Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 April 2017
Sec. Cognition

Exploring Tactile Perceptual Dimensions Using Materials Associated with Sensory Vocabulary

  • 1Department of Informatics, The University of Electro-Communications, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Kanagawa, Japan

Considering tactile sensation when designing products is important because the decision to purchase often depends on how products feel. Numerous psychophysical studies have attempted to identify important factors that describe tactile perceptions. However, the numbers and types of major tactile dimensions reported in previous studies have varied because of differences in materials used across experiments. To obtain a more complete picture of perceptual space with regard to touch, our study focuses on using vocabulary that expresses tactile sensations as a guiding principle for collecting material samples because these types of words are expected to cover all the basic categories within tactile perceptual space. We collected 120 materials based on a variety of Japanese sound-symbolic words for tactile sensations, and used the materials to examine tactile perceptual dimensions and their associations with affective evaluations. Analysis revealed six major dimensions: “Affective evaluation and Friction,” “Compliance,” “Surface,” “Volume,” “Temperature,” and “Naturalness.” These dimensions include four factors that previous studies have regarded as fundamental, as well as two new factors: “Volume” and “Naturalness.” Additionally, we showed that “Affective evaluation” is more closely related to the “Friction” component (slipperiness and dryness) than to other tactile perceptual features. Our study demonstrates that using vocabulary could be an effective method for selecting material samples to explore tactile perceptual space.

Introduction

The sense of touch is a growing interest in the fields of product design (Citrin et al., 2003; Peck and Childers, 2003; Grohmann et al., 2007), clothing (Na and Kim, 2001; Workman, 2010; Rahman, 2012), and cosmetics (Barnes et al., 2004; Nakatani et al., 2013). Specifically, it is relevant for designing products because the feel of products when handled can greatly affect a customer's final decision to purchase the product (Choi and Jun, 2007). Indeed, affective attachment experienced during tactile exploration has been recognized as a strong driver of product preference (Millar and Millar, 1996; Schifferstein, 2006; Sonneveld and Schifferstein, 2008; Peck and Shu, 2009). For designers, grasping the relationship between surface attributes and the psychological responses from customers is critical for designing the surface texture of a product.

Psychophysical studies contain a large body of literature regarding the dimensions of tactile perception (Yoshida, 1968; Lyne et al., 1984; Hollins et al., 1993, 2000; Picard et al., 2003; Gescheider et al., 2005; Bergmann Tiest and Kappers, 2006; Yoshioka et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2009; Guest et al., 2012; Okamoto et al., 2013). However, how tactile perceptual space is determined and how it is related to affective evaluations are still central questions both in perception psychology and in design applications that rely on touch. A critical problem affecting the results of past studies has been the criteria for selecting material samples. Most studies have tested only a limited number of material samples in specific areas (e.g., manufacturing products or textured fabrics). In fact, the numbers and types of major tactile dimensions reported in past studies have differed depending on the material samples used in the experiments (see Okamoto et al., 2013 for a review). In product design, although material has been a central point of research and practice for decades (Manzini, 1986; Ashby and Johnson, 2009; see Karana et al., 2015 for more details), as Karana et al. (2015) points out, a systematic method for defining and designing material experiences is lacking. This is likely because most studies take a particular material as a starting point and explore its engineering properties for potential use in a product.

The current study revisits tactile perceptual dimensions and their associations with affective evaluations by exploring tactile perceptual space using a variety of material samples that were selected with a different guiding principle. In contrast to the Material Driven Design Method proposed by Karana et al. (2015), which starts with a tangible material to facilitate the design process, our method begins with structuring tactile perceptual space. To explore as broad a tactile perceptual space as possible, we focused on sensory vocabulary called “sound symbolic words” (SSWs) as a guiding principle, and collected a varied sample of materials according to SSWs related to touch. Having detailed and reliable vocabulary is important for meaningful descriptions of perceptual experiences (Osgood, 1952; Bhushan et al., 1997; Guest et al., 2011), and we hypothesized that analyzing sensory vocabulary could be an effective way to investigate perceptual space (Malt and Majid, 2013). SSWs are adjective-like words that have associations between sound and meaning. The existence of SSWs has been demonstrated in a wide variety of languages (Köhler, 1929; Sapir, 1929; Bolinger, 1950; Hinton et al., 1994; Nuckolls, 1999; Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001; Schmidtke et al., 2014). For example, English words starting with “sl-” such as “slime,” “slush,” “slop,” “slobber,” “slip,” and “slide” symbolize something smooth or wet (Bloomfield, 1933). Semantic distinctions between tactile SSWs called “ideophones” have been reported to express tactile sensations in detail in Gbeya (a language of the Central African Republic; Samarin, 1967), and reproduce salient psychophysical dimensions in Siwu (a language of Ghana; Dingemanse and Majid, 2012). Japanese has a lexical class of SSWs called “mimetics” (Kita, 1997) or “onomatopoeia” that are used to express vivid sensations in everyday life (e.g., “sara-sara” represents a dry and smooth sensation like hair, and “zara-zara” represents a dry and rough sensation like sand paper). Recently, there has been a growing interest in using SSWs to explore perceptual space. Doizaki et al. (2016) proposed a system that automatically estimates multidimensional ratings of touch from a single SSW that is spontaneously and intuitively expressed by a user, and visualizes tactile perceptual space using the estimated SSW ratings. Their study provides an alternative method for estimating the fine quality of tactile sensations. SSWs can be used to examine other perceptual spaces as well. Sakamoto and Watanabe (2015) investigated sound symbolism in taste by analyzing SSWs spontaneously and intuitively used by participants to express tastes/textures and showed that SSWs could be important indexes for investigating differing levels of gustatory perceptual dimensions, including emotional evaluations like pleasant/unpleasant.

