Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 07 March 2022
Sec. Behavioral Endocrinology
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.826831

Selectivity and Sociality: Aggression and Affiliation Shape Vole Social Relationships

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Colgate University, Hamilton, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States

The formation of selective social relationships is not a requirement of group living; sociality can be supported by motivation for social interaction in the absence of preferences for specific individuals, and by tolerance in place of social motivation. For species that form selective social relationships, these can be maintained by preference for familiar partners, as well as by avoidance of or aggression toward individuals outside of the social bond. In this review, we explore the roles that aggression, motivation, and tolerance play in the maintenance of selective affiliation. We focus on prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) as rodent species that both exhibit the unusual tendency to form selective social relationships, but differ with regard to mating system. These species provide an opportunity to investigate the mechanisms that underlie social relationships, and to compare mechanisms supporting pair bonds with mates and same-sex peer relationships. We then relate this to the role of aggression in group composition in a comparative context.

Introduction

Sociality (a.k.a. group living) takes many forms across social species, such that groups differ in size, composition, and the role of specific, selective relationships. Many attempts have been made to characterize types of social groups—for example, distinguishing between those that are large, transient, and gregarious vs. those that are smaller, more stable, and comprised of defined roles or relationships (Hinde, 1976; Lidicker and Patton, 1987; Lee, 1994; Lacey and Sherman, 2007; Clutton-Brock and Lukas, 2012; Hofmann et al., 2014; Lukas and Clutton-Brock, 2018; Kappeler, 2019; Bales et al., 2021). As would be expected from the diversity of group types, there are multiple neurobiological routes to supporting life in social groups. By taking advantage of natural variations in social behavior, we can hope to better understand both the unity and diversity in the biological underpinnings of different types of sociality. In this review we focus on lessons from selective social groups, and the interplay between affiliation and aggression in the maintenance of selectivity.

Only about 5–10% of rodent species are classified as group living (Lacey and Sherman, 2007; Lukas and Clutton-Brock, 2013). Among those, many species (including the most commonly used laboratory rodents) are gregariously social, forming loose aggregations with conspecifics without maintenance of repeated, targeted social interactions (Lee, 1994). Thus, only a small percentage of rodent species display the enduring and specific “selective” relationships that make it possible to study mechanisms underlying social bonding. Selective social preferences for known individuals are often assessed in laboratory settings using the partner preference test, a 3-h social choice test between an unfamiliar “stranger” and a familiar “partner” in a three-chambered apparatus (Williams et al., 1992; Beery, 2021). Selective preference for familiar individuals has been most commonly demonstrated in the genus Microtus, including socially monogamous prairie voles and mandarin voles, and seasonally social meadow voles (opposite-sex: Williams et al., 1992; Parker et al., 2001; Yu et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2018; same-sex: Parker and Lee, 2003; Beery et al., 2009; Beery and Shambaugh, 2021). Selectively social species such as voles spend significantly more time with partners than with strangers in these tests, whereas mice, rats, and degus do not prefer familiar individuals under typical conditions, and may prefer social novelty (Moy et al., 2004; Smith et al., 2015; Schweinfurth et al., 2017; Beery et al., 2018; Insel et al., 2020; Beery and Shambaugh, 2021; Hackenberg et al., 2021).

Depending on context and social structure, aggression can serve to either promote or reduce social behavior (Figure 1). When social relationships are stable and selective, social groups may be closed or less flexible, and therefore aggression may serve to promote social behavior among members of a social group by excluding outsiders. On the other hand, lack of familiarity preference may be a key component of flexible and gregarious social groups, such as those formed by mice, rats, degus, and spiny mice (Beery et al., 2018; Insel et al., 2020; Kelly and Seifert, 2021; Lidhar et al., 2021). In these kinds of groups, aggression reduces social behavior; conversely, reduction in aggression promotes gregariousness. We focus on the role of selective aggression in affiliative social behavior in voles, then discuss how reduction in aggression and anxiety promote social behavior in gregarious animals more broadly.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Together with affiliation, aggression shapes the presence and nature of social groups. The vast majority of rodents are solitary, such as the Syrian hamster. Differences in the amount or extent of aggression (x-axis) shape group formation, with low aggression and territoriality promoting gregarious group structures (e.g., mice, rats, and spiny mice) that may be flexible in size and composition. The generality vs. specificity (y-axis) of how aggression is targeted shapes the selectivity of groups for specific known members. Selectivity may be maintained by “prosocial” factors such as motivation for social bonds, as well as by lack of aggression and intolerance toward group members vs. strangers. Highly selective species such as prairie voles exhibit both high levels of affiliation and stranger aggression, while less selective groups such as winter-phenotype meadow voles show preference for familiar individuals in the absence of social motivation or intense aggression. Photo credits: solitary rodent (Syrian hamster; The Rohit CC-BY NC 2.0), gregarious groups (Mice; Pixabay user Kapa65; Spiny mice; Aubrey Kelly by permission), selective groups (meadow voles and prairie voles; Beery Lab).

Specificity of Aggression Promotes Selective Groups

Selective preference for familiar individuals may result from aggression toward and avoidance of unfamiliar conspecifics, as well as potential prosocial motivation toward familiar individuals (Figure 1, selective groups). Selective social motivation vs. “tolerance” can be difficult to distinguish, and understanding their relative contributions has benefited from distinct assessment of social motivation, preference, and aggression.

