Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 26 September 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 11 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00264

Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over S1 Differentially Modulates Proprioceptive Accuracy in Young and Old Adults

Toni Muffel1,2,3,4,5*† Franziska Kirsch1,6† Pei-Cheng Shih1,7 Benjamin Kalloch1,7,8 Sara Schaumberg1 Arno Villringer1,2,3,4,5,7 Bernhard Sehm1,2,9
  • 1Neuroplasticity and Motor Recovery Group, Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 2Day Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
  • 3Mind Brain Body Institute, Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 4Center for Stroke Research Berlin (CSB), Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 5International Max Planck Research School on the Life Course, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
  • 6Institute of Psychology, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 7International Max Planck Research School on Neuroscience of Communication, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 8Faculty of Computer Science and Media, Leipzig University of Applied Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 9Department of Neurology, Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Halle, Germany

Background: Proprioception is a prerequisite for successful motor control but declines throughout the lifespan. Brain stimulation techniques such as anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (a-tDCS) are capable of enhancing sensorimotor performance across different tasks and age groups. Despite such growing evidence for a restorative potential of tDCS, its impact on proprioceptive accuracy has not been studied in detail yet.

Objective: This study investigated online effects of a-tDCS over S1 on proprioceptive accuracy in young (YA) and old healthy adults (OA).

Methods: The effect of 15 min of a-tDCS vs. sham on proprioceptive accuracy was assessed in a cross-over, double blind experiment in both age groups. Performance changes were tested using an arm position matching task in a robotic environment. Electrical field (EF) strengths in the target area S1 and control areas were assessed based on individualized simulations.

Results: a-tDCS elicited differential changes in proprioceptive accuracy and EF strengths in the two groups: while YA showed a slight improvement, OA exhibited a decrease in performance during a-tDCS. Stronger EF were induced in target S1 and control areas in the YA group. However, no relationship between EF strength and performance change was found.

Conclusion: a-tDCS over S1 elicits opposing effects on proprioceptive accuracy as a function of age, a result that is important for future studies investigating the restorative potential of a-tDCS in healthy aging and in the rehabilitation of neurological diseases that occur at advanced age. Modeling approaches could help elucidate the relationship between tDCS protocols, brain structure and performance modulation.

Introduction

Proprioception – the perception of body position and movement – is a vital function for daily activities including postural stability and reaching movements (Findlater et al., 2016), which declines in the course of aging (Stelmach and Sirica, 1986; Adamo et al., 2007; Herter et al., 2014; Hughes et al., 2015). Both changes in the peripheral and central nervous system have been associated with this decline, including decreases in muscle spindle sensitivity (Kim et al., 2007) and diameter (Kararizou et al., 2005) as well as atrophy in the postcentral gyrus, parietal and insular cortices (Good et al., 2001), which all are regions associated with proprioceptive processing (Goossens et al., 2019). Among cortical regions, the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) is the most important area to process proprioceptive information (Behrens et al., 2003; Findlater et al., 2016; Armenta Salas et al., 2018). On a functional level, Goble et al. (2012a) showed largely overlapping functional networks involved in proprioception in young (YA, <35 years) and older healthy adults (OA, >65 years), but reduced activity of the putamen in OA associated with poorer proprioceptive performance. Behaviorally, age-related changes in proprioception were assessed using an arm position matching (APM) task with occluded vision, showing that OA not only exhibited lower overall proprioceptive accuracy than YA, but also increased movement duration and frequency of speed peaks, both indicative of elevated uncertainty during task execution (Adamo et al., 2007; Herter et al., 2014). Hence, across the lifespan, proprioceptive deficits appear to accumulate gradually, with YA exhibiting highest accuracy, which decreases in middle aged adults (35–50 years), and is lowest in OA (Goble, 2010; Van de Winckel et al., 2017). Impaired proprioceptive processing is associated with an increased incidence of falls in OA (Lord et al., 1999) and inaccurate performance in reaching movements (Adamo et al., 2007). Therefore, proprioceptive deficits may result in detrimental consequences for daily activities, particularly in the late life phase. The clinical relevance of this problem is stressed by the fact that neurological diseases, such as stroke, often manifest at higher age, when a decline in proprioception additionally worsens symptoms and hampers sensorimotor recovery in the presence of such conditions. About 85% of acute stroke patients and approximately 50% of patients with stroke in the chronic phase suffer from motor deficits (Rathore et al., 2002; Carlsson et al., 2018), while up to 60% of patients also exhibit somatosensory and proprioceptive deficits (Connell et al., 2008; Carey and Matyas, 2011; Lima et al., 2015; Rand, 2018). Here, proprioceptive deficits are associated with motor deficits of the affected upper extremity (Rand, 2018).

Despite these clinical implications, it is yet unknown whether or how proprioception can be improved by means of interventional methods such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). On a physiological level it was shown that tDCS modulates the proprioceptive afferent system by changing the excitability of projections to propriospinal neurons (Bradnam et al., 2011; McCambridge et al., 2014). In this study, we targeted the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) as the primary cortical area to process proprioceptive information (Behrens et al., 2003; Findlater et al., 2016) in order to modulate behavior. While tDCS has been frequently applied over M1 to facilitate learning and cortical excitability (Buch et al., 2017), less is known about somatosensory tDCS and its potential to modulate the accuracy of sensory perception. Ragert et al. (2008) demonstrated an improvement of tactile spatial discrimination induced by anodal tDCS (a-tDCS) over S1 in YA. Similar findings were demonstrated with a different (bihemispheric) electrode montage over S1 (Fujimoto et al., 2014) and with vibrotactile discrimination (Labbe et al., 2016). Up to now, there is only one study investigating the effects of tDCS on somatosensation in OA (Zhou et al., 2018). In this study, vibratory thresholds at the foot sole were decreased during a-tDCS over S1 in OA, while no young control group was assessed. To our knowledge, there are no studies investigating the differential effects of tDCS on proprioception in YA and OA.

Various mechanisms contribute to changes in brain structure and function (Jagust, 2013) which could explain differential responses to tDCS in the two age groups. Apart from aging-related changes in excitation and inhibition rates (Cheng and Lin, 2013; Legon et al., 2016) or the reorganization of cortical representations/maps (Cabeza, 2002), changes in brain morphology (e.g., cortical thinning) might determine intensity and distribution of electrical fields (EF) and thereby the effect of tDCS (Mahdavi et al., 2018). Simulations of EF are gaining increasing importance when assessing individual or group-wise differences in response to tDCS. A recent study has established a direct connection between the tDCS-induced field strength in SM1 and modulations of brain connectivity and neurotransmitter concentrations in YA (Antonenko et al., 2019). However, individual simulations and their relationships to induced modulations on a behavioral level have not been compared between YA and OA so far.

