Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 08 April 2024
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 16 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2024.1361244

Age-related changes in static balance in older women aged in their early sixties to their late eighties: different aging patterns in the anterior–posterior and mediolateral directions

Shun Sasagawa* Ai Arakawa Aimi Furuyama Yasuo Matsumoto
  • Department of Human Sciences, Faculty of Human Sciences, Kanagawa University, Yokohama, Japan

Objective: The aim of this study was to cross-sectionally investigate how static balance changes throughout the aging process in older women aged from their early sixties to their late eighties.

Methods: Forty-six older women (aged 62–89 years) were requested to stand barefoot and quietly on a force platform for 30 s with their eyes either open or closed. During the trials, the position of the center of foot pressure (CoP) and the acceleration of the body’s center of mass (ACC) were measured. The root mean square (RMS) of the CoP and ACC values was calculated to evaluate the amplitude of postural sway and the level of regulatory activity, respectively. The mean power frequency of the ACC was also calculated to represent the temporal characteristics of regulatory activity.

Results: In the anterior–posterior direction, there was no significant relationship between the RMS of CoP and the participants’ age, whereas the RMS of ACC significantly increased with increasing age. In the mediolateral direction, however, the RMS of CoP significantly increased with increasing age, whereas the RMS of ACC did not change with age. The mean power frequency of ACC did not exhibit any age-related change in either the anterior–posterior or the mediolateral direction.

Conclusion: The results indicate that static balance in older women aged in their early sixties to their late eighties exhibits distinctly contrasting aging patterns between the anterior–posterior and mediolateral directions. To prevent falls in older women, it is necessary to elucidate the physiological mechanisms responsible for the increase in mediolateral sway that occurs throughout old age.

1 Introduction

For more than 15 years, Japan has been a super-aged society: in 2021, 28.9% of the total Japanese population was aged 65 years and older (Japanese Cabinet Office, 2022). In an aging society, falls among older people are a major health problem. According to a survey by the Tokyo Fire Department (2020), fall accidents accounted for more than 80% of emergency transport cases of older people, of which 40% were diagnosed as moderate or severe cases requiring hospitalisation. Even if falling does not result in severe injury, the fear of falling can decrease activities of daily living of older people, thereby creating a vicious cycle that increases the risk of falls (Scheffer et al., 2008). Notably, older women have been reported to be at higher risk of hip fracture (Farmer et al., 1984) and are more likely to develop a fear of falling after experiencing a fall (Vellas et al., 1997) than men. Therefore, to prevent falls and to improve the quality of life of older people, especially of older women, it is important to advance the current understanding of aging effects on their balance abilities. Because the number of emergency transport cases per unit age of population has been reported to increase dramatically as age increased from 65 to 95 years (Tokyo Fire Department, 2020), it is likely that even in old age, balance abilities change throughout the aging process and these changes are closely related to risk of fall in older people. Therefore, to prevent falls of older people, it is especially important to investigate age-related changes in static balance throughout old age. Although numerous attempts have been made to compare the balance abilities of older individuals to those of younger people (see Quijoux et al., 2021 for a review), a few attempts have been made to examine the age-related changes in balance throughout old age. Recently, Brech et al. (2022), who evaluated the age-related changes on static balance in 400 women aged 50–89 years, have reported that the older women in their eighties exhibited greater displacement and range of the center of foot pressure (CoP) in the mediolateral (ML) direction and faster planar mean velocity of CoP than the women in their fifties and sixties. However, because Brech et al. (2022) have examined only displacement-related measures (i.e., displacement and range of CoP) for each of the anterior–posterior (AP) and ML directions, direction-dependency of the age-related changes in static balance is still ambiguous. Furthermore, because Brech et al. (2022) have not examined any of frequency-domain measures, age-related changes in the temporal aspects of static balance remains subject to debate. Therefore, in the present study, our aim was to cross-sectionally investigate how static balance in older women aged in their early sixties to their late eighties changes throughout the aging process, in both the AP and ML directions, using multiple measures (i.e., displacement-related measure, acceleration-related measure, and frequency-related measure) that enabled us to capture a wide range of spatio-temporal features of postural control.