In this paper, we therefore focused on Japanese SSWs to investigate tactile perceptual dimensions. Japanese has a large number of SSWs for expressing touch sensations, and the vocabulary constitutes a linguistic system with rigid phonological constraints and a strong systematic association between form and meaning that is constituted socially (Hamano, 1998). For example, Japanese words that express a sense of smoothness often use the consonant /s/ in the first syllable as in “sara-sara” and “sube-sube,” while those expressing roughness often use /j/ or /z/ in the first syllable, as in “jari-jari,” “jori-jori,” and “zara-zara” (Watanabe et al., 2012). Japanese SSWs generally have a fixed structure in which a combination of two syllables is repeated in the form of CVCV-CVCV (C, consonant; V, Vowel). New SSWs can be created only by combining phonemes. The variation of syllables (especially the first syllable) is expected to correspond to distinct sensory categories (Hamano, 1998), and we should be able to systematically explore tactile perceptual space by encompassing materials associated with all Japanese phonemes that are located at the beginning of the words, at least to the extent that the perceptual space is one that the Japanese language can express. We therefore regarded Japanese SSWs as a useful index that can cover a wide range within tactile perceptual space, and used them to collect a variety of tactile material samples.

Our study differs critically from previous studies on tactile perceptual dimensions in that the criteria for collecting material samples is based on sensory vocabulary, not on physical properties, with tactile perceptual space approached from a phenomenological perspective (perceptual classification and its labeling), rather than a physical one. Phenomenology is mostly used to provide a better understanding of the way people perceive the world, and phenomenological methods have been introduced in fields of design that aim at providing design-relevant knowledge on how to design new artifacts (see Deckers et al., 2012). In this study, we chose 120 material samples corresponding to Japanese SSWs for expressing tactile sensations with a systematic association between sounds and meanings. Using the 120 materials, we performed psychophysical experiments to understand tactile perceptual dimensions and their associations with affective evaluations.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Electro-Communications, Tokyo, Japan, and adhered to the tenets of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki (most recently amended in 2008). All participants provided written informed consent before the experiments. Experimental protocols and written informed consent were presented to the ethics committee.

SSWs as an Index for Collecting Tactile Materials

We generated Japanese SSWs to be used as an index for collecting tactile material samples that represented as large a tactile perceptual space as possible. Initially, we made SSW-like expressions by combining all Japanese syllables. We created SSW-like expressions in the typical two-syllable-repeated form (e.g., “saka-saka,” “saki-saki,” “saku-saku,” “sake-sake,” and “sako-sako”) and added all types of special phonemes used in Japanese SSWs (syllabic nasals /N/, choked sounds /Q/, long vowels /R/, and adverbs ending in /Li/). From this considerably large number of SSW-like expressions (14,584), three 21-year old native Japanese speakers (two male and one female) who use SSWs in daily life selected words that would be acceptable as Japanese tactile SSWs. One-hundred and ten of these SSW expressions were chosen by two or three speakers and their degree of conventionality was tested using a Google search on June 6, 2014. As a result, more than 100,000 search results were obtained for all of the 110 SSWs. The first syllables of the 110 words covered all Japanese consonants except /l/, which is never used as the first syllable in conventional tactile SSWs, indicating that we can systematically explore tactile perceptual space by collecting materials associated with the first syllables.

To analyze the relationships among the 110 SSWs, we used a system that can convert a tactile SSW into rating scores for multiple touch-related adjectives. In a previous study (Doizaki et al., 2016), we built a database of sound symbolic associations between SSW phonemes and their sensory impressions. The database utilizes 26 pairs of opposing adjectives (e.g., hard/soft, wet/dry) that were selected based on the results of separate psychological experiments in which participants evaluated impressions of SSWs. Using this database, here we estimated a given SSW's ratings for each of the 26 pairs adjectives by averaging the impressions given in the database for each phoneme of the SSW. To intuitively grasp the relationships between the 110 SSWs, we performed a principal component analysis of the 110 SSWs using scores for 6 of the 26 pairs (“hard/soft,” “rough/smooth,” “bumpy/flat,” “sticky/slippery,” “wet/dry,” and “warm/cold”), which constitute the basic tactile dimensions (see Okamoto et al., 2013). Then, we generated a distribution diagram of the SSWs using the first and second principal components as the horizontal and vertical axes, respectively.

Collecting Materials

Five experts in linguistics and psychology who have experience in tactile psychophysical experiments (including the two authors) selected 120 tactile materials whose textures and properties could be expressed by one or more of the 110 SSWs. Thus, the 110 SSWs were assigned to the 120 materials. These assignments were then confirmed by four additional naïve participants.