In the present study, we investigated (1) possible effects of a-tDCS over S1 on proprioceptive accuracy in YA and OA and (2) the strength and distribution of the induced electrical fields (EF) in these groups based on modeling of individual brain structure. We hypothesized to elicit differential tDCS-induced behavioral changes as a function of age. Specifically, we expected that a-tDCS has only little to no measurable influence on performance in the YA group, since their proprioceptive accuracy may be physiologically optimized, resulting in a ceiling level performance. In OA, however, we expected lower baseline performance in proprioceptive accuracy and a more pronounced positive effect of a-tDCS, as compared to the YA group. Furthermore, we expected the individually modeled field strengths to be related to performance changes and to differ between groups, based on individual brain morphologies.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the in-house database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig. Health status was assessed by a medical examination prior to study enrolment. Persons were excluded from participation if they had contraindications for tDCS or any psychiatric or neurological disorders. In total, 45 participants were included and allocated to the experimental groups: “young adults” (YA, n = 21, 10 females) for ages 18–35 (mean age: 27.0 ± 2.4 years) and “old adults” (OA, n = 24, 12 females) for ages 60 to 80 years (mean age: 69.4 ± 4.9 years). Only right-handed healthy adults were included in this study, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). Sex was distributed equally between groups (χ2(1, 45) = 0.03, p = 0.87). The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig and written informed consent was obtained from all participants after extensive briefing prior to study enrolment and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental Design and Procedure

Age-dependent effects of a-tDCS on proprioceptive accuracy were tested using a double-blind, cross-over design with the factors age group (YA vs. OA) and stimulation condition (sham vs. a-tDCS). For familiarization, all participants underwent a training session prior to testing (Figure 1). Stimulation effects were assessed on two individual days separated by one week (mean pause = 7.2 ± 0.4 days) to avoid carry-over effects. The experimental procedures were identical on both days except for the applied tDCS type (sham vs. a-tDCS), the order of which was counterbalanced across participants. The experiment was carried out by two experimenters: one performed all measurements, interacted with the participants and was blinded for the stimulation type, the other assisted during electrode placement and operated and supervised the DC stimulator.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure. All participants underwent neurological examinations and task familiarization before study enrolment. Proprioceptive performance was assessed in two different sessions separated by at least 7 days. In each session, participants rated their levels of attention, wakefulness and pain on visual analog scales (VAS) before and after tDCS. All participants received either a-tDCS (15 min) or sham tDCS (30 s) concurrent to robotic measurements of proprioception by means of the arm position matching task (APM). In both conditions, stimulation commenced at the beginning of the calibration process for the robotic system. The order of stimulation was pseudo-randomized and balanced for all participants. Assessments of proprioceptive performance always started 9 min after stimulation onset.

Testing sessions began with a short questionnaire regarding tDCS safety and 10-point visual analog scales (VAS) for attention, wakefulness and pain. After tDCS preparations (described in the next section), the exoskeleton (described in section “Proprioceptive Assessment”) was mechanically adjusted to the participant and individual adjustment parameters were saved for the second test session. After tDCS was turned on, the robotic system was calibrated to account for individual limb-segment length and participants were again provided with the task instructions. The proprioceptive task started exactly 9 min after stimulation onset.

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation

The 10–20 system (Oostenveld and Praamstra, 2001) was used for electrode positioning. The anode (5 × 5 cm2) was placed over S1 (C3’, approximately at 10% of the nasion-inion distance posterior C3, according to Ragert et al., 2008; see Figure 2). The cathode (7 × 5 cm2) was placed over the contralateral orbit (Nitsche et al., 2008; Woods et al., 2016). After scalp preparation, electrodes were positioned using Ten20 conductive electrode paste (Weaver and Company, Aurora, CO, United States) and supported by a rubber band for good adhesion and low impedances, which were consistently held below 10 kΩ.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. tDCS montage and electric field simulation. (A) Electrode montage and simulated current flow. Stimulation electrodes are displayed in dark gray. The anode was positioned over the primary somatosensory cortex (S1, 5 × 5 cm2) based on the 10–20 system and the cathode (5 × 7 cm2, blue) was placed over the contralateral orbit. Yellow lines represent the main current trajectories obtained from simulation. (B) Simulated electric field distribution. The tDCS setup of our study was simulated on the MNI152 head template, allowing a descriptive evaluation of electrical field distribution and strength. The simulation indicates electric field maxima in the left primary motor (M1) and somatosensory (S1) cortices (darker red areas), as well as weaker electrical fields in the left frontal premotor cortex and superior and inferior parietal lobules (brighter red areas). The right prefrontal areas beneath the cathode also exhibit a stronger electrical field strength. Regions within the gray matter volume with an electrical field strength of 0.1 V/m or higher were defined as stimulation hot spots and highlighted in red. Electrodes are displayed in pale gray. A = anterior, I = inferior, L = Left, P = posterior, R = right, S = superior.

Stimulation setup and parameters were based on previously published protocols that reported facilitatory effects of a-tDCS on somatosensory function (Ragert et al., 2008) and were adjusted for the requirements of this experiment. In the verum condition (a-tDCS), 1 mA of direct current was delivered for 15 min. In the placebo condition (sham), 1 mA was applied for 30 s (Gandiga et al., 2006). In both conditions, current was faded in and out for 30 s each. Current was delivered by a battery-driven DC stimulator (Neuroconn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany).

To verify the chosen electrode setup for the stimulation of S1, we performed a simulation of the current distribution and current flow for the mentioned tDCS setup (Figure 2A) using SimNIBS 2.1 (Thielscher et al., 2015) and the included MNI head model. According to the simulation, the selected setup elicited a strong EF within the postcentral (S1) and precentral (M1) gyri and the inferior parietal lobule (Figure 2B), representing the area underneath the anode. Moreover, a second smaller field maximum could be identified at the contralateral prefrontal interhemispheric ridge, located underneath the cathode. Accordingly, the applied setup should have resulted in a unilateral stimulation of the primary sensorimotor cortices with a larger involvement of parietal brain regions.

Proprioceptive Assessment

Behavioral assessments were performed using the KINARM exoskeleton (BKIN Technologies Ltd., Kingston, ON, Canada; Figure 3), a robotic system recording elbow and shoulder movements at a spatial resolution in the millimeter range and at temporal resolution of 1000 Hz (Ball et al., 2009). To investigate proprioceptive accuracy, the APM task (Dukelow et al., 2010, 2012; Herter et al., 2014; Kenzie et al., 2014; Semrau et al., 2015a, b), measuring static joint position sense, was used. During a single trial (Figure 4A), the robot moves the participant’s right arm to one of nine possible positions (denoted targets, Figures 3, 4B) in the workspace. This movement follows a straight path and is passive for the participant (therefore the passive side). Participants are instructed to actively mirror-match the end position of the right arm displacement with their left arm, while visual perception of hand, elbow and shoulder is blocked. Accordingly, the left side is denoted active side. If participants consider their performance accurate, they notify the experimenter and the trial ends (Figure 4A). Trials are self-paced and participants are instructed to focus on accuracy only. A new trial starts with a passive movement from the current arm position to a different target. The complete APM assessment consists of 54 trials, with each of the nine targets (Figure 3) being approached six times in a random order.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Experimental setup (A: top view, B: side view). Participants are seated in the exoskeleton with their arms supported against gravity by two arm rests, allowing only planar movements. Targets are spaced 10 cm apart from each other and the central target, determining the overall positioning of the workspace, which is adjusted for each participant individually at 90° elbow flexion and 30° horizontal shoulder abduction (A). Visual information regarding upper extremity position are withheld through a closable mirror and an apron which is wrapped around the shoulders (B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Arm position matching task. (A) Timeline of a single trial. Upon trial start, the right arm is moved passively by the robot to one of nine possible targets (see also panel B), while vision is blocked. Participants are instructed to focus on the perception of the right arm position in order to move their left arm to mirror-match the position of the right arm as accurately as possible. Participants notify the experimenter when they subjectively reached the accurate position to end one trial and start the next one from that end position. (B) Workspace and accuracy measures. The workspace contains nine targets for the left (active side) and right (passive side) arm. Absolute errors (AE) are defined as the absolute spatial distance in (cm) between left and right hand positions. Elbow angle errors (EAE) represent the absolute angular differences in flexion angles between the right elbow angle (EAR, passive) and left elbow angle (EAL, active). Similarly, shoulder angle errors (SAE) are defined as the absolute angular differences in horizontal shoulder abduction between the right (SAR, passive) and left (SAL, active) shoulder joints.

