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00155-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brownstein, J., Krastoshevsky, O., McCollum, C., Kundamal, S., Matthysse, S., Holzman, P. S., et al. (2003). Antisaccade performance is abnormal in schizophrenia patients but not in their biological relatives. Schizophr. Res. 63, 13–25. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00438-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burton, H., Diamond, J. B., and McDermott, K. B. (2003). Dissociating cortical regions activated by semantic and phonological tasks: a FMRI study in blind and sighted people. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1965–1982. doi:10.1152/jn.00279.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burton, H., McLaren, D. G., and Sinclair, R. J. (2006). Reading embossed capital letters: an fMRI study in blind and sighted individuals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 325–339. doi:10.1002/hbm.20188

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burton, H., Sinclair, R. J., and Dixit, S. (2010). Working memory for vibro-tactile frequencies: comparison of cortical activity in blind and sighted individuals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1686–1701. doi:10.1002/hbm.20966

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Butler, P. D., Abeles, I. Y., Weiskopf, N. G., Tambini, A., Jalbrzikowski, M., Legatt, M. E., et al. (2009). Sensory contributions to impaired emotion processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 1095–1107. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Butler, P. D., Martinez, A., Foxe, J. J., Kim, D., Zemon, V., Silipo, G., et al. (2007). Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain 130, 417–430. doi:10.1093/brain/awl233

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Butler, P. D., Silverstein, S. M., and Dakin, S. C. (2008a). Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 40–47. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Butler, P. D., Tambini, A., Yovel, G., Jalbrzikowski, M., Ziwich, R., Silipo, G., et al. (2008b). What's in a face? Effects of stimulus duration and inversion on face processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 103, 283–292. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Butler, P. D., Zemon, V., Schechter, I., Saperstein, A. M., Hoptman, M. J., Lim, K. O., et al. (2005). Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 495–504. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.495

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calkins, M. E., Curtis, C. E., Iacono, W. G., and Grove, W. M. (2004). Antisaccade performance is impaired in medically and psychiatrically healthy biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 71, 167–178. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calkins, M. E., and Iacono, W. G. (2000). Eye movement dysfunction in schizophrenia: a heritable characteristic for enhancing phenotype definition. Am. J. Med. Genet. 97, 72–76. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(200021)97:1<72::AID-AJMG10>3.0.CO;2-L

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calkins, M. E., Iacono, W. G., and Curtis, C. E. (2003). Smooth pursuit and antisaccade performance evidence trait stability in schizophrenia patients and their relatives. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 49, 139–146. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00101-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calkins, M. E., Iacono, W. G., and Ones, D. S. (2008). Eye movement dysfunction in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analytic evaluation of candidate endophenotypes. Brain Cogn. 68, 436–461. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calvert, C., Spence, C., and Stein, B. (2003). The Handbook of Multisensory Processes. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Cannon, T. D., Huttunen, M. O., Lonnqvist, J., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Pirkola, T., Glahn, D., et al. (2000). The inheritance of neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 67, 369–382. doi:10.1086/303006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carpenter, P. A., and Eisenberg, P. (1978). Mental rotation and the frame of reference in blind and sighted individuals. Percept. Psychophys. 23, 117–124. doi:10.3758/BF03208291

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castagna, F., Montemagni, C., Maria Milani, A., Rocca, G., Rocca, P., Casacchia, M., et al. (2012). Prosody recognition and audiovisual emotion matching in schizophrenia: the contribution of cognition and psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 205, 192–198. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castronovo, J., and Seron, X. (2007). Semantic numerical representation in blind subjects: the role of vision in the spatial format of the mental number line. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 60, 101–119. doi:10.1080/17470210600598635

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cattaneo, Z., and Vecchi, T. (2011). Blind Vision. London: The MIT Press.

Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Cornoldi, C., Mammarella, I., Bonino, D., Ricciardi, E., et al. (2008). Imagery and spatial processes in blindness and visual impairment. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1346–1360. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583. doi:10.1093/brain/awl004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavezian, C., Danckert, J., Lerond, J., Dalery, J., d'Amato, T., and Saoud, M. (2007). Visual-perceptual abilities in healthy controls, depressed patients, and schizophrenia patients. Brain Cogn. 64, 257–264. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clack, D. (1990). Learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency: a link? J. Learn Disabil. 23, 266. doi:10.1177/002221949002300501

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coello, Y., and Delevoye-Turrell, Y. (2007). Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 667–683. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, L. G., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, A., Corwell, B., Falz, L., Dambrosia, J., et al. (1997). Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature 389, 180–183. doi:10.1038/39792

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, L. G., Weeks, R. A., Sadato, N., Celnik, P., Ishii, K., and Hallett, M. (1999). Period of susceptibility for cross-modal plasticity in the blind. Ann. Neurol. 45, 451–460. doi:10.1002/1531-8249(199904)45:4<451::AID-ANA6>3.0.CO;2-B

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coleman, M. J., Cestnick, L., Kras-toshevsky, O., Krause, V., Huang, Z., Mendell, N. R., et al. (2009). Schizophrenia patients show deficits in shifts of attention to different levels of global-local stimuli: evidence for magnocellular dysfunction. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 1108–1116. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp090

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Collignon, O., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., Bastien, D., and Veraart, C. (2007). Functional cerebral reorganization for auditory spatial processing and auditory substitution of vision in early blind subjects. Cereb. Cortex 17, 457–465. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj162

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Conklin, H. M., Curtis, C. E., Calkins, M. E., and Iacono, W. G. (2005). Working memory functioning in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: cognitive functioning shedding light on etiology. Neuropsychologia 43, 930–942. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cornoldi, C., Cortesi, A., and Preti, D. (1991). Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people. Mem. Cognit. 19, 459–468. doi:10.3758/BF03199569

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cornoldi, C., Tinti, C., Mammarella, I. C., Re, A. M., and Varotto, D. (2009). Memory for an imagined pathway and strategy effects in sighted and in totally congenitally blind individuals. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 130, 11–16. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Craig, E. M. (1973). Role of mental imagery in free recall of deaf, blind, and normal sunjects. J. Exp. Psychol. 97, 249–253. doi:10.1037/h0034007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crawford, T. J., Haeger, B., Ken-nard, C., Reveley, M. A., and Henderson, L. (1995a). Saccadic abnormalities in psychotic patients. I. Neuroleptic-free psychotic patients. Psychol. Med. 25, 461–471. doi:10.1017/S0033291700033389

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crawford, T. J., Haeger, B., Ken-nard, C., Reveley, M. A., and Henderson, L. (1995b). Saccadic abnormalities in psychotic patients. II. The role of neuroleptic treatment. Psychol. Med. 25, 473–483. doi:10.1017/S0033291700033390

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daban, C., Krebs, M. O., Bourdel, M. C., Willard, D., Loo, H., Olie, J. P., et al. (2004). Effects of atypical neuroleptics on alertness and visual orienting in stabilized schizophrenic patients: a preliminary study. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 7, 255–263. doi:10.1017/S1461145704004250

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Danckert, J., and Ferber, S. (2006). Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia 44, 987–1006. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Danckert, J., Saoud, M., and Maruff, P. (2004). Attention, motor control and motor imagery in schizophrenia: implications for the role of the parietal cortex. Schizophr. Res. 70, 241–261. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dandona, L., and Dandona, R. (2006). What is the global burden of visual impairment? BMC Med. 4:6. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-4-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daniel, M.-P., Dibo-Cohen, C. M., Carité, L., Boyer, P., and Denis, M. (2007). Dysfunction of spatial cognition in schizophrenic patients. Spat. Cogn. Comput. 7, 287–309. doi:10.1080/13875860701608368