Static balance ability is usually assessed using the CoP time series obtained using a force platform measurement. When analyzing the CoP time series, it should be noted that it contains information on both the position and acceleration of the center of body mass (CoM) (Kuo et al., 1998; Zatsiorsky and Duarte, 2000); positional fluctuations of the CoM are reflected in low-frequency CoP sway below 0.5 Hz, whereas fluctuations of CoM acceleration are reflected in high-frequency sway above 0.5 Hz (Zatsiorsky and Duarte, 2000). The information on CoM position and CoM acceleration have different meanings in the control of upright balance. The amplitude of positional fluctuations of the CoM represents the level of stability achieved by the postural control system (Hufschmidt et al., 1980), which can be evaluated by displacement-related sway measures such as the root mean square (RMS) of the CoP (Maurer and Peterka, 2005). In contrast, the amplitude of CoM acceleration describes the level of regulatory activity (Hufschmidt et al., 1980), which can be characterised by velocity-related sway measures such as the mean velocity of the CoP (Masani et al., 2014). The CoM acceleration is not only indirectly estimable from the CoP time series; it can also be directly obtained from the horizontal ground reaction force according to Newton’s Second Law of Motion (McClenaghan et al., 1996; Masani et al., 2007; Yu et al., 2008; Sasagawa et al., 2009; Oba et al., 2015). This directly obtained CoM acceleration (ACC) has been suggested to be an alternative force platform measure for assessing changes in the postural control system. In fact, recent studies have reported that the ACC has a higher sensitivity to changes in the postural control system with development (Oba et al., 2015), aging (McClenaghan et al., 1996; Masani et al., 2007) and disease (Yu et al., 2008). In this study, we therefore investigated the effects of aging on different aspects of static balance using measurement of both the CoP and the ACC.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

To determine the sample size needed for the study, we conducted an a priori power analysis in G∗power using a bivariate normal model from the exact family with a two-tailed test. Correlation ρ (H1), α error probability, power (1 – β error probability) and correlation ρ (H0) were set to 0.40, 0.05, 0.80 and 0.00, respectively. The correlation ρ (H1) of 0.4 was based on a previous study that showed a correlation coefficient of 0.42 between the age of older participants and the sway amplitude in the ML direction (Lord et al., 1999). The required minimum sample size was calculated to be 46. Participants were recruited through a flyer distributed throughout the local area. The inclusion/exclusion criteria to participate in the study were older women who could stand for a few minutes without standing aids. To provide an opportunity of checking their balance abilities for community-dwelling older women, we did not use strict inclusion/exclusion criteria to participate in the study. Eighty-four independent community-dwelling women aged between 54 and 89 years participated in the study. They gave written informed consent after receiving a detailed explanation of the study. The procedures used in this study were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the institutional ethics committee (no. 2021–34). For the analysis, we excluded the data of participants who: (1) were under 60 years old (N = 2); (2) had a reported history of neurological disorders (N = 2); or (3) were receiving treatment for neurological or musculoskeletal disorders (N = 18). We also excluded the data of participants such as who could not accomplish the entire trial due to fatigue, who made voluntary movements during the trials, or who wore tights or stockings (N = 16). Eventually, data from 46 individuals 62–89 years old (mean ± standard deviation (SD): age 79.2 ± 6.1 years, height 151.4 ± 5.7 cm, body mass 50.4 ± 7.2 kg) were used in the analysis.

2.2 Procedure and measurements

The participants stood barefoot and quietly on a force platform (TFG-4060; Tec Gihan, Kyoto, Japan) with their feet parallel to each other (heel-to-heel distance of 20 cm) and with their arms at their sides. They stood with their eyes open (EO) for 30 s, then for 30 s with their eyes closed (EC); this was repeated three times. During the EO condition, the participants were asked to gaze at a target placed at eye level 2.5 meters in front of them. The ground reaction force data were sampled at 100 Hz through an analog-digital converter (PowerLab 16/35, ADInstruments, Sydney, Australia) for offline analysis.

