Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neural Circuits, 21 December 2022
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2022.1027831

An early midbrain sensorimotor pathway is involved in the timely initiation and direction of swimming in the hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpole

  • Medway School of Pharmacy, University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom

Vertebrate locomotion is heavily dependent on descending control originating in the midbrain and subsequently influencing central pattern generators in the spinal cord. However, the midbrain neuronal circuitry and its connections with other brainstem and spinal motor circuits has not been fully elucidated. Vertebrates with very simple nervous system, like the hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpole, have been instrumental in unravelling fundamental principles of locomotion and its suspraspinal control. Here, we use behavioral and electrophysiological approaches in combination with lesions of the midbrain to investigate its contribution to the initiation and control of the tadpole swimming in response to trunk skin stimulation. None of the midbrain lesions studied here blocked the tadpole’s sustained swim behavior following trunk skin stimulation. However, we identified that distinct midbrain lesions led to significant changes in the latency and trajectory of swimming. These changes could partly be explained by the increase in synchronous muscle contractions on the opposite sides of the tadpole’s body and permanent deflection of the tail from its normal position, respectively. We conclude that the tadpole’s embryonic trunk skin sensorimotor pathway involves the midbrain, which harbors essential neuronal circuitry to significantly contribute to the appropriate, timely and coordinated selection and execution of locomotion, imperative to the animal’s survival.

Introduction

Animal survival relies heavily on the selection and execution of appropriate and well-timed motor actions. These could include walking, swimming, maintaining balance, moving of the eyes, running away from a predator, breathing, chewing, reaching out for objects and many more depending on the species and its environment (Orlovsky et al., 1999). In vertebrates, an important role of the central nervous system (CNS) is the generation and execution of locomotion, one of the most fundamental and extensively studied motor actions that enables full body propulsion (Grillner and El Manira, 2020).

The spinal central pattern generator (CPG) circuits are responsible for the generation of the basic locomotor rhythm, by establishing the appropriate sequence of muscle activation combined with reciprocal muscle inhibition (Buchanan and Grillner, 1987; Kiehn, 2006, 2016; Goulding, 2009; Roberts et al., 2010). In order to meet everchanging behavioral demands, the activity of spinal CPG is modulated by descending inputs originating in highly distributed supraspinal neuronal circuits (Dubuc et al., 2008; Lemon, 2008; El Manira and Grillner, 2014; Bouvier et al., 2015; Daghfous et al., 2016; Hsu et al., 2017; Arber and Costa, 2018, 2022; Ferreira-Pinto et al., 2018; Ruder and Arber, 2019; Grillner and El Manira, 2020).

The midbrain is an integral part of the survival brain network, and its role has been found to influence behavioral motor actions by projecting onto other parts of the brainstem, which in turn modulate CPG circuit activity in the spinal cord (Koutsikou et al., 2014, 2015, 2017; Arber and Costa, 2018, 2022; Ferreira-Pinto et al., 2018; Grillner and El Manira, 2020). In particular, the locomotor command systems in the midbrain (MLR: mesencephalic locomotor region), initially identified in cats (Shik et al., 1966, 1969), are conserved within the vertebrate lineage and play a multifaceted role in the control of locomotion (Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013; Grillner and El Manira, 2020; Carbo-Tano et al., 2022). Additionally, the descending circuitry of the midbrain nucleus of the medial longitudinal fascicle (nMLF) is the origin of the commands that regulate steering and posture during locomotion in zebrafish larvae (Severi et al., 2014; Thiele et al., 2014; Wang and McLean, 2014).

As in all vertebrates, supraspinal activity in the hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpole is present at initiation and during swimming (Roberts et al., 2010, 2019; Buhl et al., 2012; Koutsikou et al., 2018). A dedicated descending pathway via the tadpole’s midbrain is responsible for sensory activation and modulation of swimming following light dimming (Roberts, 1978; Foster and Roberts, 1982; Jamieson and Roberts, 1999, 2000). More specifically, a decrease in light intensity (light dimming) leads to excitation of the pineal eye photoreceptors, which in turn activate pineal ganglion cells with bilateral axons that could contact the diencephalic/mesencephalic descending (D/MD) neurons located in the midbrain. Axons of the D/MD neurons project ipsilaterally to the hindbrain where they could excite the CPG and mediate or modulate the swim response (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999).

Anatomical evidence indicates that the tadpole’s midbrain might also be involved in the initiation of swimming in response to trunk skin stimulation. Axons of trunk skin sensory Rohon-Beard neurons contact sensory pathway neurons, dla (dorsolateral ascending) and dlc (dorsolateral commissural), whose axons project to both sides of the hindbrain (Roberts and Clarke, 1982; Sillar and Roberts, 1988, 1992; Li et al., 2001). However, most dla and some dlc axons also project to midbrain (Li et al., 2001). We understand that dla and dlc sensory pathway neurons initiate or accelerate swimming by amplifying excitation (Li et al., 2001, 2004b). However, the function of the dla and dlc ascending projections to the midbrain is currently unclear. The presence of these projections suggests that (i) the midbrain might be essential part of the trunk skin sensorimotor pathway, even at this early stage of development and (ii) there could be transitory as well as longer-lasting influences of midbrain neurons on hindbrain- and/or spinal cord-driven motor activity.

Although research has shown that the midbrain is an integral part of the supraspinal motor control network, from lower vertebrates to mammals, its underlying neuronal circuitry, and interactions with the rest of the brainstem and spinal cord are not fully elucidated. Here we present the first investigation on the role of midbrain in the initiation and maintenance of trunk skin-evoked swimming in the young X. laevis tadpole. We use touch-evoked or electrical trunk skin stimulation and midbrain lesions to assess the importance of midbrain in mediating and/or modulating the swimming of the hatchling tadpole. We present evidence for changes in latency to initiation and trajectory of swimming as well as changes to tail postural control following distinct midbrain lesions.

Materials and methods

Animal ethics and surgery

All experimental procedures were carried out under the relevant guidelines and approved by the University of Kent Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB). Xenopus laevis embryos were supplied by the European Xenopus Resource Centre (EXRC; Portsmouth, UK) and kept at 16°C in tap water treated with commercially available aquarium water conditioner. All experiments were carried out on embryos at developmental stage 37/38 (Nieuwkoop and Faber, 1956), at room temperature (RT: 19°C).

All surgical procedures on the tadpoles were performed in a small custom-made dish filled with saline (NaCl 115mM, KCl 3mM, CaCl2 2mM, NaHCO3 2.4mM, MgCl2 1mM, HEPES 10mM; pH 7.4). Tadpoles were briefly anesthetised in 0.1% MS-222 (Ethyl 3-aminobenzoate methanesulfonate; Sigma-Aldrich, UK) and pinned to a small rotating Sylgard block. Dissections were performed with sharpened custom-made tungsten needles under a dissection microscope. Figures 1A, B shows the area of the tadpole’s CNS exposed in preparation for the procedures described in detail below.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Midbrain lesions and design of behavioral experiments. Dorsal view of the Xenopus laevis tadpole at developmental stage 37/38. (A) Diagram of the tadpole’s anatomical features and key CNS regions. Blue dotted outline represents the area where the skin was opened to allow access to the brain. (B) The tadpole viewed under a dissection microscope with pins running across the body, through the eyes and the notochord just caudal to the obex. Blue dotted line denotes the area of open skin with the brain visible. (C) Representation of the midbrain lesions. Ctrl (Control, orange); trigeminal nerve transection at the level of the otic capsule. The trigeminal nerve transections were performed on all experimental groups. MHB (Midbrain-Hindbrain Border lesion, purple); a transverse lesion through the MHB. ML (Midline lesion, green); lesion along the midline of the midbrain. Ipsi MHB + ML (Left/Ipsilateral MHB and ML lesion, blue); combination of left MHB and ML lesions. Contra MHB + ML (Right/Contralateral MHB and ML lesion, red); combination of right MHB and ML lesions. (D) Schematic diagram of the behavioural set up with (E) a zoomed in section of the tadpole’s starting position (not drawn to scale). (F) Illustration of the tail angle measurement used in subsequent figures. Red dot indicates the level of the anus, referred to as the starting point of the tail (G) The tadpoles starting position from a dorsal view. The tadpole is positioned upright into a groove carved onto a Sylgard block. Stimulation is applied on the left side of the tadpole’s trunk skin, at the level of the anus, using a glass pipette with an attached fine rabbit hair (CUSTOM–MADE). (H) Individual frames taken from high-speed videos recorded at 420 frames per second (fps). A short poke with a single rabbit hair to trunk skin receptors on the left side (*; time = 0 ms) initiated a swim response at 129 ms. (I) Individual video frames of a tadpole’s swim trajectory captured from a 30 fps recording. In this example the control animal trial starts at stimulation (0 s) to the end of the tadpole’s swim cycle (5.6 s). A novel tracking software, FastTrack Software, was used to detect and trace the tadpole’s body using an automatic tracking algorithm. (J) Schematic showing the path taken (black), the distance (blue) and the displacement (green). Here, the displacement is described as the shortest distance from the starting position to the next frame (position), with frame intervals indicated as F1, F2 and F3, with F4 representing the final displacement. The distance here is described as the measure of the path taken from the starting to the final position. Abbreviations: Ctrl, control; MHB, midbrain-hindbrain border; ML, midbrain midline; Ipsi MHB + ML, left/ipsilateral midbrain-hindbrain border and midbrain midline; Contra MHB + ML, right/contralateral midbrain-hindbrain border, and midbrain midline.

