Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 15 July 2020
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 12 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2020.00197

Heart Rate Variability Mainly Relates to Cognitive Executive Functions and Improves Through Exergame Training in Older Adults: A Secondary Analysis of a 6-Month Randomized Controlled Trial

Patrick Eggenberger1,2* Simon Annaheim1 Kerstin A. Kündig1,2 René M. Rossi1 Thomas Münzer3,4 Eling D. de Bruin2,5
  • 1Empa, Swiss Federal Laboratories for Materials Science and Technology, Laboratory for Biomimetic Membranes and Textiles, St. Gallen, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Health Sciences and Technology, Institute of Human Movement Sciences and Sport, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Geriatrische Klinik St. Gallen, St. Gallen, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Geriatric Medicine, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Heart rate variability (HRV) mirrors autonomic nervous system activities and might serve as a parameter to monitor health status in older adults. However, it is currently unknown which functional health measures, including cognitive, physical, and gait performance parameters, are most strongly related to HRV indices. This knowledge would enable implementing HRV assessments into health monitoring routines and training planning for older adults. Simultaneous cognitive–motor and exergame training may be effective to improve HRV indices but has not been investigated yet. Eighty-nine healthy older adults (≥70 years of age) were randomized into three groups: (1) virtual reality video game dancing, i.e., exergaming (DANCE); (2) treadmill walking with simultaneous verbal memory training (MEMORY); or (3) treadmill walking only (PHYS). Strength and balance exercises complemented each program. Over 6 months, two weekly 1-h training sessions were performed. HRV indices (standard deviation of N–N intervals, SDNN; root mean square of successive R–R interval differences, RMSSD; and absolute power of high-frequency band (0.15–0.4 Hz), HF power) and various measures of cognitive, physical, and gait performance were assessed at baseline and after 3 months and 6 months. Multiple linear regression analyses with planned comparisons were calculated. At baseline, 8–12% of HRV variance was significantly explained by cognitive executive functions and leg strength (inversely related). Verbal long-term memory, aerobic and functional fitness, and gait performance did not contribute to the model (SDNN: R2 = 0.082, p = 0.016; RMSSD: R2 = 0.121, p = 0.013; HF power: R2 = 0.119, p = 0.015). After 6 months, DANCE improved HRV indices, while MEMORY and PHYS did not (time × intervention interactions: first-contrast DANCE/MEMORY vs. PHYS: SDNN p = 0.014 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.020 and RMSSD p = 0.052 one-tailed (trend), ΔR2 = 0.007; second-contrast DANCE vs. MEMORY: SDNN p = 0.002 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.035, RMSSD p = 0.017 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.012, and HF power p = 0.011 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.013). We conclude that mainly cognitive executive functions are associated with HRV indices and that exergame training improves global and parasympathetic autonomic nervous system activities in older adults. Periodic assessments of HRV in older citizens could be particularly beneficial to monitor cognitive health and provide indications for preventative exercise measures.

Introduction

Health monitoring that aims to introduce preventative strategies is increasingly important in the rapidly growing population of older citizens. Adequate and accurate health monitoring approaches could thereby help to reduce individual suffering and overall healthcare costs. A promising and easily measurable parameter to monitor current health status and predict future health outcomes of older adults is heart rate variability (HRV). HRV indices mirror global, sympathetic, and parasympathetic activities of the autonomic nervous system (Malik, 1996). Low HRV values are usually indicative of compromised health and increased mortality (Ernst, 2017; Kemp et al., 2017). It has been proposed that physiological systems that oscillate within a range of states (which is the case for high HRV) can adapt flexibly to various inputs, whereas dysregulated systems are locked in a specific pattern (similar to low HRV; Thayer et al., 2012). As such, HRV measures may reflect the adaptivity of the brain–body system (Ernst, 2017). Furthermore, HRV indices are associated with numerous domains of cognitive and physical functioning in older adults (Albinet et al., 2010; Ogliari et al., 2015; Freitas et al., 2018).

Different domains of cognitive functioning, including global cognition, executive functions, reaction time, and processing speed, are related to HRV indices in older adults. Frewen et al. (2013) reported that older adults with lower HRV recordings also performed worse in a global cognition test, independently of confounders. Similarly, reduced HRV values in older adults were associated with lower performance in global cognitive functioning (Al Hazzouri et al., 2014). A recent meta-analysis indicated that HRV indices may be applied as biomarkers of top-down self-regulation processes, such as executive functioning, emotion regulation, and effortful control (Holzman and Bridgett, 2017). Additionally, lower resting 10-second HRV recordings were linked to slower reaction times and processing speed in older adults independently of possible confounders including medication, cardiovascular risk factors, and comorbidities (Mahinrad et al., 2016).

Reduced HRV, particularly the index standard deviation of normal-to-normal beat intervals (SDNN), is related to poorer physical functional status in activities of daily living (ADL) and predicts the risk of future functional decline of older adults, as reported in a study with 5,042 participants (Ogliari et al., 2015). Low physical capabilities, assessed with functional tests, including single-leg stance for balance, gait speed, and chair rises for leg strength, were associated with decreased HRV values in obese older adults (Liao et al., 2017). Additionally, HRV data (SDNN) predicted longevity in persons at very old age (86 ± 14 years), in contrast to an annual health examination that was not associated with this outcome (Kurita et al., 2013). HRV is also a good predictor of mortality due to chronic heart failure, which is a condition related to autonomic dysfunction (Nolan et al., 1998). Furthermore, HRV appears to be sensitive to exercise training-induced adaptations and, therefore, may be suited to monitor beneficial effects of preventative and therapeutic interventions. Particularly, aerobic exercise training was repeatedly shown to modulate HRV indices in older adults. A recent meta-analysis containing 12 studies and 329 participants beyond age 60 years demonstrated positive effects of aerobic exercise training on the global HRV index SDNN. Thereby, improvements in HRV were linearly increasing with higher exercise frequency (Raffin et al., 2019).

Supported by this previous research, HRV indices seem to provide a promising measure to assess cognitive and physical functional health risk factors in early preclinical phases. Hence, HRV monitoring may enable the early adoption of preventative interventions, such as exercise and cognitive training. In addition, it provides a measure that is sensitive to follow-up exercise training-induced adaptations.

However, thus far it remains unknown how much of the variance in HRV indices is associated with different cognitive, physical, and gait performance parameters, reflecting functional health of older adults. This knowledge would be important in order to implement HRV measures into a monitoring routine for older adults at risk for developing cognitive or physical health issues and inform about the potentially important exercise programming components. Exercise-training modalities that simultaneously incorporate both cognitive and motor aspects can be hypothesized to have a different effect compared to exercise programs targeting only physical components. However, such combined cognitive–motor programs have not yet been investigated in relation to HRV in older adults. Based on the interconnection of HRV with cognitive and physical health factors outlined above, combined cognitive–motor training through exergame training potentially bears larger effects on HRV indices compared to exclusively physical (aerobic) training.

Therefore, the aims of this study were: (1) to investigate, in a cross-sectional analysis, which cognitive, physical, and gait parameters explain the variance in HRV indices at baseline; and (2) to investigate in a longitudinal analysis whether cognitive–motor training induces different adaptations in HRV indices compared to exclusively physical training.

We hypothesized: (1) that variance in HRV indices is explained by; (a) cognitive parameters related to executive functions and verbal memory; (b) physical parameters related to physical functioning, aerobic endurance, and leg strength; and (c) gait parameters related to executive functions and, furthermore, (2) that exercise training-induced adaptations in HRV indices primarily occur in combined cognitive–motor training interventions and particularly in training forms which include aspects of executive functioning (i.e., exergame training).