CrossRef Full Text

Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.

de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., de Jong, S. J., Masthoff, E. D., Trompenaars, F. J., and Hodiamont, P. (2005). Multisensory integration of emotional faces and voices in schizophrenics. Schizophr. Res. 72, 195–203. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Vignemont, F., Zalla, T., Posada, A., Louvegnez, A., Koenig, O., Georgieff, N., et al. (2006). Mental rotation in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 15, 295–309. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Degenaar, M. (1996). Molyneux's Problem: Three Centuries of Discussion on the Perception of Forms. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science 284, 970–974. doi:10.1126/science.284.5416.970

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delevoye-Turrell, Y., Giersch, A., and Danion, J. M. (2003). Abnormal sequencing of motor actions in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from grip force adjustments during object manipulation. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 134–141. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dreben, E. K., Fryer, J. H., and McNair, D. M. (1995). Perceptual and conceptual information processing in schizophrenia and depression. Percept. Mot. Skills 80, 447–465. doi:10.2466/pms.1995.80.2.447

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dzik, D. (1966). Vision and the juvenile delinquent. J. Am. Optom. Assoc. 37, 461–468.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dzik, D. (1975). Optometric intervention in the control of juvenile delinquents. J. Am. Optom. Assoc. 46, 629–634.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ebisch, S. J., Salone, A., Ferri, F., De Berardis, D., Romani, G. L., Ferro, F. M., et al. (2012). Out of touch with reality? Social perception in first-episode schizophrenia. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 394–403. doi:10.1093/scan/nss012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eisenberg, D. P., and Berman, K. F. (2010). Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 258–277. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elvevag, B., and Goldberg, T. E. (2000). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 14, 1–21.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ettinger, U., Kumari, V., Crawford, T. J., Corr, P. J., Das, M., Zachariah, E., et al. (2004). Smooth pursuit and antisaccade eye movements in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 38, 177–184. doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00105-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Faraone, S. V., Kremen, W. S., Lyons, M. J., Pepple, J. R., Seidman, L. J., and Tsuang, M. T. (1995). Diagnostic accuracy and linkage analysis: how useful are schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 1286–1290.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fatemi, S. H., and Folsom, T. D. (2009). The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, revisited. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 528–548. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn187

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fazel, S., Gulati, G., Linsell, L., Geddes, J. R., and Grann, M. (2009). Schizophrenia and violence: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 6:e1000120. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000120

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

ffytche, D. H. (2009). Visual hallucinations in eye disease. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 22, 28–35. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e32831f1b3f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

ffytche, D. H., and Howard, R. J. (1999). The perceptual consequences of visual loss: ‘positive’ pathologies of vision. Brain 122(Pt7), 1247–1260. doi:10.1093/brain/122.7.1247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fletcher, P. C., and Frith, C. D. (2009). Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 48–58. doi:10.1038/nrn2536

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fourneret, P., de Vignemont, F., Franck, N., Slachevsky, A., Dubois, B., and Jeannerod, M. (2002). Perception of self-generated action in schizophrenia. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 7, 139–156. doi:10.1080/13546800143000212

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Daléry, J., d'Amato, T., et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 454–459. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.454

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome. Clin. Neurosci. 3, 89–97.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Frith, C. D. (2005). The self in action: lessons from delusions of control. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 752–770. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000a). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 355, 1771–1788. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0734

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000b). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 357–363. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00052-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frith, C. D., Friston, K. J., Liddle, P. F., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1992). PET imaging and cognition in schizophrenia. J. R. Soc. Med. 85, 222–224.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fuller, R. L., Luck, S. J., Braun, E. L., Robinson, B. M., McMahon, R. P., and Gold, J. M. (2009). Impaired visual working memory consolidation in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 23, 71–80. doi:10.1037/a0013854

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fusar-Poli, P., Perez, J., Broome, M., Borgwardt, S., Placentino, A., Caverzasi, E., et al. (2007). Neurofunctional correlates of vulnerability to psychosis: a systematic review and meta analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 465–484. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaebel, W., Ulrich, G., and Frick, K. (1987). Visuomotor performance of schizophrenic patients and normal controls in a picture viewing task. Biol. Psychiatry 22, 1227–1237. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(87)90030-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gee, D. G., and Cannon, T. D. (2011). Prediction of conversion to psychosis: review and future directions. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 33(Suppl. 2), s129–s142.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Genderson, M. R., Dickinson, D., Diaz-Asper, C. M., Egan, M. F., Weinberger, D. R., and Goldberg, T. E. (2007). Factor analysis of neurocognitive tests in a large sample of schizophrenic probands, their siblings, and healthy controls. Schizophr. Res. 94, 231–239. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gilbert, S. J., and Walsh, V. (2004). Vision: the versatile ‘visual’ cortex. Curr. Biol. 14, R1056–R1057. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glahn, D. C., Therman, S., Manninen, M., Huttunen, M., Kaprio, J., Lonnqvist, J., et al. (2003). Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 624–626. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01641-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 6, 348–357.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gordon, E., Coyle, S., Anderson, J., Healey, P., Cordaro, J., Latimer, C., et al. (1992). Eye movement response to a facial stimulus in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 626–629. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(92)90251-T

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottesman, I. I., and Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (2001). Family and twin strategies as a head start in defining prodromes and endophenotypes for hypothetical early-interventions in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 51, 93–102. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00245-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottesman, I. I., and Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 636–645. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gougoux, F., Lepore, F., Lassonde, M., Voss, P., Zatorre, R. J., and Belin, P. (2004). Pitch discrimination in the early blind. Nature 430, 309. doi:10.1038/430309a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Balke, M., Hajsamou, S., Ruhrmann, S., Schultze-Lutter, F., Daumann, J., et al. (2007). Orienting of attention in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia, prodromal subjects and healthy relatives. Schizophr. Res. 97, 35–42. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goyal, M. S., Hansen, P. J., and Blakemore, C. B. (2006). Tactile perception recruits functionally related visual areas in the late-blind. Neuroreport 17, 1381–1384. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000227990.23046.fe

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Granholm, E., and Verney, S. P. (2004). Pupillary responses and attentional allocation problems on the backward masking task in schizophrenia. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 52, 37–51. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., Braff, D. L., and Mintz, J. (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”. Schizophr. Bull. 26, 119–136. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gregory, R. L., and Wallace, J. (1963). Recovery from Early Blindness: A Case Study. Cambridge, UK: Heffer and Sons.

Gross, G. (1969). “Prodrome und vorposten syndrome schizophrener Erkrankungen,” in Schizophrenie und Zyklothymie, ed. G. Huber (Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag), 177–187.

Gross, G. (1989). The ‘basic’ symptoms of schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry. Suppl. 7, 21–25; discussion 37–40.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Haenschel, C., Bittner, R. A., Haertling, F., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Maurer, K., Singer, W., et al. (2007). Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 1229–1240. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1229

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halari, R., Mehrotra, R., Sharma, T., Ng, V., and Kumari, V. (2006). Cognitive impairment but preservation of sexual dimorphism in cognitive abilities in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 141, 129–139. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halfwassen, J. (2006). Der Aufstiegzum Einen [The Ascent to the One]. Leipzig: De Gruyter.