2.3 Data processing and analysis

From the measured ground reaction forces, the CoP position in the AP and ML directions were calculated for each participant. We also calculated the ACC in both the AP and ML directions by dividing the horizontal ground reaction forces by the participants’ body mass (Masani et al., 2007; Yu et al., 2008; Sasagawa et al., 2009; Oba et al., 2015). The CoP and ACC time series were digitally smoothed using a fourth-order, dual-pass, low-pass Butterworth filter (MATLAB signal processing toolbox, MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States) with a cut-off frequency of 5.0 Hz (Prieto et al., 1996). The fluctuations of the CoP and ACC were evaluated by the RMS. The RMS of CoP (CoPRMS) represents the amplitude of postural sway, or the effectiveness of postural control (Hufschmidt et al., 1980). In contrast, the RMS of ACC (ACCRMS) describes the level of regulatory activity generated by the postural control system (Hufschmidt et al., 1980; Morasso and Schieppati, 1999). In the analysis, we also performed decomposition of the CoP sway into their low-frequency (LF) and high-frequency (HF) components (Figure 1). The LF component was obtained by smoothing the raw CoP time series with a cut-off frequency of 0.5 Hz (Caron et al., 1997; Loram and Lakie, 2002; Yamada-Yanagawa et al., 2022). By subtracting the LF component from the low-pass filtered CoP time series, we then obtained the HF component.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Typical example of decomposition of the CoP sway (grey line) into the LF (cyan line) and HF (magenta line) components. CoP, center of foot pressure; HF, high frequency; LF, low frequency.

For the CoP, ACC, LF component and HF component time series, we performed spectral analyses (MATLAB signal processing toolbox, MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States). The data for a single 30-s trial were detrended and then divided into five segments with 50% overlap (10 s of data in each segment). A fast Fourier transform algorithm was used for each segment to calculate the power spectral density after passing it through a Hamming window, which resulted in a frequency resolution of 0.1 Hz. The power spectral density function of the individual segments was ensemble-averaged into the function of a single trial. The temporal characteristics of these time series were quantified by the mean power frequency (MPF).

2.4 Statistical analysis

The average value across three 30-s trials was used as a representative value of the EC and EO condition for each participant. In the text and tables, data are presented as mean ± SD. To allow comparison of the data regardless of differences in body size, we performed normalisation on the RMS of CoP-and ACC-related variables by dividing the RMS by the participants’ body height (BH). Before comparing each variable, the normality of the data was verified with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test showed that all variables were normal. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to examine the relationship between the participants’ age and CoP-or ACC-related variables. An absolute correlation coefficient < 0.20 was considered as very weak, that between 0.20–0.39 as weak, that between 0.40–0.59 as moderate, that between 0.60–0.79 as strong, and that >0.80 as very strong (Evans, 1996). A raw p < 0.05 and a Benjamini–Hochberg false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) of <15% were used to indicate statistical significance. A false discovery rate threshold of 15% was chosen to avoid a type II error. In the text, tables and figures, raw p-values are reported.

3 Results

Figure 2 shows the scatter plots of the CoPRMS against the participants’ age for all participants (N = 46). In the AP direction, the CoPRMS had no significant correlation with age during either the EO or EC condition (EO: r = 0.176, p = 0.241; EC: r = 0.242, p = 0.106). In contrast, in the ML direction, the CoPRMS had a significant weak positive correlation with age during both the EO and EC conditions (EO: r = 0.393, p = 0.007; EC: r = 0.298, p = 0.045). The reverse was evident when the relationships between the participants’ age and the ACCRMS were examined, as shown in Figure 3. In the AP direction, the ACCRMS had a significant moderate positive correlation with age during both the EO and EC conditions (EO: r = 0.498, p < 0.001; EC: r = 0.434, p = 0.003). In contrast, in the ML direction, the ACCRMS had no significant correlation with age during either the EO or EC condition (EO: r = 0.266, p = 0.074; EC: r = 0.260, p = 0.081).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatter plots of the CoPRMS against the participants’ age (left-hand panels; AP direction, right-hand panels; ML direction). The open and filled circles indicate the EO and EC conditions, respectively. In the AP direction, the CoPRMS had no significant correlation with age (EO: r = 0.176, p = 0.241; EC: r = 0.242, p = 0.106). In the ML direction, the CoPRMS had a significant positive correlation with age (EO: r = 0.393, p = 0.007; EC: r = 0.298, p = 0.045). AP, anterior–posterior; BH, body height; CoPRMS, amplitude of postural sway; EC, eyes open; EO, eyes closed; ML, mediolateral.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatter plots of the ACCRMS against the participants’ age (left-hand panels: AP direction, right-hand panels: ML direction). The open and filled circles indicate the EO and EC conditions, respectively. In the AP direction, the ACCRMS had a significant positive correlation with age (EO: r = 0.498, p < 0.001; EC: r = 0.434, p = 0.003). In the ML direction, the ACCRMS had no significant correlation with age (EO: r = 0.266, p = 0.074; EC: r = 0.260, p = 0.081). ACCRMS, level of regulatory activity; AP, anterior–posterior; BH, body height; EC, eyes open; EO, eyes closed; ML, mediolateral.