Both trigeminal nerves were severed at the level of the otic capsule (Figures 1A, C), to block skin impulses entering the nervous system and initiating swimming (Roberts, 1996; James, 2009; James and Soffe, 2011). The contribution of the hatchling tadpole’s midbrain to the control of movement was assessed through lesions as depicted in Figure 1C. These included: (i) a transverse lesion along the midbrain-hindbrain border (MHB), (ii) lesion along the midbrain’s midline (ML), (iii) lesion along the midbrain’s midline in combination with a transverse lesion on the left/Ipsilateral (Ipsi MHB + ML), and (iv) the right/contralateral (Contra MHB + ML) side of the MHB.

Behavior

Following the above preparatory procedures, all animals were allowed to recover in saline for 10 min at RT. Behavioral experiments were carried out in a Petri dish (diameter 90 mm) filled with saline, which was shallow enough to prevent animals from swimming in an upward spiral (Jamieson and Roberts, 2000; Roberts et al., 2000). The behavioral setup used is illustrated in Figures 1D, E. Briefly, animals were positioned dorsal side up within a groove made into a Sylgard block, which was affixed in the centre of the Petri dish. The Petri dish was illuminated from below by an LED light to enhance the contrast of the tadpole’s silhouette during video recording. A digital camera (Exilim EX-FH100, Casio) was positioned above this setup and was used to record high-speed videos in black and white mode. Videos were recorded at 420 fps for experiments in which latency to swim initiation was studied. Additionally, 30 fps videos were recorded in experiments where the swimming trajectory was analysed.

The angle of the tail was measured before swimming (at rest). Using the ImageJ software (NIH), we determined the tail angle based on the deviation of the tail from a straight line running from the front of the tadpole’s head (gray dot; Figure 1F) through the beginning of the tail (red dot; Figure 1F) to the most caudal tip of the tail (gray dot; Figure 1F).

A short poke with a fine (rabbit) hair was manually delivered to the tadpole’s trunk skin on the left side of its body to initiate swimming (Figure 1G). Each animal was allowed recovery time of 2 min between trials. Latency videos were analysed using the ImageJ software. The latency (ms) to swim initiation was calculated based on the number of video frames from trunk skin stimulation (time 0 ms on Figure 1H) to the first head bend (time 129 ms on Figure 1H). The direction of head movement was also recorded as either on the ipsilateral or contralateral side in relation to the stimulus (ipsilateral to the stimulus on Figure 1H).

FastTrack Software, an open-source tracking software (Gallois and Candelier, 2021), was used to detect and track the tadpole in experiments on swimming trajectory (Figure 1I). A text file containing the X-Y position of the tadpole’s body across frames was generated and kinematic analysis was preformed using MS Excel. The following equation was used to calculate the displacement:

(X0-Xn)2+(Y0-Yn)2

whereby, X0 and Y0 is the starting position of the tadpole and n is the position of the tadpole in the frame under analysis (schematic illustration shown in Figure 1J).

We aimed to record and subsequently analyse three trials per behavioral test per tadpole. However, some animals ceased to respond after one or two trials. In addition, post-experimentation some trials could not be analysed because the FastTrack software could not fully detect the animal. These challenges account for the different number of trials reported across groups in the corresponding Results section below.

Electrophysiology

Additional preparatory procedures for the electrophysiological recordings of fictive swimming included removal of the skin covering the trunk muscles on both sides of the tadpole’s body for access to the myotomal clefts. After surgery, animals were allowed to recover and tested for robust swimming prior to being paralyzed in α-bungarotoxin (0.01M, Invitrogen) for 50 min at RT. Animals were pinned on a rotating Sylgard block in a small recording dish filled with saline (Figure 2B). Two borosilicate glass suction electrodes (diameter ∼50 μm) were attached to both sides of the tadpole’s body approximately at the level of the 4th cleft to record ventral root (VR) activity (Figures 2A, B). Such positioning of the VR recording electrodes permits accurate capturing of the side of fictive swimming initiation (Figures 2C, D showing single raw examples of ipsilateral and contralateral starts, respectively). A third glass suction electrode was attached to the trunk skin on the right side of the body, at the level of the anus, to deliver electrical stimuli to the trunk skin. A schematic view of the electrode positions can be seen in Figure 2A (Koutsikou et al., 2018; Messa and Koutsikou, 2021). Electrical stimulation was delivered via a custom-made TTL pulse generator automatically driven through software (Signal 7, CED, Cambridge, UK). Threshold stimulation was set as the smallest stimulus (both in intensity -V- and duration -ms-) that led to swim initiation. Suprathreshold stimulation was defined as the intensity of threshold stimulus + 1V, with the same duration as of threshold stimulus. Threshold stimulation was verified in each animal prior to experimental recordings. Subsequently, suprathreshold stimulus was calculated for every animal accordingly. All animals started swimming with a threshold stimulus in the range of 3.5–4.5 V and 0.25–0.4 ms. The electrical signal from left and right VR was amplified, 50/60 Hz noise was eliminated via a noise eliminator (HumBug, Digitimer, UK), and data were acquired through Power 1401 mkII (CED, Cambridge, UK) in Signal 7 (CED, Cambridge, UK) and displayed as shown in Figures 2C, D.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experimental setup for fictive swimming. (A) Dorsal view of the tadpole with recording and stimulating electrodes in position. Left and right VR (ventral root) electrodes were positioned facing each other approximately at the 4th myotomal cleft. The stimulating electrode was always positioned on the right side at the level of the anus. Red dashed line denotes the area of open skin with the brain visible. Red dashed square area indicates the area presented in panel (B). (B) Picture of the trunk of a tadpole pinned to the Sylgard block during extracellular VR recordings. Right and left VR and stimulating electrodes, are positioned as described in A. (C) Example of an ipsilateral start of fictive swimming in response to electrical trunk skin stimulation (red arrowhead). The first VR burst is recorded on the right side of the body (right VR, ipsilateral; black arrowhead). (D) Example of a contralateral start of fictive swimming in response to electrical trunk skin stimulation (red arrowhead). The first VR burst is recorded on the left side of the body (contralateral, left VR; black arrowhead). In all experiments stimulation was delivered on the right side of the tadpole’s body.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using Prism 9 (GraphPad). Data were tested for normality using Shapiro-Wilk test, with normality criterion set at p < 0.05. The statistical tests are as stated throughout the Results. To describe the central tendency and variability, median with interquartile range values (IQR: 25–75 percentile) were used unless stated otherwise. Statistical analysis was not performed on data reported as percentages.

Results

None of the lesions described here abolished the swim response. This agrees with previous findings showing that the tadpole can generate episodes of sustained swimming after removal of the CNS rostral to the seventh rhombomere (Li et al., 2006). However, the specific consequences of such lesions have not previously been examined in the hatchling tadpole.

The midbrain contributes to the latency and side of first motor response in freely moving animals

Following a gentle touch to the tadpole’s trunk skin, swimming is initiated. With the use of high-speed videos recorded at 420 fps, the latency (ms) from the touch stimulus to swim initiation was measured (Figure 3A). The latency to swim initiation measured in MHB-lesioned animals (n = 14 tadpoles) was significantly longer compared to controls (n = 10 control tadpoles; p < 0.0001, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test; control group median: 104.8, IQR: 73.2–128.0 ms vs MHB-lesioned median: 156.0, IQR: 125.0–243.5 ms). Tadpoles with the Contra MHB + ML lesion (n = 9 tadpoles) also showed significantly longer latencies compared to control animals (n = 10 control tadpoles; p = 0.0011, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test; control vs Contra MHB + ML median: 126.2, IQR: 104.8–198.8 ms; Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Midbrain lesions lead to changes in latency and side of swim initiation in freely moving animals. (A) Latency (ms) to the start of swimming in response to trunk skin stimulation. Ctrl: 104.8, 73.2–128.0 ms, MHB: 156.0, 125.9–243.5 ms, midline: 113.1, 76.79–173.8 ms, Ispi MHB + ML: 121.4, 79.76–157.7 ms, Contra MHB + ML: 126.2, 104.8–198.8 ms. (B) Percentage occurrence (% total number of trials for each experimental group) of the first behavioral motor response in relation to the side of the stimulus. Ctrl: 41% ipsilateral, 59% contralateral; MHB: 37% ipsilateral, 63% contralateral; Midline: 63% ipsilateral, 37% contralateral; Ipsi MHB + ML: 24% ipsilateral, 76% contralateral; Contra MHB + ML: 72% ipsilateral, 28% contralateral. (C) Latency to the initiation of swimming in ipsilateral vs contralateral starts. Ctrl ipsilateral 109.5, 83.3–151.2 ms, contralateral 97.6, 59.5–109.5 ms; MHB ipsilateral 191.7,150.0–251.2 ms, contralateral 145.2, 109.5–228.6 ms; Midline ipsilateral 113.1, 71.4–171.4 ms, contralateral 115.5, 84.5–180.4 ms; Ipsi MHB + ML ipsilateral 128.6, 98.8–170.2 ms, contralateral 119.1, 75.0–158.3 ms; Contra MHB + ML ipsilateral 142.9, 104.8–204.8 ms, contralateral 119.1, 92.3–189.3 ms. Ctrl: n = 10 tadpoles, trials = 32; MHB: n = 14 tadpoles, trials = 38; Midline: n = 11 tadpoles, trials = 38; Ipsi MHB + ML: n = 12 tadpoles, trials = 38; Contra MHB + ML: n = 9 tadpoles, trials = 36. For boxplot (A, C) all data are reported as median and 25–75 percentile, single data points are plotted, the middle horizontal line represents median latency, the box represents the interquartile range (IQR, 25–75 percentile), and the error bars represent the minimum and maximum values. ****p < 0.0001, **p < 0.01 for Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test.