Haller, R., Kemmler, G., Kocsis, E., Maetzler, W., Prunlechner, R., and Hinterhuber, H. (2001). Schizophrenia and violence. Results of a comprehensive inquiry in an Austrian providence. Nervenarzt 72, 859–866. doi:10.1007/s001150170020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hardoy, M. C., Carta, M. G., Catena, M., Hardoy, M. J., Cadeddu, M., Dell'Osso, L., et al. (2004). Impairment in visual and spatial perception in schizophrenia and delusional disorder. Psychiatry Res. 127, 163–166. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haroun, N., Dunn, L., Haroun, A., and Cadenhead, K. S. (2006). Risk and protection in prodromal schizophrenia: ethical implications for clinical practice and future research. Schizophr. Bull. 32, 166–178. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbj007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hawkins, K. A., Addington, J., Keefe, R. S., Christensen, B., Perkins, D. O., Zipurksy, R., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological status of subjects at high risk for a first episode of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 67, 115–122. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heaton, R., Paulsen, J. S., McAdams, L. A., Kuck, J., Zisook, S., Braff, D., et al. (1994). Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics. Relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 469–476. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060033003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hegel, G. W. F. (1807). System der Wissenschaft. Erster Teil, Die Phänomenologie des Geistes. Bamberg und Würzburg: Joseph Anton Goebhardt.

Held, R., Ostrovsky, Y., de Gelder, B., Gandhi, T., Ganesh, S., Mathur, U., et al. (2011). The newly sighted fail to match seen with felt. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 551–553. doi:10.1038/nn.2795

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heller, M. A., and Kennedy, J. M. (1990). Perspective taking, pictures, and the blind. Percept. Psychophys. 48, 459–466. doi:10.3758/BF03211590

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heydebrand, G. (2006). Cognitive deficits in the families of patients with schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 277–281. doi:10.1097/01.yco.0000218598.38807.b0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hollins, M. (1989). Understanding blindness: An integrative approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Hove and London.

Hotting, K., and Roder, B. (2004). Hearing cheats touch, but less in congenitally blind than in sighted individuals. Psychol. Sci. 15, 60–64. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501010.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hotting, K., Rosler, F., and Roder, B. (2004). Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study. Exp. Brain Res. 159, 370–381. doi:10.1007/s00221-004-1965-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hugdahl, K., Bodner, T., Weiss, E., and Benke, T. (2003). Dichotic listening performance and frontal lobe function. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 58–65. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00210-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hugdahl, K., Ek, M., Takio, F., Rintee, T., Tuomainen, J., Haarala, C., et al. (2004). Blind individuals show enhanced perceptual and attentional sensitivity for identification of speech sounds. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 28–32. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hughes, D. F. (2013). Charles Bonnet syndrome: a literature review into diagnostic criteria, treatment and implications for nursing practice. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 20, 169–175. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2850.2012.01904.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hull, T., and Mason, H. (1995). Performance of blind children on digit-span tests. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 89, 166–169.

Hutton, P., Parker, S., Bowe, S., and Ford, S. (2012). Prevalence of violence risk factors in people at ultra-high risk of developing psychosis: a service audit. Early Interv. Psy-chiatry 6, 91–96. doi:10.1111/j.1751-7893.2011.00307.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hutton, S. B., Huddy, V., Barnes, T. R., Robbins, T. W., Crawford, T. J., Kennard, C., et al. (2004). The relationship between antisaccades, smooth pursuit, and executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 553–559. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.07.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Imhof, M. (2010). Listening to voices and judging people. Int. J. Listen. 24, 1–15. doi:10.1080/10904010903466295

CrossRef Full Text

Insel, T. R. (2010). Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature 468, 187–193. doi:10.1038/nature09552

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jansen, H., Mannhaupt, G., Marx, H., and Skowronek, H. (2002). Biele-felder Screening zur Früherkennung von Lese-Rechschreibschwierigkeiten (BISC). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Javitt, D. C. (2009). Sensory processing in schizophrenia: neither simple nor intact. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 1059–1064. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, S. C., Lowery, N., Kohler, C., and Turetsky, B. I. (2005). Global-local visual processing in schizophrenia: evidence for an early visual processing deficit. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 937–946. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kallimani, D., Theleritis, C., Evdokimidis, I., Stefanis, N. C., Chatzimanolis, I., and Smyrnis, N. (2009). The effect of change in clinical state on eye movement dysfunction in schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry 24, 17–26. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.08.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 11163–11170. doi:10.1073/pnas.1005062107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karoumi, B., Saoud, M., d'Amato, T., Rosenfeld, F., Denise, P., Gutknecht, C., et al. (2001). Poor performance in smooth pursuit and antisaccadic eye-movement tasks in healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 101, 209–219. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00227-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karoumi, B., Ventre-Dominey, J., Vighetto, A., Dalery, J., and d'Amato, T. (1998). Saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 77, 9–19. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(97)00126-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kathmann, N., Hochrein, A., Uwer, R., and Bondy, B. (2003). Deficits in gain of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia and affective disorder patients and their unaffected relatives. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 696–702. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.696

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kauffman, T., Theoret, H., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Braille character discrimination in blindfolded human subjects. Neuroreport 13, 571–574. doi:10.1097/00001756-200204160-00007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kazui, H., Ishii, R., Yoshida, T., Ikezawa, K., Takaya, M., Tokunaga, H., et al. (2009). Neuroimaging studies in patients with Charles Bonnet Syndrome. Psychogeriatrics 9, 77–84. doi:10.1111/j.1479-8301.2009.00288.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kebir, O., Ben Azouz, O., Amado, I., and Tabbane, K. (2008). Inhibition of return in schizophrenia: a review. Encephale 34, 263–269. doi:10.1016/j.encep.2007.07.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kebir, O., Ben Azouz, O., Rabah, Y., Dellagi, L., Johnson, I., Amado, I., et al. (2010). Confirmation for a delayed inhibition of return by systematic sampling in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 176, 17–21. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keedy, S. K., Ebens, C. L., Keshavan, M. S., and Sweeney, J. A. (2006). Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of eye movements in first episode schizophrenia: smooth pursuit, visually guided saccades and the oculomotor delayed response task. Psychiatry Res. 146, 199–211. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.01.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keefe, R. S., and Harvey, P. D. (2012). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 213, 11–37. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-25758-2_2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerr, N. H. (1983). The role of vision in “visual imagery” experiments: evidence from the congenitally blind. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 112, 265–277. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.112.2.265

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, D., Zemon, V., Saperstein, A., Butler, P. D., and Javitt, D. C. (2005). Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia: harmonic analysis. Schizophr. Res. 76, 55–65. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.10.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kimhy, D., Corcoran, C., Harkavy-Friedman, J. M., Ritzler, B., Javitt, D. C., and Malaspina, D. (2007). Visual form perception: a comparison of individuals at high risk for psychosis, recent onset schizophrenia and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 97, 25–34. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klosterkotter, J., Gross, G., Huber, G., and Steinmeyer, E. M. (1997). Are self-perceivable neuropsychological deficits in patients with neuroses or personality disorder diagnoses indicative of later schizophrenia?. Nervenarzt 68, 196–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Klosterkotter, J., Hellmich, M., Stein-meyer, E. M., and Schultze-Lutter, F. (2001). Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 158–164. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.158

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kluwe-Schiavon, B., Sanvicente-Vieira, B., Kristensen, C. H., and Grassi-Oliveira, R. (2013). Executive functions rehabilitation for schizophrenia: a critical systematic review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 91–104. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knapp, M. L., and Hall, J. A. (2002). Nonverbal Communication in Human Interaction. Boston, MA: University of California: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.