In the AP direction, the MPF of CoP (CoPMPF) had a significant weak positive correlation with the participants’ age (EO only: r = 0.308, p = 0.037) (Table 1). In contrast, in the ML direction, the CoPMPF had a significant weak negative correlation with age (EO: r = −0.394, p = 0.007; EC: r = −0.299, p = 0.044) (Table 1). A naïve interpretation of these results might lead to the conclusion that, in the AP direction, the frequency of regulatory activity increased with increasing age, whereas, in the ML direction, the frequency decreased with age. However, this is not necessarily true. In fact, the MPF of ACC (ACCMPF) had no significant correlation with age in either the AP or ML direction (Table 1). The mechanisms of significant age-related changes in the CoPMPF can be explained if the CoP sway is decomposed into the LF and HF components before considering the age-related changes in their amplitude (i.e., the RMS) and frequency (i.e., the MPF). From Table 1, we can see that the MPF of both the LF and HF components had no significant correlation with age in either the AP or ML direction. In the AP direction, however, the RMS of the HF component had a significant weak-to-moderate positive correlation with age (EO: r = 0.423, p = 0.003; EC: r = 0.395, p = 0.007) (Table 2). In contrast, in the ML direction, the RMS of the LF component had a significant weak-to-moderate positive correlation with age (EO: r = 0.425, p = 0.003; EC: r = 0.323, p = 0.029) (Table 2). In summing up, the LF and HF components of the CoP sway did not change their temporal characteristics with age in either direction. However, the LF component in the ML direction and the HF component in the AP direction slightly increased their amplitude with increasing age. Thus, with advancing age, the LF component in the ML direction and the HF component in the AP direction increased their relative dominance in the frequency spectrum of the CoP sway, thereby resulting in lower CoPMPF and higher CoPMPF values in the ML and AP directions, respectively.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the MPF of CoP-and ACC-related variables and their correlations with the participants’ age.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of the RMS of LF and HF components and their correlations with the participants’ age.

4 Discussion

The results showed that, in the AP direction, the amplitude of postural sway (measured by the CoPRMS) had no significant correlation with the participants’ age (Figure 2, left-hand panels), whereas the amplitude of regulatory activity (measured by the ACCRMS) had a significant moderate positive correlation with age (Figure 3, left-hand panels). Our findings are in line with previous studies comparing static balance between younger and older adults (Prieto et al., 1996; Masani et al., 2007; Kouzaki and Masani, 2012). For example, Masani et al. (2007) demonstrated that there was no statistical difference in the SD of CoP between the younger (29.0 ± 7.7 years) and older (72.7 ± 5.6 years) adult groups, whereas there was a significant difference in the SD of ACC between the two groups. The findings of this and other previous studies suggest that, in the AP direction, the postural control system in older individuals maintains the amplitude of postural sway within a certain range by increasing the level of regulatory activity. In other words, the level of regulatory activity associated with a certain level of stability may increase with advancing age.

