Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 05 November 2020
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 12 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2020.593833

Contributions of Hippocampal Volume to Cognition in Healthy Older Adults

  • 1Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, College of Public Health and Health Professions, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 2Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 3Department of Neuroscience, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 4Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, School of Mind, Brain and Behavior, College of Science, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 6Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 7Department of Neurology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 9Department of Biostatistics, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 10Neuroscience Graduate Interdisciplinary Program, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 11Physiological Sciences Graduate Interdisciplinary Program, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 12Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium (AAC), Phoenix, AZ, United States

Objective: The association between hippocampal volume and memory is continuing to be characterized in healthy older adults. Prior research suggests smaller hippocampal volume in healthy older adults is associated with poorer episodic memory and processing speed, as well as working memory, verbal learning, and executive functioning as measured by the NIH Toolbox Fluid (Fluid Cognition Composite, FCC) and Crystalized Cognition Composites (CCC). This study aimed to replicate these findings and to evaluate the association between: (1) hippocampal asymmetry index and cognition; and (2) independent contributions of the left and right hippocampal volume and cognition in a large sample of healthy older adults.

Participants and Methods: One-hundred and eighty-three healthy older adults (M age = 71.72, SD = 5.3) received a T1-weighted sequence on a 3T scanner. Hippocampal subfields were extracted using FreeSurfer 6.0 and combined to provide left, right, and total hippocampal volumes. FCC subtests include Dimensional Change Card Sort, Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention, List Sorting, Picture Sequence Memory, and Pattern Comparison. CCC subtests include Picture Vocabulary and Oral Reading Recognition. Multiple linear regressions were performed predicting cognition composites from the total, left and right, and asymmetry of hippocampal volume, controlling for sex, education, scanner, and total intracranial volume. Multiple comparisons in primary analyses were corrected using a false discovery rate (FDR) of p < 0.05.

Results: FCC scores were positively associated with total (β = 0.226, FDR q = 0.044) and left (β = 0.257, FDR q = 0.024) hippocampal volume. Within FCC, Picture Sequence Memory scores positively associated with total (β = 0.284, p = 0.001) and left (β = 0.98, p = 0.001) hippocampal volume. List Sorting scores were also positively associated with left hippocampal volume (β = 0.189, p = 0.029).

Conclusions: These results confirm previous research suggesting that bilateral hippocampal volume is associated with FCC, namely episodic memory. The present study also suggests the left hippocampal volume may be more broadly associated with both episodic and working memory. Studies should continue to investigate lateralized hippocampal contributions to aging processes to better identify predictors of cognitive decline.

Introduction

The hippocampus is a bilateral medial temporal lobe structure known for its important role in episodic learning and memory function, or the ability to remember ongoing experiences (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997; Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998). Hippocampal volume has been associated with domains of cognition such as processing speed, working memory, spatial navigation, and abstract reasoning (Reuben et al., 2011; Lövdén et al., 2012; O’Shea et al., 2016; Gorbach et al., 2017). These cognitive domains are particularly vulnerable to a decline in non-pathological aging (Salthouse, 2009), and therefore have been further studied in the context of hippocampal volume reduction in cognitive aging.

O’Shea et al. (2016) examined the relationship between cognitive functioning and hippocampal volume using NIH Toolbox’s fluid and crystalized cognition composite (CCC) scores in a sample of 93 older adults. The authors found smaller bilateral hippocampal volume was associated with poorer performance on fluid cognitive functioning. The greatest magnitude of association was with episodic memory and processing speed, but also included working memory, verbal learning, and executive functions. Findings from this study were consistent with prior research supporting a clear role of the hippocampus in episodic memory and also suggest hippocampal volume may play a broader role in cognitive aging other than memory, such as processing speed.

While O’Shea et al. (2016) assessed combined bilateral hippocampal volume, previous studies have demonstrated asymmetry in hippocampal volume in healthy older adults, with the right volume typically larger than left (Woolard and Heckers, 2012; Hou et al., 2013). Additionally, there are differences in hippocampal volume asymmetry across dementia disease progression, notably in mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Through a meta-analysis, Shi et al. (2009) evaluated the asymmetry of the hippocampus in healthy older adults, older individuals with MCI, and those with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Their findings revealed an overall pattern of right greater than left asymmetry in all groups. However, adults with MCI had the most pronounced right greater than left asymmetry when compared to healthy adults and those with AD (Shi et al., 2009).