Materials and Methods

Study Design and Participants

This is a secondary data analysis of a randomized controlled trial, comprising a 6-month training intervention and three parallel intervention groups (Eggenberger et al., 2015a,b). Measurements were performed at baseline (pre-training) and after 3 (mid-training) and 6 months (post-training), respectively (Figure 1). Data collection and training took place at Geriatrische Klinik, St. Gallen, Switzerland. The ethics committee of the Canton St. Gallen, Switzerland, approved the study protocol (EKSG 12/092), and the study was registered at Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN70130279. The planned methods remained unchanged after trial commencement. Reporting of this study adheres to the CONSORT guidelines (Moher et al., 2010).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of the study design. Notes: assessments of HRV indices, cognitive, physical, and gait performance were conducted at each test time-point. Abbreviations: DANCE, virtual reality video game dancing; HRV, heart rate variability; MEMORY, treadmill walking with simultaneous verbal memory training; PHYS, treadmill walking.

Healthy older adults (≥70 years of age) were recruited from the community and residential homes between August until September 2012. The training intervention started in October 2012 and ended at the end of March 2013. Details on recruitment procedures, eligibility criteria, a priori sample size estimation, and randomization are summarized previously (Eggenberger et al., 2015a,b). Related to the eligibility criteria, particularly older adults with health conditions such as dementia, stroke, or recent head injury that could potentially confound the results of cognitive performance outcomes were excluded.

Training

Training sessions were performed twice weekly for 1 h with a pause of 1–2 days to allow recovery. Fifty-two training sessions were completed within 6 months (26 weeks). Each of the three intervention groups’ sessions contained 20 min of strength and 20 min of balance training based on current recommendations for physical fitness and fall prevention for older adults (American College of Sports Medicine et al., 2009; Sherrington et al., 2011; Granacher et al., 2012). Additionally, the DANCE group performed 20 min of virtual reality video game dancing, i.e., exergaming (Impact Dance Platforms, Positive Gaming BV, Haarlem, the Netherlands; StepMania Software), the MEMORY group performed 20 min of treadmill walking with concurrent memory training (presented on a computer screen), and the PHYS group performed 20 min of treadmill walking only. This resulted in two training intervention arms including simultaneous cognitive–motor training (DANCE and MEMORY) and one training intervention arm comprising exclusively physical training (PHYS), which served as an active control group. Thereby, the virtual reality video game dance training combines cognitive aspects of attention and executive functions with motor coordination, whereas the treadmill walking memory training comprised verbal memory exercises. Further details of each training intervention were reported elsewhere (Eggenberger et al., 2015a,b).

Measurements

Heart Rate Variability (Primary Outcome)

HRV was assessed before the 6-min-walk test (6-MWT). Participants were instructed to sit in a comfortable position on a chair without speaking for 5 min, which is the standard time period for short-term recordings (Malik, 1996). During this time, beat-to-beat R–R intervals were recorded by means of a wearable heart rate monitor and chest belt (RS800CX Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland). This device provides valid and reliable test–retest HRV recordings (Williams et al., 2017). Based on the R–R intervals, the following eight HRV indices were calculated: the time-domain indices SDNN (ms), pNN50 (percentage of adjacent normal-to-normal intervals that differ by more than 50 ms, %), and RMSSD (root mean square of successive R–R interval differences, ms); the frequency-domain indices LF power (absolute power of low-frequency band, ms2, 0.04–0.15 Hz), HF power (absolute power of high-frequency band, ms2, 0.15–0.4 Hz), and the ratio of LF to HF power (%); and non-linear indices SD1 (Poincaré-plot standard deviation perpendicular to the line of identity, ms) and SD2 (Poincaré-plot standard deviation along the line of identity, ms). SDNN is an estimate of overall or global HRV and is influenced by both the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system; LF power and SD2 are not only pure measures of sympathetic drive but also affected by parasympathetic activity; and the LF/HF ratio intends to estimate the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic effects, whereas pNN50, RMSSD, HF power, and SD1 represent high-frequency variation in heart rate and are interpreted as proxy for the activity in the parasympathetic or vagally mediated division of the autonomic nervous system (Malik, 1996; Ernst, 2017; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017).

Secondary Outcome Measures

Seven cognitive performance tasks, which cover different cognitive domains, were chosen to identify associations with HRV indices. These “paper-and-pencil” tasks included the Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B; Lezak et al., 2012) to assess the executive function “shifting” (Miyake et al., 2000) and the Executive Control Task (Baller et al., 2006) to assess working memory or the executive function “updating” (Miyake et al., 2000). Furthermore, three different parallel versions of the Paired-Associates Learning task (Baller et al., 2006) were used to assess visual long-term memory, while the German version (Härting et al., 2000) of the Logical Memory subtest (Story Recall, story A, no delayed recall) from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) was applied to assess verbal long-term memory. The Trail Making Test Part A (TMT-A; Lezak et al., 2012) and the Digit Symbol Substitution Task (DSST) from Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981) were used to assess information processing speed. Finally, the Age Concentration Test A with three different parallel versions (Gatterer, 2008) was applied to assess attention (the original test was adapted such that the test result was calculated as “number of correct figures” divided by “time”). Results of training-induced adaptations in cognitive parameters were presented in Eggenberger et al. (2015a).

Four physical performance parameters were selected to identify associations with HRV indices. These comprised the 6-MWT walking distance, the total score of the Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB), the five chair-rise test time (5-CR), and the balance test score. The 6-MWT was used to assess functional aerobic endurance performance (ATS Committee on Proficiency Standards for Clinical Pulmonary Function Laboratories, 2002). Participants had to walk as far as possible on a 30-m walking course within 6 min. Lower-extremity functioning and general functional fitness were measured with the SPPB, which includes a balance test, a 3-m-walk test, and the 5-CR (Guralnik et al., 1994). Thereby, the 3-m-walk test assesses preferred walking speed, whereas the 5-CR reflects functional leg strength. The standard SPPB balance test was extended by adding two increased levels of difficulty to avoid potential ceiling effects (Eggenberger et al., 2015b).

Six temporal and spatial gait parameters were selected to identify associations with HRV indices, including “step time variability” and “step length variability” at preferred walking speed, as well as gait velocity under single- (ST) and dual-task (DT) conditions and at preferred and fast walking speeds: “velocity preferred-ST,” “velocity fast-ST,” “velocity preferred-DT,” and “velocity fast-DT.” The parameters were chosen based on findings, which have demonstrated their relation to brain functional or structural aspects (Tian et al., 2017; Ghanavati et al., 2018; Lucas et al., 2018). The GAITRite electronic walkway (CIR Systems, Havertown, PA, USA) with Platinum Version 4.0 software was used to assess gait parameters. A detailed description of the applied gait assessment procedure and the results of training-induced adaptations in gait and physical performance parameters have been reported in Eggenberger et al. (2015b).

HRV Data Processing

Electrocardiogram (ECG) and R-peaks were recorded at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz, providing a 1-ms temporal resolution for each R–R interval (Williams et al., 2017). Polar ProTrainer 5 software (Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland) was used to transmit the recorded data from the watch to the computer. The first 30 s of the recording were deleted to reduce initial motion artifacts, and the subsequent 5 min were used for the analyses. Two different methods were applied for artifact reduction: (a) interpolation of artifacts with Kubios HRV analysis software (version 2.2, Kubios Oy, Kuopio, Finland; settings: custom = 0.2, interpolation rate = 4 Hz); and (b) deletion of artifacts with Matlab software (version R2016b, MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA), i.e., R–R intervals <200 ms, >2,000 ms, or >20% interval to interval deviation were removed. Based on the criteria above, the artifact examination with Matlab software yielded a mean of 1.2% (±3.1%) artifacts per R–R interval recording (i.e., 5-min measurements). R–R interval time series with ≥80% non-artifact R–R intervals were used for further analyses (Peltola, 2012), while the time series below this value (and the associated HRV indices) were replaced with the group mean value. This procedure resulted in two 5-min measurements that needed to be replaced (1.1% of all measurements).