Excitability due to sensory stimulation on one side of the skin is transmitted to both sides of the hindbrain and can initiate swimming on either side of the tadpole’s body (Buhl et al., 2012, 2015; Roberts et al., 2019; Ferrario et al., 2021; Messa and Koutsikou, 2021). In this study, following left trunk skin stimulation, control tadpoles (n = 10) initiated swimming more often on the unstimulated side (contralateral to the stimulus; Figure 3B) at percentages of 41% ipsilateral (13/32 trials) and 59% contralateral (19/32 trials). This relationship between ipsilateral and contralateral swim initiations has been altered to varying degrees following the different midbrain lesions. More specifically, MHB-lesioned animals (n = 14) showed a slight increase in the percentage of contralateral starts (37% ipsilateral 14/38 trials vs 63% contralateral 24/38 trials). Interestingly, the partial lesion to the MHB in combination with the ML lesion (Ispi MHB + ML; n = 12 tadpoles; Figure 2B) increased even further the percentage of contralateral swim starts (24% ipsilateral 9/38 trials vs 76% contralateral 29/38 trials). In contrast to these lesions favoring contralateral starts, the ML (n = 11 tadpoles; 63% ipsilateral, 24/38 trials vs 37% contralateral, 14/38 trials) and the Contra MHB + ML lesions (n = 9 tadpoles; 72% ipsilateral 26/36 trials vs 28% contralateral 10/36 trials) led animals to more frequent initiation of swimming on the ipsilateral side (Figure 3B).

The latency data were further analysed in relation to the side of swim start (Figure 3C; number of tadpoles as mentioned above). Within each tadpole group, there was no significant difference in the initiation latency between ipsilateral vs contralateral starts (p > 0.05, individual Mann-Whitney test within each tadpole group comparing ipsilateral vs contralateral latency: ms; Figure 3C). However, comparisons across groups revealed that the ipsilateral and contralateral latencies for only the MHB-lesioned animals differed significantly when compared to the respective values obtained from the control animals (Figure 3C; p = 0.0011; control ipsilateral median: 109.5, IQR: 83.3–151.2 ms vs MHB ipsilateral median: 191.7, IQR: 150.0–251.2 ms; p < 0.0001; control contralateral median: 97.6, IQR: 59.5–109.5 ms vs MHB contralateral median: 145.2, IQR: 109.5–228.6 ms; Mann-Whitney test). Although Figure 3A shows that tadpoles with the Contra MHB + ML lesion exhibited overall significantly longer latencies when compared to control animals, this significance was lost here, most likely due to the lack of power in the number of data points, when the data were categorised according to the side of swim starts in Figure 3C. Overall, the complete disconnection of the midbrain (MHB) from the rest of the brainstem had the greatest effect on latency, indicating the involvement of the midbrain in the modulation of motor initiation. Furthermore, the change to the side of the initial body bend, for ML- and Contra MHB + ML-lesioned animals, suggests the importance of possible midbrain commissural connections (currently unknown) in determining the side of the first motor response.

The midbrain contributes to the latency, side of first motor response and alternating initiation of fictive swimming

In the behavioral experiments above, the MHB lesion caused the most significant change in latency to initiation of swimming when compared to control animal responses. For this reason, we used the MHB-lesioned vs control tadpoles to further investigate the reasons behind this change to swim latency. In this set of experiments, with electrical stimulation delivered on trunk skin at threshold intensities, MHB-lesioned tadpoles (n = 7) showed shorter latency to first ventral root (VR) activity in comparison to control animals (n = 5 tadpoles; Figure 4A; Mann-Whitney test p = 0.0049, control median: 100.9, IQR: 91.0–117.8 ms vs MHB lesioned median: 48.1, IQR: 36.2–61.4 ms). This contrasts with the behavioural data presented in Figure 3A, where MHB lesions led to significant increase in swim latency. When electrical stimulation was delivered at suprathreshold intensities, latency to the first VR activity was slightly longer but did not differ significantly in MHB-lesioned animals (n = 7) compared to control group (n = 5 tadpoles; Figure 4D; p = 0.8249, Mann-Whitney test; control median: 27.8, IQR: 24.4–95.8; MHB median: 35.2, IQR: 24.3–56.9). These differences might be due to changes in both stimulus strength and modality (mechanical vs electrical stimulus).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Midbrain-hindbrain border (MHB) lesion leads to changes in latency and side of fictive swim initiation. (A) Latencies (ms) to the first VR burst after threshold electrical stimulus was delivered to the trunk skin in control and MHB-lesioned animals. Mann-Whitney test p = 0.0049, controls 100.9, 91.02–117.8 ms vs MHB lesioned 48.1, 36.24–61.4 ms; data reported as median, 25–75 percentile. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 22; MHB: n = 7 tadpoles, trials = 29. (B) Percentage occurrence (% total number of trials for each experimental group) of the first VR burst after threshold electrical stimulation in control (orange) and MHB-lesioned animals (violet). Ctrl ipsilateral 63.6% (14/22 trials), contralateral 36.4% (8/22 trials); MHB ipsilateral 34.5% (10/29 trials), contralateral 51.7% (15/29 trials), synchrony 13.8% (4/29 trials). (C) Latencies (ms) to the first VR burst after threshold electrical stimulus delivered to the trunk skin in control (orange) and MHB-lesioned (violet) animals. Solid circles represent latencies to ipsilateral first VR burst, open circles represent latencies to contralateral first VR burst, and crosses represent VR bursts recorded simultaneously on both sides of the body. Mann-Whitney test for the ctrl group, p = 0.365; Kruskal-Wallis test for the MHB-lesioned group, p = 0.1859. Ctrl ipsilateral 101.9, 93.1–112.0 ms, contralateral 67.4, 23.6–132.8 ms; MHB ipsilateral 49.8, 39.6–64.0 ms, contralateral 50.7, 36.5–83.6 ms, synchrony 36.2, 35.9–39.8 ms. Data are reported as median, 25–75 percentiles. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 22; MHB: n = 7, trials = 29. (D) Latencies (ms) to the first VR burst after suprathreshold electrical stimulus delivered to the trunk skin in control and MHB-lesioned tadpoles. Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.8249; ctrl: 27.82, 24.35–95.81 ms; MHB: 35.16, 24.28–56.94 ms. Data reported as median, 25–75 percentile. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 27; MHB: n = 7, trials = 33. (E) Percentage occurrence (% total number of trials for each experimental group) of the first VR burst after suprathreshold electrical stimulation of control (orange) and MHB-lesioned animals (violet). Ctrl ipsilateral 25.9% (20/27 trials), contralateral 74.1% (7/27 trials); MHB ipsilateral 39.4% (13/33 trials), contralateral 42.4% (14/33 trials), synchrony 18.2% (6/33 trials). (F) Latencies (ms) to the first VR burst after suprathreshold electrical stimulus delivered to the trunk skin of control (orange) and MHB-lesioned animals (violet). Solid circles represent latencies to ipsilateral first VR burst, open circles represent latencies to contralateral first VR burst, and crosses represent VR bursts recorded simultaneously on both sides of the body. Mann-Whitney test for the ctrl group, p < 0.0001: ipsilateral 107.9, 97.0–120.3 ms, contralateral 25.5, 23.9–32.7 ms; Kruskal-Wallis test for MHB group, p = 0.2108: ipsilateral 36.5, 23.3–3.2 ms, contralateral 43.6, 24.8–95.7 ms, synchrony 25.8, 12.7–36.1 ms. All data are reported as median, 25–75 percentile. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 27; MHB-lesioned: n = 7, trials = 33). (G) Latencies (ms) to the first alternating VR burst (i.e., the first burst indicative of the start of fictive swimming) after a threshold electrical stimulus delivered to the trunk skin of control and MHB-lesioned tadpoles. Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.0049; ctrl: 100.9, 91.0–117.8 ms; MHB: 52.1, 42.0–71.1 ms. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 22; MHB: n = 7, trials = 29. (H) Latencies (ms) to the first alternating VR burst after a suprathreshold electrical stimulus delivered to the trunk skin of control and MHB-lesioned tadpoles. Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.4879; ctrl: 27.8, 24.4–95.8; MHB: 48.9, 25.1–61.1 ms. Ctrl: n = 5 tadpoles, trials = 27; MHB: n = 7, trials = 33. (I) Latency (ms) for asynchronous starts (synchrony data are omitted) after a threshold stimulation and according to the side of the first VR burst (Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.8079; ipsilateral 54.6, 42.1–63.5 ms; contralateral: 51.4, 40.6–83.5 ms; n = 7 tadpoles, trials = 29). (J) Latency to asynchronous starts after a suprathreshold stimulation and according to the side of first VR burst (Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.4654; ipsilateral 48.7, 24.8–57.6 ms, contralateral 51.6, 25.2–81.3 ms; n = 7 tadpoles, trials = 33). In all panels, data collected on controls are in orange, data collected on MHB lesioned animals are in violet. In panels (A,C,D,F–J) single data points are plotted; boxes indicate 5–95 percentile; middle horizontal line in each box represents median value; error bars indicate minimum and maximum values. In panels (B,E); filled bars: ipsilateral first VR burst; white bars: contralateral first VR burst; grey bars: synchronous VR first burst. All data reported in the figure legend are expressed as median, 25–75 percentile. **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001 for Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test.