In addition to volume asymmetry, lateralized cognitive functioning as it relates to hippocampal volume has been explored in pathological populations. Delaney et al. (1980) compared epileptic patients with seizure foci originating in either the left or right temporal lobe on verbal and non-verbal memory tasks. Their findings revealed a dissociation between verbal and non-verbal memory deficits, such that patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy were more impaired on non-verbal memory tasks, while patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy were more impaired on verbal memory tasks. In a small sample (n = 36) of healthy older adults and adults with MCI, Müller et al. (2005) found a relationship between left hippocampal volume only and a verbal memory task in the MCI group. These findings suggest cognitive function laterality associated with hippocampal volume in non-healthy patients, although this same relationship has yet to be studied in large samples of healthy older adults.

In summary, the relationship between bilateral hippocampal volume and performance in specific cognitive domains (i.e., episodic memory, verbal learning, processing speed, and executive function) has been characterized by only a few studies in healthy older adults (Woolard and Heckers, 2012; O’Shea et al., 2016). However, previous research shows hemispheric differences in hippocampal functioning, and asymmetry in hippocampal volume decline in the dementia process (Delaney et al., 1980; Müller et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2009). There is currently a gap in the literature characterizing the relationship between asymmetry/lateral hippocampal volume and cognitive functioning in healthy older adults. We must first understand cognitive differences in lateral hippocampal volume in healthy aging to further understanding of pathological aging.

The present study aims to fill this gap by first replicating findings from O’Shea et al. (2016) with a larger independent sample, and then by assessing the relationship of asymmetry and laterality of hippocampal volume and cognitive functioning in healthy older adults. We hypothesized that there would be a positive association between bilateral hippocampal volume and fluid cognitive functioning, namely episodic memory and processing speed. Given a lateralized relationship between cognitive functioning and hippocampal volume in pathological adults, we predict that there will also be a similar relationship in healthy older adults. Specifically, we predict that verbally loaded memory tasks may have a more robust relationship with left hippocampal volume.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred and eighty-three older adults (mean age = 71.72; SD = 5.3) were drawn from two ongoing randomized clinical trials with identical inclusion/exclusion criteria (R01AG054077; K01AG050707) across the University of Florida (UF; n = 126) and the University of Arizona (AU; n = 57; Woods et al., 2018). Participant demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. Only baseline data were included in this study. The participants were recruited through newspaper advertising, flyers, and community outreach. All participants provided written informed consent before study procedures began, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the University of Florida and the University of Arizona. Participants were excluded for left-handedness, if they were outside the age range of 65–89, had a history of brain or head injury that resulted in a loss of consciousness for greater than 20 min, identified a pre-existing neurological condition, psychiatric disorder, MRI contraindications (e.g., medical devices or implants not approved for 3T MRI), diagnosis of neurodegenerative brain disease (i.e., AD, Parkinson’s disease, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis), or self-reported difficulties in thinking and/or memory. At the in-person screening visit, participants were further screened for MCI through the use of the Unified Data Set (UDS) of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC; Weintraub et al., 2009) and administration of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). If they were equal to or below 1.5 standard deviations on any cognitive domain including general cognition, visuospatial functioning, executive functioning/working memory, or language, they were not included. For more detail on inclusion/exclusion criteria, refer Woods et al. (2018).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics.

Study Procedures

At the study baseline visit, all participants completed a neuropsychological battery that included the NIH Toolbox—Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB). All neuropsychological tasks were administered at an onsite clinical research facility by trained study staff. The NIHTB-CB battery and MRI scan were completed on the same day.

NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery

The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery is a brief yet valid and comprehensive assessment of cognitive functioning in a variety of domains (Heaton et al., 2014). For this study, we utilized the unadjusted standard scores of the Fluid Cognition Composite (FCC) and the Crystalized Cognitive Composite (CCC). FCC is comprised of cognitive domains that have been shown to decline with age, while CCC is comprised of language-based assessments that are stable or even improve with age (Horn and Cattell, 1967).

The FCC is comprised of five domains of cognitive functioning. The domains and description of NIHTB-CB subtask used to assess these domains are as follows:

Processing Speed—Pattern Comparison Task asks participants to rapidly match visual patterns.

Attention/Inhibition—Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention task assess visual attention and inhibition by asking participants to determine the direction of a target arrow that is flanked by stimuli on left and right.