After the artifact correction in Kubios or deletion in Matlab, respectively, the HRV processing was performed according to the procedure described by Gąsior et al. (2016). Thereby, data was imported into Kubios where the HRV indices were extracted. Detrending (settings: method = smoothn priors, Lambda = 1,000, fc = 0.029 Hz) and power spectral analysis by means of fast Fourier transformation (settings: HF range = 0.15–0.4 Hz, window width = 60 s, window overlap = 50%) were applied. In order to remove bias on HRV resulting from varying resting heart rates, the time and frequency-domain indices were normalized with respect to the mean R–R interval using the following formulae: SDNN/mean R–R2, pNN50/mean R–R7, RMSSD/mean R–R3, LF power/mean R–R2, HF power/mean R–R4, and LF to HF power ratio/mean R–R2 (Sacha, 2013; Gąsior et al., 2016). SD1 and SD2 were not normalized, as no corresponding procedure was described by the authors.

Statistical Analyses

We performed one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) to determine group differences at baseline (pre-training). Multiple linear regression analyses (stepwise backward method) were applied to analyze if the HRV indices (as individual dependent variables) are associated with specific cognitive, physical, or gait parameters at baseline (pre-training). Initially, the cognitive, physical, and gait parameters were analyzed separately with one backward regression analysis for each of these three groups of predictors. Based on these initial calculations, three HRV indices in which most of the variance could be explained were selected and used for all further analyses. Furthermore, the five cognitive, physical, and gait parameters that explained most of the variance in HRV were selected and combined in one final regression analysis to predict the HRV indices. Thereby, age and body mass index (BMI) of the participants were included as additional predictors in the regression model. Hence, for each of the three selected HRV indices, four multiple linear regression analyses were calculated in total. Due to the use of preplanned hypotheses and the exploratory nature of this study, we did not adjust p-values to account for possible multiple comparison effects (Perneger, 1998; Streiner and Norman, 2011; Armstrong, 2014). This procedure was applied: (a) to the data set edited with artifact interpolation in Kubios; and (b) to the data set edited by deletion of artifacts in Matlab to evaluate if the different data processing methods would produce comparable results in terms of which predictors are most important.

The effects of the 6-month training intervention on the three selected HRV indices were analyzed by means of multiple linear regression analysis with planned comparisons, including orthogonal contrasts and polynomial trend coding. Based on our predefined hypotheses, we produced contrast-coding variables. In the first contrast, we compared the two combined cognitive–motor training intervention groups (DANCE and MEMORY) with the exclusively physical training group (PHYS). In the second contrast, the two cognitive–motor training intervention groups were compared against each other (DANCE vs. MEMORY). Effect code variables were created for every group’s individuals to account for subject effects. Missing HRV data related to measurement artifacts (see “HRV Data Processing” section) and from participants who attended the full testing period but missed particular test items due to health constraints or personal obligations were replaced by the mean value of the group at the respective time point of measurement and are specified in the results section (simple mean imputation method). Supplementary mediation analyses were performed (Field, 2018; Bherer et al., 2019) to evaluate if changes in cognitive executive functions (TMT-B and Executive Control Task) mediated exergame training (DANCE) effects on changes in HRV indices. All statistical analyses were executed with IBM SPSS Statistics software for Windows (version 25, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) with a significance level of α = 0.05. For mediation analyses, the PROCESS macro for SPSS was used (Hayes, 2013). Effect sizes in multiple linear regression analyses were defined as small for R2 = 0.02 and ΔR2 = 0.01, medium for R2 = 0.13 and ΔR2 = 0.06, and large for R2 ≥ 0.26 and ΔR2 ≥ 0.14. Magnitude of effect size r (derived from ANOVA) was considered as small for r = 0.10, medium for r = 0.30, and large for r ≥ 0.50 (Cohen, 1988).

Results

Seventy out of 89 participants completed the full 6-month intervention (21.4% attrition) and were included for statistical analyses. For a detailed description of the participant flow, including time points and reasons for dropouts, see Eggenberger et al. (2015b). The number of dropouts per intervention group was equally distributed, and therefore, the final analyses were conducted using only the data of participants who completed the entire 6-month intervention. Missing HRV data from the included 70 participants comprised eight values at pretest (2 in DANCE, 4 in MEMORY, 2 in PHYS), 2 values at the 3-month test (1 in MEMORY, 1 in PHYS), and 11 values at the 6-month test (5 in DANCE, 5 in MEMORY, 1 in PHYS). No participant had more than one missing value, except for one participant in the MEMORY group showing two missing values (pre- and 6-month tests). Baseline demographic characteristics of all individuals of the three intervention groups are illustrated in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline demographic characteristics.

Cross-sectional Analyses

The three initial regression analyses that were performed separately for each group of predictors (i.e., cognitive, physical, or gait parameters, respectively) revealed that most variance could be explained in the three HRV indices SDNN, RMSSD, and HF power. Therefore, these three indices were included for further analyses and will be reported. They also represent the most commonly used HRV indices (Ernst, 2017). Representative results of the three separate initial backward regression analyses are presented for the RMSSD index in Table 2. Similar results were found for SDNN and HF power and are therefore not reported. Based on these results, for analysis: (a), including the data set edited with artifact interpolation in Kubios, the final regression analysis was performed with the following seven parameters: TMT-B, Story Recall, step length variability (at preferred single task walking speed), SPPB, 5-CR, age, and BMI. The corresponding results are illustrated in Table 3. For analysis: (b), comprising the data set edited by deletion of artifacts in Matlab, the final regression analysis included TMT-B, Story Recall, gait velocity (at preferred dual task walking speed), SPPB, 5-CR, age, and BMI. From these analyses, the models that included only significant parameters (or trends to significance, i.e., p < 0.10, if no significance was attained) were selected. The results from analysis (a) (Kubios) vs. (b) (Matlab) revealed no differences in which parameters were significantly contributing to the models. Therefore, only the results based on the data set with artifact interpolation in Kubios are reported and this data set will be used for the following longitudinal analyses.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Initial linear regression models to predict HRV with three separate groups of predictors, including cognitive, physical, and gait parameters, respectively.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Final linear regression models to predict HRV indices with combined cognitive, physical, and gait parameters.

Longitudinal Analyses

At baseline (pre-training), we found no significant differences of any of the HRV indices between the three intervention groups (SDNN: F(2,69) = 0.039, p = 0.962, two-tailed, r = 0.03; RMSSD: F(2,69) = 0.824, p = 0.443, two-tailed, r = 0.15; HF power: F(2,69) = 0.242, p = 0.786, two-tailed, r = 0.08). The linear global time effect, which represents the effect when the entire sample across all three intervention groups is merged, indicated a significant increase in SDNN from pre-training to 6-month test (F(1,134) = 4.81, p = 0.015 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.020). For the other two HRV indices (RMSSD and HF power), no significant linear global time effect was found.