Knauff, M., and May, E. (2006). Mental imagery, reasoning, and blindness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 59, 161–177. doi:10.1080/17470210500149992

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koethe, D., Gerth, C. W., Neatby, M. A., Haensel, A., Thies, M., Schneider, U., et al. (2006). Disturbances of visual information processing in early states of psychosis and experimental delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol altered states of consciousness. Schizophr. Res. 88, 142–150. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koethe, D., Kranaster, L., Hoyer, C., Gross, S., Neatby, M. A., Schultze-Lutter, F., et al. (2009). Binocular depth inversion as a paradigm of reduced visual information processing in prodromal state, antipsychotic-naive and treated schizophrenia. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 259, 195–202. doi:10.1007/s00406-008-0851-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kooyman, I., Walsh, E., Stevens, H., Burns, T., Tyrer, P., Tattan, T., et al. (2012). Criminal offending before and after the onset of psychosis: examination of an offender typology. Schizophr. Res. 140, 198–203. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krebs, M. O., Gut-Fayand, A., Amado, I., Daban, C., Bourdel, M. C., Poirier, M. F., et al. (2001). Impairment of predictive saccades in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 12, 465–469. doi:10.1097/00001756-200103050-00009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krueger, F., Landgraf, S., van der Meer, E., Deshpande, G., and Hu, X. (2011). Effective connectivity of the multiplication network: a functional MRI and multivariate Granger Causality Mapping study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1419–1431. doi:10.1002/hbm.21119

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krueger, F., Spampinato, M. V., Pardini, M., Pajevic, S., Wood, J. N., Weiss, G. H., et al. (2008). Integral calculus problem solving: an fMRI investigation. Neuroreport 19, 1095–1099. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328303fd85

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kujala, T., Alho, K., and Naatanen, R. (2000). Cross-modal reorganization of human cortical functions. Trends Neurosci. 23, 115–120. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01504-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kupers, R., Fumal, A., de Noordhout, A. M., Gjedde, A., Schoenen, J., and Ptito, M. (2006). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex induces somatotopically organized qualia in blind subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13256–13260. doi:10.1073/pnas.0602925103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurachi, M., Matsui, M., Kiba, K., Suzuki, M., Tsunoda, M., and Yamaguchi, N. (1994). Limited visual search on the WAIS Picture Completion test in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 12, 75–80. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(94)90086-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Land, M. F., and Furneaux, S. (1997). The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 352, 1231–1239. doi:10.1098/rstb.1997.0105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Berthoz, A., Krebs, M. O., and van der Meer, E. (2012). Cognitive identity in schizophrenia: vision, space, and body perception from prodrome to syndrome. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 2, 1–20.

Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Bour-del, M. C., Leonardi, S., and Krebs, M. O. (2008). Memory-guided saccade abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and their healthy, full biological siblings. Psychol. Med. 38, 861–870. doi:10.1017/S0033291707001912

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Brucks, M., Krueger, F., Krebs, M. O., and van der Meer, E. (2011a). Inflexible information acquisition strategies mediate visuo-spatial reasoning in stabilized schizophrenia patients. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 12, 608–619. doi:10.3109/15622975.2010.544329

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Purkhart, R., Ries, J., Olie, J. P., and van der Meer, E. (2011b). Visuo-spatial cognition in schizophrenia: confirmation of a preference for local information processing. Schizophr. Res. 127, 163–170. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., Steingen, J., Eppert, Y., Niedermeyer, U., van der Meer, E., and Krueger, F. (2011c). Temporal information processing in short- and long-term memory of patients with schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 6:e26140. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., Krebs, M. O., Olie, J. P., Committeri, G., van der Meer, E., Berthoz, A., et al. (2010a). Real world referencing and schizophrenia: are we experiencing the same reality? Neuropsychologia 48, 2922–2930. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Landgraf, S., van der Meer, E., and Krueger, F. (2010b). Resource allocation of cognitive processes for neuronal activity underlying mathematical cognition: a multi-method study. Int. J. Math. Educ. 42, 579–590. doi:10.1007/s11858-010-0264-7

CrossRef Full Text

Lane, B. A. (1980). The relationship of learning disabilities to juvenile delinquency: current status. J. Learn Disabil. 13, 425–434. doi:10.1177/002221948001300803

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Langdon, R., and Ward, P. (2008). Taking the perspective of the other contributes to awareness of illness in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 1003–1011. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lawrence, B. M., Myerson, J., and Abrams, R. A. (2004). Interference with spatial working memory: an eye movement is more than a shift of attention. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 488–494. doi:10.3758/BF03196600

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laws, K. R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of Wisconsin Card Sort studies in schizophrenia: general intellectual deficit in disguise. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 4, 1–30. doi:10.1080/135468099396025 discussion 31–35.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leigh, R. J., and Zee, D. S. (1999). The Neurology of Eye Movements. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.

Leitman, D. I., Foxe, J. J., Butler, P. D., Saperstein, A., Revheim, N., and Javitt, D. C. (2005). Sensory contributions to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 56–61. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., Auther, A. M., Correll, C. U., and Cornblatt, B. A. (2003). The assessment of “prodromal schizophrenia”: unresolved issues and future directions. Schizophr. Bull. 29, 717–728. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., McLaughlin, D., Auther, A., Nakayama, E., Hovey, L., et al. (2006). Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 863–871. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lessard, N., Pare, M., Lepore, F., and Lassonde, M. (1998). Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects. Nature 395, 278–280. doi:10.1038/26228

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levy, D. L., Bowman, E. A., Abel, L., Krastoshevsky, O., Krause, V., and Mendell, N. R. (2008). Does performance on the standard anti-saccade task meet the co-familiality criterion for an endopheno-type. Brain Cogn. 68, 462–475. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levy, D. L., O'Driscoll, G., Matthysse, S., Cook, S. R., Holzman, P. S., and Mendell, N. R. (2004). Antisaccade performance in biological relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 71, 113–125. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lewald, J. I, Meister, G., Weidemann, J., and Topper, R. (2004). Involvement of the superior temporal cortex and the occipital cortex in spatial hearing: evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 828–838. doi:10.1162/089892904970834

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leygraf, N. (1988). Psychisch Kranke Straftäter – Epidemiologie und Aktuelle Praxis des Psychiatrischen Maßregelvollzugs. Heidelberg: Springer.

Linden, D. E., Habes, I., Johnston, S. J., Linden, S., Tatineni, R., Subramanian, L., et al. (2012). Real-time self-regulation of emotion networks in patients with depression. PLoS ONE 7:e38115. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038115

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Longevialle-Henin, R., Bourdel, M. C., Willard, D., Loo, H., Olie, J. P., Poirier, M. F., et al. (2005). Visuospatial context processing in untreated schizophrenic patients and relation to disorganization syndrome. Encephale 31, 323–329.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Loughland, C. M., Williams, L. M., and Gordon, E. (2002). Schizophrenia and affective disorder show different visual scanning behavior for faces: a trait versus state-based distinction. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 338–348. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01356-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Loughland, C. M., Williams, L. M., and Harris, A. W. (2004). Visual scanpath dysfunction in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia probands: evidence for a vulnerability marker? Schizophr. Res. 67, 11–21. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00094-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lüer, G., Lass, U., and Shallo-Hoffmann, J. (1988). Eye Movement Research: Physiological and Psychological Aspects. Toronto: Hogrefe.