Data Analysis

If not otherwise stated, all analyses were performed in SPSS 18 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). An alpha value of 5% was used for all inferential statistics.

Visual Analog Scales

10-point visual analog scales (VAS) for attention (1 not at all attentive – 10 very attentive), wakefulness (1 very tired – 10 very awake), and pain (1 no pain – 10 strong pain) were obtained before (pre) and after (post) stimulation. Pre-post changes for each VAS were calculated and then compared between stimulation conditions using paired-sample t-tests. All participants tolerated the stimulation well and there were no adverse events during experimental procedures. During stimulation, no significant pre-post changes occurred neither in attention (t(44) = –0.57, p = 0.57), nor in wakefulness (t(44) = 0.38, p = 0.71), nor in pain (t(44) = 1.43, p = 0.16). There were no significant differences (pre vs. post stimulation) between conditions in levels of attention (pre: t(44) = 0.00, p = 1.00; post: t(44) = 0.31, p = 0.76), wakefulness (pre: t(44) = 0.70, p = 0.95; post: t(44) = –0.86, p = 0.39), or pain (pre: t(44) = 1.35, p = 0.18; post: t(44) = 1.07, p = 0.29). There were no significant differences in pre or post VAS between age groups.

Blinding

Both participants and the primary experimenter were blinded regarding the applied stimulation condition and were only debriefed after the second testing session. To assess the efficiency of blinding, participants had to indicate whether or not they thought they had been stimulated after each session and to provide a measure of their certainty. Blinding efficiency was then assessed using the McNemar test, which revealed no significant differences in the reported perception of stimulation between a-tDCS and sham (x2 = 3.32, p = 0.19). Out of 45 participants, 48.9% perceived stimulation correctly in the a-tDCS and 42.2% in the sham condition. The mean reported certainty of the participants was 6.4 ± 2.6 (scale: 1 unsure – 10 absolutely certain) during a-tDCS and 6.1 ± 2.6 during sham. There was no difference between conditions regarding the certainty (t(44) = –0.82, p = 0.42). We conclude that the blinding approach was effective for two reasons: firstly, only about half of the participants correctly identified the applied protocol and secondly, the mean certainty was moderate – participants were either absolute sure or tended to select a rating around 5 to indicate that they are guessing.

Learning Effects

To assess potential learning from the first to the second session, irrespective of the applied stimulation, we compared the performance between the two sessions. None of the outcome measures (AE, Var, EAE, SAE) differed significantly between the first and second session. We therefore conclude that the behavioral results are specific to the applied a-tDCS.

Task Duration

The APM task was designed to measure static limb position sense and participants are instructed to focus on position matching accuracy only, effectively making trials self-paced. Although this approach should remove temporal aspects like a speed-accuracy-tradeoff, we also considered the total duration of task execution here. APM task durations did not differ between the two stimulation conditions (main effect: F(1, 43) = 3.50, p = 0.557) nor groups (main effect: F(1, 43) = 0.118, p = 0.73).

Proprioceptive Performance (Accuracy and Variability)

The performance in position matching was quantified by calculating the mean absolute difference between passive and active elbow and shoulder joint angles (elbow angle error [EAE] and shoulder angle error [SAE], respectively; Figure 4B) at the end of each trial (Adamo et al., 2007; Fuentes and Bastian, 2010). Both parameters are a measure of the participant’s perceived arm position. In addition, the standard deviation of both accuracy parameters across all trials (VEAE and VSAE, respectively) was calculated to assess the variability of performance. All four parameters were calculated for the a-tDCS and sham conditions using MATLAB R2017b (MathWorks, Inc., United States). Repeated measures analyses of variances (RM-ANOVAs) with stimulation condition (a-tDCS vs. sham) as within-participant factor and age group (YA vs. OA) as between-group factor was used. Post hoc paired t-tests were applied when necessary to compare means between samples, p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni method.

Individual EF Simulations

To assess the distribution and magnitude of the induced EF between participants, we conducted individual EF simulations using SimNIBS 2.0 (Thielscher et al., 2015). For 35 participants (16 YA, mean age: 27.12 ± 2.5 years; 19 OA, mean age: 70.2 ± 4.2 years), high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical brain scans were procured from the participants database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig. Scans were chosen to be in temporal proximity to the proprioceptive assessments (mean difference: 4.6 ± 8.3 months).

Individual volumetric head models were generated using the mri2mesh script included in SimNIBS. The final head models exhibited five compartments representing the scalp, skull, cerebrospinal-fluid (CSF), gray matter and white matter. Due to an inconsistent estimation of the skull thickness, we pursued an alternative approach uncoupled from mri2mesh for the segmentation of the scalp, skull and CSF for all subjects. A multi-atlas-based approach was employed which computed a head segmentation in a majority voting process (Kalloch et al., 2019), resulting in a more robust representation of the thickness of the skull and CSF. Aside from this change in the segmentation routines, any further processing was done utilizing the SimNIBS mri2mesh pipeline again.

After obtaining the head model, we geometrically defined and positioned the electrodes in the SimNIBS graphical user interface. The electrodes were modeled as 5 × 5 cm2 (anode) and 7 × 5 cm2 (cathode) patch electrodes with an additional gel layer and a power connector. The positions of the electrodes were determined using our own customized plugin for the 3D modeling software Blender (Blender Foundation, Blender Institute, Amsterdam1). By interactively marking fiducial points (nasion, inion and both tragi) on the scalp surface of the individual subject, a 10–20 coordinate grid was generated which allowed a reliable electrode positioning. The anode was positioned at 10% of the nasion-inion distance behind C3 [as described in Ragert et al. (2008)], the cathode at Fp2. Determined electrode coordinates were then used for electrode placement in SimNIBS. All current outlets were modeled at the posterior sides of the electrodes. For all electrode placements, head measurements obtained during the actual proprioceptive assessments were used to aid and validate the virtual placement. We used the standard isotropic conductivities for the compartments as predefined in SimNIBS:

σ s k i n =  0.465 s m , σ s k u l l =  0.01 s m ,
σ C S F =  1.654 s m , σ G M =  0.275 s m ,
σ W M =  0.126 s m , σ e l e c t r o d e =  1.0 s m .

