Event Abstract

High rates of language impairment in vulnerable populations: the case for improving cross-sector awareness of Developmental Language Disorder.

  • 1 University of Greenwich, United Kingdom
  • 2 University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 3 Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, United Kingdom

Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) is a neurodevelopmental condition in which children do not achieve typical levels of language skill. These children may experience difficulties with structural aspects (i.e. syntactic, lexical and phonological problems) as well as pragmatic aspects of language. The prevalence rate of DLD is approximately 7.5% (Norbury et al., 2016). Thus, in a typical UK classroom of 30 students, we can expect on average two children to have DLD. Despite its relative prevalence compared to more widely known conditions such as autism (which has an estimated prevalence of around 1%; Baird et al., 2006), DLD is not well known. This is partly due to a historic lack of agreement on the criteria and terminology for DLD; recent efforts have been made to reach consensus on these issues (Bishop et al., 2016). However, the goal of increased awareness of DLD is not yet reached, and in the meantime, poor awareness of DLD has implications for children’s wellbeing and long-term outcomes. It is unsurprising, given the oral language and literacy demands of most education systems, that children with DLD show poorer academic outcomes compared to typically developing children (Johnson, Beitchman, & Brownlie, 2010). However, children with DLD may also face problems with their attendance: children who are school-refusers have poorer language skills overall, and a higher incidence of language impairments (Naylor, Staskowsi, Kenney & King, 1994). Language problems are also associated with exclusions from school; excluded children show higher rates of expressive language difficulties (Ripley & Yuill, 2005), and studies of mainstream secondary school pupils facing exclusion suggest that two thirds have language problems, with one third showing severe language problems (Clegg, Stackhouse, Finch, Murphy, & Nicholls, 2009). Of the 15 children assessed for this study, only one had received speech and language therapy (for a stammer); none of the other children had been assessed by a speech and language therapist. All children had been excluded three or more times, and were at risk of being permanently excluded from school. Language impairment is also elevated in mental health services. The high rate of language problems in children and adolescents accessing support for emotional and behavioural problems has been summarised in a meta-analysis of 22 studies (Hollo, Wehby, & Oliver, 2014): approximately 81% of children accessing help for an emotional-behavioural disorder have below-average language proficiency, and that 47% of children show moderate to severe language problems. None of the children in the studies had previously been assessed for speech or language problems. As yet, there is no data to indicate whether the high rate of language problems in these services has an impact on progress in psychological therapy. Language problems also show relationships to involvement with the criminal justice system. Language-impaired youth have higher rates of arrests and convictions (Brownlie et al., 2004), and the rate of language impairment in young offender institutions is elevated (see Anderson, Hawes, & Snow, 2016 for a systematic review). A recent study found that 47% of the 93 young offenders assessed showed language skills significantly below what would be expected for their age, and 20% would have met the criteria for language impairment (Hughes et al., 2017). The majority of those identified as having a language impairment had not previously worked with a speech and language therapist. It is thus clear that in a variety of different vulnerable populations we see higher rates of language impairment than would be expected given the prevalence in the general population, and that many of these cases go undetected. It is important to emphasise that many children with DLD go on to have good outcomes, and that language impairment is not deterministic of the problems discussed here (see Johnson et al, 2010). This is important, as it suggests firstly that there are factors that moderate the relationship between language problems and wellbeing, and secondly because it offers hope that, with appropriate identification and support, children