Lysaker, P. H., Daroyanni, P., Ringer, J. M., Beattie, N. L., Strasburger, A. M., and Davis, L. W. (2007). Associations of awareness of illness in schizophrenia spectrum disorder with social cognition and cognitive perceptual organization. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 195, 618–621. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e318093f437

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Manoach, D. S., White, N., Lindgren, K. A., Heckers, S., Coleman, M. J., Dubal, S., et al. (2005). Intact hemispheric specialization for spatial and shape working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 78, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitch, H. J. (1997). Klinische Neuropsychologie. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Marmor, G. S., and Zaback, L. A. (1976). Mental rotation by the blind: does mental rotation depend on visual imagery. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 515–521. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.2.4.515

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marvel, C. L., Schwartz, B. L., and Rosse, R. B. (2004). A quantitative measure of postural sway deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 68, 363–372. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazhari, S., Badcock, J. C., Waters, F. A., Dragovic, M., Badcock, D. R., and Jablensky, A. (2010). Impaired spatial working memory maintenance in schizophrenia involves both spatial coordinates and spatial reference frames. Psychiatry Res. 179, 253–258. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDowell, J. E., Brenner, C. A., Myles-Worsley, M., Coon, H., Byerley, W., and Clementz, B. A. (2001). Ocular motor delayed-response task performance among patients with schizophrenia and their biological relatives. Psychophysiology 38, 153–156. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3810153

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDowell, J. E., and Clementz, B. A. (1996). Ocular-motor delayed-response task performance among schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychobiology 34, 67–71. doi:10.1159/000119294

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDowell, J. E., and Clementz, B. A. (1997). The effect of fixation condition manipulations on antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: studies of diagnostic specificity. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 333–344. doi:10.1007/PL00005702

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McGorry, P. D. (2010). Risk syndromes, clinical staging and DSM V: new diagnostic infrastructure for early intervention in psychiatry. Schizophr. Res. 120, 49–53. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McGorry, P. D., Yung, A. R., and Phillips, L. J. (2003). The “close-in” or ultra high-risk model: a safe and effective strategy for research and clinical intervention in prepsychotic mental disorder. Schizophr. Bull. 29, 771–790. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McKay, S., and Brumback, R. A. (1980). Relationship between learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency. Percept. Mot. Skills 51, 1223–1226. doi:10.2466/pms.1980.51.3f.1223

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Melzer, K. (2001). Psychisch kranke Straftäterinnen: Frauen im Maßregelvollzug. [Mentally Disordered Female Perpetrators: Women in Forensic Psychiatry]. Frankfurt, Main: Lang.

Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Buodo, G., Leone, V., and Palomba, D. (2011). Neurofeedback training for tourette syndrome: an uncontrolled single case study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 36, 281–288. doi:10.1007/s10484-011-9169-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Micoulaud-Franchi, J. A., Fakra, E., Cermolacce, M., and Vion-Dury, J. (2012). Towards a new approach of neurophysiology in clinical psychiatry: functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback applied to emotional dysfunctions. Neurophysiol. Clin. 42, 79–94. doi:10.1016/j.neucli.2011.12.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milea, D., Lobel, E., Lehericy, S., Leboucher, P., Pochon, J. B., Pierrot- Deseilligny, C., et al. (2007). Prefrontal cortex is involved in internal decision of forthcoming saccades. Neuroreport 18, 1221–1224. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3281e72ce7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, T. J., McGlashan, T. H., Rosen, J. L., Cadenhead, K., Cannon, T., Ventura, J., et al. (2003). Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr. Bull. 29, 703–715. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040

CrossRef Full Text

Miller, T. J., McGlashan, T. H., Rosen, J. L., Somjee, L., Markovich, P. J., Stein, K., et al. (2002). Prospective diagnosis of the initial prodrome for schizophrenia based on the structured interview for prodromal syndromes: preliminary evidence of interrater reliability and predictive validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 863–865. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.863

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milner, A. D., and Goodale, M. A. (2006). The Visual Brain in Action. New York: Oxford University Press.

Minassian, A., Granholm, E., Verney, S., and Perry, W. (2005). Visual scanning deficits in schizophrenia and their relationship to executive functioning impairment. Schizophr. Res. 74, 69–79. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. Cereb. Cortex 18, 262–271. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morgan, M. J. (1977). Molyneux's Question: Vision, Touch and Philosophy of Perception. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Moyer, R. S., and Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature 215, 1519–1520. doi:10.1038/2151519a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muchnik, C., Efrati, M., Nemeth, E., Malin, M., and Hildesheimer, M. (1991). Central auditory skills in blind and sighted subjects. Scand. Audiol. 20, 19–23. doi:10.3109/01050399109070785

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muller, N., Riedel, M., Eggert, T., and Straube, A. (1999). Internally and externally guided voluntary sac-cades in unmedicated and medicated schizophrenic patients. Part II. Saccadic latency, gain, and fixation suppression errors. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 249, 7–14. doi:10.1007/s004060050059

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nedopil, N. (2007). Forensische Psychiatrie: Klinik, Begutachtung und Behandlung zwischen Psychiatrie und Recht [Forensic Psychiatry: Clinic, Evaluation and Treatment in-between Psychiatry and Law.]. Stuttgart: Thieme.

Nelson, B., Yung, A. R., Bech-dolf, A., and McGorry, P. D. (2008). The phenomenological critique and self-disturbance: implications for ultra-high risk (“prodrome”) research. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 381–392. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm094

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nieman, D., Becker, H., van de Fliert, R., Plat, N., Bour, L., Koel-man, H., et al. (2007). Antisaccade task performance in patients at ultra high risk for developing psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 95, 54–60. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Johnson, J. K., McKinley, M., Loewy, R., O'Brien, M., et al. (2006). Neurocognitive performance and functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophr. Res. 84, 100–111. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Zin-berg, J., Johnson, J. K., O'Brien, M., and Cannon, T. D. (2007). The course of neurocognition and social functioning in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 772–781. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitschke, J., Osterheider, M., and Mokros, A. (2011). Schizophrenic diseases, psychosis and homicide: the importance of community psychiatry for the prevention of offences. Psychiatr. Prax. 38, 82–86. doi:10.1055/s-0030-1248603

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nowara, S. (1993). “Disziplinierungsinstanzen Psychiatrie und Justiz,” in Spinnt die Frau? Ein Lesebuch; zur Geschlechterfrage in der Psychiatrie, eds D. Schneider and G. Tergeist (Bonn: Psychiatrie Verlag), 219–228.

Ochoa, S., Usall, J., Cobo, J., Labad, X., and Kulkarni, J. (2012). Gender differences in schizophrenia and first-episode psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. Schizophr. Res. Treatment 2012:916198. doi: 10.1155/2012/916198

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Donnell, B. F., Swearer, J. M., Smith, L. T., Nestor, P. G., Shenton, M. E., and McCarley, R. W. (1996). Selective deficits in visual perception and recognition in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 153, 687–692.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ofan, R. H., and Zohary, E. (2007). Visual cortex activation in bilingual blind individuals during use of native and second language. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1249–1259. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olsen, K. A., and Rosenbaum, B. (2006a). Prospective investigations of the prodromal state of schizophrenia: assessment instruments. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 113, 273–282. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00698.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olsen, K. A., and Rosenbaum, B. (2006b). Prospective investigations of the ‘prodromal state of schizophrenia: review of studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 113, 247–272. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00698.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, S., and Holzman, P. S. (1992). Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 975–982. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120063009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, S., and Holzman, P. S. (1993). Association of working memory deficit and eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 11, 55–61. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(93)90038-K

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, S., Holzman, P. S., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Spatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 821–828. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950220031007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, S., Puschel, J., Sauter, B. H., Rentsch, M., and Hell, D. (2002). Spatial selective attention and inhibition in schizophrenia patients during acute psychosis and at 4-month follow-up. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 498–506. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01278-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, S., Puschel, J., Sauter, B. H., Rentsch, M., and Hell, D. (2003). Visual object working memory function and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 59, 261–268. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00209-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parnas, J., Jansson, L., Sass, L., and Handest, P. (1998). Self-experience in the prodromal phases of schizophrenia: a pilot study of first admissions. Neurol. Psychiatry Brain Res. 6, 107–116.