Evaluating Subjective Impression

Sixty naïve participants (30 males and 30 females; age range, 19–26 years) took part in the experiments. All were native Japanese speakers, unaware of the purpose of the experiments, and none had any known abnormalities in verbal or tactile sensory systems or in any particular skills with respect to touch. They visited a laboratory at the University of Electro-Communications for 1 day to take part in the experiment.

The 60 participants were randomly classified into two groups (n = 30 for each) to reduce the number of materials each participant had to touch. Sixty materials were assigned to each group. Participants sat in front of a box with an 8 × 10 cm opening (the material box), and touched a material through the opening using the index finger of their dominant hand. Participants could not see the material while they were touching, and were asked at this time to rate the material on each of the 26 pairs of adjectives (Table 1). Figure 1 shows an illustration of the experimental setup.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of 26 adjective pairs.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental setup.

The six pairs of adjectives in the left column are those given as basic tactile dimensions (Okamoto et al., 2013). The other 20 pairs of adjectives are related to material-oriented and affective evaluations. Most of the 26 pairs of adjectives are included in the 262-word English vocabulary for expressing tactile sensations (Guest et al., 2011). Ratings were made on a seven-point Likert-like scale (e.g., −3 = very smooth, 3 = very rough). Participants could freely run their fingers along the surface or press their finger into the material to ascertain a variety of material properties (Lederman and Klatzky, 1987). No time limit was given for rating. The experimenter placed one material in the material box, and only replaced it with the next material after the participant had touched and rated it. The materials were presented in random order. The experiment took about 50–60 min for each participant.

Results

SSWs as an Index for Collecting Tactile Materials

Figure 2 is the scatter plot of the 110 SSWs based on the first and second principal components as the horizontal and vertical axes, respectively. The cumulative contribution ratio of the first and second principal components was 80.0%. We have added the six pairs of basic tactile adjectives to the diagram based on the principal component loadings. Additionally, SSWs expressing closely related sensations are located close to each other on the distribution diagram. The diagram is thus a spatial map that reveals of how tactile sensations are categorized by Japanese language.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatter plot of 110 identified SSWs. The repetition of syllables in the SSWs are omitted (CVCV-CVCV is expressed as CVCV). SSW, sound symbolic word; C, consonant; V, Vowel.

Material Collection

The 120 materials are listed with their compositions and the associated SSWs in Appendix in Supplementary Material. Images of the materials, such as particle, metal, ceramic, glass, spring, elastomer, soft-urethane, gel, rubber, leather, polyurethane, paper, fabric, clay, polystyrene, rough paper, stone, and others are given in Figure 3. Most of the material samples were cut into 6 × 6 cm squares and stacked into 2-mm thick layers. The rocks and sand were placed loose in a container.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Images of the 120 materials used in the experiment.

Evaluating Subjective Impressions

Each of 30 participants gave 26 ratings (one for each pair of adjectives) for half of the 120 material samples and the other 30 participants did the same for the other half of the samples. We conducted a factor analysis to detect basic structures in the relationships among adjectives and to classify them. More specifically, principal component analysis was applied to mean values of the 26 adjective ratings. This type of factor analysis is a common statistical method for describing variables via the lowest number of unobserved variables called factors. Although this method may simply cluster similar categorical words together and depends on the words pairs selected for the analysis, it remains useful and has been employed in numerous psychophysical studies, including tactile studies (see a review by Okamoto et al., 2013). Results of the analysis are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor loadings for the 26 adjective pairs.

We selected factors having an eigenvalue >1.0 and confirmed that the first six factors accounted for 91.0% of the total variance. Factor 1 was an “Affective evaluation” dimension, with high loadings on scales for Comfortable/Uncomfortable, Good/Bad, Pleasant/Irritating, Clean/Dirty, Relieved/Uneasy, Familiar/Unfamiliar, Ordinary/Eccentric, Calm/Intense, and Impressive/Unimpressive. Factor 1 also included the “Friction” components of Slippery/Sticky, Repulsive/Non-repulsive, and Wet/Dry such that positive affective evaluations were strongly associated with non-frictional features such as slipperiness, non-resistance, and dryness. Factor 2 was a “Compliance” dimension, with high loadings on scales for Hard/Soft, Elastic/Non-elastic, Stretchy/Non-stretchy, Sharp/Dull, and Firm/Fragile. This dimension also included the adjective pair Strong/Weak. It is reasonable that a hard, non-elastic, non-stretchy, sharp, and firm object is perceived as strong. Factor 3 was a “Surface” dimension, with high loadings on Bumpy/Flat, Smooth/Rough, and Regular/Irregular. These adjective pairs are related to surface texture or geometry. Factor 4 was a “Volume” dimension, with high loadings on Heavy/Light, and Thick/Thin. This dimension is a shape property that was independent of the material properties. This property has not been addressed in previous studies, most of which focused on material properties and either only allowed participants to touch material surfaces or only considered flat materials (Okamoto et al., 2013). The adjective pair Luxury/Cheap also had a high loading for this dimension, although it was also related to the “Affective evaluation and Friction” dimension. Factor 5 was a “Temperature” dimension with a high loading for the scale Warm/Cold. Factor 6 was a “Naturalness” dimension, with a high loading for the Natural/Artificial scale. This dimension has not been highlighted in any previous study.