Executive Functioning—Dimensional Card Sort task assesses set-shifting by asking participants to match cards based on switching between multiple rules and strategies.

Working Memory—List Sorting task asks participants to sort lists of visual and auditory stimuli from smallest to largest.

Episodic Memory—Picture Sequence Memory task asks participants to remember a sequence of thematically related pictures.

The CCC is comprised of two domains assessing language functioning. The description of NIHTB-CB subtask used to assess this domain is as follows:

Oral Reading Recognition Task—The participant is asked to read and pronounce words as accurately as possible.

Picture Vocabulary Test—The participant is asked to match auditorily presented words to pictures.

Image Acquisition

All participants received a high-resolution 3D T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence on a 3-Tesla Siemens Magnetom Prisma Scanner with a 64-channel head coil at the Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory at the University of Florida and a 3-Tesla Siemens Magnetom Skyra scanner with a 32-channel head coil at the University of Arizona. Both study sites followed identical sequence and scanning procedures, including reducing head motion through the use of foam padding, and the use of earplugs to reduce scanner noise. Scanning parameters are as follows: echo time (TE) = 2.26 ms; repetition time (TR) = 1,800 ms; flip angle = 8°; FOV = 256 × 256 × 176 mm, voxel size = 1 mm3.

Image Processing

T1-weighted images were processed with the FreeSurfer software version 6.0, freely available to download at http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/. All images were processed using the “recon-all” pipeline, which briefly includes motion correction and averaging (Reuter et al., 2010), skull stripping (Ségonne et al., 2004), Talaraich transformation, intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), gray/white matter segmentation, and topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001; Ségonne et al., 2007). A more detailed description of this process can be found at Fischl et al. (2001) and Ségonne et al. (2004).

Within FreeSurfer 6.0, the hippocampal segmentation module was applied to extract 12 subfields of bilateral hippocampi that were summed to produce total hippocampal volume (Iglesias et al., 2015, 2016; Saygin et al., 2017). This tool uses Bayesian inference from a probabilistic atlas built with ultra-high resolution ex vivo MRI data (~0.1 mm isotropic)1. While this tool segments hippocampal and amygdala subfields, only total left and right hippocampal volumes derived from subfield segmentation were used in this study. This tool has been validated against manual segmentation in older adult brains and was shown to have higher accuracy in segmentation due to the use of ex vivo segmentation techniques that better match with histological studies (Iglesias et al., 2015; Schmidt et al., 2018). Additionally, this atlas is better able to discriminate between AD and control participants using whole hippocampal volume, suggesting total hippocampal measurement may be more accurate than the previous in vivo mask (Iglesias et al., 2015). While hippocampal subfields have been shown to differentially decline with age (Zheng et al., 2018), we were interested in replicating previous findings with a larger independent sample (O’Shea et al., 2016) and exploring differences in left-right total hippocampal volume and cognitive functioning, and thus chose to only assess whole hippocampal volume.

See Figure 1 for a visual depiction of hippocampal volume.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Bilateral hippocampal region of interest overlaid onto the T1 image. (A) Axial, (B) coronal, and (C) right sagittal view of combined hippocampal subfields in radiological display convention.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in SPSS software version 25. UF and UA study sites were compared on demographic factors, then combined. Outlier analyses were conducted on dependent variables. Any participants who had cognitive z-scores in any subtest greater than three standard deviations away from the mean were excluded in further analyses. NIHTB-CB descriptive statistics can be found in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. NIH Toolbox-Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) fluid cognition and crystalized cognition composites and subtests.

To measure asymmetry, an asymmetry index was calculated as the ratio of the absolute difference of left from right hippocampal volume to the sum of left and right hippocampal volume (Sarica et al., 2018). Therefore, values closer to zero represent no asymmetry. The absolute difference was calculated to represent the magnitude of asymmetry that does not indicate laterality.

Asymmetry Index=| LR |L+R

Primary analysis assessed total, asymmetry index, and lateralized contribution of hippocampal volume to NIHTB-CB cognitive composites. Linear regressions were performed predicting FCC and CCC from combined, asymmetry ratio, then left and right hippocampal volume separately, totaling to eight separate regression models. Sex, education, scanner type, and total intracranial volume were entered into all models as covariates. Multiple comparisons in primary analysis were corrected using a false discovery rate (FDR) of p < 0.05. Significant primary analyses were followed up by secondary linear regressions to assess NIHTB-CB subtest performance with hippocampal volume variables.