We also identified the side of the first VR burst (Figures 2C, D showing examples of ipsilateral and contralateral starts, respectively). The control group activated the ipsilateral side first in 63.6% of the trials (14/22 trials), whilst the first VR burst appeared on the contralateral side in 36.4% of the trials (8/22 trials; Figure 4B). Additionally, it was revealed that tadpoles with lesioned MHB failed to start efficient swimming (with alternating VR activity). Instead, synchronous first VR bursts were recorded in 13.8% of the trials (synchrony in 4/29 trials; referred as “synchrony” in Figure 4B), and our previous work: (Ferrario et al., 2021), which in a behavioral setting would have prevented the initiation of swimming. The latency to fictive swimming initiation was then grouped and plotted based on the side of the first VR recorded (Figure 4C). Although no significant difference was identified (Figure 4C; control: p = 0.365, Mann-Whitney and MHB-lesioned animals: p = 0.1859, Kruskal-Wallis), the latencies for synchronous start in MHB-lesioned tadpoles appeared less variable than latencies reported for contralateral or ipsilateral initiation in both lesioned and control animals (Figure 4C; control ipsilateral median: 101.9, IQR: 93.1–112.0 ms; control contralateral median: 67.44, IQR: 23.6–132.8 ms; MHB ipsilateral median: 49.8, IQR: 39.6–64.0 ms; MHB contralateral median: 50.7, IQR: 36.5–83.6 ms; MHB synchrony median: 36.3, IQR: 35.9–39.8 ms).

In contrast to the response following threshold stimulation, control animals switched the side of first VR burst to the contralateral side [Figure 4E; contralateral 74.07% vs ipsilateral 25.93% (20/27 vs 7/27 trials, respectively)]. In MHB-lesioned tadpoles, the percentages of trials with ipsilateral vs contralateral initiation were similar following suprathreshold stimulation [ipsilateral 39.4% (13/33 trials) vs contralateral 42.4% (14/33 trials)]. The percentage of synchronous initiations recorded was slightly higher in comparison to that recorded following threshold skin stimulation of MHB-lesioned animals (Figure 4E; 18.2% (6/33 trials) synchronous VR activity). At suprathreshold trunk skin stimulation, control tadpoles showed shorter latency to fictive swimming when started their movement on the contralateral side (Figure 4F; p < 0.0001, Mann-Whitney test; ipsilateral median: 107.9, IQR: 97.0–120.3 ms vs contralateral median: 25.5, IQR: 23.9–32.7 ms). Following MHB lesions, tadpoles failed to diversify their swim response. The latency to initiation of swimming was not significant across ipsilateral, contralateral, and synchronous fictive swim initiations (Figure 4F; p = 0.2108, Kruskal-Wallis test; ipsilateral median: 36.5, IQR: 23.3–53.3 ms vs contralateral median: 43.6, IQR: 24.8–95.7 ms vs synchrony median: 25.8, IQR: 12.7–36.1 ms).

When VR activity appears synchronously on both sides of the tadpole’s trunk, the alternating contraction of antagonist muscles necessary for movement initiation cannot be achieved. The electrophysiological experiments here give us the opportunity to also identify and easily separate the latency to the first VR activity, post synchrony, which is the one eventually leading to alternating fictive swimming. The latency of the first alternating VR was also measured and plotted for all MHB-lesioned animals vs the control group. At threshold stimulation, MHB-lesioned animals showed a significantly shorter latency compared to controls despite some of the alternating VR firing commencing post-synchrony (Figure 4G; p = 0.0049, Mann-Whitney test; control median: 100.9, IQR: 91.0–117.8 ms; MHB lesion median: 52.2, IQR: 42.0–71.1 ms). On the contrary, at suprathreshold stimulation, latency values were not significant different between the two groups (Figure 4H; p = 0.4879, Mann-Whitney test; control median: 27.8, IQR: 24.4–95.8 ms; MHB lesion median: 48.9, IQR: 25.1–61.1 ms).

When the latency values for alternating starts were plotted according to the side of initiation, MHB-lesioned tadpoles showed comparable latencies for both ipsilateral and contralateral fictive swim initiation following both threshold (Figure 4I; p = 0.8079, Mann-Whitney test; ipsilateral median: 54.6, IQR: 42.1–63.5 ms; contralateral median: 51.4, IQR: 40.6–83.5 ms) and suprathreshold electrical trunk skin stimuli (Figure 4J; p = 0.4654, Mann-Whitney test; ipsilateral median: 48.7, IQR: 24.8–57.6 ms; contralateral median: 51.6, IQR: 25.2–81.3 ms).

Both sets of experimental work described above, revealed that varying degrees of midbrain lesion led to changes in swim behavior. First, latencies to the initiation of swimming have been altered and it appears that the synchronous contraction of trunk muscles on opposite sides of the tadpole’s body might be in part responsible for such changes. Furthermore, we observed stark alterations to the side of swim initiation in midbrain-lesioned animals, in some instance leading to a reversal of the control behavior.

Midbrain lesions affect the trajectory and maintenance of swimming in freely moving animals

The following experiments on freely moving animals focused on exploring the contribution and role of midbrain in the maintenance of trajectory and further aspects of sustained swimming (Figures 1I, J). Tadpole swim behavior was initiated in response to mechanical touch (fine rabbit hair) on the trunk skin (Figure 1G). The swim trajectories for each animal group were captured through video recording (Figure 1I) and subsequently traced, and plotted (Figures 1J, 5A, B). Representative examples from each experimental group are shown in Figure 5A (the trajectories of two animals per group are shown). The trajectories of control animals followed a forward direction as the tadpoles swam away from the starting position (Figure 5A1). In contrast, the trajectories of midbrain-lesioned animals had a turning/circular pattern, pivoting around a specific area of the arena multiple times (often close to the starting point; Figures 5A2–A5). Notably, Contra MHB + ML lesioned animals started briefly in a forward direction, which would turn into circular movement until the end of the swim episode (Figure 5A5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Midbrain lesions lead to stark changes in the trajectory of tadpole swimming (A). Example swim trajectories (n = 2) for each animal group extracted from the FastTrack Software. The starting position is symbolised by the black dot. (B) Displacement time graphs of each animal trial (coloured lines) to show the tadpoles deviation from the starting position for each animal group (See section “Materials and methods” for calculation). The average is represented by the black line for each animals group. An increase in displacement refers to the tadpole swimming away from the starting position, while a decrease represents the tadpole swimming closer to the starting position. (C) Boxplot of the final displacement and total distance travelled for each animal trial within the respective animal group. For panels (B,C), Control: n = 10, trials = 17; MHB: n = 9, trials = 12; Midline: n = 8, trials = 12; Ipsi MHB + ML: n = 9, trials = 14; Contra MHB + ML: n = 10, trials = 14. For boxplot, single data points are plotted, the middle horizontal line represents the median value, the box represents the interquartile range (IQR, 25–75 percentile), and the extended vertical bars represent the minimum and maximum values.

Swim trajectories were further analysed quantitatively by calculating the displacement for each tadpole per video frame (Figure 5B; see section “Materials and methods” for details). The displacement indicates the deviation of the tadpole’s swim position from the starting point (centre of the arena; 0 s) in each video frame up to the final frame when tadpoles ceased swimming (Figure 1J). In control animals (Figure 5B1; n = 10 tadpoles, 17 trials), the steep increase in displacement over a relatively short period of time represents the forward direction of swimming as the tadpole swims further away from its starting position. Control animals showed an average peak of displacement at 37.9 mm (black line in Figure 5B1) due to the physical dimension of the arena used, which had a radius of 45 mm. Indeed, control tadpoles took mostly forward trajectories, quickly reached the wall of the Petri dish. On the other hand, the displacement of midbrain-lesioned animals, swimming predominantly in a circular pattern, was characterised by peaks and troughs indicative of repetitive circular movement of the animal throughout the entire swim cycle (Figures 5B2–B5). MHB- (n = 9 tadpoles, 12 trials) ML- (n = 8 tadpoles, 12 trials) and Ipsi MHB + ML-lesioned animals (n = 9 tadpoles, n = 14 trials) adopted a consistent circular-shaped trajectory (Figures 5B2–B4), while Contra MHB + ML-lesioned tadpoles (n = 10 tadpoles, n = 14 trials) had a more irregular trajectory, characterised by a brief forward start and wider circular movements (Figure 5B5).

Figure 5C summarizes these observations by presenting the median final displacement and total distance travelled for each experimental group (Figure 5C). The median values for displacement and total distance travelled by control animals are almost equal (Figure 5C1), as the control tadpole would constantly move away from the starting point, in an almost straight trajectory (Figures 5A1, B1). In contrast, midbrain-lesioned animals showed very different values for final displacement and total distance travelled (Figures 5C2–C5). In fact, midbrain-lesion tadpoles covered longer distances, however, due to the circular trajectories their final displacement values are smaller, because their swim episode ended nearer the starting position.