Discussion

Our study collected 120 material samples based on 110 sensory words related to tactile sensations. Results yielded six major dimensions for tactile perceptions and evaluations, which included almost all perceptual factors mentioned in other studies (Okamoto et al., 2013), plus several additional ones. This suggests that our vocabulary-based material collection covered a wider range of perceptual space compared with commonly used physical-property-based collection methods, although the arbitrariness of connections between words and materials might not be negligible. The novel point of this study is that the tactile perceptual space was approached from a phenomenological viewpoint rather than from a physical viewpoint, and was tested in a psychophysical experiment. We believe that the combination of material collection using language and its psychophysical validation can contribute to revealing a representation of human engagement with the tactile world.

We focused on sensory vocabulary for collecting material samples, and specifically on SSWs as opposed to normal adjectives. One SSW expresses rich and delicate information that must be otherwise expressed by combinations of two or more normal adjectives. Additionally, there are a larger number of SSWs than normal adjectives in Japanese for expressing tactile sensations. This suggests that Japanese SSWs describe a wider range of tactile sensations than normal Japanese adjectives. In fact, Sakamoto and Watanabe (2013) showed that more than 20 Japanese SSWs were evoked by the tactile sensations for 40 materials, while only 15 normal Japanese adjectives were evoked for the same materials. Moreover, there is a systematic and strong association between the first syllable of Japanese SSWs and the category of tactile sensation (Watanabe et al., 2012). Further, SSWs might describe sensory qualities with a finer resolution given that different types of SSWs were associated with imitation metals and real metals (Sakamoto et al., 2016). Here, we therefore used Japanese phonemes to express tactile experience as an index for collecting material samples.

The main result of the current study is that the 26 pairs of adjectives could be described by six factors. This result is not simply a measure of the conceptual closeness of the adjectives that describe the tactile features, but is also a measure of the co-occurrence of tactile dimensions among the 120 materials in terms of the 26 adjective pairs. Because we collected materials based on sensory vocabulary, the nature of the materials varied considerably and the six factors reflect a broad range of perceptual dimensions. Although we cannot overcome the limitations of vocabulary or exclude bias of linguistic descriptions, our method thus provides benefits over those used in previous studies.

Fundamental tactile dimensions obtained in previous studies did not completely match what we found here, but were somehow complementary (see Okamoto et al., 2013). We found that the six dimensions “Affective evaluation and Friction,” “Compliance,” “Surface,” “Volume,” “Temperature,” and “Naturalness” are dominant factors in human tactile perceptual experience. The three perceptual dimensions (“Compliance,” “Surface,” and “Temperature”) have been frequently observed in other studies, with “Compliance” and “Surface” being regarded as robust (Hollins et al., 1993, 2000; Bergmann Tiest and Kappers, 2006), and thermal information being known to play a critical role in material perception (Ho and Jones, 2006; Tiest and Kappers, 2009; Tiest, 2010). Note that the “Surface” dimension could be further divided into sub-dimensions (coarse and fine) in terms of corresponding sensory channels (Hollins and Rinser, 2000), and that the “Temperature” dimension is unique in terms of its functional connectivity in the brain because temperature information is processed primarily through the insular cortex, not primary somatosensory cortex (Peltz et al., 2011). Given that the perceptual characteristic of these three dimensions are obtained through different movements (pushing, tracing, and static touch; Lederman and Klatzky, 1987), regarding them as essential and independent dimensions might be reasonable.

The first dimension extracted in our study, “Affective evaluation and Friction,” includes almost all affective evaluations and a few perceptual adjectives. Objects and their meanings that are evaluated using multiple adjectives are known to be categorized primarily with affective criterion (Sakamoto and Utsumi, 2014). This is consistent with our finding that affective evaluations are included in the primary dimension. The first dimension also included adjective pairs pertaining to frictional perceptions (Slippery/Sticky and Repulsive/Non-repulsive) and moisture (Wet/Dry). Interestingly, Slippery/Sticky and Wet/Dry have been obtained as important tactile factors in several other studies, although they have never been extracted together in a single experiment (Okamoto et al., 2013). Additionally, although evaluations of friction and roughness seem to share some underlying mechanisms (Smith et al., 2002), given that roughness was insensitive to the frictional status of the surfaces (Taylor and Lederman, 1975; Nonomura et al., 2009; Skedung et al., 2011), the frictional evaluations in the first dimension appear to be independent of the “Surface” dimension.

Little is known about the relationship between affective evaluations and perceptual factors (Essick et al., 2010), although studies on single perceptual properties—mostly on roughness (Choi and Jun, 2007; Kitada et al., 2012)—have been performed. Studies on multiple tactile properties have indicated that users prefer smooth sandpaper, cardboard, and paper (seven materials; Ekman et al., 1965), slippery and hard cardboard, flexible materials, and laminate boards (37 materials; Chen et al., 2009), smooth and soft materials encountered in everyday life (48 materials; Klöcker et al., 2012), and soft and smooth virtual surfaces, where only roughness and compliance were varied (Hilsenrat and Reiner, 2011). In our study, crystalline limestone (#100) and the ceramic dish (#17) were highly associated with evaluations of “Slippery” and “Comfortable,” while ether-based polyurethane (#23) and methylcellulose (#117) were highly associated with “Sticky” and “Uncomfortable.”