Finally, the electrical field strength was calculated for the entire head volume.

For subsequent analyses and visualization, data were processed in ParaView 5.4 (Sandia National Laboratory, Kitware Inc., and Los Alamos National Laboratory). EF strength values were extracted from 3 predefined regions of interest (ROI) within S1, our main target area, as well as M1 and the contralateral frontal lobe, to serve as control areas on an individual basis. We defined the S1 and M1 masks anatomically informed and based on the Human Brainnetome Atlas (Fan et al., 2016) in MNI space. The S1 mask was created by the intersection of the postcentral gyrus mask of the atlas and a 3 cm diametric sphere around the upper limb coordinate in S1 (MNI coordinates: –40, –27, 54; Mayka et al., 2006). The M1 mask represents the intersection of the precentral gyrus mask of the atlas and a sphere of, again, 3 cm at the upper limb coordinate in M1 (MNI coordinates: –37, –25, 64; Mayka et al., 2006). To transform these two anatomically defined ROI masks from MNI space into the individual space of the subject MRI data, we spatially normalized the skull-stripped brain MRI data first by a linear registration using FSL FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002), followed by a non-linear registration in ANTs (Avants et al., 2011) to the MNI152 1 mm brain template. We then co-registered the ROI mask into the individual space by inverting the before determined normalization transformations. The third ROI was defined relative to the individual position of the frontal electrode of each subject. We projected the coordinate of the center of mass of the frontal electrode to the cortical gray matter compartment and used the resulting coordinate as the center of the 3 cm spherical frontal ROI. Since these operations were done in subject space, no transformation was necessary in this case. From each of the ROI masks in subject space, we extracted only the portion of the electrical field data of the head model that was located within both the respective ROI mask and the gray matter volume compartment per subject (Figure 6B) and computed its mean value. Additionally, we co-registered the field data of the entire head model of each subject into MNI space again using FLIRT and ANTs and the before determined normalization transformations to be able to visualize the EF pattern at the cortical surface on a group-level, as illustrated in Figure 6A.

The extracted values were compared between groups using an independent samples t-test. To assess a potential relationship between the induced EF and behavioral changes through tDCS, a change score was calculated for both mean outcome parameter using the formula %ΔEAE or SAE = 100 (EAE or SAE anodal – EAE or SAE sham)/EAE or SAE sham using either EAE or SAE values for computations, respectively (Ameli et al., 2009). Higher values indicate inferior performance due to an increase in error rate through a-tDCS. The change scores were then correlated to the field strength values in each group using Spearman’s linear correlation.

Results

Proprioceptive Performance (Accuracy and Variability)

Main Effect Stimulation Condition

Performance in matching accuracy (EAE: F(1, 43) = 0.476, p = 0.49, η2 = 0.01; SAE: F(1, 43) = 2.711, p = 0.11, η2 = 0.06) or variability (VEAE: F(1, 43) = 0.014, p = 0.91, η2 < 0.001; VSAE: F(1, 43) = 0.229, p = 0.64, η2 = 0.005) did not differ significantly between the two stimulation conditions across both groups.

Main Effect Age Group

The two groups did not differ significantly in matching accuracy (EAE: F(1, 43) = 2.89, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.06; SAE: F(1, 43) = 2.61, p = 0.11, η2 = 0.06) or variability (VEAE: F(1, 43) = 1.31, p = 0.26, η2 = 0.03; VSAE: F(1, 43) = 2.84, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.06).

Interaction Effect

A differential effect of a-tDCS on performance was demonstrated by a significant interaction between age group and stimulation condition for all four outcome parameters (EAE: F(1, 43) = 5.22, p = 0.027, η2 = 0.11; SAE: F(1, 43) = 5.07, p = 0.029, η2 = 0.11; VEAE: F(1, 43) = 6.77, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.14; VSAE: F(1, 43) = 6.79, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.14), with YA exhibiting slight reductions in EAE and SAE whereas OA showed marked increases through stimulation (see Figure 5 for individual modulations and group means). Post hoc t-tests revealed that during sham, both age groups performed on a similar level (mean EAEYA = 6.68 ± 3.04°, mean EAEOA = 7.09 ± 3.68°, t(43) = 0.41, p = 0.87; mean SAEYA = 6.45 ± 2.67°, mean SAEOA = 6.68 ± 2.64°, t(43) = 0.289, p = 0.774). During stimulation, age groups diverged significantly in performance (EAE: t(36.8) = 2.687, p = 0.011; SAE: t(43) = 2.498, p = 0.016): while YA slightly improved under a-tDCS (mean EAEYA = 5.91 ± 2.27°, mean SAEYA = 6.21 ± 2.24°), the performance of the OA decreased (mean EAEOA = 8.52 ± 4.09°, mean SAEOA = 8.23 ± 3.05°).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Performance changes through a-tDCS. No significant main effect for the factors stimulation condition (sham vs. a-tDCS) and age group (young adults vs. older adults) was identified. For all four outcome measures, there was a significant interaction between age group and stimulation condition, indicating differential effects of a-tDCS in young and old adults. Gray lines represent individual change trajectories for EAE and SAE in each group, thicker lines represent group means (error bars provided as ±1 standard error of the mean).

Individual EF Simulations

The simulation of individual EF indicated significantly stronger EF strengths in the primary target region S1 in the YA group when compared to the OA group (t(33) = –3.80, p = 0.001, two-sample t-test, Figure 6B). The general pattern of EF distributions across participants is similar when compared between groups, however, the YA group showed markedly higher field strengths and a wider distribution of elevated EF values in both primary sensorimotor (S1 ROI mean: YA = 0.097 ± 0.025 V/m, OA = 0.074 ± 0.08 V/m, M1 ROI mean: YA = 0.097 ± 0.024 V/m, OA = 0.074 ± 0.008 V/m) and frontal regions (Frontal ROI mean: YA = 0.096 ± 0.023 V/m, OA = 0.077 ± 0.012 V/m) of the cortex (Figure 6A). In comparison, the OA group also showed field strength maxima in S1 and M1, albeit less intense, but only low EF strengths were detected in supplementary motor area, premotor cortex and parietal areas. Likewise, the frontal field appeared to focus along the frontopolar interhemispheric ridge and shared the general pattern as in the YA group, but showed otherwise less intense field strengths within the distribution area. In both age groups, no significant correlation between the induced EF strength and the behavioral change scores could be established at a corrected p-level < 0.05.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Individual electrical field simulations. (A) Whole-brain electrical field distribution. While the general pattern of electrical field distributions is similar in both groups, the difference map suggests that electrical fields at the cortical surface are stronger in young (YA; displayed in red) as compared to the old adults (OA; displayed in blue) group. (B) Electrical field strength in target region S1. A cortical gray matter ROI mask (marked in blue, left panel) of the hand and elbow region of S1 was used to compare field strength magnitudes between groups. A significant difference in electrical field strengths could be identified, with YA receiving higher field values in the ROI mask. A = anterior, I = inferior, L = Left, P = posterior, R = right, S = superior.