Results

Replication of Total Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

After FDR adjustment of the p-value (q-value), total hippocampal volume significantly positively predicted the FCC (q < 0.05) but was not related to the CCC. Therefore, we did not explore the relationship between subtest performance within CCC and total hippocampal volume. Out of the subtests within FCC, total hippocampal volume was significantly positively associated with the episodic memory subtest, picture sequence memory (p < 0.001). Total hippocampal volume did not significantly predict any other subtest within FCC. Refer Table 3 for complete statistical output and Figures 2A, 3A for scatter plot visual representation.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regressions of total hippocampal volume and NIH Toolbox—Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Regressions of hippocampal volume and NIHTB cognition composites. (A) Total hippocampal volume; (B) left hippocampal volume; (C) right hippocampal volume; (D) asymmetry index; R2 reflects variance explained from the partial correlation between hippocampal volume and NIHTB-CB score; X-axis = residual of hippocampal volume after controlling for sex, education, scanner, and total intracranial volume; Y-axis = standard score of NIHTB-CB variables.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regressions of hippocampal volume and significant subtests. (A) Total hippocampal volume; (B,C) left hippocampal volume; R2 reflects variance explained from the partial correlation between hippocampal volume and NIHTB-CB score; X-axis = residual of hippocampal volume after controlling for sex, education, scanner, and total intracranial volume; Y-axis = standard score of Fluid Cognition Composite (FCC) subtest variables.

Asymmetry of Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

After FDR adjustment, the asymmetry index of the hippocampus did not significantly predict FCC or CCC. Therefore, the relationship of subtest scores and asymmetry of the hippocampal volume was not explored for either cognition composite. Refer Table 4 for complete statistical output and Figure 2D for scatter plot visual representation.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regressions of asymmetry index and NIHTB-CB.

Left and Right Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

After FDR adjustment, left hippocampal volume significantly positively predicted FCC (q < 0.05), but not CCC. The right hippocampal volume did not significantly predict the FCC or CCC. Therefore, the relationship between subtest performance and hippocampal volume was only explored with left hippocampal volume for the FCC subtests. Out of the FCC subtests, left hippocampal volume was significantly positively associated with the working memory subtest, list sorting (p < 0.05), and the episodic memory subtest, picture sequence memory (p < 0.001). Left hippocampal volume did not significantly predict any other subtest within FCC. Refer Table 5 for complete statistical output and Figure 2B (left) and Figure 2C (right) for NIHTB-CB composite scatter plot visual representation, and Figures 3B,C for NIHTB FCC subtest scatter plot visual representation.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Regressions of lateral hippocampal volume and NIHTB-CB.

Discussion

Smaller hippocampal volume in older adults is related to poorer cognitive functioning in domains of episodic memory, verbal learning, processing speed, and executive function (Woolard and Heckers, 2012; O’Shea et al., 2016). Additionally, previous research suggests that cognitive functions associated with the hippocampus may be lateralized, and this cognitive asymmetry may change with the aging process and progression into dementia (Delaney et al., 1980; Müller et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2009). This study sought to replicate and extend O’Shea et al.’s (2016) findings with a larger, and independent sample, confirming that greater hippocampal volume is associated with better fluid cognitive functioning as measured by the NIHTB-CB in older adults. We further sought to characterize the relationship between lateral and asymmetrical hippocampal volume and cognitive functioning in older adults.

Total Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

Greater total hippocampal volume was related to higher scores on NIHTB FCC, after we controlled for sex, education, and total intracranial volume. Further, the episodic memory subtest (picture sequence memory) within the FCC showed the strongest relationship with total hippocampal volume. This replicates findings from O’Shea et al. (2016), who also found a positive relationship between total hippocampal volume and episodic memory. The replication of this relationship confirms the association between total hippocampal volume and episodic memory performance in aging. Prior data in patient populations with hippocampal sclerosis and hippocampal degeneration have also demonstrated the role of hippocampal volume in episodic memory (Sexton et al., 2010; Saghafi et al., 2018). However, our larger sample did not show a relationship between total hippocampal volume and processing speed, which O’Shea et al. (2016) found to be at near the same magnitude as episodic memory. Possibly this relationship was not robust enough to withstand a larger sample, and a possibly more accurate methodology of volume extraction (Iglesias et al., 2015).