Highly Selective Partnerships: Affiliation and Aggression in Monogamous Prairie Voles

Prairie voles are socially monogamous rodents that have been well-studied for their pair bonding behavior and biparental care (reviewed in Carter, 2017; Gobrogge et al., 2017; Walum and Young, 2018; Kenkel et al., 2021). Prairie voles are native to grasslands, primarily in the midwestern United States, where repeated live trapping has revealed that social groups are often comprised of male-female reproductive pairs, characterized by overlapping home ranges, shared nests, territoriality, and mate-guarding (Getz et al., 1993; Getz and Carter, 1996; Madrid et al., 2020). In addition to mate partnerships, prairie voles can also form non-reproductive relationships with same-sex conspecifics or “peers” (DeVries et al., 1997; Beery et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2019); these female-female relationships are selective and enduring, much like pair bonds with mates. Laboratory studies indicate that relationships in prairie voles are maintained by stranger aggression and reinforced by social reward, leading to high levels of selectivity (Figure 1, highly selective partnership).

Selective aggression occurs in males and females, in the field and lab, and is directed at same- and opposite-sex conspecifics (reviewed in Young et al., 2011). It may promote mate-guarding, territory defense, and more generally maintain the pair bond. Experiments in male prairie voles have elucidated the timing and target-specificity of this response. Aggression is elevated following cohabitation with a mate; males housed with a female for 24 h exhibited significantly more stranger-directed aggression in a resident-intruder test than unmated males (Winslow et al., 1993; Insel et al., 1995; Wang et al., 1997). Although mated males exhibited aggression toward strangers of both sexes, female-directed aggression after a 24-h cohousing interval was less intense than male-directed aggression (Wang et al., 1997). However, after 2 weeks of cohabitation with a mate, males displayed intense aggression toward even female strangers (Aragona et al., 2006).

Maternal/pregnant females and females paired with males (with or without mating for different intervals) also displayed increased aggression toward female strangers compared to females housed with females (Bowler et al., 2002). This aggression peaked after 12 days of opposite-sex cohabitation. In trios consisting of two sibling or non-sibling females and an unrelated male housed together for 3 days (both sexes sexually experienced), non-sibling female groups exhibited significantly more female-female aggression than sibling female groups (Firestone et al., 1991). This suggests that the presence of a male reduces female tolerance for unfamiliar females.

The mechanisms underlying both affiliation and aggression in prairie vole social relationships have been extensively explored (Table 1), with many signaling pathways involved in both processes. For example, the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin are both implicated in social bond formation in prairie voles, as well as in aggression. Oxytocin has a long-appreciated role in pair bond formation in females (Williams et al., 1992; Insel and Hulihan, 1995) and more recently in males (Liu et al., 2001; Ophir et al., 2012; Duclot et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2016). There is growing appreciation that oxytocin mediates both prosocial and antisocial behaviors, and there is evidence for overlapping neural circuitry mediating these seemingly opposing but similarly oxytocin-dependent processes (Bartz et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2011; van Anders et al., 2013; Beery, 2015; Anacker et al., 2016a; de Jong and Neumann, 2018; Steinman et al., 2019). Several studies highlight a role of oxytocin in aggressive behavior in prairie voles. Female prairie voles treated with oxytocin shortly after birth exhibited more aggression toward a same-sex stranger in a neutral arena after brief exposure to a male, compared to control or oxytocin antagonist-treated females (Bales and Carter, 2003). Oxytocin receptor density in the BNST of female prairie voles was associated with increased aggression toward unfamiliar voles (Beery et al., 2021), and in males, an Oxtr genotype that is associated with striatal oxytocin receptor expression and bond formation (King et al., 2016; Ahern et al., 2021) is also strongly associated with aggression toward strangers (Vahaba et al., 2021). Oxytocin also mediates aggression and social anxiety in many other rodent species, including rats, mice, naked mole rats, California mice, hamsters, and Mongolian gerbils (reviewed in Campbell, 2008; de Jong and Neumann, 2018; Steinman et al., 2019).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. A summary of the signaling pathways involved in prairie vole affiliation and aggression.

The role of vasopressin in aggressive behavior was one of its earliest known social functions in the brains of mammals (Albers, 2012). In male prairie voles, activation of V1aR receptors in the ventral pallidum is necessary for pair bond formation (Lim and Young, 2004). Pair bonding also leads to an increase of vasopressin in the anterior hypothalamus of male prairie voles, where activation of V1a receptors promotes aggressive behavior, while blockade of V1a receptors decreases aggressive behavior (Gobrogge et al., 2009). Across species, the role of vasopressin in aggression is a topic of extensive study.

Selective post-mating aggression in prairie voles is also mediated by dopaminergic and opioid signaling pathways. Pair bond maintenance was associated with upregulation of dopamine D1 type receptors in the nucleus accumbens in male prairie voles (Aragona et al., 2006), and with upregulated mRNA expression of genes encoding D1-type receptors in males and females (Resendez et al., 2016). Peripheral blockade of κ-opioid receptors, as well as blockade in the nucleus accumbens shell, abolished selective aggression in prairie vole males and females (Resendez et al., 2012). Furthermore, κ-opioid receptors and D1 receptors interacted to maintain pair bonds (Resendez et al., 2016). Dopamine and vasopressin in the nucleus accumbens, anterior hypothalamus, and central amygdala have also been found to be involved in maintenance-related aggression in male prairie voles (reviewed in Gobrogge and Wang, 2011).