In the ML direction, the sway amplitude had a significant weak positive correlation with age (Figure 2, right-hand panels), whereas the amplitude of regulatory activity did not have a significant correlation with age (Figure 3, right-hand panels). This slightly increased sway amplitude with increasing age in the ML direction is consistent with the recent report of Brech et al. (2022) in which statistically significant difference was found in the ML directional CoP displacement between the women in their sixties and eighties. However, our present results are partly contrary to a previous study (Prieto et al., 1996) in which no significant differences were found in any of ten measures based on the ML directional component of the CoP (including the CoPRMS) between the younger (26.4 ± 4.9 years) and older (68.0 ± 1.3 years) adult groups. The discrepancy between the previous (Prieto et al., 1996) and present studies may be attributed partly to the difference in the age of the participants; in the present study they were substantially older (79.2 ± 6.1 years) than the participants of Prieto et al. (1996). This discrepancy, at the same time, suggests that the CoPRMS in the ML direction may increase steeply after 70 years of age. The slightly increased sway amplitude together with unchanged levels of regulatory activity in the ML direction further suggests that the postural control system is not sensitive to the sway amplitude in this direction, which is in marked contrast to the control in the AP direction. Researchers (Day et al., 1993; Winter et al., 1996) have pointed out that the balance controls in the AP and ML directions are completely different processes involving two independent neuromuscular groups. This difference in the balance control between the AP and ML directions is due largely to a difference in the structure of the human body between the sagittal and frontal planes. In particular, independent motion is possible at the ankle, knee and hip joints in the sagittal plane, whereas this is not possible in the frontal plane because of the mechanical linkage of the ankle and hip joints (Day et al., 1993). Furthermore, it is important to note that a loss of balance in the ML direction can more easily lead to a fall because the loaded supporting leg is in the direction of fall (Winter et al., 1996). In fact, it has been shown that the ML directional CoPRMS in a blindfolded condition was the best predictor of future fall risk in older people (Maki et al., 1994). Other studies have also demonstrated that the ML directional sway in narrow stance was increased in older people with a history of falls (Lord et al., 1999; Melzer et al., 2004). Combined with the findings of the previous studies (i.e., relationship between the ML directional sway and the falling risk of older people) and that of our present study (i.e., slightly increased ML directional sway with increasing age), we are now confident that the changes in ML balance occurring throughout old age can provide an insight into the increasing incidence of falls with age.

The results further showed that the CoPMPF had a significant weak positive correlation with the participants’ age in the AP direction, whereas the CoPMPF had a significant weak negative correlation with age in the ML direction (Table 1). However, this result does not mean that the frequency of regulatory activity changes with advancing age. Rather, the changes in CoPMPF observed in old age result from the marginal increase in the amplitude (but not frequency) of the LF (in the ML direction) and HF (in the AP direction) components (Table 2). In fact, the ACCMPF had no significant correlation with age in either the AP or ML direction (Table 1). Nevertheless, in the AP direction, the ACCMPF of the older participants (1.112 ± 0.274 Hz, the data from the EO and EC conditions were averaged) tended to be lower than those of young adults (1.3 ± 0.2 Hz) and children (1.6 ± 0.2 Hz) reported in Oba et al. (2015) [note that the ACCMPF values reported in Oba et al., 2015 were also obtained by averaging the data from eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions]. The difference in ACCMPF in the AP direction between the older participants and younger ones is presumably due to the redundancy in the multi-joint dynamics inherent in the sagittal plane as discussed above. When the human upright stance in the sagittal plane is modelled as a double-link inverted pendulum consisting of the leg and head–arms–trunk segments (Sasagawa et al., 2009, 2014), there are two coordination modes between these segments known as the in-phase and anti-phase coordination modes (Suzuki et al., 2012). The in-phase coordination mode, in which the leg and head–arms–trunk segments move in the same direction, has an eigenfrequency of below 0.5 Hz (Günther et al., 2009, 2011; Suzuki et al., 2012, 2016; Morasso et al., 2019). In contrast, the anti-phase coordination mode, in which the two segments move in opposite directions, has an eigenfrequency of around 1.0 Hz (Günther et al., 2009, 2011; Suzuki et al., 2012, 2016; Morasso et al., 2019). Therefore, the lower ACCMPF seen in the older participants suggests that the in-phase coordination mode increases its relative dominance in controlling upright balance, and thus these individuals sway more like a single-link inverted pendulum rotating around the ankle joint during quiet standing.

In conclusion, we found that static balance in older women aged from their early sixties to their late eighties exhibits distinctly contrasting aging patterns between the AP and ML directions; i.e., an unchanged sway amplitude with an increased level of regulatory activity in the AP direction, and a slightly increased sway amplitude with an unchanged level of regulatory activity in the ML direction. Based on the literature and our present findings, we suggest that to prevent falls in older women, it is necessary to elucidate physiological mechanisms responsible for the increase in the ML sway throughout old age.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Ethics Committee of Kanagawa University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AA: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AF: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) [grant number 17K01479] to SS and a Takahashi Industrial and Economic Research Foundation grant [grant number 12-003-40] to YM.