Asymmetry of Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

There is a reliable and reproducible finding of right-greater-than-left asymmetry of the hippocampus in healthy older adults (Shi et al., 2009; Sarica et al., 2018). Post hoc analyses confirmed this in our sample when comparing lateralized volume using a paired samples t-test (t(182) = 6.248, p < 0.001). However, our findings show that this volumetric asymmetry is not related to fluid cognitive functioning in healthy older adults. Through a meta-analytical review of literature, Shi et al. (2009) were able to show that this asymmetry was greatest in the MCI portion of a trajectory to Alzheimer’s dementia, although other studies have shown greater asymmetry in a population with dementia due to AD (Sarica et al., 2018). A cognitive relationship with structural asymmetry may not yet be evident in healthy older adults. Similar results have been found in a study assessing the asymmetry of hippocampal volume and its relationship to learning and memory (Sarica et al., 2018). Sarica et al. (2018) focused on measures of learning and memory extracted from the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Nevertheless, they also did not find an association between an asymmetry index and cognitive functioning in healthy older adults. Interestingly, they were able to show this association only in a population with dementia due to AD. Taken together, our findings support previous literature suggesting that structural asymmetry of the hippocampus, while evident, is not necessarily related to differences in cognitive functioning assessed over a range of cognitive domains in cognitively healthy adults.

Lateralization of Hippocampal Volume and NIHTB-CB

Despite no evidence of a relationship between structural asymmetry and cognition in this sample, our findings did suggest cognitive laterality. Left hippocampal volume had a positive association with fluid cognition scores, specifically the episodic and working memory subtests of the NIHTB FCC. Interestingly, right hippocampal volume showed no association with either fluid or crystallized cognition composites from the NIHTB. Left hippocampal volume is thought to relate to verbally-loaded memory tasks, while right hippocampal volume is thought to be related to spatially-loaded memory tasks (Delaney et al., 1980; Bonner-Jackson et al., 2015; Ezzati et al., 2016). Given our measure of episodic memory has a prominent verbal component, we predicted that left hippocampal volume would most strongly predict episodic memory. These results confirm previous research showing a positive association between verbally-based immediate and delayed measures of episodic memory and left hippocampal volume in healthy older adults (Ezzati et al., 2016).

Our findings also showed a positive relationship between working memory and left hippocampal volume. While the key neural correlates of working memory in aging are traditionally thought to include the function of the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior parietal lobes, and insula (Suzuki et al., 2018), other studies have suggested that the hippocampus may also play a role in this memory system. Through the use of combined functional MRI and intracranial electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings in epilepsy patients and healthy controls, Axmacher et al. (2007) demonstrated sustained activity in medial temporal lobes during visual working memory maintenance, and the amount of activation was related to working memory load. Some have argued that hippocampal involvement in maintenance is related to the process of long-term memory because maintenance predicts the number of items consolidated (Schon et al., 2004; Axmacher et al., 2007; Leszczynski, 2011). However, our working memory measure was derived from the sum of total items correctly sorted, rather than maintenance. Therefore, our findings expand upon this notion by suggesting that in cognitive aging left hippocampal volume may also be associated with aspects of working memory that is independent of long-term consolidation. The NIHTB-CB FCC working memory subtest also requires sorting of information presented in a visual and auditory format, rather than predominately verbal (Tulsky et al., 2014). Given this measure of working memory only associated with the left hippocampal volume, the left hippocampus may play a broader role in working memory functioning that isn’t limited to verbally presented information.

No cognition composite was significantly related to the right hippocampal volume. That is not to say that the right hippocampal volume does not play a role in cognition. Notably, FCC does explain ~2% of the variance in the right hippocampal volume. With increased power and broader cognitive assessments, possibly a significant relationship would arise. None of the subtests that comprise the NIHTB cognition composites relies on spatial memory or spatial navigation; a skill that is related to right hippocampal volume (Nedelska et al., 2012). Spatial memory and navigation deficits can be an early sign of cognitive dysfunction suggestive of AD (Dudas et al., 2005; Laczó et al., 2018). The current study may be limited in its ability to assess key components of right hippocampal functioning in healthy older adults due to a lack of spatially-based measures that comprise typical cognitive batteries. Although the NIHTB picture sequence memory subtest does have visual and verbal components, this subtest does not assess spatial memory or navigation (Dikmen et al., 2014). This limitation is represented across cognitive batteries and is partly due to a lack of validated spatial navigation assessments for clinical use (Possin, 2010). Considering volumetric asymmetry may not be a sensitive method of understanding the relationship between hippocampal volume and cognition in healthy older adults, the ability to independently assess the contribution of lateral hippocampi to cognitive functioning is perhaps crucial for documenting the nature of the pathological cognitive decline.

