Event Abstract

Understanding the Bilingual Advantage: Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control

  • 1 Centre de Recherche de l'Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Canada
  • 2 University of Nebraska at Kearney, United States
  • 3 University of the Balearic Islands, Spain

Bilingualism has been associated with an age-related cognitive advantage (1,2). In comparison to monolinguals, bilingual speakers show an added advantage in managing interference (1,3,5,8). An interesting way to study cognitive control is by performing analysis of the response time distribution. Based on the temporal dynamics of the cognitive control mechanism, Braver (4) proposed a “dual mechanism of control (DMC),” which consists of two modes of cognitive control: proactive and reactive. Within a response time distribution, the slow responses reflected reactive control mechanisms and the fast responses proactive control mechanisms (4). The current study provides a methodological innovation to highlight strategy differences between bilingual and monolingual elderly populations. The goal of this research was to gain an in-depth understanding of the behavioral implications of the cognitive control mechanism by breaking performance on an executive control task down into its constituents, and thus to examine the DMC framework (4) and to dissociate the reactive and proactive modes of control by directly comparing their behavioral and neural signatures. Native-French monolinguals (n = 10; mean age = 74.5 years) and French-English bilinguals (n = 10; mean age = 74.2 years) participated in the study. They completed the experimental protocol, which consisted of an interview schedule (LEAP-Q), the experimental task (Simon task inside the MRI scanner) and a linguistic and neuropsychological assessment battery. In line with previous studies (6,7), we examined Response Time (RT) distribution, by studying the fast (Fast RTs) and slow response times (slow RTs) across conditions for each participant (9). A mixed ANOVA with Language group (Monolinguals and Bilinguals) as the between-subject factor and Trial (Slow RTs and Fast RTs) and Condition (Congruent, Neutral and Incongruent) as the within-subject factors was done. For the fMRI analysis, image preprocessing was performed using SPM12 following standard procedures. The design matrix consists of two groups (Monolinguals, Bilinguals), three conditions (Congruent, Incongruent and Neutral), and two trial types (Fast RTs, Slow RTs). A t-test (K ≥ 20, p ≤0.001) was conducted on the contrast images of interest. In addition, functional connectivity analysis was done using a small-world approach (2). Behavioral data indicated differences between the groups only on the slow RTs, indicating a group difference for the congruent (monolinguals > bilinguals; p = .00) and incongruent conditions (monolinguals > bilinguals; p = .00). The fMRI results show that the bilinguals performed efficiently in a proactive mode, as less activation and degree of node distribution were observed for the bilinguals, whereas monolinguals showed less activation of the brain areas in reactive mode, which they use more often because of aging. There is evidence that with age, individuals depend more on the reactive control strategy for performance and the proactive control strategy is more vulnerable to cognitive aging (6). Thus, the lifelong use of two languages protects the proactive control strategy. The study adds to the debate regarding the existence and possible mechanism of the effect of bilingual language experience on cognitive function with aging.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) for funding, along with the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Santé, Government of Quebec for post-doctoral fellowship (to T.D.).

References

1. Ansaldo, A. I., Ghazi-Saidi, L., & Adrover-Roig, D. (2015). Interference control in elderly bilinguals: Appearances can be misleading. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37, 455–470. 2. Berroir, P., Ghazi-Saidi, L., Dash, T., Adrover-Roig, D., Benali, H., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2017). Interference control at the response level: Functional networks reveal higher efficiency in the bilingual brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 4–16. 3. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. 4. Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 106–113. 5. Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. 6. Czernochowski, D., Nessler, D., & Friedman, D. (2010). On why not to rush older adults – relying on reactive cognitive control can effectively reduce errors at the expense of slowed responses. Psychophysiology, 47, 637–646. 7. Fassbender, C., Hester, R., Murphy, K., Foxe, J. J., Foxe, D. M., & Garavan, H. (2009). Prefrontal and midline interactions mediating behavioural control. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 181–187. 8. Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F. I., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170–181. 9. Gupta, R., Kar, B. R., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Development of task switching and post-error-slowing in children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 38.

Keywords: bilingualism, cognitive control, Aging - old age - seniors, Neuroimaging, proactive cognitive control

Conference: Academy of Aphasia 57th Annual Meeting, Macau, Macao, SAR China, 27 Oct - 29 Oct, 2019.

Presentation Type: Platform presentation

Topic: Eligible for student award

Citation: Dash T, Berroir P, Ghazi-Saidi L, Adrover-Roig D and Ansaldo A (2019). Understanding the Bilingual Advantage: Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 57th Annual Meeting. doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.01.00120

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 06 May 2019; Published Online: 09 Oct 2019.

* Correspondence: Dr. Tanya Dash, Centre de Recherche de l'Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Quebec, H3W 1W5, Canada, tani.dash@gmail.com