Acknowledgments

We thank Yuzaburo Omori for his help in the execution of this study. We also thank Takumi Kitano, Kokoa Miura, and Chinari Seno for their help in data acquisition. We thank Lorraine Law, BSc, from Edanz (https://jp.edanz.com/ac) for editing a draft of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Brech, G. C., Bobbio, T. G., Cabral, K. N., Coutinho, P. M., Castro, L. R., Mochizuki, L., et al. (2022). Changes in postural balance associated with a woman's aging process. Clinics (São Paulo) 77:100041. doi: 10.1016/j.clinsp.2022.100041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caron, O., Faure, B., and Breniere, Y. (1997). Estimating the Centre of gravity of the body on the basis of the Centre of pressure in standing posture. J. Biomech. 30, 1169–1171. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(97)00094-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, B. L., Steiger, M. J., Thompson, P. D., and Marsden, C. D. (1993). Effect of vision and stance width on human body motion when standing: implications for afferent control of lateral sway. J. Physiol. 469, 479–499. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019824

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, J. D. (1996). Straightforward Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. Pacific Grove, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.

Google Scholar

Farmer, M. E., White, L. R., Brody, J. A., and Bailey, K. R. (1984). Race and sex differences in hip fracture incidence. Am. J. Public Health 74, 1374–1380. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.74.12.1374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Günther, M., Grimmer, S., Siebert, T., and Blickhan, R. (2009). All leg joints contribute to quiet human stance: a mechanical analysis. J. Biomech. 42, 2739–2746. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.08.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Günther, M., Muller, O., and Blickhan, R. (2011). Watching quiet human stance to shake off its straitjacket. Arch. Appl. Mech. 81, 283–302. doi: 10.1007/s00419-010-0414-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hufschmidt, A., Dichgans, J., Mauritz, K. H., and Hufschmidt, M. (1980). Some methods and parameters of body sway quantification and their neurological applications. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 228, 135–150. doi: 10.1007/BF00365601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Japanese Cabinet Office (2022). Japanese Cabinet Office’s Annual Report on the Ageing Society in 2022. Available at: https://www8.cao.go.jp/kourei/english/annualreport/2022/pdf/2022.pdf (Accessed December 25, 2023).

Google Scholar

Kouzaki, M., and Masani, K. (2012). Postural sway during quiet standing is related to physiological tremor and muscle volume in young and elderly adults. Gait Posture 35, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, A. D., Speers, R. A., Peterka, R. J., and Horak, F. B. (1998). Effect of altered sensory conditions on multivariate descriptors of human postural sway. Exp. Brain Res. 122, 185–195. doi: 10.1007/s002210050506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loram, I. D., and Lakie, M. (2002). Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: position control by small, ballistic-like, throw and catch movements. J. Physiol. 540, 1111–1124. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013077

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S. R., Rogers, M. W., Howland, A., and Fitzpatrick, R. (1999). Lateral stability, sensorimotor function and falls in older people. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 1077–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb05230.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maki, B. E., Holliday, P. J., and Topper, A. K. (1994). A prospective-study of postural balance and risk of falling in an ambulatory and independent elderly population. J. Gerontol. 49, M72–M84. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.2.M72

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masani, K., Vette, A. H., Abe, M. O., and Nakazawa, K. (2014). Center of pressure velocity reflects body acceleration rather than body velocity during quiet standing. Gait Posture 39, 946–952. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.12.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masani, K., Vette, A. H., Kouzaki, M., Kanehisa, H., Fukunaga, T., and Popovic, M. R. (2007). Larger center of pressure minus center of gravity in the elderly induces larger body acceleration during quiet standing. Neurosci. Lett. 422, 202–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.06.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, C., and Peterka, R. J. (2005). A new interpretation of spontaneous sway measures based on a simple model of human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 189–200. doi: 10.1152/jn.00221.2004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcclenaghan, B. A., Williams, H. G., Dickerson, J., Dowda, M., Thombs, L., and Eleazer, P. (1996). Spectral characteristics of aging postural control. Gait Posture 4, 112–121. doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(95)01040-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Melzer, I., Benjuya, N., and Kaplanski, J. (2004). Postural stability in the elderly: a comparison between fallers and non-fallers. Age Ageing 33, 602–607. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afh218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morasso, P., Cherif, A., and Zenzeri, J. (2019). Quiet standing: the single inverted pendulum model is not so bad after all. PLoS One 14:e0213870. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213870