Future Directions and Limitations

While this article focused solely on cognitively intact older adults, there is a need to extend these analyses to include low functioning older adults. More accurate quantification of how lateralized and asymmetrical hippocampal volume contribute to the trajectory of cognitive decline is possible by utilizing a longitudinal method. Additionally, despite known right hippocampal involvement in spatial memory and spatial navigation, and decline of these abilities in AD, these measures are not part of the core functional battery of the NIHTB-CB. Including spatial navigation measures regularly in cognitive batteries will allow us to better quantify changes of right hippocampal volume and function.

While our sample includes a wide range of education (12–21 years), the mean education was about 16 years. In 2015, only 27% of older adults reported attaining a bachelor’s degree or more (U.S. Census Bureau, 2015). Despite controlling for educational attainment in our statistical models, education is a known predictor of cognitive reserve and therefore may have influenced our findings (Farfel et al., 2013). Future studies would benefit from confirming the relationship of hippocampal volume and cognition in a population of older adults that reflects national educational demographics. Additionally, rates of dementia differ between racial and ethnic groups. Non-white individuals develop dementia at a younger age and a higher prevalence (National Research Council, and Committee on Population, 2004; DeCarli et al., 2008). Considering the sample used for this study was predominately non-Hispanic whites, future studies would benefit from exploring racial differences in lateralized hippocampal volume and cognitive functioning.

Conclusion

The findings from this study contribute to our understanding of the role of lateral and asymmetrical hippocampal volume with multiple domains of cognitive functioning in healthy older adults. This work replicates the relationship between total hippocampal volume and episodic memory. Furthermore, these results show that the strongest relationship of predominately verbal-based episodic memory is with the left hippocampus; suggesting laterality of cognitive functioning in the context of healthy aging. Interestingly, we observed that the asymmetry of the hippocampi appears to not yet be related to cognitive functioning. However, that is not to say asymmetry may not be related as the trajectory of cognitive decline progresses. These findings help to better understand the role of the hippocampus in specific domains of cognitive functioning in healthy older adults and therefore may serve as a foundation to better understand the continuum from healthy to pathological aging.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: data are managed under the data sharing agreement established with NIA and the parent R01 clinical trial Data Safety and Monitoring Board in the context of an ongoing Phase III clinical trial (ACT study, R01AG054077). All trial data will be made publicly available 2 years after completion of the parent clinical trial, per NIA and DSMB agreement. Requests for baseline data can be submitted to the ACT Publication and Presentation (P&P) Committee and will require submission of a data use, authorship, and analytic plan for review by the P&P committee (ajwoods@phhp.ufl.edu). Requests to access these datasets should be directed to ajwoods@ufl.edu.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Florida Institutional Review Board; University of Arizona Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CH, AO’S, JK, AA, NE, HH, EB, EV, PB, SD, GA, and AW contributed text to the manuscript. CH, JK, and AO’S contributed to data analysis and processing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We would like to acknowledge support by the National Institute of Aging/National Institutes of Health (T32AG020499, K01AG050707, R01AG054077, P30AG019610, and T32AG061892), the University of Florida Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory Clinical Translational Research, the state of Arizona and Arizona Department of Health Services, the McKnight Brain Research Foundation, and National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (T32HL134621).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/HippocampalSubfieldsAndNucleiOfAmygdala

References

Axmacher, N., Mormann, F., Fernandez, G., Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2007). Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 27, 7807–7816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0962-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonner-Jackson, A., Mahmoud, S., Miller, J., and Banks, S. J. (2015). Verbal and non-verbal memory and hippocampal volumes in a memory clinic population. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 7:61. doi: 10.1186/s13195-015-0147-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeCarli, C., Reed, B. R., Jagust, W. J., Martinez, O., Ortega, M., and Mungas, D. (2008). Brain behavior relationships amongst African Americans, caucasians and hispanics. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 22, 382–391. doi: 10.1097/wad.0b013e318185e7fe