The above observations (Figure 5) also highlight that the midbrain lesions, despite altering the tadpole’s swim trajectory, still permit the animal to swim for a considerable amount of time and spontaneously stop. We further investigate the effect of midbrain lesions on the total duration of swim episodes, in all experimental groups.

Figure 6A shows all data on duration of swim episodes irrespective of the way each animal’s swimming ceased. Control animals swam for 3.3 ± 0.3 s (mean ± SEM; s) after trunk skin stimulation (Figure 6A). The most significant increase in swim duration was seen in animals with complete disconnection of the midbrain from the rest of the brainstem (MHB; p < 0.0001, ANOVA/Dunnett’s; MHB: 22.7 ± 3.8 s). The ML and Ipsi MHB + ML lesions also led to overall significantly longer swim episodes, compared to control animals (ML: 10.99 ± 1.32 s; p = 0.0127, ANOVA/Dunnett’s and p = 0.0003, ANOVA/Dunnett’s, Ipsi MHB + ML: 13.5 ± 1.9 s). Consequently, the distance travelled will also increase in lesioned tadpoles (Figure 6B). Control tadpoles travelled 47.4, 41.9–63.7 mm (median, IQR), though the size of the arena (radius 45 mm) is a limiting factor here. MHB-lesioned animals travelled the greatest distance of 187.5, 99.7–307.5 mm (median, IQR; p < 0.0001, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test). Similarly, ML, Ipsi MHB + ML and Contra MHB + ML animals all travelled various distances than control (p = 0.0005, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test; ML median: 113.7, IQR: 79.4–189.6 mm; p < 0.0001, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test; Ipsi MHB + ML median: 123.9, IQR: 97.7–195.7 mm; p = 0.0070, Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test; Contra MHB + ML median: 105.8, IQR: 77.4–138.3 mm).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Midbrain lesions affect sustained swimming. (A) The average duration of swim episodes in control and midbrain lesioned animals. Controls swam for 3.3 ± 0.3 s after stimulation. MHB: 22.7 ± 3.8 s; ML: 11.0 ± 1.3 s; Ipsi MHB + ML: 13.5 ± 1.9 s; Contra MHB + ML: 6.3 ± 0.6 s. Reported as mean ± SEM. Single data points are plotted; the middle horizontal line represents the mean and the extended vertical bars represent the SEM. (B) Controls travelled 47.4, 41.9–63.7 mm (note the size of the arena had a radius of 45 mm). MHB: 187.5, 99.7–307.5 mm; ML: 113.7, 79.4–189.6 mm; Ipsi MHB + ML: 123.9, 97.7–195.7 mm; Contra MHB + ML: 105.8, 77.4–138.3 mm. Sample size identical to those in Figure 5. For panel (A) Results are reported as mean ± SEM; single data points are plotted, the horizontal line represents mean duration, error bars represent the SEM. For panel (B) Results are reported as median and IQR (25–75 percentile); for boxplot, single data points are plotted, the middle horizontal line represents median distance, the box represents the interquartile range (IQR, 25–75 percentile), and the extended vertical bars represent the minimum and maximum values. ****p < 0.0001, ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01 and *p < 0.05 for ANOVA/Dunnett’s and Kruskal-Wallis/Dunn’s test.

The midbrain contributes to the postural orientation of the tail

It has been shown that in larva zebrafish excitability in the midbrain nMLF (nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus) provides postural control for tail orientation, which in turn influences the direction (via steering) of swimming (Thiele et al., 2014). We investigated if the midbrain activity of the hatchling tadpole is essential in postural control of the tail, which in part might explain the changes seen in the swim trajectory of midbrain-lesioned animals. Tail deflection was measured as described in Methods and Figure 1F, while the tadpole was at rest, positioned into the Sylgard block groove with dorsal side up with its tail unrestrained, off the Sylgard block. A one-way ANOVA revealed that midbrain lesions resulted in increased tail deviation when compared to tail position of control animals (Figure 7; F4,46 = 10.4, p < 0.001). In particular, statistical significance was recorded between the control tadpoles’ mean tail angle (mean ± S.E.M: 0.9° ± 1.1°; n = 5 tadpoles; 12 trials) and those of the Ipsi MHB + ML (p = 0.0003, ANOVA/Dunnett’s; Figure 7A; mean ± S.E.M: 5.1° ± 8.7°; n = 5 tadpoles; 10 trials) and Contra MHB + ML lesioned groups (p < 0.0001, ANOVA/Dunnett’s; Figure 7A; mean ± S.E.M: 7.8° ± 13.3°; n = 4 tadpoles; 8 trials). The unrestrained tail position of control animals (n = 5 tadpoles, 12 trials), at rest, deviated by 0.9o ± 1.1o (mean ± SEM) with the greatest angle to the left at 8.2o and right at 5.1o (Figures 7A, B1, C1). The Contra MHB + ML tadpoles showed the most significant change in the position of the tail with a mean deviation angle of 7.8o ± 13.3o (mean ± SEM; n = 4 tadpoles, 8 trials), and the greatest angle change to the left and right at 61.1 and 35.6o, respectively (Figures 7A, B5, C5).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Midbrain lesions cause tail deflections. (A) The mean tail angle at rest (no stimulus applied), prior to trunk skin stimulation. Ctrl: 0.9o ± 1.1o, n = 5 tadpoles, 12 trials; MHB: 3.1o ± 3.1o, n = 6, 13 trials; ML: 4.8o ± 7.0o, n = 4, 8 trials; Ipsi MHB + ML: –5.1o ± 8.7o, n = 5, 10 trials; Contra MHB + ML: –7.8o ± 13.3o, n = 4, 8 trials. Reported as mean ± SEM. Single data points are plotted, the middle horizontal line represents the mean, and the extended vertical bars represent the SEM. ****p < 0.0001, ***p = 0.003, ANOVA/Dunnett’s. (B) A single example of the tadpole’s body posture from still images of resting tadpoles, representative of each experimental group. (C) Circular plot of the frequency distribution of the tail angle before swim starts. Each circular segment represents the number of values. Bin width is 5.8o.

Discussion

The midbrain is an integral part of the supraspinal motor control network, shown to command locomotor activity from simple vertebrates to mammals (Shik et al., 1966, 1969; Sirota et al., 2000; Cabelguen et al., 2003; Uematsu et al., 2007; Dubuc et al., 2008; Jordan et al., 2008; Smetana et al., 2010; Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013; Daghfous et al., 2016). However, the midbrain neuronal circuitry and its connections with other supraspinal and spinal motor circuits are not fully understood. In this study we sought to investigate the role of midbrain in the control of coordinated locomotor behavior in the Xenopus laevis tadpole, a simple animal at a very early stage of development. We summarise our findings and propose a midbrain sensorimotor pathway, which gets activated in response to trunk skin stimulation (Figure 8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Summary diagram of known and proposed midbrain neural circuitry. Diagram of the brain and spinal cord of the Xenopus laevis tadpole at developmental stage 37/38, showing already known and newly proposed neuronal connections involved in the initiation of swimming in response to photic and mechanical stimuli. Mechanosensory Rohon-Beard (RB) neurons innervate the skin of the trunk and tail. The central axons of the RB neurons transmit excitation via sensory interneurons namely dorsolateral commissural (dlc) and dorsolateral ascending (dla) interneurons. Both interneurons project ascending axons to the hindbrain and midbrain. The second sensory pathway presented in this diagram involves the excitation of the pineal eye photoreceptors in response to light dimming. Axons of pineal ganglion cells form synapses on both sides of the midbrain. Midbrain diencephalic/mesencephalic descending (D/MD) neurons are excited by the pineal ganglion cells, and project caudally into the hindbrain and spinal cord where they could excite Central Pattern Generators (CPGs) directly, or via a novel hindbrain population of sensory memory neurons (hindbrain extension neurons; hexNs), which have been proposed to extend the sensory signal between the sensory pathway and reticulospinal neurons of the swimming CPG. The proposed midbrain interneurons (mINs), if different to D/MD neurons, may also project to the opposite side of the midbrain and/or directly or indirectly to hexNs and the CPG. Each neuron type population and colour coding are displayed in the list. Circles represent groups of cell bodies, single lines represent axons, and triangles represent excitatory synapses. Proposed neuronal connections are indicated by dashed lines.

In the hatchling tadpole (developmental stage 37/38), the first evidence on the exact role of midbrain in the initiation of swimming (locomotion) came from studies on the pineal eye, situated above the midbrain-forebrain border (Foster and Roberts, 1982; Jamieson, 1997; Jamieson and Roberts, 1999). Light dimming excites pineal eye photoreceptors and axons of pineal ganglion cells project caudally into the ventral midbrain, without reaching the hindbrain and spinal cord. HRP (horseradish peroxidase) application in the hindbrain and spinal cord, discovered a cluster of midbrain neurons, so-called diencephalic/mesencephalic descending (D/MD) neurons (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999). The D/MD neurons were in a position where they could be contacted by the pineal ganglion cell axons and their activity was similar to that of the pineal. Furthermore, the axonal projections of the D/MD neurons to hindbrain and spinal cord (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999) indicate that midbrain neurons of the hatchling tadpole could make direct ipsilateral connections and excite the central pattern generator (CPG) network. Midbrain neuron – CPG connections remain unidentified in the tadpole.

