Event Abstract

Passive Sentence Comprehension in Thai Agrammatic Speakers

  • 1 University of Potsdam, Linguistics, Germany
  • 2 University of Groningen, Clinical Linguistics, Netherlands
  • 3 University of Eastern Finland, Linguistics, Finland

Several studies have reported that sentence comprehension in agrammatic aphasia is relatively impaired. In particular, semantically reversible sentences whose arguments are in their derived, not in their base, positions (e.g. object relative clauses, object clefts, and passives) are shown to be less correctly understood than their counterparts (e.g. subject relative clauses, subject clefts, and actives). According to the Derived Order Problem Hypothesis (DOP-H) (Bastiaanse & Van Zonneveld, 2005; 2006), comprehension of derived order sentences is poorer than that of the base order ones. Many studies have confirmed this (Bastiaanse & Thompson, 2003; Bastiaanse & Van Zonneveld, 2005; 2006; Yarbay Duman, Ozgirgin, Altinok & Bastiaanse, 2011). Yet, the focus of these previous studies has primarily been on Indo-European languages. Scarce attention has been given to Southeast Asian ones, such as Thai. As Thai uses a particular type of passives (passives with adversative verbs, or passive-bias verbs), a derived order, as frequently as actives, a base order, it is not clear yet whether the same impairment is observed. This raises two questions: (1) Are there similarities or differences in active and passive sentence comprehension in Thai agrammatic speakers? (2) Is comprehension of passive sentences with adversative verbs (passive-bias verbs), whose arguments are in a derived order, impaired in Thai agrammatic speakers? In our study, we examined not only the actives and passives sentence comprehension, but also the interaction effect of word orders (actives and passives) and verb types (adversative verbs and non-adversative verbs). We administered a two-choice sentence-picture matching task on 4 agrammatic speakers and 4 non-brain-damaged speakers. Our test items were categorized into 4 conditions on a 2x2 design (word orders x verb types). According to the DOP-H, for agrammatic speakers, a good performance is predicted for sentences in base order—actives—while a poor performance is predicted for sentences in derived order—passives. However, we hypothesize that there will be an interaction effect with frequency. In brief, the derived order sentences will be more difficult to comprehend in passives with non-adversative verbs, where they are less frequently used, than passives with adversative verbs. The passive sentences with adversative verbs will also be comprehended less well than the active sentences (both with adversative and non-adversative verbs) due to the high frequency of adversative passive sentences in Thai. As for the actives base order, it is hypothesized that active sentences with non-adversative verbs will be comprehended as equally well as the active sentences with adversative verbs. Having gathered the data, and performed a generalized linear model, we found that, as predicted by the DOP-H, the agrammatic speakers have more problems comprehending sentences in a derived order (passives) than in a base order (actives) condition. The interaction effect of sentence types and verb types was observed. Participants’ performances vary depending on both word order and verb frequency. Further, as hypothesized, comprehension of passive construction with non-adversative verbs is more challenging than that of passive construction with adversative verbs (passive-bias verbs). The finding implies that it is not only the derived order that is a problem in Thai individuals with agrammatic aphasia, but also frequency and/or lexical bias—verb types—that play a role in sentence comprehension.

Figure 1

Acknowledgements

My sincere gratitude goes to all of my supervisors, Prof. Dr. Frank Burchert, Dr. Alexandre Nikolaev, and Prof. Dr. Roelien Bastiaanse. Despite the long distance of each professor’s whereabouts—Germany, Finland, and the Netherlands—, their continuous support and guidance did not cease to stop. I thank my colleagues and my coordinators from the Erasmus Mundus program in Clinical Linguistics (EMCL) for making me feel like home wherever I am.

References

Abuom, T., Shah, E., & Bastiaanse, R. (2013). Sentence comprehension in Swahili-English agrammatic speakers. Journal of Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 355–370.

Anjarningsih, H. Y., Haryadi-Soebadi, R. D., Gofir, A., & Bastiaanse, R. (2012). Characterising agrammatism in Standard Indonesian. Aphasiology, 26, 757–784.

Avrutin, S. (2006). Weak syntax. In Amunts, K. & Grodzinsky, Y. (eds.) Broca’s region. (pp 49-62). New York: Oxford Press.

Bastiaanse, R., Bouma, G., & Post, W. (2009). Linguistic complexity and frequency in agrammatic speech production. Brain and Language, 109, 18-28.

Bastiaanse, R., & Thompson, C.K. (2003). Verb and auxiliary movement in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 84, 286-305.

Bastiaanse, R. & Edwards, S. (2004). Word order and finiteness in Dutch and English Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 89, 91-107.