Corticosteroids also play an important role in prairie vole social behavior. For example, acute corticosterone administration has a sexually dimorphic effect on prairie vole pair bonding, facilitating the formation of partner preferences by males for females (without inducing male territorial aggression), but inhibiting bonding of females to males (DeVries et al., 1996; Blondel and Phelps, 2016). While the involvement of gonadal steroid hormones in aggression is well established in multiple species (reviewed in Soma et al., 2008), there is some evidence suggesting that they are less important in prairie vole aggression as well as affiliation (reviewed in Carter and Perkeybile, 2018). For example, castration failed to inhibit aggression or pair bonding in adult male prairie voles (Demas et al., 1999; Carter and Perkeybile, 2018). However, neonatal castration did successfully disrupt pair bonding (Cushing et al., 2003).

Although aggression mediates selectivity in prairie vole mate and peer relationships, the relative importance of prosocial motivation varies across relationship type. Prairie vole mate relationships rely on dopamine and opioid signaling (Aragona et al., 2003, 2006; Resendez et al., 2012, 2016), and are associated with behavioral reward in socially conditioned place preference (sCPP) tests and operant studies using a social reward (Ulloa et al., 2018; Goodwin et al., 2019; Beery et al., 2021). While dopamine signaling is not essential for the formation of peer relationships in prairie voles (Lee and Beery, 2021), female prairie voles find familiar peers more motivating than strangers or an empty chamber, and condition toward socially associated cues in the sCPP test (Beery et al., 2021; Lee and Beery, 2021). Because prairie voles direct their affiliation and aggression at specific targets, both aspects of behavior likely contribute to their highly selective social groups. Increased affiliation toward members of an ingroup and decreased affiliation toward outgroup individuals has also been demonstrated in humans (e.g., Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006; Dreu et al., 2010, 2011; Oaten et al., 2011; Hruschka and Henrich, 2013; Neuberg and Schaller, 2016) and other species.

Selective Groups: Selectivity and Flexibility in Meadow Vole Social Relationships

Meadow voles are not socially monogamous, and because they are closely related to, yet behaviorally distinct from prairie voles, they provide an opportunity to assess similarities and differences in behavior and mechanism across species and mating systems (e.g., Wang et al., 1994; Kingsbury et al., 2012; Inoue et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2019; Beery et al., 2021). Meadow voles also provide a within-species opportunity to examine mechanisms underlying variation in social grouping, as they exhibit seasonal shifts in social behavior. Meadow voles live in grassy fields, woodlands, and marshes throughout most of Canada and northern and eastern parts of the United States. Wild meadow voles are intolerant of conspecifics during the reproductive season: In the summer (long day lengths, LD), females in particular form exclusive territories and (except for mating) are aggressive toward adult voles of both sexes. However, in the winter (short day lengths, SD), meadow voles form tolerant, mixed-sex communal huddling groups, have overlapping home ranges, and share nests with conspecifics (Madison, 1980; McShea and Madison, 1984; Ferkin and Seamon, 1987; Madison and McShea, 1987). These groups typically begin with undispersed family members joined by immigrant males (Madison et al., 1984), but by early January, predation and acceptance of new group members lead to groups that are no longer kin-based. In the Spring, groups close to new members (Madison and McShea, 1987). Meadow vole winter social groups are thus selective, but somewhat flexible (Figure 1).

Seasonal changes in social organization in wild meadow voles are mirrored in the laboratory under changing photoperiods: short day lengths promote greater social huddling and reduced anxiety behavior (Parker and Lee, 2003; Ossenkopp et al., 2005; Beery et al., 2008, 2009; Lee et al., 2019). Because of this daylength-dependent variation in social group formation, meadow voles have been used to study the environmental and neural factors that alter the propensity to form selective same-sex relationships (Beery and Zucker, 2010; Beery et al., 2014; Ondrasek et al., 2015; Anacker et al., 2016a; Beery, 2019).

Meadow voles exhibit selective preferences for familiar same-sex and opposite-sex individuals in partner preference tests (Parker and Lee, 2003; Beery et al., 2009; Stetzik et al., 2018; Beery and Shambaugh, 2021), and can simultaneously form such preferences for multiple familiar individuals (Beery et al., 2009). Interestingly, relative preference for the partner vs. the stranger is sometimes found in both SD- and LD-housed females (Ondrasek et al., 2015; Goodwin et al., 2019), although SD-housed females may huddle more than their LD-housed counterparts with both partners and strangers (Beery et al., 2008). Increased social huddling, including tolerance of strangers, is an important factor in winter group formation, as meadow vole groups accommodate immigration throughout the winter before becoming fixed in early spring (Madison and McShea, 1987). Meadow voles thus exhibit both selective, familiarity-based affiliation, and increased social flexibility and gregariousness during winter months.

While meadow voles naturally cohabit in winter social groups, social reward does not appear to play an important role in group formation. Meadow voles housed in SDs in the lab show no sign of social reinforcement in either sCPP tests (Goodwin et al., 2019) or in operant conditioning tests with social rewards (Beery et al., 2021). Partner preferences for same-sex peers are also dopamine-independent in this species (Beery and Zucker, 2010). Thus, social motivation does not seem to explain winter grouping behavior.