Parnas, J., Vianin, P., Saebye, D., Jansson, L., Volmer-Larsen, A., and Bovet, P. (2001). Visual binding abilities in the initial and advanced stages of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 103, 171–180. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.00160.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pascual-Leone, A., and Hamilton, R. (2001). The metamodal organization of the brain. Prog. Brain Res. 134, 427–445. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(01)34028-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Penfield, W. (1955). The twenty-ninth Maudsley lecture: the role of the temporal cortex in certain psychical phenomena. J. Ment. Sci. 101, 451–465.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Perry, W., Heaton, R. K., Potterat, E., Roebuck, T., Minassian, A., and BraffD. L. (2001). Workingmemory in schizophrenia: transient “online” storage versus executive functioning. Schizophr. Bull. 27, 157–176. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006854

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phillips, M. L., and David, A. S. (1997). Visual scan paths are abnormal in deluded schizophrenics. Neuropsychologia 35, 99–105. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00061-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phillips, W. A., and Silverstein, S. M. (2003). Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 65–138. doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Picard, H., Amado, I., Bourdel, M. C., Landgraf, S., Olie, J. P., and Krebs, M. O. (2009). Correlates between neurological soft signs and saccadic parameters in schizophrenia. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 676–681. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.03.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Picard, H., Amado, I., Mouchet-Mages, S., Olie, J. P., and Krebs, M. O. (2008). The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: an update of clinical, cognitive, and functional evidences. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 155–172. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pichora-Fuller, M. K., and Singh, G. (2006). Effects of age on auditory and cognitive processing: implications for hearing aid fitting and audiologic rehabilitation. Trends Amplif. 10, 29–59. doi:10.1177/108471380601000103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Muri, R. M., Nyffeler, T., and Milea, D. (2005). The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1039, 239–251. doi:10.1196/annals.1325.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piskulic, D., Olver, J., Norman, T., and Maruff, P. (2007). Behavioral studies of spatial working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a quantitative review. Psychiatry Res. 150, 111–121. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.03.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plato. (380 BC). Plato: The Republic, Timaeus and Critias. (Translation by G. Burges, 1854). London: H.G. Bohn.

Poirier, C., Collignon, O., Scheiber, C., Renier, L., Vanlierde, A., Tranduy, D., et al. (2006). Auditory motion perception activates visual motion areas in early blind subjects. Neuroimage 31, 279–285. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., Early, T. S., Reiman, E., Pardo, P. J., and Dhawan, M. (1988). Asymmetries in hemispheric control of attention in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45, 814–821. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800330038004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., and Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychol. Rev. 83, 157–171. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Postma, A., Zuidhoek, S., Noordzij, M. L., and Kappers, A. M. (2007). Differences between early-blind, late-blind, and blindfolded-sighted people in haptic spatial-configuration learning and resulting memory traces. Perception 36, 1253–1265. doi:10.1068/p5441

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pozar, L. (1982). Effect of long-term sensory deprivation on recall of verbal material. Stud. Psychol. (Bratisl.) 24, 311.

Previc, F. H. (1998). The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychol. Bull. 124, 123–164. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.123

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pukrop, R., and Klosterkotter, J. (2010). Neurocognitive indicators of clinical high-risk states for psychosis: a critical review of the evidence. Neurotox. Res. 18, 272–286. doi:10.1007/s12640-010-9191-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pukrop, R., Ruhrmann, S., Schultze-Lutter, F., Bechdolf, A., Brockhaus-Dumke, A., and Klosterkotter, J. (2007). Neurocognitive indicators for a conversion to psychosis: comparison of patients in a potentially initial prodromal state who did or did not convert to a psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 92, 116–125. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Putzar, L., Goerendt, I. Lange, K., Rosler, F., and Roder, B. (2007). Early visual deprivation impairs multisensory interactions in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1243–1245. doi:10.1038/nn1978

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Radant, A. D., Claypoole, K., Wingerson, D. K., Cowley, D. S., and Roy-Byrne, P. P. (1997). Relationships between neuropsychological and oculomotor measures in schizophrenia patients and normal controls. Biol. Psychiatry 42, 797–805. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00464-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Radant, A. D., Dobie, D. J., Calkins, M. E., Olincy, A., Braff, D. L., Cadenhead, K. S., et al. (2007). Successful multi-site measurement of antisaccade performance deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 89, 320–329. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P. (1995). Compensatory plasticity and sensory substitution in the cerebral cortex. Trends Neurosci. 18, 36–43. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(95)93948-W

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raz, N., Amedi, A., and Zohary, E. (2005). V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1459–1468. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raz, N., Striem, E., Pundak, G., Orlov, T., and Zohary, E. (2007). Superior serial memory in the blind: a case of cognitive compensatory adjustment. Curr. Biol. 17, 1129–1133. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reich, L., Maidenbaum, S., and Amedi, A. (2012). The brain as a flexible task machine: implications for visual rehabilitation using noninvasive vs. invasive approaches. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 25, 86–95. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834ed723

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reichenberg, A., Weiser, M., Rabinowitz, J., Caspi, A., Schmeidler, J., Mark, M., et al. (2002). A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 2027–2035. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Resnikoff, S., Pascolini, D., Etya'Ale, D., Kocur, I., Pararajasegaram, R., Pokharel, G. P., et al. (2004). Global data on visual impairment in the year 2002. Bull. World Health Organ. 82, 844–851.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Reuter, B., Herzog, E., and Kathmann, N. (2006). Antisaccade performance of schizophrenia patients: evidence of reduced task-set activation and impaired error detection. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 122–130. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., Kusmierek, A., Spence, C., and Schicke, T. (2007). Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 4753–4758. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607158104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., and Neville, H. J. (2003). “Developmental plasticity,” in Plasticity and Rehabilitation, eds J. Grafman, and I. Robertson (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 231–270.

Roder, B., and Rosler, F. (2003). Memory for environmental sounds in sighted, congenitally blind and late blind adults: evidence for cross-modal compensation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 50, 27–39. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00122-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., Rosler, F., Henninghausen, E., and Nacker, F. (1996). Event-related potentials during auditory and somatosensory discrimination in sighted and blind human subjects. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain. Res. 4, 77–93. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(96)00024-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Neville, H. J. (2001). Auditory memory in congenitally blind adults: a behavioral-electrophysiological investigation. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain. Res. 11, 289–303. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00002-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Spence, C. (2004). Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind. Curr. Biol. 14, 121–124. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2003.12.054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B., Teder-Salejarvi, W., Sterr, A., Rosler, F., Hillyard, S. A., and Neville, H. J. (1999). Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 400, 162–166. doi:10.1038/22106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosenberg, D. R., Sweeney, J. A., Squires-Wheeler, E., Keshavan, M. S., Cornblatt, B. A., and Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1997). Eye-tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York high-risk project: diagnostic specificity and the role of attention. Psychiatry Res. 66, 121–130. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(96)02975-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ross, D. E., Buchanan, R. W., Med-off, D., Lahti, A. C., and Thaker, G. K. (1998). Association between eye tracking disorder in schizophrenia and poor sensory integration. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 1352–1357.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rybakowski, J. K., and Borkowska, A. (2002). Eye movement and neuropsychological studies in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Schizophr. Res. 54, 105–110. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00357-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sadato, N., Okada, T., Honda, M., and Yonekura, Y. (2002). Critical period for cross-modal plasticity in blind humans: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 16, 389–400. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Graf-man, J., Deiber, M. P., Ibanez, V., and Hallett, M. (1998). Neural networks for Braille reading by the blind. Brain 121(Pt 7), 1213–1229. doi:10.1093/brain/121.7.1213