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delaney, R. C., Rosen, A. J., Mattson, R. H., and Novelly, R. A. (1980). Memory function in focal epilepsy: a comparison of non-surgical, unilateral temporal lobe and frontal lobe samples. Cortex 16, 103–117. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(80)80026-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikmen, S. S., Bauer, P. J., Weintraub, S., Mungas, D., Slotkin, J., Beaumont, J. L., et al. (2014). Measuring episodic memory across the lifespan: NIH toolbox picture sequence memory test. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 611–619. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudas, R. B., Clague, F., Thompson, S. A., Graham, K. S., and Hodges, J. R. (2005). Episodic and semantic memory in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 43, 1266–1276. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ezzati, A., Katz, M. J., Zammit, A. R., Lipton, M. L., Zimmerman, M. E., Sliwinski, M. J., et al. (2016). Differential association of left and right hippocampal volumes with verbal episodic and spatial memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia 93, 380–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farfel, J. M., Nitrini, R., Suemoto, C. K., Grinberg, L. T., Ferretti, R. E. L., Leite, R. E. P., et al. (2013). Very low levels of education and cognitive reserve: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology 81, 650–657. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a08f1b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Liu, A., and Dale, A. M. (2001). Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorbach, T., Pudas, S., Lundquist, A., Orädd, G., Josefsson, M., Salami, A., et al. (2017). Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline. Neurobiol. Aging 51, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heaton, R. K., Akshoomoff, N., Tulsky, D., Mungas, D., Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S., et al. (2014). Reliability and validity of composite scores from the NIH toolbox cognition battery in adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 588–598. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, J., and Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystalized intelligence. Acta Psychologica 26, 107–129.

Google Scholar

Hou, G., Yang, X., and Yuan, T.-F. (2013). Hippocampal asymmetry: differences in structures and functions. Neurochem. Res. 38, 453–460. doi: 10.1007/s11064-012-0954-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iglesias, J. E., Augustinack, J. C., Nguyen, K., Player, C. M., Player, A., Wright, M., et al. (2015). A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. NeuroImage 115, 117–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iglesias, J. E., Van Leemput, K., Augustinack, J., Insausti, R., Fischl, B., and Reuter, M. (2016). Bayesian longitudinal segmentation of hippocampal substructures in brain MRI using subject-specific atlases. NeuroImage 141, 542–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laczó, J., Parizkova, M., and Moffat, S. D. (2018). Spatial navigation, aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Aging 10, 3050–3051. doi: 10.18632/aging.101634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leszczynski, M. (2011). How does hippocampus contribute to working memory processing? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:168. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lövdén, M., Schaefer, S., Noack, H., Bodammer, N. C., Kühn, S., Heinze, H.-J., et al. (2012). Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 620.e9–620.e22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, M. J., Greverus, D., Dellani, P. R., Weibrich, C., Wille, P. R., Scheurich, A., et al. (2005). Functional implications of hippocampal volume and diffusivity in mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage 28, 1033–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council, and Committee on Population. (2004). Critical Perspectives on Racial and Ethnic Differences in Health in Late Life. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Google Scholar