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morasso, P. G., and Schieppati, M. (1999). Can muscle stiffness alone stabilize upright standing? J. Neurophysiol. 82, 1622–1626. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1622

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oba, N., Sasagawa, S., Yamamoto, A., and Nakazawa, K. (2015). Difference in postural control during quiet standing between young children and adults: assessment with Center of Mass Acceleration. PLoS One 10:e0140235. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prieto, T. E., Myklebust, J. B., Hoffmann, R. G., Lovett, E. G., and Myklebust, B. M. (1996). Measures of postural steadiness: differences between healthy young and elderly adults. I.E.E.E. Trans. Biomed. Eng. 43, 956–966. doi: 10.1109/10.532130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quijoux, F., Nicolai, A., Chairi, I., Bargiotas, I., Ricard, D., Yelnik, A., et al. (2021). A review of center of pressure (COP) variables to quantify standing balance in elderly people: algorithms and open-access code. Physiol. Rep. 9:e15067. doi: 10.14814/phy2.15067

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasagawa, S., Shinya, M., and Nakazawa, K. (2014). Interjoint dynamic interaction during constrained human quiet standing examined by induced acceleration analysis. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 313–322. doi: 10.1152/jn.01082.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasagawa, S., Ushiyama, J., Kouzaki, M., and Kanehisa, H. (2009). Effect of the hip motion on the body kinematics in the sagittal plane during human quiet standing. Neurosci. Lett. 450, 27–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.11.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheffer, A. C., Schuurmans, M. J., Van Dijk, N., Van Der Hooft, T., and De Rooij, S. E. (2008). Fear of falling: measurement strategy, prevalence, risk factors and consequences among older persons. Age Ageing 37, 19–24. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm169

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, Y., Morimoto, H., Kiyono, K., Morasso, P. G., and Nomura, T. (2016). Dynamic determinants of the uncontrolled manifold during human quiet stance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:618. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00618

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, Y., Nomura, T., Casadio, M., and Morasso, P. (2012). Intermittent control with ankle, hip, and mixed strategies during quiet standing: a theoretical proposal based on a double inverted pendulum model. J. Theor. Biol. 310, 55–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.06.019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tokyo Fire Department (2020). Tokyo fire Department’s Report on Emergency Transportation for Elderly People. Available at: https://www.tfd.metro.tokyo.lg.jp/lfe/topics/202009/kkhansoudeta.html (Accessed December 25, 2023).

Google Scholar

Vellas, B. J., Wayne, S. J., Romero, L. J., Baumgartner, R. N., and Garry, P. J. (1997). Fear of falling and restriction of mobility in elderly fallers. Age Ageing 26, 189–193. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.3.189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, D. A., Prince, F., Frank, J. S., Powell, C., and Zabjek, K. F. (1996). Unified theory regarding a/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. J. Neurophysiol. 75, 2334–2343. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.6.2334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada-Yanagawa, A., Sasagawa, S., Nakazawa, K., and Ishii, N. (2022). Effects of occasional and habitual wearing of high-heeled shoes on static balance in young women. Front Sports Act Living 4:760991. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2022.760991

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, E., Abe, M., Masani, K., Kawashima, N., Eto, F., Haga, N., et al. (2008). Evaluation of postural control in quiet standing using center of mass acceleration: comparison among the young, the elderly, and people with stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 1133–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.10.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatsiorsky, V. M., and Duarte, M. (2000). Rambling and trembling in quiet standing. Mot. Control. 4, 185–200. doi: 10.1123/mcj.4.2.185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: posture, quiet standing, center of pressure, fall, women

Citation: Sasagawa S, Arakawa A, Furuyama A and Matsumoto Y (2024) Age-related changes in static balance in older women aged in their early sixties to their late eighties: different aging patterns in the anterior–posterior and mediolateral directions. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1361244. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1361244

Received: 05 January 2024; Accepted: 28 March 2024;
Published: 08 April 2024.

Edited by:

Guang H. Yue, Kessler Foundation, United States

Reviewed by:

Kiran Karunakaran, Kessler Foundation, United States
Elisabeth Kaminski, Leipzig University, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Sasagawa, Arakawa, Furuyama and Matsumoto. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shun Sasagawa, sasagawa@kanagawa-u.ac.jp

Download