Differences Between Cognitive–Motor and Exclusively Physical Training (First Contrast)

The first contrast, i.e., the cognitive–motor intervention groups (DANCE and MEMORY) vs. the exclusively physical intervention group (PHYS), yielded a significant linear time×intervention interaction for SDNN (F(1,134) = 5.00, p = 0.014 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.020) and a trend to significance for RMSSD (F(1,134) = 2.68, p = 0.052 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.007). These interactions reflect an increase in the two HRV indices in the cognitive–motor groups, while PHYS remained unchanged.

Differences Between the Two Cognitive–Motor Training Groups (Second Contrast)

In the second contrast, the two cognitive–motor intervention groups (DANCE vs. MEMORY) were compared. For all three investigated HRV indices, a significant linear time × intervention interaction was evident: SDNN (F(1,134) = 8.69, p = 0.002 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.035), RMSSD (F(1,134) = 4.63, p = 0.017 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.012), and HF power (F(1,134) = 5.47, p = 0.011 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.013). These interactions reflect an increase in all three HRV indices in DANCE, while MEMORY remained unchanged. The longitudinal development of the three HRV indices, from baseline (pre-training) to 6-month test, is illustrated in Figures 24, respectively.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Exercise training-induced adaptations in SDNN. Notes: significant improvements of SDNN were shown in the first contrast (DANCE/MEMORY vs. PHYS; p = 0.014 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.020) and in the second contrast (DANCE vs. MEMORY; p = 0.002 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.035). Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: SDNN, standard deviation of N–N intervals; DANCE, virtual reality video game dancing; MEMORY, treadmill walking with simultaneous verbal memory training; PHYS, treadmill walking.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Exercise training-induced adaptations in RMSSD. Notes: significant improvements of RMSSD were shown in the first contrast (DANCE/MEMORY vs. PHYS; trend p = 0.052 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.007) and in the second contrast (DANCE vs. MEMORY; p = 0.017 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.012). Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: RMSSD, root mean square of successive R–R interval differences; DANCE, virtual reality video game dancing; MEMORY, treadmill walking with simultaneous verbal memory training; PHYS, treadmill walking.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Exercise training-induced adaptations in HF power. Notes: a significant improvement of HF power was shown in the second contrast (DANCE vs. MEMORY; p = 0.011 one-tailed, ΔR2 = 0.013). Error bars indicate ± standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: HF power, absolute power of high-frequency band (0.15–0.4 Hz); DANCE, virtual reality video game dancing; MEMORY, treadmill walking with simultaneous verbal memory training; PHYS, treadmill walking.

Supplementary Analysis: Mediation of Exergame Training Effects on Changes in HRV Through Changes in Cognitive Executive Functions

No significant indirect effect of training intervention (i.e., DANCE vs. MEMORY/PHYS) on changes in standardized HRV indices (z-scores) through mediation of changes in standardized cognitive executive functions was evident: change in SDNN mediated by change in TMT-B and Executive Control Task, respectively: b = 0.034, 95% BCa CI [–0.078, 0.193] and b = 0.019, 95% BCa CI [–0.041, 0.094]; change in RMSSD mediated by change in TMT-B and Executive Control Task, respectively: b = –0.015, 95% BCa CI [–0.119, 0.060] and b = –0.040, 95% BCa CI [–0.159, 0.056]; and change in HF power mediated by change in TMT-B and Executive Control Task, respectively: b = –0.004, 95% BCa CI [–0.071, 0.059] and b = –0.031, 95% BCa CI [–0.144, 0.046]. Significant mediation was indicated when confidence intervals did not include zero (Field, 2018).

Discussion

Our study aimed: (1) to investigate which cognitive, physical, and gait parameters explain the variance in HRV indices; and (2) to investigate whether cognitive–motor training induces different adaptations in HRV indices compared to exclusively physical training. The two main findings were that 8–12% of variance in each of the selected HRV indices (SDNN, RMSSD, and HF power) was explained by cognitive executive functions (TMT-B) and leg strength (5-CR, inversely related). Furthermore, our results indicate that 6 months of exergame training (DANCE) markedly increased all three HRV indices, while DT treadmill walking (MEMORY) and exclusively physical training (PHYS) did not affect HRV in older adults.

Cross-sectional Analyses

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate a combination of different parameters of cognitive, physical, and gait performance as predictors of HRV in older adults. We hypothesized that variance in HRV is explained by: (a) cognitive parameters related to executive functions and verbal memory; (b) physical parameters related to physical functioning, aerobic endurance, and leg strength; and (c) gait parameters related to executive functions.

Cognitive executive functioning (TMT-B) was the most prominent significant predictor of variance in all three assessed HRV indices (SDNN, RMSSD, and HF power). This finding is in line with previous cross-sectional studies and a recent meta-analysis, which demonstrated that HRV indices may mirror executive functions in several age groups (N = 14,347) and that this association becomes even stronger in older people (Holzman and Bridgett, 2017). Similarly, higher performance in dual tasks with great demands on executive functioning was closely related to increased parasympathetic activity in very fit older adults (i.e., Master Athletes; Dupuy et al., 2018). These and our own findings highlight the role of fitness level on cardiac autonomic control and executive functioning. Additionally, the results support the “neuro-visceral integration model,” which proposes that cognitive executive functions are linked to cardiac autonomic regulation and both are controlled by the prefrontal cortex together with other brain areas (Thayer et al., 2009, 2012).

Verbal long-term memory (story recall) did not significantly contribute to variance in HRV in our study. In contrast, verbal learning and memory were linked with very-low-frequency (VLF) HRV from 24-h measurements in 416 middle-aged male twins (mean age 55 ± 2.9 years; Shah et al., 2011). Equally, Frewen et al. (2013) reported that mainly the memory recall and language sub-domains of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) were responsible for the relationship of the MoCA with HRV (i.e., SDNN, LF, and LF/HF ratio) in a large cohort of older adults (N = 4,763, mean age 61.7 ± 8.3 years). Interestingly, patients with mild cognitive impairments (MCI) demonstrate parasympathetic deficits, i.e., reduced HF power, and patients with greater autonomic dysfunction exhibit more pronounced neuropsychological deficits, probably due to early structural and functional neural alterations of the central autonomic network related to Alzheimer’s disease (Collins et al., 2012).

Leg strength (5-CR), but not overall functional fitness (SPPB total score, comprising gait speed, 5-CR, and balance), emerged as a significant inversely related predictor of variance in the two parasympathetic HRV indices RMSSD and HF power. Hence, better leg strength (i.e., shorter time on the 5-CR test) was associated with lower or worse HRV indices and vice versa. Comparably, Matos et al. (2020) recently reported that SDNN and RMSSD were inversely correlated with hand-grip strength in 28 older adults (r = –0.30 and –0.23, respectively); although the correlation was not statistically significant, the result seems worth mentioning due to the medium effect size. Hand-grip strength dynamometry and functional leg strength assessments are both valid tools to measure muscle strength and are feasible for sarcopenia screening in home settings of community-dwelling older adults (Mijnarends et al., 2013). Sarcopenia is a condition dependent on the quality of the mechanism of corticospinal interaction and central drive to skeletal muscle (Gennaro et al., 2020) and is characterized by low muscle strength, muscle mass, and physical performance and accompanied by increased cardiac risk (Cruz-Jentoft et al., 2019). To our knowledge, no study has previously investigated the specific association of leg strength with HRV. Therefore, further research is warranted to substantiate or refute this finding. Nevertheless, our result which showed that overall functional fitness did not explain variance in HRV contradicts previous studies with older adults. For instance, Graham et al. (2019) recorded long-term HRV (approximately 2.5 h) during ADL with a wrist-worn photoplethysmography (PPG) sensor. They observed significant correlations of HRV with two tests of physical functioning, including the SPPB and the Timed Up and Go test (TUG) in 52 older adults between 65 and 95 years of age. In a 5-year longitudinal assessment among 985 US older adults (mean age 71 ± 5 years), higher total leisure-time activity, as well as walking distance and pace, was prospectively related to more favorable HRV indices SDNN and ultra-low-frequency power (ULF power, <0.003 Hz) recorded over 24 h (Soares-Miranda et al., 2014). Comparably, older adults (mean age 75.7 ± 0.2 years) who followed a long-term sportive lifestyle exhibited increased 5-min global HRV (SDNN) and parasympathetic HRV indices (e.g., RMSSD and HF power) compared to their active but non-sportive counterparts; Buchheit et al., 2004). Moreover, older adults affected by sarcopenia showed reduced modulation of the parasympathetic autonomic nervous system as assessed with 5-min time-domain and nonlinear HRV indices (Freitas et al., 2018). Similarly, a shift toward sympathetic predominance was evident in frail elderly women, suggesting that HRV monitoring may be of value for the prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of the frailty syndrome (Katayama et al., 2015).