There is also evidence to support the involvement of the tadpole midbrain in the initiation and maintenance of swimming in response to trunk skin stimulation. Axons of the trunk skin sensory Rohon-Beard (RB) neurons excite dorsolateral ascending (dla) and dorsolateral commissural (dlc) sensory pathway neurons (Roberts et al., 2010, 2019), whose axons have been shown to also reach the midbrain [see Figure 8 and (Li et al., 2001)]. This suggests that the midbrain might play a significant role in the trunk skin sensorimotor pathway. However, connections from the sensory pathway to midbrain neurons remain unknown. The sensory pathway neurons (dla and dlc) initiate or accelerate swimming by amplifying excitation (Li et al., 2001, 2004b), however, their contribution to the rostral CNS does not appear to influence the generation of tadpole swimming. The hatchling tadpole can still generate episodes of sustained swimming after removal of the rostral CNS (Li et al., 2006), as well as, the disconnection of the midbrain from the rest of the brainstem and spinal cord. We also confirm this here, in both behavioral and electrophysiological experiments transection along the midbrain-hindbrain (MHB) border does not prevent the generation of swimming per se. So, what might be the role of the midbrain within the trunk skin sensorimotor pathway?

To answer this question, we used a combination of midbrain lesions and trunk skin stimulation, in behavioral and electrophysiological settings, to identify the specific consequences of each midbrain lesion on kinematic parameters associated with the hatchling tadpole swimming. We reveal that the tadpole relies heavily on the midbrain for the timely and effective initiation and maintenance of swimming.

Timely and coordinated initiation of swimming is dependent on the midbrain

Our behavioral experiments show that following trunk skin stimulation control tadpoles initiate swimming at variable latencies and with preference for a contralateral start, which agrees with previous studies (Buhl et al., 2015; Koutsikou et al., 2018; Ferrario et al., 2021; Messa, 2022). Midbrain lesions affected both of those parameters to varying degrees, without preventing the generation and maintenance of locomotion. This agrees with studies on salamander and lamprey, at later stages of development, where just unilateral excitation of the MLR could lead to bilateral activation of reticulospinal neurons (Ryczko et al., 2016). The MHB (Midbrain/Hindbrain Border) and the Contra MHB + ML (right/Contralateral side of the MHB + Midline) lesions led to significant increases in the swim latency when the left side of the trunk skin was stimulated. First, this is in accordance with tadpole studies that identified a population of caudal hindbrain descending interneurons (hdINs) being sufficient for driving swimming in this animal even in the absence of the rostral brainstem (Li et al., 2006; Soffe et al., 2009). Second, studies have demonstrated that the contralateral to the stimulus side, which in our experiments is lesioned in MHB and Contra MHB + ML tadpoles, is the fastest and strongest (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999; Buhl et al., 2015). In support, the ML and Ipsi MHB + ML (left/ipsilateral to the stimulus) lesions, did not lead to significant increases in latency, most likely because in these lesions and unlike in the ones of MHB and Contra MHB + ML, sensory information, especially via the dlc pathway, could still reach the midbrain on the side contralateral to the stimulus and permit top-down descending control to be exerted by the fastest, contralateral pathway.

The behavioral results also indicate that disruption of input and output information from the contralateral midbrain, here in ML and Contra MHB + ML lesions, had stark effects on the side of swim initiation. Tadpoles with these lesions mostly responded to trunk skin stimulation with ipsilateral initiations (here the intact side of the CNS). This suggests that the blockade of the strongest, dlc pathway carrying sensory information to the contralateral midbrain together with the lack of (i) communication between the two sides of the midbrain and (ii) descending control from the contralateral midbrain force the animal to ipsilateral motor bias. Such functional lateralization and asymmetric motor behaviors have been seen in many species (Rogers, 2009), however, the underlying neural substrates and connecting pathways remain unknown. Recently, identification of an intrinsic lateralized behavior in zebrafish larvae showed that the left/right motor bias is dependent on neurons in the diencephalon that project to the habenula (Horstick et al., 2020). The rest of the lesions utilized here caused minor changes to the ratio of contralateral vs ipsilateral starts, thus sustaining the contralateral motor bias. Overall, these data highlight that even at this early stage of development, the midbrain can modulate the side and latency of swim initiation in response to trunk skin stimulation, which makes it essential to the timely and efficient initiation of this behavioral motor response.

The significant changes in latency to swim initiation following MHB lesions were further investigated in experiments of fictive swimming in hatchling tadpoles. As soon as swimming is initiated, the neuronal activity of hindbrain and spinal cord CPG alternates rhythmically between the two sides via reciprocal inhibition, which will be maintained during ongoing swimming (Roberts et al., 2010; Moult et al., 2013). Here we show that MHB-lesioned tadpoles exhibit synchronous oscillations at initiation, referred to as “synchrony,” a phenomenon that has been previously observed in the hatchling tadpole (Kahn and Roberts, 1982; Li et al., 2014). Simultaneous ventral root (VR) activity on opposite sides of the trunk is indicative of immobility until the animal can engage in an antiphase oscillatory muscle activity, which in a behavioral setting can lead to full body propulsion. Our data suggest that in the hatchling tadpole midbrain descending control is necessary for the generation of antiphasic activity patterns by contributing to the avoidance of synchrony during swim initiation. However, the synchronous VR events recorded from MHB animals do not fully explain the increase in latency to swim initiation observed in behavioral experiments. This is due to threshold stimulation of MHB tadpoles leading to shorter latencies despite the increase in synchronous events. It is unknown how the tadpole midbrain interacts with the brainstem and spinal cord to avoid synchronous initiations. We suggest that the midbrain might directly or indirectly promote downstream unilateral inhibition, which limits the firing of motoneurons and other CPG interneurons (Li et al., 2004a; Koyama et al., 2016; Liu and Hale, 2017; Koyama and Pujala, 2018). This explanation is also in line with studies showing that blockade of glycinergic inhibition leads to synchrony in neonatal rats (Cowley and Schmidt, 1995) and lamprey (Cohen and Harris-Warrick, 1984).

The midbrain is essential for the discrimination of stimulus strength

Shik et al. (1966, 1969) demonstrated, on the decerebrate cat, that MLR stimulation at different strengths produces different motor behaviors. Low-strength stimulation produced walking, while an increase in the stimulus strength led to trot and gallop type movements. The increase in stimulus strength leads to increase in recruitment of active MLR neurons. Similarly, we show in the young Xenopus that different stimulus strengths lead to distinct motor outcomes with the midbrain being essential for this sensory discrimination. The hatchling tadpole can discriminate stimulus saliency because electrical stimulation at different intensities (threshold vs suprathreshold) led to distinct patterns of swim initiation in control tadpoles. Suprathreshold stimulation shortened the median swim initiation latency of control animals to more than half, in comparison to threshold stimulation. In addition, the stronger stimulus led to a contralateral motor bias with a swim latency significantly shorter in comparison to the ipsilateral motor response latency (in the fewer occasions when the ipsilateral side started first). This difference between ipsilateral and contralateral latencies is not present in control tadpoles stimulated at threshold intensities. These data also agree with previously published results, that show suprathreshold stimulation leads to contralateral motor initiation bias, with no overlapping latencies between ipsilateral and contralateral motor responses (Zhao et al., 1998). The authors showed very short delay glycinergic IPSPs in motoneurons ipsilaterally to stimulation, whilst no inhibition was found in motoneurons on the contralateral side (Zhao et al., 1998). Interestingly, this glycinergic inhibition was reported only at higher strength stimuli and no IPSPs were recorded following threshold stimulation (Zhao et al., 1998). Based on the short latency of these IPSPs the authors proposed that ascending interneurons (aINs) might be responsible for the ipsilateral inhibition prior to the initiation of swimming (Zhao et al., 1998), which implies that dlas (the ipsilateral sensory pathway neurons) form synaptic contacts with aINs. This connection could explain how aINs would be responsible for the short-delayed inhibition recorded in ipsilateral motoneurons (Zhao et al., 1998). Our data here do not provide the underlying mechanistic differences, however, from an ethological point of view it is advantageous for the animal to move away from the source of a stimulus that can be potentially threatening to its survival.

The MHB lesion prevented all animals from maintaining the discrimination between different strengths of stimulation. The lesioned tadpoles responded to either stimulus (threshold vs suprathreshold) with similar latency, which differs from data from control animals responding with a faster swim initiation to suprathreshold stimulus.

Midbrain control of posture and swim trajectory

The variable change or loss of behavioral motor functions of the hatchling tadpole, due to distinct midbrain lesions, also include the swim trajectory, displacement, duration, and distance of swimming. Our data show that any type of ablation in the midbrain results in a deflection of the tail. This suggests that the midbrain is a source of activity before any external stimulus is applied and locomotion is initiated in response to that. However, the midbrain neurons responsible for this activity have not been identified. Our findings are consistent with previous literature in zebrafish, demonstrating that ablation of midbrain nMLF neurons also causes pronounced deflections of the tail (Gahtan et al., 2005; Thiele et al., 2014). We propose that the changes observed in the swim kinematics mentioned above are due to tail deflection, which changes the yaw angle between the head and the tail resulting in loss of postural control that cannot be sustained even after partial loss of midbrain descending control.