Social tolerance—mediated via seasonal reduction in anxiety and aggression—provides a better explanation for winter sociality, and seasonal changes in tolerance and aggression have been documented in multiple field studies (Madison, 1980; McShea and Madison, 1984; Ferkin and Seamon, 1987; Madison and McShea, 1987; McShea, 1990). Laboratory studies provide several lines of support for this hypothesis. Behaviorally, meadow voles housed in short day lengths exhibit greater social interaction with strangers and less anxiety-like behavior (Ossenkopp et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2019), while exogenous stressors that elevate corticosterone signaling impair the formation of new non-reproductive relationships (Anacker et al., 2016b). Anxiety may be modulated by photoperiod-mediated changes in HPA axis activity: glucocorticoid metabolites are lower in SD-housed voles and neural receptor densities of corticotropin releasing factor receptors (CRF1 and CRF2) change with day length in opposing directions, consistent with seasonal decline in anxiety (Beery et al., 2014; Anacker et al., 2016b). Decreased anxiety and increased investigation likely support the formation of groups, as well as the continued slow acceptance of new members throughout early winter.

Interspecific Comparisons

The relative importance of factors promoting vs. opposing social interaction differs across vole species, with both selective aggression and social motivation playing a larger role in prairie voles compared to meadow voles. While both prairie and meadow voles form partner preferences for same-sex peers, studies comparing peer relationships in these species indicate that prairie voles spend more time in contact with a partner than do meadow voles (Beery and Shambaugh, 2021). As described above, social reward plays a role in mate and peer relationships in prairie but not meadow voles, and social reward may play a larger role in mate vs. peer relationships in the former species (Goodwin et al., 2019; Beery et al., 2021; Lee and Beery, 2021). Specific affiliation plays an important role in determining who is in a group.

Just as factors promoting social interaction differ between species, so do factors preventing interaction. Selective aggression toward unfamiliar same-sex conspecifics was higher in social interaction tests in prairie voles than in meadow voles (Lee et al., 2019), and female prairie voles exhibited significantly more stranger-directed aggression than meadow voles in operant conditioning trials when they gained access to the stranger chamber (Beery et al., 2021). Like prairie vole mate relationships, peer relationships in this species are maintained in part by aggression toward unfamiliar individuals. In contrast, social tolerance is an important feature of meadow vole peer affiliation, demonstrated by low aggression toward unfamiliar conspecifics, and suggested by field data on winter tolerance. This low aggression may be particularly important in shaping group size, or how closed groups are to new members. Together, affiliation and aggression may substantially shape social groups, thereby contributing to potential interspecific differences in the nature of these groups.

Reduction of Aggression and Anxiety Promotes Gregarious Interactions

For gregarious groups, familiarity does not play a central role in group formation. Instead, gregarious social groups may result from a generalized lack of aggression, together with prosocial factors (e.g., motivation) that more actively promote grouping (Figure 1, gregarious groups). In many species, there is an inverse relationship between aggression and social behavior—that is, aggression and social anxiety drive social avoidance, and a tendency to be solitary, while the absence of aggression in combination with other factors may lead to the formation of social groups. Opposing roles of affiliation and aggression/anxiety have been documented in rodents and birds (detailed below), among many other taxonomic groups.

In rodents, the relationship between anxiety and social interaction is so well-defined that the primary use of the social interaction test is as an indicator of anxiety, with low social interaction (e.g., sniffing or grooming) associated with high anxiety (reviewed in File and Seth, 2003). This association also forms the basis for experimental manipulations investigating anxiogenic and anxiolytic effects of specific factors of interest, especially in conjunction with other common tests of anxiety-like behavior in rodents. For example, in rats, greater social contact in the social interaction test is often correlated with lower anxiety in measures such as the elevated plus maze and light-dark box (Starr-Phillips and Beery, 2014; Sparling et al., 2018). Rats that exhibited higher anxiety-like behavior were also slower to learn helping behavior in the form of freeing a trapped conspecific from a restrainer (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2014).

Aggression is also associated with reduced social behavior. Studies in rats have shown that central oxytocin reduces aggression and increases affiliative behavior, although endogenous oxytocin signaling also plays a critical role in promoting aggression (e.g., Calcagnoli et al., 2015; Oliveira et al., 2021). Aggression has also been hypothesized to shape group size in birds, many of which exhibit seasonal differences in social behavior, from exclusive territories in the breeding season to flocks of several to thousands of individuals in the non-breeding season (Goodson and Kingsbury, 2011; Goodson et al., 2012a,b). There is evidence that oxytocin (Goodson et al., 2009) and vasopressin (Kelly et al., 2011) support larger flock sizes in gregarious zebra finches. Furthermore, in comparisons across territorial and gregarious (i.e., flocking) finch species, nonapeptides have been found to mediate flocking behavior. For example, differences in oxytocin receptor binding density in the lateral septum were found between territorial and gregarious finches (Goodson et al., 2009). Thus, while amount/extent of aggression may determine group sizes—as in seasonally flocking finches—specificity of aggression—as in voles—shapes group membership.

Conclusion

The role of aggression in social behavior is dependent on species and social context. Aggression can reduce social interactions by promoting avoidance and solitary living. Conversely, reduction in aggression may support group living in the form of gregarious and flexible social groups. Aggression also plays an essential role in promoting selective social behavior—at topic that has received less attention. In prairie voles, aggression toward strangers enhances selectivity toward partners. The winter transition to sociality in species such as meadow voles may rely on relaxation of selective aggression as well as reduction in anxiety to promote larger and more flexible groups.