Gait performance, particularly step length variability at preferred walking speed, was another predictor that did not make a significant contribution to the combined regression models in our study. This outcome conforms with the only previous investigation of associations of HRV with measures of gait from Aerts et al. (2009). The authors did not find any significant correlation of gait performance and HRV in their sample of healthy older adults and Parkinson’s disease patients (n = 15 and 18, respectively). Nevertheless, we suggest that future studies may still further investigate this interrelation, since gait performance and gait control are interrelated not only with cognitive executive functions but also with prefrontal and cingulate cortex activity (Cohen et al., 2016; Sakurai et al., 2017). Therefore, one might have expected to find an association of gait parameters with HRV indices, based on their shared reliance on partially the same brain areas, including the prefrontal and cingulate cortices (Thayer et al., 2009; Tian et al., 2017; Ghanavati et al., 2018; Lucas et al., 2018).

In contrast to previous research, aerobic fitness (6-MWT) was not a relevant predictor for HRV in our regression models. Dorey et al. (2019) reported higher HF power in aerobically fit compared to aerobically unfit older adults. Thereby, aerobic fitness was determined as VO2 peak in a graded cycle ergometer test. Also in senior athletes, parasympathetic HRV was significantly higher than in sedentary older adults and correlated positively with maximal aerobic capacity (Yataco et al., 1997). Because resting heart rate was also lower in older adults with better physical fitness (Buchheit et al., 2004; Dupuy et al., 2018), our findings may be linked to the fact that we have normalized HRV indices for average R–R interval duration to adjust for interindividual differences in resting heart rate. This procedure appears to be crucial to improving reproducibility and comparability of HRV recordings, since already a minimal change in heart rate by 1 bpm results in considerable alterations of HRV indices by 16.5% on average (Gąsior et al., 2016). Comparably, Monfredi et al. (2014) suggested that changes in HRV due to altered morbidity and mortality are substantially associated with concurrent changes in heart rate. Therefore, the authors emphasized that the results of studies failing to properly adjust HRV for interindividual heart rate differences would have to be reevaluated. Nonetheless, also in a supplementary analysis, using the non-normalized HRV data of our study (results not reported), the relevance of the 6-MWT as a predictor of HRV did not increase.

Longitudinal Analyses

We further hypothesized that exercise training-induced adaptations in HRV indices primarily occur in cognitive–motor training interventions (i.e., DANCE and MEMORY) and particularly in DANCE, which includes training aspects of executive functioning. We indeed found specific improvements of HRV indices after 6 months of exergame training (DANCE), but not after DT treadmill walking (MEMORY) and following exclusively physical training (PHYS). Since baseline values in nine different tests of cognitive performance were equal among all three intervention groups, as reported previously (Eggenberger et al., 2015a), we exclude the possibility that unequally distributed factors related to cognitive and brain health (e.g., cerebrovascular disease, obesity, hypertension, and diabetes mellitus) may have affected this outcome. The small to medium effect size of our exergame intervention on HRV is in the same range of effects commonly found in aerobic endurance training interventions in healthy older adults, as reported in a meta-analysis (Raffin et al., 2019). Therefore, the effects in our study appear to be domain-specific and of clinical and practical importance (Durlak, 2009). To our knowledge, the effect of exergame training on HRV indices has not been investigated previously in healthy older adults. Nevertheless, our results are coherent with two recent trials applying exergame paradigms in individuals with chronic stroke (Sampaio et al., 2016) and women with fibromyalgia (Villafaina et al., 2020). Sampaio et al. (2016) performed 6 weeks (20 sessions) of a virtual reality-based dance video game in a small sample of eleven patients with chronic hemiparetic stroke (age 61.7 ± 4.3 years) and found improvements in autonomic modulation assessed with LF power, HF power, and LF/HF HRV indices which were accompanied by increased VO2max (computed from the YMCA submaximal cycle ergometer test). Meanwhile, Villafaina et al. (2020) recently conducted a randomized controlled trial including 50 women with fibromyalgia. The intervention group (age 54.0 ± 8.5) completed two weekly 1-h exergame sessions over 24 weeks and displayed improved SDNN and other HRV indices compared to the passive control group.

Supplementary mediation analysis revealed that changes in cognitive executive functions did not mediate exergame training effects (DANCE vs. MEMORY/PHYS) on changes in HRV. Nevertheless, such a mediation effect appeared possible since the exergame training in this study particularly improved cognitive executive functions as reported previously (Eggenberger et al., 2015a) and similar findings were summarized in recent meta-analytic and review articles (Stanmore et al., 2017; Stojan and Voelcker-Rehage, 2019). Moreover, another study demonstrated that behavioral training effects on cognitive executive functions are reflected in brain functional modulations in the prefrontal cortex area as assessed with functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) during challenging walking (Eggenberger et al., 2016). Thereby, the prefrontal cortex mediates the association of cognitive executive functions with HRV (Yoo et al., 2018). Neuroimaging and pharmacological studies have demonstrated a link between HRV and prefrontal neural function (Thayer et al., 2009, 2012). Therefore, the potential role of changes in cognitive executive functions as a mediator of specific exergame training effects on HRV warrants further investigation in order to substantiate or refute this finding. In summary, together with the cross-sectional associations of HRV with cognitive executive functions found in the present study, the outcome of our longitudinal analyses does further support the notion that HRV particularly mirrors executive functioning.

The two intervention groups MEMORY and PHYS both comprised treadmill walking as an aerobic exercise-training component. However, they did not increase HRV indices, which conflicts with previous research. Recent meta-analytic results show that studies including exclusively aerobic-type training mostly demonstrated improvements in HRV in older adults and particularly in the global HRV index SDNN (Raffin et al., 2019). In some trials, aerobic training also specifically increased the parasympathetic mediated HRV indices (i.e., RMSSD and HF power) with a concomitant enhancement of cognitive executive functions (Albinet et al., 2010, 2016). Similarly, in a study including 118 sedentary, overweight, hypertensive women (mean age 64.8 ± 4.2 years), 6 months of low-to-moderate intensity aerobic training raised parasympathetic HRV indices, despite VO2max remaining unchanged (Earnest et al., 2012). Raffin et al. (2019) proposed in their meta-analysis that long-term aerobic exercise interventions with a high training frequency, i.e., four or more sessions per week over several months, are the key to positively affecting HRV in older adults, rather than the duration and intensity of the single sessions. It appears that exclusively aerobic exercise-based interventions require a much higher training frequency to induce positive adaptations of HRV, compared to our twice-weekly exergame intervention (DANCE). Therefore, our conflicting result may be the consequence of the relatively low training frequency of aerobic-type exercise in MEMORY and PHYS, i.e., twice weekly, and the relatively low fraction of specific aerobic endurance training volume, i.e., 20 min per session. However, the training still improved aerobic endurance assessed with the 6-MWT as reported elsewhere (Eggenberger et al., 2015b). Furthermore, the opposing result may again be related to normalizing HRV indices with average R–R intervals in our study. Thereby, the normalizing procedure may have leveled out any effects on HRV that could be linked to the possibly lowered resting heart rate after the training intervention. Similar to our exclusively physical training group which comprised strength, balance, and treadmill walking (PHYS), the few available strength training studies appeared not to have an impact on HRV in older adults (Forte et al., 2003; Madden et al., 2006; Gerhart et al., 2017) or even reduced HRV (Melo et al., 2008). The authors suggested that diverging mechanisms mediate beneficial effects of strength compared to aerobic training on the cardiovascular system.