The association between “sticky” and “uncomfortable” has been reported in studies examining the feeling of disgust, with mushiness, stickiness, and sliminess being related to feelings of disgust from the viewpoint of pathogen avoidance (Curtis and Biran, 2001). Additionally, experiments have shown that wet stimuli and stimuli resembling biological consistencies (stickiness) are evaluated as more disgusting (Oum et al., 2011). Here, we also extracted factors pertaining to cognitive evaluations (Good/Bad) in the first dimension. Three functions—evolutional (pathogen avoidance), biological (mate choice), and social (moral judgment)—have been suggested to be deeply related to feelings of disgust (Tybur et al., 2009). Given that stickiness is related to a feeling of disgust within a biological context (Clean/Dirty), feeling Good/Bad could be included in the same dimension.

The fourth dimension “Volume” is related to aspects of an object's shape, such as thickness (Thick/Thin), weight (Heavy/Light), and its valuation (Luxury/Cheap). For example, crystalline limestone (#99) and gray granite (#101) were evaluated as thick, heavy, and expensive objects, while polypropylene film (#106) and denim (#87) were evaluated as thin, light, and cheap. Although valuation is deeply related to touch (for example, increased duration of physical contact leads to higher valuations in a manner similar to the duration of actual ownership (Wolf et al., 2008), little attention has been paid to valuation in touch research. Our interpretation of the fourth dimension is that thicker and heavier objects tend to be perceived as more luxurious, although valuation is also related to the “Affective evaluation and Friction” dimension. Considering the relationship between size and perceived quality (e.g., Yan et al., 2014), this interpretation is reasonable.

Analysis revealed “Naturalness” as a sixth major dimension of tactile evaluations. The concept of naturalness has been used widely in studies of sensory perception, but the meaning is somehow ambiguous and an objective definition has been lacking. In this study, we defined naturalness as “the feeling that an object is something derived from nature” (Overvliet and Soto-Faraco, 2011), and asked participants to rate “Naturalness” on a seven-point scale ranging from “very natural” to “very artificial.” Objects that were rated the most natural were granite (#102) and water (#114), which are natural materials, while objects rated the most artificial were aluminum product (#13) and a stainless-steel coil spring (#21). The feeling of naturalness is likely to be assessed from information gathered by multiple modalities, which can provide complementary knowledge about materials. For example, the contributions of vision and touch have been experimentally validated in perceiving the naturalness of wood (Overvliet and Soto-Faraco, 2011). Our results (Table 2) indicate that the “Regular/Irregular” categorization within the “Surface” dimension, and “Elastic/Non-elastic” in the “Compliance” dimension are also related to “Naturalness.” This notion agrees with the intuition that something irregular and elastic is made from natural materials. At present, naturalness is a highly appreciated material property for food and medicine (e.g., Rozin et al., 2004), but only a few studies have examined tactile naturalness as a critical factor in product preference. Further, systematic research is needed to investigate the relationship between the concept of naturalness and tactile perceptual features.

Because the factors extracted from our results were based on data averaged among 60 participants whose age ranged from 19 to 26 years, our results might only reflect the tactile perceptual space of young people. The ability to sense tactile properties is known to decline with aging due to changes in the density and distribution of mechanoreceptors (Stevens and Patterson, 1995). Thus, tactile perceptual space might differ between young and older people. Additionally, differences in skin properties might influence perceptual space. Understanding these types of individual differences in perceptual space is an important issue when designing the tactile properties of products.

In our experiment, we asked participants to rate their impressions of materials and evaluate affective properties through the sense of touch alone (without visual information) because we wanted to examine the representation of tactile perceptual space. However, vision plays an obviously important complimentary role in the perception of objects in our daily lives, and interest in the contributions and interactions of visual and tactile modalities in object perception has been growing. While participants have been reported to be able to retrieve similar information through visual and tactile modalities regarding surface roughness (Bergmann Tiest and Kappers, 2007) and three-dimensional shape (Gaissert and Wallraven, 2012), visual and tactile modalities were shown to be related to distinct perceptual properties in a free-sorting task: shape for vision and substances for touch (Klatzky et al., 1987). Another study has shown that material properties primarily obtained through touch (i.e., hardness and roughness) are crucial for material perception, and the interplay between visual and tactile senses is required for precision (Baumgartner et al., 2013). Our brain is known to integrate visual and tactile information when estimating material properties of an object. When estimating the size (Ernst and Banks, 2002) and the surface roughness (Lederman et al., 1986) of an object through both modalities, these two inputs are averaged. However, visual information can be used to generate prior tactile expectations before touching an object. When they are mismatched, this can cause a contrast effect, such as in the size-weight illusion (Ross, 1969), in which the contrast between expectation and actual percept is emphasized. Investigating visual and tactile perceptual space using SSW is an intriguing direction of future research.