Discussion

We investigated the effect of a-tDCS over S1 on proprioceptive performance in YA and OA. We showed that tDCS differentially modulates task performance in an age-dependent manner: while at baseline there was no statistical difference between the two age groups regarding their proprioceptive matching accuracy, a-tDCS induced a slight performance increase in the YA group and a more prominent performance decrease in the OA group, leading to a significant difference in performance under stimulation. Additionally, the same stimulation setup induced different EF in the two groups, with YA receiving significantly higher EF strengths in S1 and in control areas. However, a direct linear relationship between induced field strength and the behavioral modulation could not be identified in our sample. Our findings highlight the impact of aging on the modulatory influence of tDCS on proprioceptive performance. Future studies are needed to elucidate age-related neuroanatomical, neurophysiological and cognitive-behavioral factors that might underlie these differential results.

Even during healthy aging individuals often undergo a decline in perceptual, motor and cognitive functions. A better knowledge of underlying mechanisms and new ways to prevent or even restore this functional decline is a major goal of current aging research. We here focused on proprioceptive function, since this modality is a prerequisite for successful motor control. Age-related decline of proprioception has been shown to impair locomotor function (Berard et al., 2012), balance (Maki and McIlroy, 2006; Kaminski et al., 2013) and goal-directed movements of the upper limbs (Sarlegna, 2006). In addition, proprioceptive deficits significantly hamper the recovery process of deficient motor function in the presence of neurological diseases.

Despite the clinical significance, it remains a challenge to assess proprioceptive performance in a sensitive and specific way. Available tests include ipsilateral and contralateral matching, that require active motion, as well as psychophysical threshold methods of purely passive movements. These methods represent measures, that, despite, their target modality proprioception, also inherently cover other aspects of neural processing. For example, ipsilateral and contralateral matching methods both require active motion and consequently are influenced by additional sensorimotor processes (Elangovan et al., 2014). Furthermore, ipsilateral matching and psychophysical threshold hunting require a higher demand on working memory, which has been found to influence the results especially in elderly populations (Goble et al., 2012b). Since the target of this study was to investigate age-related processes, we used a robotic APM task which assesses proprioceptive performance by matching interlimb accuracy with low working memory demand. This task has been used extensively in previous studies and has been shown to be sensitive to proprioceptive deficits in both aging and clinical populations (Dukelow et al., 2010; Herter et al., 2014). In the present sample, however, a significant difference in matching performance could not be established, in contrast to both the hypothesis of this study and previous findings (Stelmach and Sirica, 1986; Adamo et al., 2007; Herter et al., 2014). The reason to this could be that the sample size used in the current study was not large enough to establish age-related performance differences (in the absence of tDCS). Furthermore, differences regarding the influence of aging on proprioceptive accuracy across studies are likely reflecting the diversity of experimental designs. For instance, previous studies have assessed proprioception with other tasks and outcome measures that are likely more rater-dependent and cognitively demanding, as discussed above, than the robotic approach presented here (e.g., joint angle error vs. location of the hand; Herter et al., 2014), which could have resulted in more apparent group differences. Furthermore, such differences are also more prominent and thus easier detectable in the lower limb (Proske and Gandevia, 2012; Henry and Baudry, 2019), but the upper limb was assessed in the present study. For instance, age-related difference in the elbow matching study by Adamo et al. (2007) were on average below 2°.

In contrast to previous publications and our hypothesis (Ragert et al., 2008; Labbe et al., 2016; Summers et al., 2016), we did not show an overall beneficial effect of a-tDCS. Notably, these other studies used different experimental designs, including other assessments of somatosensory perception, and slightly different tDCS protocols, impeding a direct comparison between results. However, our findings do provide evidence that individual factors such as age might crucially influence both strength and directionality of tDCS effects (Heise et al., 2014; Craig and Doumas, 2017). In OA, findings of tDCS-induced modulations are not consistent across studies: while some report beneficial effects on behavioral performance (Learmonth et al., 2015; von Rein et al., 2015), others show negative responses to a-tDCS in learning tasks, such as dynamic balance learning (Kaminski et al., 2017). Our findings extend the notion of missing efficacy in OA and suggest that a-tDCS can actually impair function. Taken together, the healthy aging brain might respond fundamentally different to a-tDCS on a behavioral level.

While the underlying mechanisms are not yet understood, our result is interesting in the context of a recent study that investigated effects of tDCS on short intracortical inhibition, a measure that is considered to reflect GABAergic inhibition (Heise et al., 2014). While a-tDCS induced a release of inhibition in YA, an increase in inhibition was observed in OA. While such opposing effects were not demonstrated on a behavioral level in the same study, this result raises the notion that a-tDCS effects on the balance of excitation/inhibition in the cortex might be essentially different in YA vs. OA. Indeed, the “baseline” balance of excitation and inhibition is different between the two groups (Cabeza, 2002; Luebke et al., 2004) and could be one explanation of differential tDCS-induced effects between age cohorts. Another study used TMS to investigate temporal dynamics of responses after a-tDCS and showed that there is a delayed response on cortical excitability in OA (Fujiyama et al., 2014). Hence, both timing and direction of tDCS-induced neuroplastic changes vary with respect to age, a notion that might have contributed to the effects observed in our study.

Despite such local electrophysiological differences of tDCS effects, a complementary explanation could be that different brain networks are engaged in YA vs. OA while performing the same task (Cabeza, 2002; Zimerman et al., 2014). Therefore, the response to targeted stimulation of specific cortical areas might essentially vary as a function of age. For example, it is a well-described phenomenon that OA activate larger brain networks and recruit additional contra- and ipsilateral brain areas in order to perform a motor task similarly to YA (Ward, 2003; Seidler et al., 2010) and that such reorganizations are highly individual (Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). While such evidence in the somatosensory domain is scarcer, one study showed activity increases in contralateral S1 and decreases in S2 and the cingular cortex during tactile stimulation in OA which correlated negatively with perceptual performance (Lenz et al., 2012; Brodoehl et al., 2013). In the context of our results, it is tempting to speculate that a stimulation of overactivated hubs in elderly might have interfered with the respective network and in consequence decreased performance.

A general limitation inherent to tDCS is the low spatial resolution. Hence, other areas than the primary target area might be responsible for the induced effects (Prehn and Floel, 2015). An alternative explanation of our findings might relate to the electrode setup. While the anode was attached over the left S1, the “reference” cathodal electrode was placed over the supraorbital region of the forehead, which represents the current standard for a-tDCS setups (Nitsche et al., 2008; Buttkus et al., 2011). It cannot be ruled out that also the cathode influenced cortical activity in the frontopolar cortex. To prevent this problem, we used an enlarged reference electrode to decrease current density and thereby render stimulation inefficient (Nitsche et al., 2008). However, the EF simulation showed high field strengths in the frontal pole and interhemispheric ridge. In the frontal ROI, which was selected as the focus point of the cathode here, no group-specific difference could be identified that could explain the detrimental behavioral effect in the OA group. In the present study, we chose a unilateral setup based on previously published protocols for S1 and M1 stimulation, as we sought to stimulate left S1 and investigate the modulatory effects of a-tDCS on proprioception in this area. However, our simulation showed a non-focal distribution of the induced EF in the whole sensorimotor system (see also next paragraph). The application of a more focused stimulation protocol, i.e., a high-definition or multi-electrode approach with a small anode and a Laplacian array of small return electrodes (e.g., Villamar et al., 2013), could have delivered stronger current loads to S1, thereby exerting stronger or different modulatory effects on proprioceptive accuracy, as highlighted in previous research investigating the effects of high-definition tDCS over M1 (Kuo et al., 2013). A possible mechanism of action that drives differential effects between the two approaches might be a selective interaction with center-surround inhibition in the sensorimotor system (Beck and Hallett, 2011).