Identity of midbrain neurons involved in the trunk skin sensorimotor pathway

The light dimming sensorimotor pathway, from the pineal eye to D/MD neurons to the brainstem and spinal CPG and motoneurons (see summary in Figure 8) exhibits latencies from light dim to fictive swimming of 70–110 ms (Roberts, 1990). This range of onset latencies is very similar to the fictive swim latencies following trunk skin stimulation (Koutsikou et al., 2018). So the similarity in onset latencies between the two sensorimotor pathways indicates that the D/MD neurons might also be involved in the trunk skin sensorimotor pathway as third order neurons (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999), following synaptic connections from dla and dlc sensory pathway neurons which are activated by RB cells (Roberts et al., 2010) and whose axons can reach the midbrain [see summary in Figure 8; (Li et al., 2001)].

Although we cannot exclude the possibility that D/MD neuronal excitability contributes to trunk skin-evoked swimming, their contribution alone does not fully explain our findings. The ML (midbrain midline) lesion used in this study severs commissural connections between the two sides of the midbrain and leads to significant changes in kinematics of swim behavior in response to trunk skin stimulation. Previous data have shown that the identified D/MD neurons involved in the light dim pathway, do not have commissural axons (Jamieson and Roberts, 1999) and the dlc neurons that carry sensory information from the trunk skin to the brainstem do not cross the midline at the level of the midbrain (Roberts et al., 2010). Thus, we propose that the effects of ML lesion seen here are only possible if there is another group(s) of midbrain neurons that (i) respond to trunk skin excitation, via synaptic connections with dla and dlc neurons and (ii) can transfer this excitation to the opposite side at the level of the midbrain. Interestingly, HRP backfill experiments by Jamieson and Roberts (1999) identified the D/MD group in a cluster close to the midbrain-forebrain border, where these neurons could be contacted by the axons of the pineal ganglion cells (Foster and Roberts, 1982; Jamieson and Roberts, 1999). In the Jamieson and Roberts (1999) study the location of the HRP retrograde application in the hindbrain is defined as rostral to the 5th post-otic myotome. It is possible that the authors did not fully identify the population(s) of midbrain neurons that could be activated by trunk skin stimulation and have commissural axons that also descend to the hindbrain, because they restricted the area of HRP application. Based on the similarities of onset latencies described above, it is possible that the trunk skin-activated midbrain neurons might synapse onto rostral (pre-otic) hindbrain extension neurons [exNs with command properties; (Messa and Koutsikou, 2021)], whose recurrent excitatory population produces the relatively slow and variable onset to swimming in the tadpole (Koutsikou et al., 2018; Roberts et al., 2019; Ferrario et al., 2021). Further work is required to both identify the connections between midbrain neurons activated by trunk skin stimulation and their synaptic connections to exNs and supraspinal CPG.

Conclusion

Like all animals, the hatchling Xenopus tadpole needs to be able to navigate its environment and avoid predators. We identified that the midbrain of this animal contributes significantly to its survival by mediating postural control, as well as the timely and efficient swim initiation and maintenance. Thus, this animal’s primitive midbrain neurons share functions of sensorimotor descending control with midbrain and brainstem neurons in older and higher vertebrates. This makes the tadpole, with its very simple CNS, an ideal animal model to investigate the underlying circuitry of supraspinal sensorimotor control at the single cell level.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

This animal study was reviewed and approved by AWERB University of Kent.

Author contributions

ML, GM, and HJ performed experiments, analysed data, and worked and commented on the manuscript. SK conceptualised and designed the study and written, commented, and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The Physiological Society UK awarded a research grant to SK.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Arber, S., and Costa, R. M. (2018). Connecting neuronal circuits for movement. Science 360, 1403–1404. doi: 10.1126/science.aat5994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arber, S., and Costa, R. M. (2022). Networking brainstem and basal ganglia circuits for movement. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 342–360. doi: 10.1038/s41583-022-00581-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouvier, J., Caggiano, V., Leiras, R., Caldeira, V., Bellardita, C., Balueva, K., et al. (2015). Descending command neurons in the brainstem that halt locomotion. Cell 163, 1191–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, J. T., and Grillner, S. (1987). Newly identified ‘glutamate interneurons’ and their role in locomotion in the lamprey spinal cord. Science 236, 312–314. doi: 10.1126/science.3563512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buhl, E., Soffe, S. R., and Roberts, A. (2015). Sensory initiation of a co-ordinated motor response: Synaptic excitation underlying simple decision-making. J. Physiol. 593, 4423–4437. doi: 10.1113/JP270792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buhl, E., Soffe, S. R., Hull, M., and Roberts, A. (2012). “Making tadpole escape unpredictable: From behaviour to neurons,” in Proceedings of the tenth international congress of neuroethology, Bristol.

Google Scholar

Cabelguen, J. M., Bourcier-Lucas, C., and Dubuc, R. (2003). Bimodal locomotion elicited by electrical stimulation of the midbrain in the salamander Notophthalmus viridescens. J. Neurosci. 23, 2434–2439. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02434.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbo-Tano, M., Lapoix, M., Jia, X., Auclair, F., Dubuc, R., and Wyart, C. (2022). Functional coupling of the mesencephalic locomotor region and V2a reticulospinal neurons driving forward locomotion. bioRxiv [Preprint]. bioRxiv 2022.04.01.486703. doi: 10.1101/2022.04.01.486703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A. H., and Harris-Warrick, R. M. (1984). Strychnine eliminates alternating motor output during fictive locomotion in the lamprey. Brain Res. 293, 164–167. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)91464-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowley, K. C., and Schmidt, B. J. (1995). Effects of inhibitory amino acid antagonists on reciprocal inhibitory interactions during rhythmic motor activity in the in vitro neonatal rat spinal cord. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1109–1117. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.1109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daghfous, G., Green, W. W., Alford, S. T., Zielinski, B. S., and Dubuc, R. (2016). Sensory activation of command cells for locomotion and modulatory mechanisms: LESSONS from lampreys. Front. Neural Circuits 10:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubuc, R., Brocard, F., Antri, M., Fenelon, K., Gariepy, J. F., Smetana, R., et al. (2008). Initiation of locomotion in lampreys. Brain Res. Rev. 57, 172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Manira, A., and Grillner, S. (2014). Motion with direction and balance. Neuron 83, 515–517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrario, A., Palyanov, A., Koutsikou, S., Li, W., Soffe, S., Roberts, A., et al. (2021). From decision to action: Detailed modelling of frog tadpoles reveals neuronal mechanisms of decision-making and reproduces unpredictable swimming movements in response to sensory signals. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17:e1009654. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira-Pinto, M. J., Ruder, L., Capelli, P., and Arber, S. (2018). Connecting circuits for supraspinal control of locomotion. Neuron 100, 361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, R. G., and Roberts, A. (1982). The pineal eye in Xenopus laevis embryos and larvae: A photoreceptor with a direct excitatory effect on behaviour. J. Comp. Physiol. 145, 413–419. doi: 10.1007/BF00619346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gahtan, E., Tanger, P., and Baier, H. (2005). Visual prey capture in larval zebrafish is controlled by identified reticulospinal neurons downstream of the tectum. J. Neurosci. 25, 9294–9303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2678-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallois, B., and Candelier, R. (2021). FastTrack: An open-source software for tracking varying numbers of deformable objects. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17:e1008697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goulding, M. (2009). Circuits controlling vertebrate locomotion: Moving in a new direction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 507–518. doi: 10.1038/nrn2608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grillner, S., and El Manira, A. (2020). Current principles of motor control, with special reference to vertebrate locomotion. Physiol. Rev. 100, 271–320. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00015.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horstick, E. J., Bayleyen, Y., and Burgess, H. A. (2020). Molecular and cellular determinants of motor asymmetry in zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 11:1170. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14965-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, L. J., Zelenin, P. V., Orlovsky, G. N., and Deliagina, T. G. (2017). Supraspinal control of spinal reflex responses to body bending during different behaviours in lampreys. J. Physiol. 595, 883–900. doi: 10.1113/JP272714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, L. J. (2009). Initiation of motor responses in developing Xenopus laevis tadpoles. Ph.D. thesis. Bristol: University of Bristol.