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibanez, V., Deiber, M. P., Dold, G., et al. (1996). Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 380, 526–528. doi:10.1038/380526a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanders, G. S., Gallup, G. G., Hein-sen, H., Hof, P. R., and Schmitz, C. (2002). Cognitive deficits, schizophrenia, and the anterior cingu-late cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 6, 190–192. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01892-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanders, G. S., Platek, S. M., and Gallup, G. G. (2003). No blind schizophrenics: are NMDA-receptor dynamics involved. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 103–104.

Saperstein, A. M., Fuller, R. L., Avila, M. T., Adami, H., McMahon, R. P., Thaker, G. K., et al. (2006). Spatial working memory as a cognitive endophenotype of schizophrenia: assessing risk for pathophysiological dysfunction. Schizophr. Bull. 32, 498–506. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbj072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sass, L., and Parnas, J. (2001). Phenomenology of self-disturbances in schizophrenia: some research findings and directions. Philos. Psychiatry Psychol. 8, 347–356. doi:10.1353/ppp.2002.0027

CrossRef Full Text

Sass, L., and Parnas, J. (2003). Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr. Bull. 29, 427–444. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sathian, K. (2005). Visual cortical activity during tactile perception in the sighted and the visually deprived. Dev. Psychobiol. 46, 279–286. doi:10.1002/dev.20056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schadlu, A. P., Schadlu, R., and Shepherd, J. B. 3rd (2009). Charles Bonnet syndrome: a review. Curr. Opin. Ophthalmol. 20, 219–222. doi:10.1097/ICU.0b013e328329b643

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schultze-Lutter, F., Ruhrmann, S., Picker, H., von Reventlow, H. G., Daumann, B., Brockhaus-Dumke, A., et al. (2007). Relationship between subjective and objective cognitive function in the early and late prodrome. Br. J. Psychiatry. Suppl. 51, s43–51. doi:10.1192/bjp.191.51.s43

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schwabe, L., and Blanke, O. (2007). Cognitive neuroscience of ownership and agency. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 661–666. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.07.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Damaggio, G., Falcone, M., Nicolo, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2003). How to evaluate matacognitive function in psychotherapy? The metacognition assessment scale and its applications. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 10, 238–261. doi:10.1002/cpp.362

CrossRef Full Text

Silverstein, S. M., and Keane, B. P. (2011a). Perceptual organization impairment in schizophrenia and associated brain mechanisms: review of research from 2005 to 2010. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 690–699. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silverstein, S. M., and Keane, B. P. (2011b). Vision science and schizophrenia research: toward a re-view of the disorder. Editors' introduction to special section. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 681–689. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silverstein, S. M., Wang, Y., and Keane, B. P. (2013). Cognitive and neu-roplasticity mechanisms by which congenital or early blindness may confer a protective effect against schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 3:624. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simon, A. E., Cattapan-Ludewig, K., Zmilacher, S., Arbach, D., Gruber, K., Dvorsky, D. N., et al. (2007). Cognitive functioning in the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 761–771. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simon, A. E., Dvorsky, D. N., Boesch, J., Roth, B., Isler, E., Schueler, P., et al. (2006). Defining subjects at risk for psychosis: a comparison of two approaches. Schizophr. Res. 81, 83–90. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slaton, P., and Jorgensen, D. (1958). Visual screening in a county school for delinquent boys. Am. J. Optom. Arch. Am. Acad. Optom. 35, 369–372. doi:10.1097/00006324-195807000-00005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, C. W., Park, S., and Cornblatt, B. (2006). Spatial working memory deficits in adolescents at clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 81, 211–215. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, L., and Gasser, M. (2005). The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies. Artif. Life 11, 13–29. doi:10.1162/1064546053278973

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, M., Franz, E. A., Joy, S. M., and Whitehead, K. (2005). Superior performance of blind compared with sighted individuals on bimanual estimations of object size. Psychol. Sci. 16, 11–14. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00773.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smits, B. W., and Mommers, M. J. (1976). Differences between blind and sighted children on WISC verbal subtests. New Outlook Blind 70, 240–246.

Soyka, M., Albus, M., Immler, B., Kathmann, N., and Hippius, H. (2002). Psychopathology in dual-diagnosis and nonaddicted schizophrenics: are there differences? Eur. J. Health Econ. 3(Suppl. 2), S114–S120. doi:10.1007/s10198-002-0125-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Soyka, M., and Morhart-Klute, V. (2002). Aggression and violent crime associated with schizophrenia. Dtsch. Med. Wochenschr. 127, 1708–1712. doi:10.1055/s-2002-33380

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Soyka, M., Morhart-Klute, V., and Schoech, H. (2004). Delinquency and criminal offenses in former schizophrenic inpatients 7-12 years following discharge. Eur. Arch. Psy-chiatry Clin. Neurosci. 254, 289–294. doi:10.1007/s00406-004-0495-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steinert, T. (1998). Aggression und Gewalt bei Schizophrenie. Munster: Waxmann.

Stone, D. B., Urrea, L. J., Aine, C. J., Bustillo, J. R., Clark, V. P., and Stephen, J. M. (2011). Unisensory processing and multisensory integration in schizophrenia: a high-density electrical mapping study. Neuropsychologia 49, 3178–3187. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Striem-Amit, E., Bubic, A., and Amedi, A. (2012a). “Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying plastic changes and rehabilitation following sensory loss in blindness and deafness,” in The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, eds M. M. Murray and M. T. Wallace (Boca Raton, FL: Taylor and Francis Group), 395–422.

Striem-Amit, E., Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., and Amedi, A. (2012b). Reading with sounds: sensory substitution selectively activates the visual word form area in the blind. Neuron 76, 640–652. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Striem-Amit, E., Dakwar, O., Reich, L., and Amedi, A. (2012c). The large-scale organization of “visual” streams emerges without visual experience. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1698–1709. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr253

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Striem-Amit, E., Guendelman, M., and Amedi, A. (2012d). ‘Visual’ acuity of the congenitally blind using visual-to-auditory sensory substitution. PLoS ONE 7:e33136. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033136

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suchan, B. (2008). Neuroanatomi-cal correlates of processing in visual and visuospatial working memory. Cogn. Process. 9, 45–51. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0186-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sulzer, J., Sitaram, R., Blefari, M. L., Kollias, S., Birbaumer, N., Stephan, K. E., et al. (2013). Neurofeedback-mediated self-regulation of the dopaminergic midbrain. Neuroimage 75C, 176–184. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sweeney, J. A., Clementz, B. A., Haas, G. L., Escobar, M. D., Drake, K., and Frances, A. J. (1994). Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization of component eye movement abnormalities, diagnostic specificity, and the role of attention. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 222–230. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.222

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tan, H. Y., Sust, S., Buckholtz, J. W., Mattay, V. S., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Egan, M. F., et al. (2006). Dysfunctional prefrontal regional specialization and compensation in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1969–1977. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.11.1969