With respect to the question of stimulation focality, we employed modeling of our electrode setup using high-resolution MRI-based head models to obtain EF maps. With this simulation approach, we showed that S1 was among the regions with the highest electric field strengths, supporting that with our stimulation setup the target brain structure S1 was reached. Furthermore, we showed differences in EF strengths between age cohorts: YA experience stronger EF strengths than OA both in S1 and in the other ROIs (M1 and frontal cortex). Since this was found in all areas tested, it might represent a general difference between YA and OA that needs to be considered when tDCS is applied to elderly cohorts. In line with recent findings (Im et al., 2018; Mahdavi et al., 2018; Thomas et al., 2018), this difference could be caused by age-related anatomical changes such as cortical thinning or atrophy, both inducing higher intracranial CSF volumes and, as a consequence, stronger current shunting. Moreover, skull thickness changes during healthy aging (Lillie et al., 2016) could further affect the induced EF. Indeed, it is plausible that the differential effect observed in the two age groups is reflective of a current-dosage-dependency, based on aforementioned age-related structural changes of the brain. If OA indeed receive weaker EF strengths overall, increasing the induced EF either by applying stronger current (e.g., 2 mA) or by increasing the current density under the anode (see also next paragraph) could potentially reverse the observed effect pattern. To test this in detail, a dose titration protocol could be applied, as commonly used in other areas of medicine and pharmacotherapy (e.g., Turnheim, 2003). However, the present study was designed to assess potential differential effects of a defined dose of a-tDCS in two age groups. It was not designed to investigate the differential effects of incremental a-tDCS dosages. The fact that no direct relationship between field strength and behavioral modulation was found might be explained by (i) latent age-related parameters other than the induced EF that mediate the relation between the variables tested in our study and (ii) the possibility, that other brain areas then the targeted S1 significantly contributed to the effects.

Future studies should also investigate the efficacy of other tDCS setups to modulate proprioception across the life span, as they might differentially interact with neurophysiological properties. For instance, although generally believed to have a dampening effect on cortical excitability (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000), cathodal tDCS has been shown to enhance motion perception in a visual tracking task (Antal et al., 2004), possibly interacting with a reduced signal-to-noise ratio in neural processing in the OA group (Hämmerer et al., 2013). Future studies should also investigate the effect of a bilateral S1 electrode setup (Fujimoto et al., 2014) specifically in OA, since it might interfere stronger with age-related interhemispheric activation changes (Mooney et al., 2018; Strauss et al., 2019). It is also possible that afferent sensory functions are modulated in a different way by means of classical tDCS protocols as compared to efferent motor functions. Especially when considering the folded architecture of the human cortex, hand and elbow areas in M1 and S1 are located at different positions on the surface of the precentral gyrus and the anterior ventral regions of the postcentral gyrus, respectively (Mayka et al., 2006), and are therefore differently oriented in relation to EF applied through a-tDCS (Fox et al., 2004). The peculiar relationships between cortical column orientation, induced EF and behavioral modulations need to be explored further.

Taken together, the results of the present study demonstrate for the first time that aging is a critical modulator of tDCS-induced changes in proprioceptive accuracy. The results of this study are important for future studies designed to develop tDCS as a therapeutic tool to enhance sensorimotor functions in OA or in patients with neurological diseases.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TM: conception and design of the work, data collection and procurement, data analysis and interpretation (all data), drafting the article, critical revision of the article. FK: conception and design of the work, data collection, data analysis and interpretation (behavioral data), drafting the article, critical revision of the article. P-CS: data collection, data analysis and interpretation (behavioral data), critical revision of the article. BK: data analysis and interpretation (simulations), drafting the article, critical revision of the article. SS: data analysis and interpretation (simulations), critical revision of the article. AV: conception and design of the work, supervision, drafting the article, critical revision of the article. BS: conception and design of the work, supervision, data interpretation (all data), drafting the article, critical revision of the article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Moreover, all authors agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

ANOVA, analysis of variances; APM, arm position matching task; a-tDCS, anodal transcranial direct current stimulation; EAE, elbow angle error; EF, electrical field; M1, primary motor cortex; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; OA, old adult group; ROI, region of interest; S1, primary somatosensory cortex; SAE, shoulder angle error; SD, standard deviation; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; VAS, visual analog scale; VEAE, variability (standard deviation) of the elbow angle error; VSAE, variability (standard deviation) of the shoulder angle error; YA, young adult group.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.blender.org