Author Contributions

Both authors conceived, wrote, and edited this mini-review.

Funding

Publication of this manuscript was supported in part by the Colgate University Research Council (to NL). The authors were also supported by award R15MH113085 from the National Institutes of Health (to AB).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Eileen Lacey and the two reviewers for their helpful feedback on this manuscript.

References

Ahern, T. H., Olsen, S., Tudino, R., and Beery, A. K. (2021). Natural variation in the oxytocin receptor gene and rearing interact to influence reproductive and nonreproductive social behavior and receptor binding. Psychoneuroendocrinology 128:105209. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albers, H. E. (2012). The regulation of social recognition, social communication and aggression: vasopressin in the social behavior neural network. Hormones Behav. 61, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anacker, A. M. J., Christensen, J. D., LaFlamme, E. M., Grunberg, D. M., and Beery, A. K. (2016a). Septal oxytocin administration impairs peer affiliation via V1a receptors in female meadow voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology 68, 156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anacker, A. M. J., Reitz, K. M., Goodwin, N. L., and Beery, A. K. (2016b). Stress impairs new but not established relationships in seasonally social voles. Hormones Behav. 79, 52–57. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aragona, B. J., Liu, Y., Curtis, J. T., Stephan, F. K., and Wang, Z. (2003). A critical role for nucleus accumbens dopamine in partner-preference formation in male prairie voles. J. Neurosci. 23, 3483–3490. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03483.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aragona, B. J., Liu, Y., Yu, Y. J., Curtis, J. T., Detwiler, J. M., Insel, T. R., et al. (2006). Nucleus accumbens dopamine differentially mediates the formation and maintenance of monogamous pair bonds. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 133–139. doi: 10.1038/nn1613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bales, K. L., Ardekani, C. S., Baxter, A., Karaskiewicz, C. L., Kuske, J. X., Lau, A. R., et al. (2021). What is a pair bond? Hormones Behav. 136:105062. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bales, K. L., and Carter, C. S. (2003). Sex differences and developmental effects of oxytocin on aggression and social behavior in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Hormones Behav. 44, 178–184. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00154-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Ochsner, K. N., Bolger, N., Kolevzon, A., Ludwig, N., et al. (2010). Effects of oxytocin on recollections of maternal care and closeness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 21371–21375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012669107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K. (2015). Antisocial oxytocin: complex effects on social behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 6, 174–182. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K. (2019). Frank Beach award winner: neuroendocrinology of group living. Hormones Behav. 107, 67–75. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K. (2021). Familiarity and mate preference assessment with the partner preference test. Curr. Protoc. 1:e173. doi: 10.1002/cpz1.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., Christensen, J. D., Lee, N. S., and Blandino, K. L. (2018). Specificity in sociality: mice and prairie voles exhibit different patterns of peer affiliation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:50. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., Loo, T. J., and Zucker, I. (2008). Day length and estradiol affect same-sex affiliative behavior in the female meadow vole. Hormones Behav. 54, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., Lopez, S. A., Blandino, K. L., Lee, N. S., and Bourdon, N. S. (2021). Social selectivity and social motivation in voles. eLife 10:e72684. doi: 10.7554/eLife.72684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., Routman, D. M., and Zucker, I. (2009). Same-sex social behavior in meadow voles: multiple and rapid formation of attachments. Physiol. Behav. 97, 52–57. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., and Shambaugh, K. L. (2021). Comparative assessment of familiarity/novelty preferences in rodents. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:648830. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.648830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., Vahaba, D. M., and Grunberg, D. M. (2014). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor densities vary with photoperiod and sociality. Hormones Behav. 66, 779–786. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., and Zucker, I. (2010). Oxytocin and same-sex social behavior in female meadow voles. Neuroscience 169, 665–673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Ami Bartal, I., Rodgers, D. A., Bernardez Sarria, M. S., Decety, J., and Mason, P. (2014). Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. eLife 3:e01385. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blondel, D. V., and Phelps, S. M. (2016). Effects of acute corticosterone treatment on male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster): territorial aggression does not accompany induced social preference. J. Comp. Psychol. 130, 400–406. doi: 10.1037/com0000048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowler, C. M., Cushing, B. S., and Carter, C. S. (2002). Social factors regulate female–female aggression and affiliation in prairie voles. Physiol. Behav. 76, 559–566. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00755-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calcagnoli, F., Kreutzmann, J. C., de Boer, S. F., Althaus, M., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2015). Acute and repeated intranasal oxytocin administration exerts anti-aggressive and pro-affiliative effects in male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 112–121. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A. (2008). Attachment, aggression and affiliation: the role of oxytocin in female social behavior. Biol. Psychol. 77, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S. (2017). The oxytocin–vasopressin pathway in the context of love and fear. Front. Endocrinol. 8:356. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., and Perkeybile, A. M. (2018). The monogamy paradox: what do love and sex have to do with it? Front. Ecol. Evol. 6:202. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2018.00202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. S., Kumsta, R., and Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin and intergroup relations: goodwill is not a fixed pie. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108:E45. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101633108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clutton-Brock, T. H., and Lukas, D. (2012). The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals. Mol. Ecol. 21, 472–492. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05232.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cushing, B. S., Okorie, U., and Young, L. J. (2003). The effects of neonatal castration on the subsequent behavioural response to centrally administered arginine vasopressin and the expression of v1a receptors in adult male prairie voles. J. Neuroendocrinol. 15, 1021–1026. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.01097.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong, T. R., and Neumann, I. D. (2018). “Oxytocin and aggression,” in Behavioral Pharmacology of Neuropeptides: Oxytocin Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, eds R. Hurlemann and V. Grinevich (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 175–192. doi: 10.1007/7854_2017_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demas, G. E., Moffatt, C. A., Drazen, D. L., and Nelson, R. J. (1999). Castration does not inhibit aggressive behavior in adult male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Physiol. Behav. 66, 59–62.