Conclusion

To obtain a more complete picture of tactile perceptual space, this study focused on sensory vocabulary termed “sound symbolic words” (SSWs) for expressing tactile sensations. We collected a wide range of material samples based on tactile SSWs obtained through testing, and revisited tactile perceptual dimensions. Analysis revealed six major dimensions: “Affective evaluation and Friction,” “Compliance,” “Surface,” “Volume,” “Temperature,” and “Naturalness.” These dimensions include almost all factors mentioned by past studies as well two factors, “Volume” and “Naturalness” that have not been frequently found in other studies. Additionally, we showed that “Affective evaluation” is more closely related to the “Friction” component (slipperiness and dryness) than to other tactile perceptual features. We believe that tactile perceptual dimensions extracted from vocabulary-based analysis will help designers select and create the best range of textures, which is important for obtaining effective affective evaluations.

Author Contributions

MS and JW conceived the experiments. MS performed the experiment and carried out the data analyses. All authors discussed and interpreted the results, and contributed to drafts of this paper.

Funding

This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Shitsukan” (No. 23135510 and 25135713) from MEXT and a JSPS KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Innovative SHITSUKSAN Science and Technology” (No. 15H05922).

Conflict of Interest Statement

JW is employed by NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation as a research scientist conducting basic scientific research on human sensory processing. There are no patents, products in development or marketed products to declare. This does not alter the authors' adherence to the journal's policies on sharing data and materials.

The other author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00569/full#supplementary-material

References

Ashby, M., and Johnson, K. (2009). Materials and Design. The Art and Science of Material Selection in Product Design, 2nd Edn. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Barnes, C. J., Childs, T. H. C., Henson, B., and Southee, C. H. (2004). Surface finish and touch - A case study in a new human factors tribology. Wear 257, 740–750. doi: 10.1016/j.wear.2004.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner, E., Wiebel, C., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Visual and haptic representations of material properties. Multisens. Res. 26, 429–455. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergmann Tiest, W. M., and Kappers, A. M. L. (2006). Analysis of haptic perception of materials by multidimensional scaling and physical measurements of roughness and compressibility. Acta Psychol. 121, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergmann Tiest, W. M., and Kappers, A. M. L. (2007). Haptic and visual perception of roughness. Acta Psychol. 124, 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhushan, N., Rao, A. R., and Lohse, G. L. (1997). The texture lexicon: understanding the categorization of visual texture terms and their relationship to texture images. Cogn. Sci. 21, 219–246. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2102_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York, NY: Henry Holt.

Google Scholar

Bolinger, D. (1950). Rime, assonance, and morpheme analysis. Word 6, 117–136. doi: 10.1080/00437956.1950.11659374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Shao, F., Barnes, C., Childs, T., and Henson, B. (2009). Exploring relationships between touch perception and surface physical properties. Int. J. Design 3, 67–77.

Google Scholar

Choi, K., and Jun, C. (2007). A systematic approach to the Kansei factors of tactile sense regarding the surface roughness. Appl. Ergon. 38, 55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2006.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Citrin, A. V., Stem, D. E., Spangenberg, E. R., and Clark, M. J. (2003). Consumer need for tactile input: an internet retailing challenge. J. Bus. Res. 56, 915–922. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(01)00278-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, V., and Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: is hygiene in our genes? Perspect. Biol. Med. 44, 17–31. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2001.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deckers, E. J. L., Lévy, P. D., Wensveen, S. A. G., Ahn, R. M. C., and Overbeeke, C. J. (2012). Designing for perceptual crossing: applying and evaluating design notions. Int. J. Design 6, 41–55.

Google Scholar

Dingemanse, M., and Majid, A. (2012). “The semantic structure of sensory vocabulary in an African language,” in Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (Sapporo), 300–305.

Google Scholar

Doizaki, R., Watanabe, J., and Sakamoto, M. (2016). Automatic estimation of multidimensional ratings from a single sound-symbolic word and word-based visualization of tactile perceptual space. IEEE Trans. Haptics. doi: 10.1109/TOH.2016.2615923. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, G., Hosman, J., and Lindstrom, B. (1965). Roughness, Smoothness, and Preference - A study of quantitative relations in individual subjects. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 18–26. doi: 10.1037/h0021985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, M. O., and Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, 429–433. doi: 10.1038/415429a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Essick, G. K., McGlone, F., Dancer, C., Fabricant, D., Ragin, Y., Phillips, N., et al. (2010). Quantitative assessment of pleasant touch. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaissert, N., and Wallraven, C. (2012). Categorizing natural objects: a comparison of the visual and the haptic modalities. Exp. Brain Res. 216, 123–134. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2916-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gescheider, G. A., Bolanowski, S. J., Greenfield, T. C., and Brunette, K. E. (2005). Perception of the tactile texture of raised - dot patterns: a multidimensional analysis. Somatosens. Motor Res. 22, 127–140. doi: 10.1080/08990220500262018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grohmann, B., Spangenberg, E. R., and Sprott, D. E. (2007). The influence of tactile input on the evaluation of retail product offerings. J. Retail. 83, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2006.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guest, S., Dessirier, J. M., Mehrabyan, A., McGlone, F., Essick, G., Gescheider, G., et al. (2011). The development and validation of sensory and emotional scales of touch perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 531–550. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0037-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guest, S., Mehrabyan, A., Essick, G., Phillips, N., Hopkinson, A., and Mcglone, F. (2012). Physics and tactile perception of fluid - covered surfaces. J. Texture Stud. 43, 77–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-4603.2011.00318.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamano, S. (1998). The Sound Symbolic System of Japanese. Stanford, CA; Tokyo: CSLI Publications; Kuroshio.