References

Adamo, D. E., Martin, B. J., and Brown, S. H. (2007). Age-related differences in upper limb proprioceptive acuity. Percept. Mot. Skills 104(3 Pt 2), 1297–1309. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.4.1297-1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ameli, M., Grefkes, C., Kemper, F., Riegg, F. P., Rehme, A. K., Karbe, H., et al. (2009). Differential effects of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over ipsilesional primary motor cortex in cortical and subcortical middle cerebral artery stroke. Ann. Neurol. 66, 298–309. doi: 10.1002/ana.21725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kruse, W., Kincses, T. Z., Hoffmann, K. P., and Paulus, W. (2004). Direct current stimulation over V5 enhances visuomotor coordination by improving motion perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 521–527. doi: 10.1162/089892904323057263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Thielscher, A., Saturnino, G. B., Aydin, S., Ittermann, B., Grittner, U., et al. (2019). Towards precise brain stimulation: is electric field simulation related to neuromodulation? Brain Stimul. 12, 1159–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.03.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armenta Salas, M., Bashford, L., Kellis, S., Jafari, M., Jo, H., Kramer, D., et al. (2018). Proprioceptive and cutaneous sensations in humans elicited by intracortical microstimulation. eLife 7:e32904. doi: 10.7554/eLife.32904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., and Gee, J. C. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, S. J., Brown, I. E., and Scott, S. H. (2009). Performance evaluation of a Planar 3DOF robotic exoskeleton for motor assessment. J. Med. Devices 3:021002. doi: 10.1115/1.3131727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, S., and Hallett, M. (2011). Surround inhibition in the motor system. Exp. Brain Res. 210, 165–172. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2610-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., Woolrich, M. W., Smith, S. M., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., Boulby, P. A., et al. (2003). Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 750–757. doi: 10.1038/nn1075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berard, J., Fung, J., and Lamontagne, A. (2012). Impact of aging on visual reweighting during locomotion. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1422–1428. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.11.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradnam, L. V., Stinear, C. M., and Byblow, W. D. (2011). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation suppresses ipsilateral projections to presumed propriospinal neurons of the proximal upper limb. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2582–2589. doi: 10.1152/jn.01084.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodoehl, S., Klingner, C., Stieglitz, K., and Witte, O. W. (2013). Age-related changes in the somatosensory processing of tactile stimulation–an fMRI study. Behav. Brain Res. 238, 259–264. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buch, E. R., Santarnecchi, E., Antal, A., Born, J., Celnik, P. A., Classen, J., et al. (2017). Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: a consensus and critical position paper. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buttkus, F., Baur, V., Jabusch, H. C., de la Cruz Gomez-Pellin, M., Paulus, W., Nitsche, M. A., et al. (2011). Single-session tDCS-supported retraining does not improve fine motor control in musician’s dystonia. Restor Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 85–90. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol. Aging 17, 85–100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M., and Matyas, T. A. (2011). Frequency of discriminative sensory loss in the hand after stroke in a rehabilitation setting. J. Rehabil. Med. 43, 257–263.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carlsson, H., Ekstrand, E., and Brogardh, C. (2018). Sensory function, measured as active discriminative touch, is associated with dexterity after stroke. PM R 11, 821–827. doi: 10.1002/pmrj.12044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C. H., and Lin, Y. Y. (2013). Aging-related decline in somatosensory inhibition of the human cerebral cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 226, 145–152. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3420-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connell, L. A., Lincoln, N. B., and Radford, K. A. (2008). Somatosensory impairment after stroke: frequency of different deficits and their recovery. Clin. Rehabil. 22, 758–767. doi: 10.1177/0269215508090674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, C. E., and Doumas, M. (2017). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation shows minimal, measure-specific effects on dynamic postural control in young and older adults: a double blind, sham-controlled study. PLoS One 12:e0170331. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dukelow, S. P., Herter, T. M., Bagg, S. D., and Scott, S. H. (2012). The independence of deficits in position sense and visually guided reaching following stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 9:72. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-9-72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dukelow, S. P., Herter, T. M., Moore, K. D., Demers, M. J., Glasgow, J. I., Bagg, S. D., et al. (2010). Quantitative assessment of limb position sense following stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 178–187. doi: 10.1177/1545968309345267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elangovan, N., Herrmann, A., and Konczak, J. (2014). Assessing proprioceptive function: evaluating joint position matching methods against psychophysical thresholds. Phys. Ther. 94, 553–561. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, L., Li, H., Zhuo, J., Zhang, Y., Wang, J., Chen, L., et al. (2016). The human brainnetome atlas: a new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3508–3526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlater, S. E., Desai, J. A., Semrau, J. A., Kenzie, J. M., Rorden, C., Herter, T. M., et al. (2016). Central perception of position sense involves a distributed neural network - evidence from lesion-behavior analyses. Cortex 79, 42–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, P. T., Narayana, S., Tandon, N., Sandoval, H., Fox, S. P., Kochunov, P., et al. (2004). Column-based model of electric field excitation of cerebral cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 22, 1–14. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, C. T., and Bastian, A. J. (2010). Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 164–171. doi: 10.1152/jn.00494.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujimoto, S., Yamaguchi, T., Otaka, Y., Kondo, K., and Tanaka, S. (2014). Dual-hemisphere transcranial direct current stimulation improves performance in a tactile spatial discrimination task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1669–1674. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujiyama, H., Hyde, J., Hinder, M. R., Kim, S. J., McCormack, G. H., Vickers, J. C., et al. (2014). Delayed plastic responses to anodal tDCS in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:115. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goble, D. J. (2010). Proprioceptive acuity assessment via joint position matching: from basic science to general practice. Phys. Ther. 90, 1176–1184. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goble, D. J., Coxon, J. P., Van Impe, A., Geurts, M., Van Hecke, W., Sunaert, S., et al. (2012a). The neural basis of central proprioceptive processing in older versus younger adults: an important sensory role for right putamen. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 895–908. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goble, D. J., Mousigian, M. A., and Brown, S. H. (2012b). Compromised encoding of proprioceptively determined joint angles in older adults: the role of working memory and attentional load. Exp. Brain Res. 216, 35–40. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2904-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14(1 Pt 1), 21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goossens, N., Janssens, L., Caeyenberghs, K., Albouy, G., and Brumagne, S. (2019). Differences in brain processing of proprioception related to postural control in patients with recurrent non-specific low back pain and healthy controls. Neuroimage Clin. 23:101881. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämmerer, D., Li, S. C., Völkle, M., Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2013). A lifespan comparison of the reliability, test-retest stability, and signal-to-noise ratio of event-related potentials assessed during performance monitoring. Psychophysiology 50, 111–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01476.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heise, K. F., Niehoff, M., Feldheim, J. F., Liuzzi, G., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F. C. (2014). Differential behavioral and physiological effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy adults of younger and older age. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:146. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, M., and Baudry, S. (2019). Age-related changes in leg proprioception: implications for postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 122, 525–538. doi: 10.1152/jn.00067.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herter, T. M., Scott, S. H., and Dukelow, S. P. (2014). Systematic changes in position sense accompany normal aging across adulthood. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11:43. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C. M. L., Tommasino, P., Budhota, A., and Campolo, D. (2015). Upper extremity proprioception in healthy aging and stroke populations, and the effects of therapist- and robot-based rehabilitation therapies on proprioceptive function. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Im, C., Seo, H., and Jun, S. C. (2018). Stimulation effect of inter-subject variability in tDCS-Multi-scale modeling study. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2018, 3092–3095. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2018.8513056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagust, W. (2013). Vulnerable neural systems and the borderland of brain aging and neurodegeneration. Neuron 77, 219–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalloch, B., Bode, J., Kozlov, M., Pampel, A., Hlawitschka, M., Sehm, B., et al. (2019). Semi-automated generation of individual computational models of the human head and torso from MR images. Magn. Reson. Med. 81, 2090–2105. doi: 10.1002/mrm.27508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaminski, E., Hoff, M., Rjosk, V., Steele, C. J., Gundlach, C., Sehm, B., et al. (2017). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation does not facilitate dynamic balance task learning in healthy old adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:16. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaminski, E., Hoff, M., Sehm, B., Taubert, M., Conde, V., Steele, C. J., et al. (2013). Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) during complex whole body motor skill learning. Neurosci. Lett. 552, 76–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.07.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kararizou, E., Manta, P., Kalfakis, N., and Vassilopoulos, D. (2005). Morphometric study of the human muscle spindle. Anal. Quant. Cytol. Histol. 27, 1–4.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kenzie, J. M., Semrau, J. A., Findlater, S. E., Herter, T. M., Hill, M. D., Scott, S. H., et al. (2014). Anatomical correlates of proprioceptive impairments following acute stroke: a case series. J. Neurol. Sci. 342, 52–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, G. H., Suzuki, S., and Kanda, K. (2007). Age-related physiological and morphological changes of muscle spindles in rats. J. Physiol. 582(Pt 2), 525–538. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.130120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, H. I., Bikson, M., Datta, A., Minhas, P., Paulus, W., Kuo, M. F., et al. (2013). Comparing cortical plasticity induced by conventional and high-definition 4 x 1 ring tDCS: a neurophysiological study. Brain Stimul. 6, 644–648. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labbe, S., Meftah, el-M., and Chapman, C. E. (2016). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of primary somatosensory cortex on vibrotactile detection and discrimination. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1978–1987. doi: 10.1152/jn.00506.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Learmonth, G., Thut, G., Benwell, C. S., and Harvey, M. (2015). The implications of state-dependent tDCS effects in aging: behavioural response is determined by baseline performance. Neuropsychologia 74, 108–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legon, W., Punzell, S., Dowlati, E., Adams, S. E., Stiles, A. B., and Moran, R. J. (2016). Altered prefrontal excitation/inhibition balance and prefrontal output: markers of aging in human memory networks. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4315–4326. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, M., Tegenthoff, M., Kohlhaas, K., Stude, P., Hoffken, O., Gatica Tossi, M. A., et al. (2012). Increased excitability of somatosensory cortex in aged humans is associated with impaired tactile acuity. J. Neurosci. 32, 1811–1816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2722-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lillie, E. M., Urban, J. E., Lynch, S. K., Weaver, A. A., and Stitzel, J. D. (2016). Evaluation of skull cortical thickness changes with age and sex from computed tomography scans. J. Bone Miner. Res. 31, 299–307. doi: 10.1002/jbmr.2613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, N. M., Menegatti, K. C., Yu, E., Sacomoto, N. Y., Scalha, T. B., Lima, I. N., et al. (2015). Sensory deficits in ipsilesional upper-extremity in chronic stroke patients. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 73, 834–839. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20150128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S. R., Rogers, M. W., Howland, A., and Fitzpatrick, R. (1999). Lateral stability, sensorimotor function and falls in older people. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 1077–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb05230.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luebke, J. I., Chang, Y. M., Moore, T. L., and Rosene, D. L. (2004). Normal aging results in decreased synaptic excitation and increased synaptic inhibition of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 125, 277–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.01.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahdavi, S., and Towhidkhah, F., and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, (2018). Computational human head models of tDCS: influence of brain atrophy on current density distribution. Brain Stimul. 11, 104–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maki, B. E., and McIlroy, W. E. (2006). Control of rapid limb movements for balance recovery: age-related changes and implications for fall prevention. Age Ageing 35(Suppl. 2), ii12–ii18. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayka, M. A., Corcos, D. M., Leurgans, S. E., and Vaillancourt, D. E. (2006). Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 31, 1453–1474. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCambridge, A. B., Stinear, J. W., and Byblow, W. D. (2014). A dissociation between propriospinal facilitation and inhibition after bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 2187–2195. doi: 10.1152/jn.00879.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mooney, R. A., Cirillo, J., and Byblow, W. D. (2018). Adaptive threshold hunting reveals differences in interhemispheric inhibition between young and older adults. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2247–2258. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527(Pt 3), 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., and Praamstra, P. (2001). The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 713–719. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00527-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prehn, K., and Floel, A. (2015). Potentials and limits to enhance cognitive functions in healthy and pathological aging by tDCS. Front. Cell Neurosci. 9:355. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proske, U., and Gandevia, S. C. (2012). The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol. Rev. 92, 1651–1697. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ragert, P., Vandermeeren, Y., Camus, M., and Cohen, L. G. (2008). Improvement of spatial tactile acuity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 805–811. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rand, D. (2018). Proprioception deficits in chronic stroke-Upper extremity function and daily living. PLoS One 13:e0195043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rathore, S. S., Hinn, A. R., Cooper, L. S., Tyroler, H. A., and Rosamond, W. D. (2002). Characterization of incident stroke signs and symptoms: findings from the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Stroke 33, 2718–2721. doi: 10.1161/01.str.0000035286.87503.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 355–370. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarlegna, F. R. (2006). Impairment of online control of reaching movements with aging: a double-step study. Neurosci. Lett. 403, 309–314. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrau, J. A., Herter, T. M., Kiss, Z. H., and Dukelow, S. P. (2015a). Disruption in proprioception from long-term thalamic deep brain stimulation: a pilot study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:244. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrau, J. A., Herter, T. M., Scott, S. H., and Dukelow, S. P. (2015b). Examining differences in patterns of sensory and motor recovery after stroke with robotics. Stroke 46, 3459–3469. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.010750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stelmach, G. E., and Sirica, A. (1986). Aging and proprioception. Age 9, 99–103. doi: 10.1007/bf02432281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, S., Lotze, M., Floel, A., Domin, M., and Grothe, M. (2019). Changes in interhemispheric motor connectivity across the lifespan: a combined TMS and DTI study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:12. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, J. J., Kang, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2016). Does transcranial direct current stimulation enhance cognitive and motor functions in the ageing brain? A systematic review and meta- analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 25, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thielscher, A., Antunes, A., and Saturnino, G. B. (2015). Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: a useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2015, 222–225. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7318340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C., Datta, A., and Woods, A. (2018). Effect of aging on cortical current flow due to transcranial direct current stimulation: considerations for safety. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2018, 3084–3087.