An intriguing finding in the context of our study is the observation from Vesterinen et al. (2016) that HRV has potential to be used as a tool that may be able to help in designing individualized training programs. For instance, it seems that individuals (younger adults) with low HRV values rather respond with increases in endurance performance when high-volume low-intensity training is endorsed. On the opposite, trainees with high initial HRV values respond better to high-intensity training with progressively increasing training volumes and intensities (Vesterinen et al., 2016). In the present study, we developed and applied our training programs in the traditional way, i.e., based on scientific literature, guidelines, and recommendations, in combination with the practical experience of our trainers. Taking this approach means that our programs were prescribed without information on how the older adults have responded to past training. However, from previous research we know that such an approach may lead to success on a group level but might, at the same time, hide interindividual differences in training response because not all individuals respond in a similar way. Large individual differences in response to the same training program are not uncommon in research literature (Bouchard and Rankinen, 2001; Hautala et al., 2003; Vollaard et al., 2009), which points out the necessity of designing individualized training programs. Therefore, further research is warranted to investigate the potential of baseline HRV assessments as a means to individualize training prescription in older adults.

Strengths and Limitations

A methodological strength of the presented study was the comparably large number of participants and the long training period of 6 months. The following limitations need to be considered. First, the conclusions from this study are limited to the sample population that was involved, i.e., physically and cognitively healthy older adults. It may be argued that training effects could have been even higher in physically and cognitively impaired older persons. This assumption is based on the exercise-training principle “initial values,” which states that exercise training-induced improvements will be largest in persons with lower initial fitness levels (Ammann et al., 2014). Thereby, it might be particularly interesting to assess effects of exergame training on HRV in healthy older adults compared to patients with cerebrovascular pathology which is associated with reduced executive functioning (Poulin et al., 2012). Second, although participants were blinded in relation to the hypothesized study outcome, it was not possible to blind the investigators as they were involved in supervising and conducting training and test sessions. Finally, the chosen simple mean imputation method provides valid estimations of intervention effects but lowers variability and p-values (Dziura et al., 2013). Variability in the data including imputed missing values was on average 5.5% (±4.4%) lower compared to the data with missing values. Considering the overall large variability in the HRV data (standard deviation in % of mean HRV values: with imputed values = 116.2% ± 70.3%, with missing values = 123.1% ± 74.3%) leads us to conclude that the imputed missing values had a negligible effect on the presented results.

Conclusion

The present study provides important novel information supporting the predictive value of HRV indices as markers of cognitive health in older adults. To our knowledge, this is the first investigation of a combination of different parameters of cognitive, physical, and gait performance as predictors of HRV indices (SDNN, RMSSD, and HF power) representing global and parasympathetic aspects of the autonomic nervous system. Thereby, we found in the cross-sectional analysis that 8–12% of variance in each of the three HRV indices was significantly explained by cognitive executive functions and, inversely related, by leg strength. Furthermore, in the longitudinal analysis we identified that 6 months of exergame training (DANCE) markedly increased all three HRV indices, while DT treadmill walking (MEMORY) and exclusively physical training (PHYS) did not have any effects on HRV. Since the exergame training in this study specifically improved cognitive executive functioning in older adults, as reported previously (Eggenberger et al., 2015a), the outcomes of our cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses are highly coherent. We conclude that mainly cognitive executive functions are associated with HRV indices and, notably, that exergame training represents an effective training strategy to improve global and parasympathetic autonomic nervous system activities in older adults. Therefore, regularly assessing HRV in older citizens could be particularly beneficial to monitor cognitive health and provide indications for preventative exercise-training measures. Future investigations are warranted to strengthen these results and could include other potentially supplementary predictors of HRV, such as measures of brain function and structure. This will further promote our understanding of which factors of cognitive and physical health may be mirrored in HRV measurements in older adults.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the ethics committee of the Canton, St. Gallen, Switzerland (EKSG 12/092). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

PE, TM, and EB contributed with the conception and design of the study. PE recruited the participants. PE conducted and supervised the training and testing sessions. PE, SA, and KK executed the data processing. PE and KK performed the statistical analyses. PE, SA, KK, RR, TM, and EB contributed to data interpretation. PE wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed with the manuscript revision and read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was initially supported by the Zürcher Kantonalbank (grant number: ZKB-01 10-3, grant recipient: EB) within the framework of sponsoring of Movement Sciences, Sports and Nutritional Sciences at ETH Zurich. Zürcher Kantonalbank had no influence on the study design and the analyses presented in this article, had no access to the data, and did not contribute to this manuscript in any way. The secondary analysis of the study presented in this manuscript was funded by Empa, Swiss Federal Laboratories for Materials Science and Technology, Laboratory for Biomimetic Membranes and Textiles, St. Gallen, Switzerland.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the postgraduate students Marius Angst, Stefan Holenstein, Fabienne Hüppin, Manuela Kobelt, Alexandra Schättin, and Sara Tomovic for instructing trainings and helping with data acquisition. Furthermore, we would like to thank all participants for their enthusiasm, kindness, and patience during our extensive training and testing sessions.