with DLD can have good outcomes. Nonetheless, a pertinent question is why are rates of DLD increased across these different settings? There are number of plausible reasons. Firstly, some of the risk factors for poor language development and emotional and behavioural problems are overlapping, such as social disadvantage (Horwitz et al., 2003; Olak et al., 2009; Singh & Ghandour, 2012). Secondly, language problems themselves may contribute to social and psychological difficulties that increase the risk of emotional and behaviour problems. Specifically, language problems may lead children to experience social isolation, reducing opportunities for acquiring good social skills and increasing the risk of poor self-esteem and poor emotional outcomes (Craig, 1993; Redmound & Rice, 1998; Wadman, Durkin, & Conti-Ramsden, 2008). Children with language problems are also at increased risk of being bullied (Redmond, 2011), and of having poorer emotion regulation (Fujiki, Brinton & Clarke, 2002). Finally, and of relevance to an array of different professions, when their language problems are not recognised, the behaviours of children and young people with DLD may be interpreted by the adults in their environment in a way that increases the likelihood of poor outcomes. In the context of forensic interviewing, Snow and Powell (2012) argue that the presentation of language problems in young offenders may lead professionals to assume that the young person is rude, or incoherent with their account of events (and therefore likely guilty). One account given by a young person in prison stated that when asked by a judge if he was remorseful of his crime, he said no: he had never heard the word “remorseful” before, and had no idea what it meant (Talbot, 2007). If such an experience is representative of young people with communication disorders’ interactions with the criminal justice system, it is perhaps not surprising that the rate of language problems is so high in young offender institutions. Extrapolating this to the headteacher’s office or the psychiatrist’s chair, we may predict that children with DLD are similarly judged as being uncooperative, unapologetic or reticent. It would be naïve to assume that untrained professionals will readily detect language problems in their cohorts. Rather, language impairments will not always be obvious to people interacting with a person with DLD; adults have a tendency to assume that children understand more of what is being said to them than is actually the case (Sattler, Feldman, & Bohanan, 1985), and children may engage in masking behaviours that cover their language problems (Evans & Macwhinney, 1999). Indeed, teachers have quite poor sensitivity and specificity to children’s oral language impairments (Antoniazzi, Snow & Dickson-Swift, 2010). Even in settings where we might expect educational staff to have a higher level of knowledge regarding language problems, this is not necessarily the case: in a special school for children with emotional and behavioural difficulties in which the prevalence of language impairment was 74%, less than half of these children had been identified as impaired (Stringer & Lozano, 2007). Speech problems may be the most detectable issue, but children with pure speech problems actually do not show adverse outcomes, compared to typical children (Johnson et al., 2010). Possibly this is because speech problems do not affect children’s social and emotional development in the same way that a language problem does. Alternatively, perhaps a child with a speech problem is far more likely to be identified and supported than a child who has a language impairment but whose speech does not sound unusual. What happens to these children and young people if their language problems are not recognised? The increased rates of language impairment in children facing exclusion from school and in the criminal justice system paint a gloomy picture. DLD is considered a lifelong condition, and while children’s language skills may improve, they require input from specialists to help them acquire skills and strategies to compensate for their language problems (Perkins, 2001). Clearly there are many children who finish schooling and enter services aiming to remediate mental health problems or criminal behaviours without their language problems having been detected. It is therefore important that awareness of DLD is sufficient in professions that are very likely to interact with children with DLD, including mental health and forensic professionals.