Nedelska, Z., Andel, R., Laczo, J., Vlcek, K., Horinek, D., Lisy, J., et al. (2012). Spatial navigation impairment is proportional to right hippocampal volume. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 2590–2594. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121588109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Shea, A., Cohen, R. A., Porges, E. C., Nissim, N. R., and Woods, A. J. (2016). Cognitive aging and the hippocampus in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:298. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Possin, K. L. (2010). Visual spatial cognition in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase 16, 466–487. doi: 10.1080/13554791003730600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuben, A., Brickman, A. M., Muraskin, J., Steffener, J., and Stern, Y. (2011). Hippocampal atrophy relates to fluid intelligence decline in the elderly. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 56–61. doi: 10.1017/S135561771000127X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2010). Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. NeuroImage 53, 1181–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saghafi, S., Ferguson, L., Hogue, O., Gales, J. M., Prayson, R., and Busch, R. M. (2018). Histopathologic subtype of hippocampal sclerosis and episodic memory performance before and after temporal lobectomy for epilepsy. Epilepsia 59, 825–833. doi: 10.1111/epi.14036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2009). Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 650–661. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarica, A., Vasta, R., Novellino, F., Vaccaro, M. G., Cerasa, A., Quattrone, A., et al. (2018). MRI asymmetry index of hippocampal subfields increases through the continuum from the mild cognitive impairment to the Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 12:576. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, Z. M., Kliemann, D., Iglesias, J. E., van der Kouwe, A. J. W., Boyd, E., Reuter, M., et al. (2017). High-resolution magnetic resonance imaging reveals nuclei of the human amygdala: manual segmentation to automatic atlas. NeuroImage 155, 370–382. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M. F., Storrs, J. M., Freeman, K. B., Jack, C. R., Turner, S. T., Griswold, M. E., et al. (2018). A comparison of manual tracing and FreeSurfer for estimating hippocampal volume over the adult lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 2500–2513. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schon, K., Hasselmo, M. E., Lopresti, M. L., Tricarico, M. D., and Stern, C. E. (2004). Persistence of parahippocampal representation in the absence of stimulus input enhances long-term encoding: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of subsequent memory after a delayed match-to-sample task. J. Neurosci. 24, 11088–11097. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3807-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage 22, 1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ségonne, F., Pacheco, J., and Fischl, B. (2007). Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 26, 518–529. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.887364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sexton, C. E., Mackay, C. E., Lonie, J. A., Bastin, M. E., Terrière, E., O’Carroll, R. E., et al. (2010). MRI correlates of episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging. Psychiatry Res. 184, 57–62. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, F., Liu, B., Zhou, Y., Yu, C., and Jiang, T. (2009). Hippocampal volume and asymmetry in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: meta-analyses of MRI studies. Hippocampus 19, 1055–1064. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sled, J. G., Zijdenbos, A. P., and Evans, A. C. (1998). A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 17, 87–97. doi: 10.1109/42.668698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, M., Kawagoe, T., Nishiguchi, S., Abe, N., Otsuka, Y., Nakai, R., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of working memory maintenance in advanced aging: evidence from fMRI. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:358. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulsky, D. S., Carlozzi, N., Chiaravalloti, N. D., Beaumont, J. L., Kisala, P. A., Mungas, D., et al. (2014). NIH toolbox cognition battery (NIHTB-CB): list sorting test to measure working memory. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 599–610. doi: 10.1017/S135561771400040X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Census Bureau. (2015). Educational Attainment in the United States: 2015. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2016/demo/p20-578.pdf. Accessed June 19, 2020.

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., and Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277, 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5324.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weintraub, S., Salmon, D., Graff-Radford, N. R., Chui, H., DeCarli, C., Foster, N. L., et al. (2009). The Alzheimer’s disease centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS). Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 23, 91–101. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7dd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Cohen, R., Marsiske, M., Alexander, G. E., Czaja, S. J., and Wu, S. (2018). Augmenting cognitive training in older adults (the ACT study): design and methods of a phase III tDCS and cognitive training trial. Contemp. Clin. Trials 65, 19–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2017.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolard, A. A., and Heckers, S. (2012). Anatomical and functional correlates of human hippocampal volume asymmetry. Psychiatry Res. 201, 48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, F., Cui, D., Zhang, L., Zhang, S., Zhao, Y., Liu, X., et al. (2018). The volume of hippocampal subfields in relation to decline of memory recall across the adult lifespan. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:320. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, cognition, hippocampus, magnetic resonance imaging, brain volume, NIH toolbox

Citation: Hardcastle C, O’Shea A, Kraft JN, Albizu A, Evangelista ND, Hausman HK, Boutzoukas EM, Van Etten EJ, Bharadwaj PK, Song H, Smith SG, Porges EC, Dekosky S, Hishaw GA, Wu SS, Marsiske M, Cohen R, Alexander GE and Woods AJ (2020) Contributions of Hippocampal Volume to Cognition in Healthy Older Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:593833. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.593833

Received: 11 August 2020; Accepted: 05 October 2020;
Published: 05 November 2020.

Edited by:

P. Hemachandra Reddy, Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center, United States

Reviewed by:

Muthuswamy Anusuyadevi, Bharathidasan University, India
Nirnath Sah, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Hardcastle, O’Shea, Kraft, Albizu, Evangelista, Hausman, Boutzoukas, Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Song, Smith, Porges, Dekosky, Hishaw, Wu, Marsiske, Cohen, Alexander and Woods. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adam J. Woods, ajwoods@phhp.ufl.edu

Download