Google Scholar

DeVries, A. C., DeVries, M. B., Taymans, S. E., and Carter, C. S. (1996). The effects of stress on social preferences are sexually dimorphic in prairie voles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 11980–11984. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.21.11980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries, A. C., Johnson, C. L., and Carter, C. S. (1997). Familiarity and gender influence social preferences in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Can. J. Zool. 75, 295–301. doi: 10.1139/z97-037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreu, C. K. W. D., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J. J., Shalvi, S., Kleef, G. A. V., Baas, M., et al. (2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328, 1408–1411. doi: 10.1126/science.1189047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreu, C. K. W. D., Greer, L. L., Kleef, G. A. V., Shalvi, S., and Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. PNAS 108, 1262–1266. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015316108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duclot, F., Wang, H., Youssef, C., Liu, Y., Wang, Z., and Kabbaj, M. (2016). Trichostatin A (TSA) facilitates formation of partner preference in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Hormones Behav. 81, 68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., and Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 7, 333–353. doi: 10.1177/1368430204046142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferkin, M. H., and Seamon, J. O. (1987). Odor preference and social behavior in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus?: seasonal differences. Can. J. Zool. 65, 2931–2937. doi: 10.1139/z87-445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

File, S. E., and Seth, P. (2003). A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 463, 35–53. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01273-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firestone, K. B., Thompson, K. V., and Carter, C. S. (1991). Female—female interactions and social stress in praine voles. Behav. Neural Biol. 55, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(91)80125-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getz, L. L., and Carter, C. S. (1996). Prairie-vole partnerships. Am. Sci. 84, 56–62.

Google Scholar

Getz, L. L., McGuire, B., Pizzuto, T., Hofmann, J. E., and Frase, B. (1993). Social Organization of the Prairie Vole (Microtus ochrogaster). J. Mammal. 74, 44–58. doi: 10.2307/1381904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobrogge, K. L., Jia, X., Liu, Y., and Wang, Z. (2017). Neurochemical mediation of affiliation and aggression associated with pair-bonding. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 231–242. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobrogge, K. L., Liu, Y., Young, L. J., and Wang, Z. (2009). Anterior hypothalamic vasopressin regulates pair-bonding and drug-induced aggression in a monogamous rodent. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 19144–19149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908620106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobrogge, K. L., and Wang, Z. (2011). Genetics of aggression in voles. Adv. Genet. 75, 121–150. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-380858-5.00003-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., Kelly, A. M., and Kingsbury, M. A. (2012a). Evolving nonapeptide mechanisms of gregariousness and social diversity in birds. Hormones Behav. 61, 239–250. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., Wilson, L. C., and Schrock, S. E. (2012b). To flock or fight: neurochemical signatures of divergent life histories in sparrows. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 10685–10692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1203394109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., and Kingsbury, M. A. (2011). Nonapeptides and the evolution of social group sizes in birds. Front. Neuroanat. 5:13. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2011.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., Schrock, S. E., Klatt, J. D., Kabelik, D., and Kingsbury, M. A. (2009). Mesotocin and nonapeptide receptors promote estrildid flocking behavior. Science 325, 862–866. doi: 10.1126/science.1174929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodwin, N. L., Lopez, S. A., Lee, N. S., and Beery, A. K. (2019). Comparative role of reward in long-term peer and mate relationships in voles. Hormones Behav. 111, 70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackenberg, T. D., Vanderhooft, L., Huang, J., Wagar, M., Alexander, J., and Tan, L. (2021). Social preference in rats. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1002/jeab.686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinde, R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11:1. doi: 10.2307/2800384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, H. A., Beery, A. K., Blumstein, D. T., Couzin, I. D., Earley, R. L., Hayes, L. D., et al. (2014). An evolutionary framework for studying mechanisms of social behavior. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 581–589. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hruschka, D. J., and Henrich, J. (2013). Institutions, parasites and the persistence of in-group preferences. PLoS One 8:e63642. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue, K., Burkett, J. P., and Young, L. J. (2013). Neuroanatomical distribution of μ-opioid receptor mRNA and binding in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and non-monogamous meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Neuroscience 244, 122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, N., Shambaugh, K. L., and Beery, A. K. (2020). Female degus show high sociality but no preference for familiar peers. Behav. Process. 174:104102. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., and Hulihan, T. J. (1995). A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Behav. Neurosci. 109, 782–789. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., Preston, S., and Winslow, J. T. (1995). Mating in the monogamous male: behavioral consequences. Physiol. Behav. 57, 615–627. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00362-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, Z. V., Walum, H., Jamal, Y. A., Xiao, Y., Keebaugh, A. C., Inoue, K., et al. (2016). Central oxytocin receptors mediate mating-induced partner preferences and enhance correlated activation across forebrain nuclei in male prairie voles. Horm. Behav. 79, 8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kappeler, P. M. (2019). A framework for studying social complexity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73:13. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2601-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A. M., Kingsbury, M. A., Hoffbuhr, K., Schrock, S. E., Waxman, B., Kabelik, D., et al. (2011). Vasotocin neurons and septal V1a-like receptors potently modulate songbird flocking and responses to novelty. Hormones Behav. 60, 12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A. M., and Seifert, A. W. (2021). Distribution of vasopressin and oxytocin neurons in the basal forebrain and midbrain of spiny mice (Acomys cahirinus). Neuroscience 468, 16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.05.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenkel, W. M., Gustison, M. L., and Beery, A. K. (2021). A neuroscientist’s guide to the vole. Curr. Protoc. 1:e175. doi: 10.1002/cpz1.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, L. B., Walum, H., Inoue, K., Eyrich, N. W., and Young, L. J. (2016). Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene predicts brain region–specific expression and social attachment. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 160–169. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kingsbury, M. A., Gleason, E. D., Ophir, A. G., Phelps, S. M., Young, L. J., and Marler, C. A. (2012). Monogamous and promiscuous rodent species exhibit discrete variation in the size of the medial prefrontal cortex. BBE 80, 4–14. doi: 10.1159/000339247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacey, E. A., and Sherman, P. W. (2007). “The ecology of sociality in rodents,” in Rodent Societies: An Ecological & Evolutionary Perspective, eds J. O. Wolff and P. W. Sherman (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 243–254.