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tek, C., Gold, J., Blaxton, T., Wilk, C., McMahon, R. P., and Buchanan, R. W. (2002). Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 146–153. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.146

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thoma, P., Wiebel, B., and Daum, I. (2007). Response inhibition and cognitive flexibility in schizophrenia with and without comorbid substance use disorder. Schizophr. Res. 92, 168–180. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tillman, M. H., and Bashaw, W. L. (1968). Multivariate analysis of the WISC scales for blind and sighted children. Psychol. Rep. 23, 523–526. doi:10.2466/pr0.1968.23.2.523

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tinti, C., Adenzato, M., Tamietto, M., and Cornoldi, C. (2006). Visual experience is not necessary for efficient survey spatial cognition: evidence from blindness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 59, 1306–1328. doi:10.1080/17470210500214275

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uhlhaas, P. J., and Mishara, A. L. (2007). Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 142–156. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van den Stock, J., de Jong, S. J., Hodi-amont, P. P., and de Gelder, B. (2011). Perceiving emotions from bodily expressions and multisensory integration of emotion cues in schiz-ophrenia. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 537–547. doi:10.1080/17470919.2011.568790

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Velzen, J., Eardley, A. F., Forster, B., and Eimer, M. (2006). Shifts of attention in the early blind: an erp study of attentional control processes in the absence of visual spatial information. Neuropsychologia 44, 2533–2546. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Varga, M., Magnusson, A., Flekkoy, K., David, A. S., and Opjordsmoen, S. (2007). Clinical and neuropsy-chological correlates of insight in schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder: does diagnosis matter. Compr. Psychiatry 48, 583–591. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.06.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vecchi, T., Tinti, C., and Cornoldi, C. (2004). Spatial memory and integra-tion processes in congenital blindness. Neuroreport 15, 2787–2790.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Vercammen, A., Knegtering, H., Liemburg, E. J., den Boer, J. A., and Aleman, A. (2010). Functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal region in schizophrenia: effects of rTMS treatment of auditory hallucinations. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 725–731. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.12.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Versmissen, D., Myin-Germeys, I., Janssen, I., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Campo, J. A., et al. (2007). Impairment of self-monitoring: part of the endophenotypic risk for psychosis. Br. J. Psychiatry. Suppl. 51, s58–s62. doi:10.1192/bjp.191.51.s58

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voglmaier, M. M., Seidman, L. J., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Dickey, C. C., Shenton, M. E., and McCar-ley, R. W. (2005). A comparative profile analysis of neuropsychological function in men and women with schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr. Res. 74, 43–49. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Von Senden, M. (1932). Space and Sight: The Perception of Space and Shape in the Congenitally Blind Before and After Operation. Kiel: J. A. Barth.

Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gal-linat, J., Heinz, A., and Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain 133, 3104–3112. doi:10.1093/brain/awq152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vrtunski, P. B., Lewis, K. L., and Meltzer, H. Y. (1993). Visual feedback in force control of schizophrenics. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 667–670. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(93)90162-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Walla, P. (2008). Olfaction and its dynamic influence on word and face processing: cross-modal integration. Prog. Neurobiol. 84, 192–209. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wallace, M. T., Perrault, T. P., Hairston, W. D., and Stein, B. (2004). Visual experience is necessary for the development of multisensory integration. J. Neurosci. 24, 9580–9584. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-04.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Warren, D. H. (1994). Blindness and Children: An Individual Differences Approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Warren, D. H., and Pick, H. L. (1970). Intermodality relations in localization in blind and sighted people. Percept. Psychophys. 8, 430–432. doi:10.3758/BF03207040

CrossRef Full Text

Waters, F. A., and Badcock, J. C. (2010). First-rank symptoms in schizophrenia: reexamining mechanisms of self-recognition. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 510–517. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weeks, R., Horwitz, B., Aziz-Sultan, A., Tian, B., Wessinger, C. M., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2000). A positron emission tomographic study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J. Neurosci. 20, 2664–2672.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

WHO. (2007). The ICD-10 International Classification of Diseases and Related 511] Health Problems, 10th Edn. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Williams, L. E., Light, G. A., Braff, D. L., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2010). Reduced multisensory integration in patients with schizophrenia on a target detection task. Neuropsychologia 48, 3128–3136. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, L. M., Loughland, C. M., Gordon, E., and Davidson, D. (1999). Visual scanpaths in schizophrenia: is there a deficit in face recognition? Schizophr. Res. 40, 189–199. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00056-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witt, K., van Dorn, R., and Fazel, S. (2013). Risk factors for violence in psychosis: systematic review and meta-regression analysis of 110 studies. PLoS ONE 8:e55942. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055942

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wittgenstein, L. (1921). Logisch-Philosophische Abhandlung, Tractatus logico-philosophicus. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.

Wolitzky, R., Goudsmit, N., Goetz, R. R., Printz, D., Gil, R., Harkavy-Friedman, J., et al. (2006). Etiological heterogeneity and intelligence test scores in patients with schizophrenia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 167–177. doi:10.1080/13803390500360315

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wood, S. J., Pantelis, C., Proffitt, T., Phillips, L. J., Stuart, G. W., Buchanan, J. A., et al. (2003). Spatial working memory ability is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychol. Med. 33, 1239–1247. doi:10.1017/S0033291703008067

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yung, A. R., Phillips, L. J., Yuen, H. P., Francey, S. M., McFarlane, C. A., Hallgren, M., et al. (2003). Psychosis prediction: 12-month follow up of a high-risk (“prodromal”) group. Schizophr. Res. 60, 21–32. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00167-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yung, A. R., Phillips, L. J., Yuen, H. P., and McGorry, P. D. (2004). Risk factors for psychosis in an ultra high-risk group: psychopathology and clinical features. Schizophr. Res. 67, 131–142. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00192-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yung, A. R., Yuen, H. P., McGorry, P. D., Phillips, L. J., Kelly, D., Dell'Olio, M., et al. (2005). Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 39, 964-971. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zampini, M., and Spence, C. (2012). “Assessing the role of visual and auditory cues in multisensory perception of flavor,” in The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, eds M. M. Murray and M. T. Wallace (Boca Raton FL: Taylor and Francis Group), 739–758.

Zimler, J., and Keenan, J. M. (1983). Imagery in the con-genitally blind: how visual are visual images. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 9, 269–282. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.9.2.269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zinkus, P. W., and Gottlieb, M. I. (1978). Learning disabilities and juvenile delinquency. Clin. Pediatr. (Phila.) 17, 775–780. doi:10.1177/000992287801701011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: schizophrenia, blindness, visual aberrations, Protection-Against-Schizophrenia (PaSZ), vision therapy, continuous diagnostic criteria, early detection, cognition

Citation: Landgraf S and Osterheider M (2013) “To see or not to see: that is the question.” The “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) model: evidence from congenital blindness and visuo-cognitive aberrations. Front. Psychol. 4:352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00352

Received: 31 January 2013; Accepted: 30 May 2013;
Published online: 01 July 2013.

Edited by:

Steven Silverstein, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, USA

Reviewed by:

Mahesh Menon, University of Toronto, Canada
José Alberto González-Hernández, Hermanos Ameijeiras Hospital, Cuba

Copyright © 2013 Landgraf and Osterheider. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Steffen Landgraf, Department of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, District Hospital, University Regensburg, Haus 45, Raum 001, Universitätsstraße 84, 93053 Regensburg, Germany e-mail: steffen.landgraf@ukr.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.