Acknowledgements

Geoff Bird was supported by the Baily Thomas Charitable Trust.

References

Anderson, S. A. S., Hawes, D. J., & Snow, P. C. (2016). Language impairments among youth offenders: A systematic review. Children and Youth Services Review, 65(April), 195–203. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.04.004 Antoniazzi, D., Snow, P., & Dickson-Swift, V. (2010). Teacher identification of children at risk for language impairment in the first year of school. International journal of speech-language pathology, 12(3), 244-252. Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., & Charman, T. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69041-7 Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., Adams, C., Archibald, L., … Whitehouse, A. (2016). CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS ONE, 11(7), 1–26. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158753 Brownlie, E. B., Beitchman, J. H., Escobar, M., Young, A., Atkinson, L., Johnson, C., ... & Douglas, L. (2004). Early language impairment and young adult delinquent and aggressive behavior. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 32(4), 453-467. Clegg, J., Stackhouse, J., Finch, K., Murphy, C., & Nicholls, S. (2009). Language abilities of secondary age pupils at risk of schoolClegg, J., Stackhouse, J., Finch, K., Murphy, C., & Nicholls, S. (2009). Language abilities of secondary age pupils at risk of school exclusion: A preliminary report. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 25(1), 123–139. http://doi.org/10.1177/0265659008098664 Craig, H. K. (1993). Social skills of children with specific language impairment: Peer relationships. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 24(4), 206-215. Evans, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive-receptive specific language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 34(2), 117-134. Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Clarke, D. (2002). Emotion regulation in children with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33(2), 102-111. Hollo, A., Wehby, J. H., & Oliver, R. M. (2014). Unidentified language deficits in children with emotional and behavioral disorders: A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 80(2), 169–186. http://doi.org/10.1177/001440291408000203 Horwitz, S. M. C., Irwin, J. R., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Bosson Heenan, J. M., Mendoza, J., & Carter, A. S. (2003). Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(8), 932–940. http://doi.org/10.1097/01.CHI.0000046889.27264.5E Hughes, N., Chitsabesan, P., Bryan, K., Borschmann, R., Swain, N., Lennox, C., & Shaw, J. (2017). Language impairment and comorbid vulnerabilities among young people in custody. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58(10), 1106–1113. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12791 Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., & Brownlie, E. B. (2010). Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: Family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1), 51–65. http://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0083) Naylor, M. W., Staskowski, M., Kenney, M. C., & King, C. A. (1994). Language disorders and learning disabilities in school-refusing adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(9), 1331-1337. Norbury, C. F., Gooch, D., Wray, C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., … Pickles, A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57(11), 1247–1257. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12573 Amone-P’Olak, K., Burger, H., Ormel, J., Huisman, M., Verhulst, F. C., & Oldehinkel, A. J. (2009). Socioeconomic position and mental health problems in pre-and early-adolescents. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology, 44(3), 231-238. Perkins, M. R. (2001). Compensatory strategies in SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 15(1–2), 67–71. http://doi.org/10.1080/026992001461334 Redmond, S. M. (2011). Peer victimization among students with specific language impairment, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42(4), 520-535. Redmond, S. M., & Rice, M. L. (1998). The socioemotional behaviors of children with SLI: Social adaptation or social deviance?. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(3), 688-700. Ripley, K., & Yuill, N. (2005). Patterns of language impairment and behaviour in boys excluded from school. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(Pt 1), 37–50. http://doi.org/10.1348/000709905X27696 Sattler, J. M., Feldman, J., & Bohanan, A. L. (1985). Parental estimates of children’s receptive vocabulary. Psychology in the Schools, 22(3), 303–307. http://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(198507)22:3<303::AID-PITS2310220312>3.0.CO;2-J Singh, G. K., & Ghandour, R. M. (2012). Impact of neighborhood social conditions and household socioeconomic status on behavioral problems among US children. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 16(SUPPL. 1), 158–169. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-012-1005-z Snow, P., & Powell, M. (2012). Youth (in) justice: Oral language competence in early life and risk for engagement in antisocial behaviour in adolescence. Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice, 435, 1-6. Stringer, H., & Lozano, S. (2007). Under identification of speech and language impairment in children attending a special school for children with emotional and behavioural disorders. Educational & Child Psychology, 24(4), 9–19. Talbot, J. (2007) No One Knows: Identifying and Supporting Prisoners with Learning Difficulties and Learning Disabilities. The Views of Prison Staff. London: Prison Reform Trust Wadman, R., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). Self-esteem, shyness, and sociability in adolescents with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(4), 938-952.

Keywords: developmental language disorder (DLD), Language, vulnerable population group, Mental Health, Policy & disabilities

Conference: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates, 10 Mar - 11 Mar, 2019.

Presentation Type: Oral Presentation (invited speakers only)

Topic: Educational Neuroscience

Citation: Hobson HM and Bird G (2019). High rates of language impairment in vulnerable populations: the case for improving cross-sector awareness of Developmental Language Disorder.. Conference Abstract: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.229.00002

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 08 Feb 2019; Published Online: 27 Sep 2019.

* Correspondence: Dr. Hannah M Hobson, University of Greenwich, Greenwich, London, SE10 9LS, United Kingdom, H.Hobson@greenwich.ac.uk