Google Scholar

Hilsenrat, M., and Reiner, M. (2011). The impact of subliminal haptic perception on the preference discrimination of roughness and compliance. Brain Res. Bull. 85, 267–270. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinton, L., Nichols, J., and Ohala, J. (eds.). (1994). Sound Symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Ho, H.-N., and Jones, L. A. (2006). Contribution of thermal cues to material discrimination and localization. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 118–128. doi: 10.3758/BF03193662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollins, M., and Rinser, S. R. (2000). Evidence for the duplex theory of tactile texture perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 695–705. doi: 10.3758/BF03206916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollins, M., Bensmaïa, S., Karlof, K., and Young, F. (2000). Individual differences in perceptual space for tactile textures: evidence from multidimensional scaling. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 1534–1544. doi: 10.3758/BF03212154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollins, M., Faldowski, R., Rao, S., and Young, F. (1993). Perceptual dimensions of tactile surface texture: a multidimensional scaling analysis. Percept. Psychophys. 54, 697–705. doi: 10.3758/BF03211795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karana, E., Barati, B., Rognoli, V., and Zeeuw van der Laan, A. (2015). Material driven design (MDD): a method to design for material experiences. Int. J. Design 9, 35–54.

Google Scholar

Kita, S. (1997). Two - dimensional semantic analysis of Japanese mimetics. Linguistics 35, 379–415. doi: 10.1515/ling.1997.35.2.379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitada, R., Sadato, N., and Lederman, S. J. (2012). Tactile perception of nonpainful unpleasantness in relation to perceived roughness: effects of inter - element spacing and speed of relative motion of rigid 2- D raised-dot patterns at two body loci. Perception 41, 204–220. doi: 10.1068/p7168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klatzky, R. L., Lederman, S., and Reed, C. (1987). There's more to touch than meets the eye: the salience of object attributes for haptics with and without vision. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 116, 356–369. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.116.4.356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klöcker, A., Arnould, C., Penta, M., and Thonnard, J. L. (2012). Rasch - built measure of pleasant touch through active fingertip exploration. Front. Neurorobot. 6:5. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2012.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt Psychology. NewYork, NY: Liveright Publishing Corporation.

PubMed Abstract

Lederman, S. J., and Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 342–368. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90008-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lederman, S. J., Thorne, G., and Jones, B. (1986). Perception of texture by vision and touch: multidimensionality and intersensory integration. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 12, 169–180. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.12.2.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyne, M. B., Whiteman, A., and Donderi, D. C. (1984). Multidimensional scaling of tissue quality. Pulp Pap. Canada 85, 43–50.

Google Scholar

Malt, B. C., and Majid, A. (2013). How though is mapped into words. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 4, 583–597.

Google Scholar

Manzini, E. (1986). The Material of Invention. Milan: Arcadia Edizioni.

Google Scholar

Millar, M. G., and Millar, K. U. (1996). The effects of direct and indirect experience on affective and cognitive responses and the attitude-behavior relation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 32, 561–579. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1996.0025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Na, Y., and Kim, C. (2001). Quantifying the handle and sensibility of woven silk fabrics. Text. Res. J. 71, 739–742. doi: 10.1177/004051750107100814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakatani, M., Fukuda, T., Sasamoto, H., Arakawa, N., Otaka, H., Kawasoe, T., et al. (2013). Relationship between perceived softness of bilayered skin models and their mechanical properties measured with a dual - sensor probe. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 35, 84–88. doi: 10.1111/ics.12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nonomura, Y., Fujii, T., Arashi, Y., Miura, T., Maeno, T., Tashiro, K., et al. (2009). Tactile impression and friction of water on human skin. Colloids Surf. B Biointerfaces 69, 264–267. doi: 10.1016/j.colsurfb.2008.11.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuckolls, J. (1999). The case for sound symbolism. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 28, 225–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, S., Nagano, H., and Yamada, Y. (2013). Psychophysical dimensions of tactile perception of textures. IEEE Trans. Haptics 6, 81–93. doi: 10.1109/TOH.2012.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osgood, C. E. (1952). The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychol. Bull. 49, 197–237. doi: 10.1037/h0055737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oum, R. E., Lieberman, D., and Aylward, A. (2011). A feel for disgust: tactile cues to pathogen presence. Cogn. Emot. 25, 717–725. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.496997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overvliet, K. E., and Soto-Faraco, S. (2011). I can't believe this isn't wood! An investigation in the perception of naturalness. Acta Psychol. 136, 95–111. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, J., and Childers, T. L. (2003). To have and to hold. The influence of haptic information on product judgments. J. Market. 67, 35–48. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.67.2.35.18612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, J., and Shu, S. B. (2009). The effect of mere touch on perceived ownership. J. Cons. Res. 36, 434–447. doi: 10.1086/598614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peltz, E., Seifert, F., DeCol, R., Dörfler, A., Schwab, S., and Maihöfner, C. (2011). Functional connectivity of the human insular cortex during noxious and innocuous thermal stimulation. Neuroimage 54, 1324–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picard, D., Dacremont, C., Valentin, D., and Giboreau, A. (2003). Perceptual dimensions of tactile textures. Acta Psychol. 114, 165–184. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, O. (2012). The influence of visual and tactile inputs on denim jeans evaluation. Int. J. Design 6, 11–25.