Google Scholar

James, L. J., and Soffe, S. R. (2011). Skin impulse excitation of spinal sensory neurons in developing Xenopus laevis (Daudin) tadpoles. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 3341–3350. doi: 10.1242/jeb.058446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, D. (1997). Synaptic transmission in the pineal eye of young Xenopus laevis tadpoles: A role for NMDA and non-NMDA glutamate and non-glutaminergic receptors? J. Comp. Physiol. A 181, 177–186. doi: 10.1007/s003590050104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, D., and Roberts, A. (1999). A possible pathway connecting the photosensitive pineal eye to the swimming central pattern generator in young Xenopus laevis tadpoles. Brain Behav. Evol. 54, 323–337. doi: 10.1159/000006632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, D., and Roberts, A. (2000). Responses of young Xenopus laevis tadpoles to light dimming: Possible roles for the pineal eye. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 1857–1867. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.12.1857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, L. M., Liu, J., Hedlund, P. B., Akay, T., and Pearson, K. G. (2008). Descending command systems for the initiation of locomotion in mammals. Brain Res. Rev. 57, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, J. A., and Roberts, A. (1982). Experiments on the central pattern generator for swimming in amphibian embryos. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 296, 229–243. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiehn, O. (2006). Locomotor circuits in the mammalian spinal cord. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 279–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiehn, O. (2016). Decoding the organization of spinal circuits that control locomotion. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 224–238. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsikou, S., Apps, R., and Lumb, B. M. (2017). Top down control of spinal sensorimotor circuits essential for survival. J. Physiol. 595, 4151–4158. doi: 10.1113/JP273360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsikou, S., Crook, J. J., Earl, E. V., Leith, J. L., Watson, T. C., Lumb, B. M., et al. (2014). Neural substrates underlying fear-evoked freezing: The periaqueductal grey-cerebellar link. J. Physiol. 592, 2197–2213. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.268714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsikou, S., Merrison-Hort, R., Buhl, E., Ferrario, A., Li, W. C., Borisyuk, R., et al. (2018). A simple decision to move in response to touch reveals basic sensory memory and mechanisms for variable response times. J. Physiol. 596, 6219–6233. doi: 10.1113/JP276356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsikou, S., Watson, T. C., Crook, J. J., Leith, J. L., Lawrenson, C. L., Apps, R., et al. (2015). The periaqueductal gray orchestrates sensory and motor circuits at multiple levels of the neuraxis. J. Neurosci. 35, 14132–14147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0261-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koyama, M., and Pujala, A. (2018). Mutual inhibition of lateral inhibition: A network motif for an elementary computation in the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49, 69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koyama, M., Minale, F., Shum, J., Nishimura, N., Schaffer, C. B., and Fetcho, J. R. (2016). A circuit motif in the zebrafish hindbrain for a two alternative behavioral choice to turn left or right. Elife 5:e16808. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N. (2008). Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 195–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125547

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. C., Soffe, S. R., and Roberts, A. (2004b). Dorsal spinal interneurons forming a primitive, cutaneous sensory pathway. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 895–904. doi: 10.1152/jn.00024.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. C., Higashijima, S., Parry, D. M., Roberts, A., and Soffe, S. R. (2004a). Primitive roles for inhibitory interneurons in developing frog spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 24, 5840–5848. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1633-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. C., Merrison-Hort, R., Zhang, H. Y., and Borisyuk, R. (2014). The generation of antiphase oscillations and synchrony by a rebound-based vertebrate central pattern generator. J. Neurosci. 34, 6065–6077. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4198-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. C., Perrins, R., Soffe, S. R., Yoshida, M., Walford, A., and Roberts, A. (2001). Defining classes of spinal interneuron and their axonal projections in hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpoles. J. Comp. Neurol. 441, 248–265. doi: 10.1002/cne.1410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. C., Soffe, S. R., Wolf, E., and Roberts, A. (2006). Persistent responses to brief stimuli: Feedback excitation among brainstem neurons. J. Neurosci. 26, 4026–4035. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4727-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y. C., and Hale, M. E. (2017). Local spinal cord circuits and bilateral mauthner cell activity function together to drive alternative startle behaviors. Curr. Biol. 27, 697–704. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messa, G. (2022). Identification of hindbrain neural substrates for motor initiation in the hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpole. Ph.D. thesis. Canterbury: University of Kent.

Google Scholar

Messa, G., and Koutsikou, S. (2021). Distributed and diverse hindbrain neuronal activity contributes to sensory processing and motor control in the Xenopus laevis tadpole. bioRxiv [Preprint]. bioRxiv 2021.06.10.447865. doi: 10.1101/2021.06.10.447865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moult, P. R., Cottrell, G. A., and Li, W. C. (2013). Fast silencing reveals a lost role for reciprocal inhibition in locomotion. Neuron 77, 129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwkoop, P., and Faber, J. (1956). Normal table of Xenopus laevis (Daudin) a systematical and chronological survey of the develop ment from the fertilized egg till the end of metamorphosis. Amsterdam: North-Holland Pub. Co.

Google Scholar

Orlovsky, G. N., Deliagina, T. G., and Grillner, S. (1999). Neruonal control of locomotion: From mollusc to man. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524052.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A. (1978). Pineal eye and behaviour in Xenopus tadpoles. Nature 273, 774–775. doi: 10.1038/273774a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A. (1990). How does a nervous system produce behaviour? A case study in neurobiology. Sci. Prog. 74, 31–51.

Google Scholar

Roberts, A. (1996). Trigeminal pathway for the skin impulse to initiate swimming in hatchling Xenopus embryos. J. Physiol. 493P, 40–41.

Google Scholar

Roberts, A., and Clarke, J. D. (1982). The neuroanatomy of an amphibian embryo spinal cord. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 296, 195–212. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A., Borisyuk, R., Buhl, E., Ferrario, A., Koutsikou, S., Li, W. C., et al. (2019). The decision to move: Response times, neuronal circuits and sensory memory in a simple vertebrate. Proc. Biol. Sci. 286:20190297. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A., Hill, N. A., and Hicks, R. (2000). Simple mechanisms organise orientation of escape swimming in embryos and hatchling tadpoles of Xenopus laevis. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 1869–1885. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.12.1869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A., Li, W. C., and Soffe, S. R. (2010). How neurons generate behavior in a hatchling amphibian tadpole: An outline. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:16. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (2009). Hand and paw preferences in relation to the lateralized brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 943–954. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruder, L., and Arber, S. (2019). Brainstem circuits controlling action diversification. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 42, 485–504. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070918-050201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryczko, D., and Dubuc, R. (2013). The multifunctional mesencephalic locomotor region. Curr. Pharm. Des. 19, 4448–4470. doi: 10.2174/1381612811319240011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryczko, D., Auclair, F., Cabelguen, J. M., and Dubuc, R. (2016). The mesencephalic locomotor region sends a bilateral glutamatergic drive to hindbrain reticulospinal neurons in a tetrapod. J. Comp. Neurol. 524, 1361–1383. doi: 10.1002/cne.23911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Severi, K. E., Portugues, R., Marques, J. C., O’malley, D. M., Orger, M. B., and Engert, F. (2014). Neural control and modulation of swimming speed in the larval zebrafish. Neuron 83, 692–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shik, M. L., Severin, F. V., and Orlovskii, G. N. (1966). [Control of walking and running by means of electric stimulation of the midbrain]. Biofizika 11, 659–666.

Google Scholar

Shik, M. L., Severin, F. V., and Orlovsky, G. N. (1969). Control of walking and running by means of electrical stimulation of the mesencephalon. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 26:549.

Google Scholar

Sillar, K. T., and Roberts, A. (1988). A neuronal mechanism for sensory gating during locomotion in a vertebrate. Nature 331, 262–265. doi: 10.1038/331262a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sillar, K. T., and Roberts, A. (1992). Phase-dependent modulation of a cutaneous sensory pathway by glycinergic inhibition from the locomotor rhythm generator in Xenopus embryos. Eur. J. Neurosci. 4, 1022–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00129.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirota, M. G., Di Prisco, G. V., and Dubuc, R. (2000). Stimulation of the mesencephalic locomotor region elicits controlled swimming in semi-intact lampreys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 4081–4092. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00301.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smetana, R., Juvin, L., Dubuc, R., and Alford, S. (2010). A parallel cholinergic brainstem pathway for enhancing locomotor drive. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 731–738. doi: 10.1038/nn.2548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soffe, S. R., Roberts, A., and Li, W. C. (2009). Defining the excitatory neurons that drive the locomotor rhythm in a simple vertebrate: Insights into the origin of reticulospinal control. J. Physiol. 587, 4829–4844. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiele, T. R., Donovan, J. C., and Baier, H. (2014). Descending control of swim posture by a midbrain nucleus in zebrafish. Neuron 83, 679–691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uematsu, K., Baba, Y., Kake, Y., Ikenaga, T., Moon, S. J., Miyai, Y., et al. (2007). Central mechanisms underlying fish swimming. Brain Behav. Evol. 69, 142–150. doi: 10.1159/000095203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W. C., and McLean, D. L. (2014). Selective responses to tonic descending commands by temporal summation in a spinal motor pool. Neuron 83, 708–721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, F. Y., Wolf, E., and Roberts, A. (1998). Longitudinal distribution of components of excitatory synaptic input to motoneurones during swimming in young Xenopus tadpoles: Experiments with antagonists. J. Physiol. 511 (Pt 3), 887–901. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.887bg.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: midbrain, locomotion, swimming, Xenopus, descending control, brainstem, sensorimotor, tadpole

Citation: Larbi MC, Messa G, Jalal H and Koutsikou S (2022) An early midbrain sensorimotor pathway is involved in the timely initiation and direction of swimming in the hatchling Xenopus laevis tadpole. Front. Neural Circuits 16:1027831. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2022.1027831

Received: 25 August 2022; Accepted: 25 November 2022;
Published: 21 December 2022.

Edited by:

Frederic Bretzner, Laval University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Tuan Vu Bui, University of Ottawa, Canada
Henrik Boije, Uppsala University, Sweden

Copyright © 2022 Larbi, Messa, Jalal and Koutsikou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stella Koutsikou, S.Koutsikou@kent.ac.uk

Present Address: Giulia Messa, ICM, Paris Brain Institute, Paris, France

Download