Google Scholar

Lee, N. S., and Beery, A. K. (2021). The role of dopamine signaling in prairie vole peer relationships. Hormones Behav. 127:104876. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N. S., Goodwin, N. L., Freitas, K. E., and Beery, A. K. (2019). Affiliation, aggression, and selectivity of peer relationships in meadow and prairie voles. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:52. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, P. C. (1994). “Social structure and evolution,” in Behaviour and Evolution, eds P. J. B. Slater and T. Halliday (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 266–303. doi: 10.1038/nature10149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidhar, N. K., Thakur, A., David, A.-J., Takehara-Nishiuchi, K., and Insel, N. (2021). Multiple dimensions of social motivation in adult female degus. PLoS One 16:e0250219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidicker, W. Z., and Patton, J. L. (1987). “Patterns of dispersal and genetic structure in populations of small rodents,” in Mammalian Dispersal Patterns: The Effects of Social Structure on Population Genetics, eds B. D. Chepko-Sade and Z. T. Halpin (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 144–161.

Google Scholar

Lim, M. M., and Young, L. J. (2004). Vasopressin-dependent neural circuits underlying pair bond formation in the monogamous prairie vole. Neuroscience 125, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Curtis, J. T., and Wang, Z. (2001). Vasopressin in the lateral septum regulates pair bond formation in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 115, 910–919. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.4.910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukas, D., and Clutton-Brock, T. (2018). Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol. Lett. 21, 1129–1134. doi: 10.1111/ele.13079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukas, D., and Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2013). The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science 341, 526–530. doi: 10.1126/science.1238677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madison, D. M. (1980). Space use and social structure in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7, 65–71.

Google Scholar

Madison, D. M., FitzGerald, R. W., and McShea, W. J. (1984). Dynamics of social nesting in overwintering meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus): possible consequences for population cycling. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15, 9–17.

Google Scholar

Madison, D. M., and McShea, W. J. (1987). Seasonal changes in reproductive tolerance, spacing, and social organization in meadow voles: a microtine model. Am. Zool. 27, 899–908. doi: 10.1093/icb/27.3.899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madrid, J. E., Parker, K. J., and Ophir, A. G. (2020). Variation, plasticity, and alternative mating tactics: revisiting what we know about the socially monogamous prairie vole. Adv. Study Behav. 52, 203–242. doi: 10.1016/bs.asb.2020.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McShea, W. J. (1990). Social tolerance and proximate mechanisms of dispersal among winter groups of meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. Anim. Behav. 39, 346–351.

Google Scholar

McShea, W. J., and Madison, D. M. (1984). Communal nesting between reproductively active females in a spring population of Microtus pennsylvanicus. Can. J. Zool. 62, 344–346. doi: 10.1139/z84-053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moy, S. S., Nadler, J. J., Perez, A., Barbaro, R. P., Johns, J. M., Magnuson, T. R., et al. (2004). Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 3, 287–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, C. D., and Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: the effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 270–282. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuberg, S. L., and Schaller, M. (2016). An evolutionary threat-management approach to prejudices. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoidance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 3433–3452. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, V. E. M., Lukas, M., Wolf, H. N., Durante, E., Lorenz, A., Mayer, A.-L., et al. (2021). Oxytocin and vasopressin within the ventral and dorsal lateral septum modulate aggression in female rats. Nat. Commun. 12:2900. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-23064-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ondrasek, N. R., Wade, A., Burkhard, T., Hsu, K., Nguyen, T., Post, J., et al. (2015). Environmental modulation of same-sex affiliative behavior in female meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Physiol. Behav. 140, 118–126. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ophir, A. G., Gessel, A., Zheng, D.-J., and Phelps, S. M. (2012). Oxytocin receptor density is associated with male mating tactics and social monogamy. Hormones Behav. 61, 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ossenkopp, K.-P. P., van Anders, S. M., Engeland, C. G., and Kavaliers, M. (2005). Influence of photoperiod and sex on locomotor behavior of meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) in an automated light-dark “anxiety” test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 869–879. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. J., and Lee, T. M. (2003). Female meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) demonstrate same-sex partner preferences. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 283–289. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. J., Phillips, K. M., and Lee, T. M. (2001). Development of selective partner preferences in captive male and female meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. Anim. Behav. 61, 1217–1226. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resendez, S. L., Keyes, P. C., Day, J. J., Hambro, C., Austin, C. J., Maina, F. K., et al. (2016). Dopamine and opioid systems interact within the nucleus accumbens to maintain monogamous pair bonds. eLife 5:e15325. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resendez, S. L., Kuhnmuench, M., Krzywosinski, T., and Aragona, B. J. (2012). κ-Opioid receptors within the nucleus accumbens shell mediate pair bond maintenance. J. Neurosci. 32, 6771–6784. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5779-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweinfurth, M. K., Neuenschwander, J., Engqvist, L., Schneeberger, K., Rentsch, A. K., Gygax, M., et al. (2017). Do female Norway rats form social bonds? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71:98.