Google Scholar

Ramachandran, V. S., and Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia - A window into perception, thought, and language. J. Conscious. Stud. 8, 3–34.

Google Scholar

Ross, H. E. (1969). When is a weight not illusory? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 21, 346–355. doi: 10.1080/14640746908400230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., Spranca, M., Krieger, Z., Neuhaus, R., Surillo, D., and Swerdlin, A. (2004). Preference for natural: instrumental and ideational/moral motivations, and the contrast between foods and medicines. Appetite 43, 147–154. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, M., and Utsumi, A. (2014). Adjective metaphors evoke negative meanings. PLoS ONE 9:e89008. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, M., and Watanabe, J. (2013). “Effectiveness of onomatopoeia representing quality of tactile texture: a comparative study with adjectives,” in Papers from the 13th National Conference of the Japanese Cognitive Linguistics Association (Tokyo), 473–485 (in Japanese).

Sakamoto, M., and Watanabe, J. (2015). Cross-modal associations between sounds and drink tastes/textures: a study with spontaneous production of sound-symbolic words. Chem. Senses 41, 197–203. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, M., Yoshino, J., Doizaki, R., and Haginoya, M. (2016). Metal-like texture design evaluation using sound symbolic words. Int. J. Des. Creat. Innov. 4, 181–194. doi: 10.1080/21650349.2015.1061449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samarin, W. J. (1967). Determining the meaning of ideophones. J. West Afr. Lang. 4, 35–41.

Sapir, E. (1929). A study of phonetic symbolism. J. Exp. Psychol. 12, 225–239. doi: 10.1037/h0070931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2006). The perceived importance of sensory modalities in product usage. A study of self-reports. Acta Psychol. 121, 41–64. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidtke, D. S., Conrad, M., and Jacobs, A. M. (2014). Phonological iconicity. Front. Psychol. 5:80. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skedung, L., Danerlöv, K., Olofsson, U., Johannesson, C. M., Aikala, M., Kettle, J., et al. (2011). Tactile perception: finger friction, surface roughness and perceived coarseness. Tribol. Int. 44, 505–512

Google Scholar

Smith, A. M., Chapman, C. E., Deslandes, M., Langlais, J. S., and Thibodeau, M. P. (2002). Role of friction and tangential force variation in the subjective scaling of tactile roughness. Exp. Brain Res. 144, 211–223. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1015-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonneveld, M. H., and Schifferstein, N. J. (2008). “The tactual experience of objects,” in Product Experience, eds H. N. J. Schifferstein and P. Hekkert (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 41–67.

Google Scholar

Stevens, J. C., and Patterson, M. Q. (1995). Dimensions of spatial acuity in the touch sense: changes over a life span. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 12, 29–47. doi: 10.3109/08990229509063140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M. M., and Lederman, S. J. (1975). Tactile roughness of grooved surfaces: a model and the effect of friction. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 17, 23–36. doi: 10.3758/BF03203993

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiest, W. M. (2010). Tactual perception of material properties. Vis. Res. 50, 2775–2782. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiest, W. M., and Kappers, A. M. L. (2009). Tactile perception of thermal diffusivity. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 481–489. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.3.481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tybur, J., Lieberman, D., and Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122. doi: 10.1037/a0015474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, J., Hayakawa, T., Matsui, S., Kano, A., Shimizu, Y., and Sakamoto, M. (2012). “Visualization of tactile material relationships using sound symbolic words,” in EuroHaptics 2012, Part II, LNCS 7283, eds P. Isokoski and J. Springare (Heidelberg: Springer), 175–180. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-31404-9_30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, J. R., Arkes, H. R., and Muhanna, W. A. (2008). The power of touch: an examination of the effect of duration of physical contact on the valuation of objects. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 3, 476–482.

Google Scholar

Workman, J. E. (2010). Fashion consumer groups, gender, and need for touch. Cloth. Text. Res. J. 28, 126–139. doi: 10.1177/0887302X09356323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, D., Sengupta, J., and Wyer, R. S. Jr. (2014). Package size and perceived quality: the intervening role of unit price perceptions. J. Cons. Psychol. 24, 4–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, M. (1968). Dimensions of tactual impressions (1). Jpn. Psychol. Res. 10, 123–137.

Google Scholar

Yoshioka, T., Bensamaïa, S. J., Craig, J. C., and Hsiao, S. S. (2007). Texture perception through direct and indirect touch: an analysis of perceptual space for tactile textures in two modes of exploration. Somatosens. Motor Res. 24, 53–70. doi: 10.1080/08990220701318163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tactile materials, tactile perceptual dimensions, sound symbolic words, affective evaluation, friction

Citation: Sakamoto M and Watanabe J (2017) Exploring Tactile Perceptual Dimensions Using Materials Associated with Sensory Vocabulary. Front. Psychol. 8:569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00569

Received: 21 December 2016; Accepted: 28 March 2017;
Published: 13 April 2017.

Edited by:

Bernhard Hommel, Leiden University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Zhuanghua Shi, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany
Bao-Liang Lu, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China

Copyright © 2017 Sakamoto and Watanabe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maki Sakamoto, maki.sakamoto@uec.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.