References

Aerts, M. B., Synhaeve, N. E., Mirelman, A., Bloem, B. R., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2009). Is heart rate variability related to gait impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease? A pilot study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 15, 712–715. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al Hazzouri, A. Z., Haan, M. N., Deng, Y., Neuhaus, J., and Yaffe, K. (2014). Reduced heart rate variability is associated with worse cognitive performance in elderly Mexican Americans. Hypertension 63, 181–187. doi: 10.1161/hypertensionaha.113.01888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albinet, C. T., Abou-Dest, A., André, N., and Audiffren, M. (2016). Executive functions improvement following a 5-month aquaerobics program in older adults: role of cardiac vagal control in inhibition performance. Biol. Psychol. 115, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albinet, C. T., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C. A., and Audiffren, M. (2010). Increased heart rate variability and executive performance after aerobic training in the elderly. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 617–624. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1393-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American College of Sports Medicine, Chodzko-Zajko, W. J., Proctor, D. N., Fiatarone Singh, M. A., Minson, C. T., Nigg, C. R., et al. (2009). American college of sports medicine position stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41, 1510–1530. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a0c95c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ammann, B. C., Knols, R. H., Baschung, P., de Bie, R. A., and de Bruin, E. D. (2014). Application of principles of exercise training in sub-acute and chronic stroke survivors: a systematic review. BMC Neurol. 14:167. doi: 10.1186/s12883-014-0167-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, R. A. (2014). When to use the Bonferroni correction. Ophthalmic. Physiol. Opt. 34, 502–508. doi: 10.1111/opo.12131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ATS Committee on Proficiency Standards for Clinical Pulmonary Function Laboratories. (2002). ATS statement: guidelines for the six-minute walk test. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 166, 111–117. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.166.1.at1102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baller, G., Brand, M., Kalbe, E., and Kessler, J. (2006). Inventar Zur Gedächtnisdiagnostik (IGD), Manual. Goettingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Bherer, L., Langeard, A., Kaushal, N., Vrinceanu, T., Desjardins-Crepeau, L., Langlois, F., et al. (2019). Physical exercise training effect and mediation through cardiorespiratory fitness on dual-task performances differ in younger-old and older-old adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz066 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchard, C., and Rankinen, T. (2001). Individual differences in response to regular physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 33, S446–S451; discussion S452–S443. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200106001-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchheit, M., Simon, C., Viola, A. U., Doutreleau, S., Piquard, F., and Brandenberger, G. (2004). Heart rate variability in sportive elderly: relationship with daily physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 36, 601–605. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000121956.76237.b5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. A., Verghese, J., and Zwerling, J. L. (2016). Cognition and gait in older people. Maturitas 93, 73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, O., Dillon, S., Finucane, C., Lawlor, B., and Kenny, R. A. (2012). Parasympathetic autonomic dysfunction is common in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2324–2333. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cruz-Jentoft, A. J., Bahat, G., Bauer, J., Boirie, Y., Bruyere, O., Cederholm, T., et al. (2019). Sarcopenia: revised European consensus on definition and diagnosis. Age Ageing 48, 16–31. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afy169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorey, T. W., O’Brien, M. W., and Kimmerly, D. S. (2019). The influence of aerobic fitness on electrocardiographic and heart rate variability parameters in young and older adults. Auton. Neurosci. 217, 66–70. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2019.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupuy, O., Bosquet, L., Fraser, S. A., Labelle, V., and Bherer, L. (2018). Higher cardiovascular fitness level is associated to better cognitive dual-task performance in Master Athletes: mediation by cardiac autonomic control. Brain Cogn. 125, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlak, J. A. (2009). How to select, calculate and interpret effect sizes. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 34, 917–928. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziura, J. D., Post, L. A., Zhao, Q., Fu, Z., and Peduzzi, P. (2013). Strategies for dealing with missing data in clinical trials: from design to analysis. Yale J. Biol. Med. 86, 343–358.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Earnest, C. P., Blair, S. N., and Church, T. S. (2012). Heart rate variability and exercise in aging women. J. Womens Health 21, 334–339. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2011.2932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Schumacher, V., Angst, M., Theill, N., and de Bruin, E. D. (2015a). Does multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training boost cognitive performance in older adults? A 6-month randomized controlled trial with a 1-year follow-up. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 1335–1349. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S87732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Theill, N., Holenstein, S., Schumacher, V., and de Bruin, E. D. (2015b). Multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training to enhance dual-task walking of older adults: a secondary analysis of a 6-month randomized controlled trial with 1-year follow-up. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 1711–1732. doi: 10.2147/cia.s91997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Wolf, M., Schumann, M., and de Bruin, E. D. (2016). Exergame and balance training modulate prefrontal brain activity during walking and enhance executive function in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:66. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, G. (2017). Heart-rate variability-more than heart beats? Front. Public Health 5:240. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2018). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. London: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Forte, R., De Vito, G., and Figura, F. (2003). Effects of dynamic resistance training on heart rate variability in healthy older women. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 89, 85–89. doi: 10.1007/s00421-002-0775-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitas, V. P., Passos, R. D. S., Oliveira, A. A., Ribeiro, I. J. S., Freire, I. V., Schettino, L., et al. (2018). Sarcopenia is associated to an impaired autonomic heart rate modulation in community-dwelling old adults. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 76, 120–124. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2018.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frewen, J., Finucane, C., Savva, G. M., Boyle, G., Coen, R. F., and Kenny, R. A. (2013). Cognitive function is associated with impaired heart rate variability in ageing adults: the Irish longitudinal study on ageing wave one results. Clin. Auton. Res. 23, 313–323. doi: 10.1007/s10286-013-0214-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gąsior, J. S., Sacha, J., Jelén, P. J., Zielinski, J., and Przybylski, J. (2016). Heart rate and respiratory rate influence on heart rate variability repeatability: effects of the correction for the prevailing heart rate. Front. Physiol. 7:356. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatterer, G. (2008). Der Alters-Konzentrations-Test. Goettingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Gennaro, F., Maino, P., Kaelin-Lang, A., Bock, K., and Bruin, E. D. (2020). Corticospinal control of human locomotion as a new determinant of age-related sarcopenia: an exploratory study. J. Clin. Med. 9:720. doi: 10.3390/jcm9030720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerhart, H., Tai, Y. L., Fennell, C., Mayo, X., and Kingsley, J. D. (2017). Autonomic modulation in older women: using resistance exercise as a countermeasure. Int. J. Exerc Sci. 10, 178–187.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ghanavati, T., Smitt, M. S., Lord, S. R., Sachdev, P., Wen, W., Kochan, N. A., et al. (2018). Deep white matter hyperintensities, microstructural integrity and dual task walking in older people. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 1488–1496. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9787-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S. A., Jeste, D. V., Lee, E. E., Wu, T. C., Tu, X., Kim, H. C., et al. (2019). Associations between heart rate variability measured with a wrist-worn sensor and older adults’ physical function: observational study. JMIR mHealth and uHealth 7:e13757. doi: 10.2196/13757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granacher, U., Muehlbauer, T., and Gruber, M. (2012). A qualitative review of balance and strength performance in healthy older adults: impact for testing and training. J. Aging Res. 2012:708905. doi: 10.1155/2012/708905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guralnik, J. M., Simonsick, E. M., Ferrucci, L., Glynn, R. J., Berkman, L. F., Blazer, D. G., et al. (1994). A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J. Gerontol. 49, M85–M94. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.2.m85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., and Kessler, J. (2000). WMS-R—Wechsler Gedächtnistest—Revidierte Fassung. Bern: Huber.