Google Scholar

Smith, C. J. W., Wilkins, K. B., Mogavero, J. N., and Veenema, A. H. (2015). Social novelty investigation in the juvenile rat: modulation by the μ-opioid system. J. Neuroendocrinol. 27, 752–764. doi: 10.1111/jne.12301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soma, K. K., Scotti, M.-A. L., Newman, A. E. M., Charlier, T. D., and Demas, G. E. (2008). Novel mechanisms for neuroendocrine regulation of aggression. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 29:3. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2007.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparling, J. E., Baker, S. L., and Bielajew, C. (2018). Effects of combined pre- and post-natal enrichment on anxiety-like, social, and cognitive behaviours in juvenile and adult rat offspring. Behav. Brain Res. 353, 40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr-Phillips, E. J., and Beery, A. K. (2014). Natural variation in maternal care shapes adult social behavior in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 1017–1026. doi: 10.1002/dev.21182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinman, M. Q., Duque-Wilckens, N., and Trainor, B. C. (2019). Complementary neural circuits for divergent effects of oxytocin: social approach versus social anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 792–801. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stetzik, L., Ganshevsky, D., Lende, M. N., Roache, L. E., Musatov, S., and Cushing, B. S. (2018). Inhibiting ERα expression in the medial amygdala increases prosocial behavior in male meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Behav. Brain Res. 351, 42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulloa, M., Portillo, W., Díaz, N. F., Young, L. J., Camacho, F. J., Rodríguez, V. M., et al. (2018). Mating and social exposure induces an opioid-dependent conditioned place preference in male but not in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Hormones Behav. 97, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vahaba, D. M., Halstead, E. R., Donaldson, Z., Ahern, T. H., and Beery, A. K. (2021). Sex differences in the reward value of familiar mates in prairie voles. Genes Brain Behav. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1101/2021.09.01.458610

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Anders, S. M., Goodson, J. L., and Kingsbury, M. A. (2013). Beyond “oxytocin = good”: neural complexities and the flipside of social bonds. Arch. Sex. Behav. 42, 1115–1118. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0134-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walum, H., and Young, L. J. (2018). The neural mechanisms and circuitry of the pair bond. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 643–654. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0072-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Hulihan, T. J., and Insel, T. R. (1997). Sexual and social experience is associated with different patterns of behavior and neural activation in male prairie voles. Brain Res. 767, 321–332. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00617-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Smith, W., Major, D. E., and De Vries, G. J. (1994). Sex and species differences in the effects of cohabitation on vasopressin messenger RNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Brain Res. 650, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91784-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. R., Catania, K. C., and Carter, C. S. (1992). Development of partner preferences in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster): the role of social and sexual experience. Hormones Behav. 26, 339–349. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(92)90004-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winslow, J. T., Hastings, N., Carter, C. S., Harbaugh, C. R., and Insel, T. R. (1993). A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature 365, 545–548. doi: 10.1038/365545a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, R., Song, Z., and Tai, F. (2018). Parent-offspring cohabitation after weaning inhibits partner preference and alters central oxytocin and dopamine systems in adult mandarin vole. J. Comp. Physiol. A 204, 593–604. doi: 10.1007/s00359-018-1262-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. A., Gobrogge, K. L., Liu, Y., and Wang, Z. (2011). The neurobiology of pair bonding: insights from a socially monogamous rodent. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 32:6. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, P., An, S., Tai, F., Wang, J., Wu, R., and Wang, B. (2013). Early social deprivation impairs pair bonding and alters serum corticosterone and the NAcc dopamine system in mandarin voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 3128–3138. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aggression, affiliation, prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster), meadow vole (Microtus pennsylvanicus), selectivity, social motivation, tolerance

Citation: Lee NS and Beery AK (2022) Selectivity and Sociality: Aggression and Affiliation Shape Vole Social Relationships. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:826831. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.826831

Received: 01 December 2021; Accepted: 14 February 2022;
Published: 07 March 2022.

Edited by:

Russell D. Romeo, Columbia University, United States

Reviewed by:

Alexander G. Ophir, Cornell University, United States
Heather B. Patisaul, North Carolina State University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Lee and Beery. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Annaliese K. Beery, abeery@berkeley.edu

Download