Bastiaanse, R., & Van Zonneveld, R. (2005). Sentence production with verbs of alternating transitivity in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 57–66.

Bastiaanse, R., & Van Zonneveld, R. (2006). Comprehension of passives in Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 96, 135-142.

Berndt, R., Mitchun, C. & Haendiges, A. (1996). Comprehension of reversible sentences in ‘agrammatism’: A meta-analysis. Cognition, 58, 289-308.

Burchert, F., De Bleser, R., & Sonntag, K. (2003). Does morphology make the difference? Agrammatic sentence comprehension in German. Brain and Language, 87, 323–342. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00132-9


Burchert, F., Swoboda-Moll, M., & De Bleser, R. (2005). Tense and agreement dissociations in German agrammatic speakers: underspecification versus hierarchy. Brain and Language, 94, 188-199.

Burchert, F., Meiner, N. & De Bleser, R. (2008). Production of non-canonical sentences in agrammatic aphasia: Limits in representation or rule application? Brain and Language, 104, 170-179.

Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572-582.

Caplan, D., & Futter, C. (1986). Assignment of thematic roles by an agrammatic aphasic patient. Brain and Language, 27, 117-135.

Drai, D., Grodzinsky, Y. (2006). The variability debate: More statistics, more linguistics. Brain & Language, 76.2, 157-170.

Filbeck, D. (1973). The Passive in Thai. Anthropological Linguistics, 15.1, 33-41.

Gahl, S., Menn, L., Ramsberger, G., Jurafsky, D. S., Elder, E., Rewega, M., & Audrey, L. H. (2003). Syntactic frame and verb bias in aphasia: Plausibility judgments of undergoer-subject sentences. Brain and Cognition, 53, 223- 228.

Goodglass, H. (1976). Agrammatism. In H. Whitaker & H.A. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in Neurolinguistics (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

Grodzinsky, Y. (1984). The syntactic characterization of agrammatism. Cognition,16, 99-120.

Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). Overarching agrammatism. In Grodzinsky, Y., L. Shapiro & D. Swinney, eds., 2000. Language and the brain. San Diego: Academic Press.

Jap, B.J., Martinez-Ferreiro, S. & Bastiaanse, R (2016) The effect of syntactic frequency on sentence comprehension in Standard Indonesian. Aphasiology, 30, 1325-1340.

Lukatela, K., Shankweiler D., & Crain, S. (1995). Syntactic processing in agrammatic aphasia by speakers of a Slavic language. Brain and Language, 49, 50-76.

Menn, L. (2000). It’s time to face a simple question: What makes canonical form simple? Brain and Language.

Menn, L., Reilly, K. F., Hayashi, M., Kamio, A., Fujita, I., & Sasanuma, S. (1998). The interaction of preserved pragmatics and impaired syntax in Japanese and English aphasic speech. Brain and Language, 61, 183–225.

Prasithrathsint, A. (2006). Development of the thùuk passive marker in Thai. In Werner Abraham and Larisa Leisio (eds), Passivization and Typology: Form and Function. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 115-131.

Pinango, M. (2000). Syntactic displacement in Broca’s agrammatic aphasia. In: R. Bastiaanse & Y. Grodzinsky (Eds.), Grammatical disorders in aphasia (pp.75-87). London: Whurr Publishers.


R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.R-project. org/.


Sudmuk, C. (2003). The thuuk construction in Thai. In Proceedings of the LFG03 Conference, ed. M. Butt and T.H. King. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Thompson, C. K. (2003). Unaccusative verb production in agrammatic aphasia: the argument structure complexity hypothesis. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 151-167.

Wongbaisaj, S. (1979). On the passive in Thai. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 9.1, 207–216.

Yarbay Duman, T., Ozgirgin, N., Altinok, N., & Bastiaanse, R. (2011). Sentence comprehension in Turkish Broca’s aphasia: An integration problem. Aphasiology, 25, 908-926.

Keywords: Derived Order Problem Hypothesis (DOP-H), Frequency, Lexical Bias, passive sentences, word order, agrammatic aphasia, Thai, sentence comprehension

Conference: Academy of Aphasia 55th Annual Meeting , Baltimore, United States, 5 Nov - 7 Nov, 2017.

Presentation Type: oral presentation

Topic: Consider for student award

Citation: Siriboonpipattana W, Burchert F, Bastiaanse R and Nikolaev A (2019). Passive Sentence Comprehension in Thai Agrammatic Speakers. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 55th Annual Meeting . doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2017.223.00102

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 02 May 2017; Published Online: 25 Jan 2019.

* Correspondence: Miss. Wilasinee Siriboonpipattana, University of Potsdam, Linguistics, Potsdam, 14476, Germany, wilasinee.sir@gmail.com