Google Scholar

Hautala, A. J., Mäkikallio, T. H., Kiviniemi, A., Laukkanen, R. T., Nissilä, S., Huikuri, H. V., et al. (2003). Cardiovascular autonomic function correlates with the response to aerobic training in healthy sedentary subjects. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 285, H1747–H1752. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00202.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Holzman, J. B., and Bridgett, D. J. (2017). Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 233–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katayama, P. L., Dias, D. P., Silva, L. E., Virtuoso-Junior, J. S., and Marocolo, M. (2015). Cardiac autonomic modulation in non-frail, pre-frail and frail elderly women: a pilot study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 27, 621–629. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0320-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Koenig, J., and Thayer, J. F. (2017). From psychological moments to mortality: a multidisciplinary synthesis on heart rate variability spanning the continuum of time. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 547–567. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurita, A., Takase, B., Kodani, E., Iwahara, S., Kusama, Y., and Atarashi, H. (2013). Prognostic value of heart rate variability in comparison with annual health examinations in very elderly subjects. J. Nippon Med. Sch. 80, 420–425. doi: 10.1272/jnms.80.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howiesen, D. B., Bigler, E. D., and Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Liao, C. D., Tsauo, J. Y., Hsiao, D. J., Liou, T. H., Huang, S. W., and Lin, L. F. (2017). Association of physical capacity with heart rate variability based on a short-duration measurement of resting pulse rate in older adults with obesity. PLoS One 12:e0189150. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, M., Wagshul, M. E., Izzetoglu, M., and Holtzer, R. (2018). Moderating effect of white matter integrity on brain activation during dual-task walking in older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 435–441. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, K. M., Levy, W. C., and Stratton, J. K. (2006). Exercise training and heart rate variability in older adult female subjects. Clin. Invest. Med. 29, 20–28.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mahinrad, S., Jukema, J. W., van Heemst, D., Macfarlane, P. W., Clark, E. N., de Craen, A. J., et al. (2016). 10-Second heart rate variability and cognitive function in old age. Neurology 86, 1120–1127. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000002499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malik, M. (1996). Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task force of the european society of cardiology and the North American society of pacing and electrophysiology. Eur. Heart J. 17, 354–381. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-474x.1996.tb00275.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matos, F. O., Vido, A., Garcia, W. F., Lopes, W. A., and Pereira, A. (2020). A neurovisceral integrative study on cognition, heart rate variability and fitness in the elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:51. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melo, R. C., Quiterio, R. J., Takahashi, A. C., Silva, E., Martins, L. E., and Catai, A. M. (2008). High eccentric strength training reduces heart rate variability in healthy older men. Br. J. Sports Med. 42, 59–63. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2007.035246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mijnarends, D. M., Meijers, J. M., Halfens, R. J., ter Borg, S., Luiking, Y. C., Verlaan, S., et al. (2013). Validity and reliability of tools to measure muscle mass, strength and physical performance in community-dwelling older people: a systematic review. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 14, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2012.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Hopewell, S., Schulz, K. F., Montori, V., Gotzsche, P. C., Devereaux, P. J., et al. (2010). CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ 340:c869. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monfredi, O., Lyashkov, A. E., Johnsen, A. B., Inada, S., Schneider, H., Wang, R., et al. (2014). Biophysical characterization of the underappreciated and important relationship between heart rate variability and heart rate. Hypertension 64, 1334–1343. doi: 10.1161/hypertensionaha.114.03782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolan, J., Batin, P. D., Andrews, R., Lindsay, S. J., Brooksby, P., Mullen, M., et al. (1998). Prospective study of heart rate variability and mortality in chronic heart failure: results of the United Kingdom heart failure evaluation and assessment of risk trial (UK-heart). Circulation 98, 1510–1516. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.98.15.1510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogliari, G., Mahinrad, S., Stott, D. J., Jukema, J. W., Mooijaart, S. P., Macfarlane, P. W., et al. (2015). Resting heart rate, heart rate variability and functional decline in old age. CMAJ 187, E442–E449. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.150462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peltola, M. A. (2012). Role of editing of R-R intervals in the analysis of heart rate variability. Front. Physiol. 3:148. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perneger, T. V. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ 316, 1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulin, V., Korner-Bitensky, N., Dawson, D. R., and Bherer, L. (2012). Efficacy of executive function interventions after stroke: a systematic review. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 19, 158–171. doi: 10.1310/tsr1902-158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffin, J., Barthelemy, J. C., Dupre, C., Pichot, V., Berger, M., Feasson, L., et al. (2019). Exercise frequency determines heart rate variability gains in older people: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. Sports Med. 49, 719–729. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01097-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacha, J. (2013). Why should one normalize heart rate variability with respect to average heart rate. Front. Physiol. 4:306. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, R., Ishii, K., Yasunaga, M., Takeuchi, R., Murayama, Y., Sakuma, N., et al. (2017). The neural substrate of gait and executive function relationship in elderly women: a PET study. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 17, 1873–1880. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampaio, L. M., Subramaniam, S., Arena, R., and Bhatt, T. (2016). Does virtual reality-based kinect dance training paradigm improve autonomic nervous system modulation in individuals with chronic stroke? J. Vasc. Interv. Neurol. 9, 21–29.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., and Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, A. J., Su, S., Veledar, E., Bremner, J. D., Goldstein, F. C., Lampert, R., et al. (2011). Is heart rate variability related to memory performance in middle-aged men?. Psychosom. Med. 73, 475–482. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182227d6a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrington, C., Tiedemann, A., Fairhall, N., Close, J. C., and Lord, S. R. (2011). Exercise to prevent falls in older adults: an updated meta-analysis and best practice recommendations. N. S. W. Public Health Bull. 22, 78–83. doi: 10.1071/nb10056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares-Miranda, L., Sattelmair, J., Chaves, P., Duncan, G. E., Siscovick, D. S., Stein, P. K., et al. (2014). Physical activity and heart rate variability in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Circulation 129, 2100–2110. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.005361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanmore, E., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., de Bruin, E. D., and Firth, J. (2017). The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stojan, R., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2019). A systematic review on the cognitive benefits and neurophysiological correlates of exergaming in healthy older adults. J. Clin. Med. 8:734. doi: 10.3390/jcm8050734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streiner, D. L., and Norman, G. R. (2011). Correction for multiple testing: is there a resolution? Chest 140, 16–18. doi: 10.1378/chest.11-0523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., and Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., and Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation and health. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 141–153. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, Q., Chastan, N., Bair, W. N., Resnick, S. M., Ferrucci, L., and Studenski, S. A. (2017). The brain map of gait variability in aging, cognitive impairment and dementia-A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 149–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesterinen, V., Hakkinen, K., Laine, T., Hynynen, E., Mikkola, J., and Nummela, A. (2016). Predictors of individual adaptation to high-volume or high-intensity endurance training in recreational endurance runners. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 26, 885–893. doi: 10.1111/sms.12530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villafaina, S., Collado-Mateo, D., Dominguez-Munoz, F. J., Gusi, N., and Fuentes-Garcia, J. P. (2020). Effects of exergames on heart rate variability of women with fibromyalgia: A randomized controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 10:5168. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61617-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollaard, N. B., Constantin-Teodosiu, D., Fredriksson, K., Rooyackers, O., Jansson, E., Greenhaff, P. L., et al. (2009). Systematic analysis of adaptations in aerobic capacity and submaximal energy metabolism provides a unique insight into determinants of human aerobic performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 1479–1486. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91453.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (WMS-R) Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Google Scholar

Williams, D. P., Jarczok, M. N., Ellis, R. J., Hillecke, T. K., Thayer, J. F., and Koenig, J. (2017). Two-week test-retest reliability of the Polar((R)) RS800CX() to record heart rate variability. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 37, 776–781. doi: 10.1111/cpf.12321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yataco, A. R., Fleisher, L. A., and Katzel, L. I. (1997). Heart rate variability and cardiovascular fitness in senior athletes. Am. J. Cardiol. 80, 1389–1391. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9149(97)00697-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, H. J., Thayer, J. F., Greening, S., Lee, T. H., Ponzio, A., Min, J., et al. (2018). Brain structural concomitants of resting state heart rate variability in the young and old: evidence from two independent samples. Brain Struct. Funct. 223, 727–737. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1519-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive–motor training, dual-task training, normalized heart rate variability, functional fitness, executive functions, verbal long-term memory, gait variability, elderly

Citation: Eggenberger P, Annaheim S, Kündig KA, Rossi RM, Münzer T and de Bruin ED (2020) Heart Rate Variability Mainly Relates to Cognitive Executive Functions and Improves Through Exergame Training in Older Adults: A Secondary Analysis of a 6-Month Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:197. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00197

Received: 01 April 2020; Accepted: 05 June 2020;
Published: 15 July 2020.

Edited by:

Dennis Qing Wang, Southern Medical University, China

Reviewed by:

Joseph M. Gullett, University of Florida, United States
Talitha Best, Central Queensland University, Australia

Copyright © 2020 Eggenberger, Annaheim, Kündig, Rossi, Münzer and de Bruin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrick Eggenberger, patrick.eggenberger@empa.ch

Download