Google Scholar

Turnheim, K. (2003). When drug therapy gets old: pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics in the elderly. Exp. Gerontol. 38, 843–853. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5565(03)00133-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Winckel, A., Tseng, Y. T., Chantigian, D., Lorant, K., Zarandi, Z., Buchanan, J., et al. (2017). Age-Related decline of wrist position sense and its relationship to specific physical training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:570. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villamar, M. F., Volz, M. S., Bikson, M., Datta, A., Dasilva, A. F., and Fregni, F. (2013). Technique and considerations in the use of 4x1 ring high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS). J. Vis. Exp. 77:e50309. doi: 10.3791/50309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Rein, E., Hoff, M., Kaminski, E., Sehm, B., Steele, C. J., Villringer, A., et al. (2015). Improving motor performance without training: the effect of combining mirror visual feedback with transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 2383–2389. doi: 10.1152/jn.00832.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, N. S. (2003). Age-related changes in the neural correlates of motor performance. Brain 126, 873–888. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., et al. (2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Lo, O. Y., Lipsitz, L. A., Zhang, J., Fang, J., and Manor, B. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances foot sole somatosensation when standing in older adults. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 795–802. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5178-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimerman, M., Heise, K. F., Gerloff, C., Cohen, L. G., and Hummel, F. C. (2014). Disrupting the ipsilateral motor cortex interferes with training of a complex motor task in older adults. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1030–1036. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tDCS, aging, proprioception, position sense, robotics, electrical field simulation

Citation: Muffel T, Kirsch F, Shih P-C, Kalloch B, Schaumberg S, Villringer A and Sehm B (2019) Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over S1 Differentially Modulates Proprioceptive Accuracy in Young and Old Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:264. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00264

Received: 10 July 2019; Accepted: 06 September 2019;
Published: 26 September 2019.

Edited by:

Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Walter Paulus, University of Göttingen, Germany
Tino Zaehle, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Muffel, Kirsch, Shih, Kalloch, Schaumberg, Villringer and Sehm. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Toni Muffel, muffel@cbs.mpg.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Download