Event Abstract

The dual origin of semantic errors in access deficits: activation deficit vs. inhibition deficit

  • 1 Johns Hopkins University, Neurology Department, School of Medicine, United States

Two sources have been identified as origins of semantic errors (e.g., “dog” for “cat”) after brain damage: damage to semantic concepts (“storage deficit”), and damage to the links between semantic concepts and lexical representations (“access deficit”; e.g., Lambon-Ralph et al., 2016). I present data suggesting that an access deficit can be further broken down into two distinct deficits: problems in activating the target (activation deficit), and problems in inhibiting competitors (inhibition deficit). These two deficits are expected to show dissociations on three categories of tests listed in Table 1. Lexical-semantic memory. Activation of lexical items supports activation of semantic features, either through feedback connections (e.g., Foygel & Dell, 2000) or simply by verbal rehearsal (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). Either way, poor lexical activation as proposed in the activation deficit should impair semantic working memory. General indirect competitive control. If lexical selection depends on domain-general inhibitory control (e.g., Shao et al., 2015), an inhibition deficit should be accompanied by impaired performance on a non-linguistic task that measures competitive inhibitory control. Naming under increased competitor activation. Any cues that increase competitor activation should promote the production of the competitor in inhibitory deficit because it cannot be properly suppressed. These cues include misleading onset cues (/d/ for cat), or switching between words that share onset or rhyme phonology (e.g., cat-cup). A special dissociation is expected for boosting semantic competitors: the extra activation of semantic space in this case should be beneficial in activation deficit, but detrimental in inhibition deficit. Methods. Participants were two individuals (XR, female, 57; QD, male, 66) with an access-deficit profile summarized in Table 2. Seven controls (Mean age = 61.14) were also tested. Lexical semantic memory was assessed using a modified version of the Category-Probe test (Freedman & Martin, 2001). Participants decided whether a word-probe belonged to the same semantic category as any of the words in a memory list (increasing length 1:6). General indirect competitive control was measured using the spatial Simon task that taps into response inhibition (Lu & Proctor, 1995). Performance under increased lexical competition was assessed using two tasks: a) a miscueing task (see Lambon-Ralph et al., 2016) where a picture was paired either with the correct first sound or the first sound of a semantic competitor. b) a word-pair reversal paradigm (Nozari et al., 2016) in which participants reversed the name of two pictures on several trials. Four conditions included unrelated pairs, semantically-related pairs or pairs overlapping in onset or rhyme. Results. Figure 1 summarizes the results that confirm the predictions in Table 1. Activation deficit is marked by worse semantic working memory, normal general inhibitory control, and relatively better performance on reversed naming when the competitor is semantically related to the target. Inhibition deficit, on the other hand, is marked by intact semantic working memory, impaired inhibitory control, and significantly worse naming accuracy under conditions that increased competitor activation by either semantic or phonological cues. Conclusion. Access deficit comprises two distinct problems: activation deficit and inhibition deficit.

Figure 1

References

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of learning and motivation, 8, 47-89.
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2007). Comparison of a single case to a control or normative sample in neuropsychology: Development of a Bayesian approach. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(4), 343-372.
Freedman, M., & Martin, R. (2001). Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(3), 193–226.
Foygel, D., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of impaired lexical access in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(2), 182-216.
Lambon-Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2016). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 2(2), 174-207.
Nozari, N., Freund, M., Breining, B., Rapp, B., & Gordon, B. (2016). Cognitive control during selection and repair in word production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(7), 886-903.
Roach, A., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Grewal, R. S., & Brecher, A. (1996). The Philadelphia naming test: Scoring and rationale. Clinical Aphasiology, 24, 121–133.
Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., Martin, R. C., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Selective inhibition and naming performance in semantic blocking, picture-word interference, and color–word Stroop tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1806.

Keywords: Aphasia, Executive Function, Inhibitory Control, access deficit, semantic errors

Conference: Academy of Aphasia 55th Annual Meeting , Baltimore, United States, 5 Nov - 7 Nov, 2017.

Presentation Type: poster or oral

Topic: Aphasia

Citation: Nozari N (2019). The dual origin of semantic errors in access deficits: activation deficit vs. inhibition deficit. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 55th Annual Meeting . doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2017.223.00100

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 17 Apr 2017; Published Online: 25 Jan 2019.

* Correspondence: Dr. Nazbanou Nozari, Johns Hopkins University, Neurology Department, School of Medicine, Baltimore, United States, bnozariiu@gmail.com