Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 April 2013
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00132

A multimodal perspective on the composition of cortical oscillations

  • 1Aston Brain Centre, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, UK
  • 2Faculty of Science and Technology, School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Devon, UK

An expanding corpus of research details the relationship between functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures and neuronal network oscillations. Typically, integrated electroencephalography and fMRI, or parallel magnetoencephalography (MEG) and fMRI are used to draw inference about the consanguinity of BOLD and electrical measurements. However, there is a relative dearth of information about the relationship between E/MEG and the focal networks from which these signals emanate. Consequently, the genesis and composition of E/MEG oscillations requires further clarification. Here we aim to contribute to understanding through a series of parallel measurements of primary motor cortex (M1) oscillations, using human MEG and in vitro rodent local field potentials. We compare spontaneous activity in the ∼10 Hz mu and 15–30 Hz beta frequency ranges and compare MEG signals with independent and integrated layers III and V (LIII/LV) from in vitro recordings. We explore the mechanisms of oscillatory generation, using specific pharmacological modulation with the GABA-A alpha-1 subunit modulator zolpidem. Finally, to determine the contribution of cortico-cortical connectivity, we recorded in vitro M1, during an incision to sever lateral connections between M1 and S1 cortices. We demonstrate that frequency distribution of MEG signals appear have closer statistically similarity with signals from integrated rather than independent LIII/LV laminae. GABAergic modulation in both modalities elicited comparable changes in the power of the beta band. Finally, cortico-cortical connectivity in sensorimotor cortex (SMC) appears to directly influence the power of the mu rhythm in LIII. These findings suggest that the MEG signal is an amalgam of outputs from LIII and LV, that multiple frequencies can arise from the same cortical area and that in vitro and MEG M1 oscillations are driven by comparable mechanisms. Finally, cortico-cortical connectivity is reflected in the power of the SMC mu rhythm.

Introduction

The development of sophisticated non-invasive human neuroimaging methods has revolutionized our approach to understanding the function of the brain. In particular, the ubiquitous magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), capable of inferring the profile of neural activity from blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) measurements, has facilitated the rapid construction of a detailed functional map of the human brain. However, as a result of the indirect nature and temporal resolution of these measurements, it is important that further comparisons with direct electrophysiological measures are made, in order to properly extrapolate the information contained within the wealth of existing literature. For more than a decade, researchers have integrated functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) information with electrophysiological measures such as magnetoencephalography (MEG), in order to improve the outcomes of these approaches (Liu et al., 1998; Dale et al., 2000; Dale and Halgren, 2001).

Early work to understand the neural basis of fMRI, focused on the comparison between MEG evoked responses and fMRI BOLD (e.g., Ahlfors et al., 1999; Dale et al., 2000). However, the advent of contemporary MEG approaches, such as beamforming, enabled the localization and reconstruction of oscillatory activity in the cortex with greatly improved spatial resolution (Vrba and Robinson, 2001; Hillebrand et al., 2005). With a focus on functionally related changes, these approaches have enabled researchers to explore the relationship between rhythmic signature of proposed importance in neural processing and the corresponding BOLD response. Parallel experiments using both language and visual tasks revealed a close correspondence between the oscillatory signatures of MEG and BOLD (Singh et al., 2002). Further studies have revealed the complex nature of these relationships, by exploring multiple oscillatory signatures in comparison with fMRI BOLD activity. Brookes et al. (2005), using a visual paradigm, demonstrated a positive correlation between BOLD and sustained fields and gamma (>30 Hz) power and a negative correlation between BOLD and alpha power; these observations are supported by a subsequent study that reported differential effects at low and high frequencies (Zumer et al., 2010). A number of further studies have since described observations of correspondence between MEG oscillations and fMRI BOLD (for a review see Singh, 2012). However, while these studies describe strong support for the comparative identification of the involvement of specific brain regions in specific function, the correspondence between modalities is not always as clear-cut. Examples from visual experiments, where gamma oscillations are implicated in the processing of features such as contrast (Hall et al., 2005) and spatial frequency (Adjamian et al., 2004), reveal discrepancies between the processing of color stimuli with MEG (Adjamian et al., 2008) compared to BOLD (Mullen et al., 2007). Similarly, the tuning characteristics for spatial frequency appear different between these modalities (Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2008, 2009). In recent years, the importance of understanding the natural variability of neuronal network activity at rest, has become a topic of great interest. Accordingly, so-called “resting state” networks have been investigated using fMRI approaches and recent studies have applied parallel fMRI and MEG to determine the oscillatory counterparts to the fMRI observations (Brookes et al., 2011).

The relationship between neuronal network oscillations and the BOLD response is further described in comparisons between BOLD signatures and invasive recordings in animals, where BOLD appears to temporally correlate well with the local field potential (LFP), for example in the gamma range (Logothetis et al., 2001). However, while these relationships provide, arguably, a more direct reflection of the neuronal network activity underlying the BOLD signal, they begin to reveal the additional complexities that exist in relating these signals. For example, experiments in the rat somatosensory cortex reveal correspondence between the BOLD signal and LFP oscillatory activity. However, the use of multi-channel depth electrodes reveals a depth dependent response to stimulation, demonstrating differential relationships between neuronal oscillations and the BOLD response (Boorman et al., 2010). The simultaneous recording of LFP activity during fMRI in the human auditory cortex reveals that the BOLD response is closely related to the gamma activity and, importantly, the interneuron activity at the same location (Nir et al., 2007).

With regard to the aim of understanding the neural basis of the fMRI signal. High spatial and temporal resolution, non-invasive measures of human brain function such as MEG, are powerful measures for understanding the electrophysiological basis of fMRI. However, the limitations in our understanding of the physiological and pharmacological basis of the MEG signal, limit the extent to which we can draw conclusions about the basis of the fMRI signal.

Previous studies have developed models of the neuronal population that form the generators of the MEG signal, based upon the cortical architecture and cell physiology (for a review see Hamalainen, 1992). Further studies suggest that the MEG signal is an aggregate of 50000 (perhaps as few as 10000) synchronously firing pyramidal cells in layer three (LIII) and layer five (LV) of the cortex (Murakami and Okada, 2006). This is particularly convenient for the purpose of comparison with oscillations in vitro, as we know that these same populations of cells give rise to the spontaneous oscillatory activity. These oscillations are driven by phasic inhibition of principal cells by interneurons and are well described in both modeling studies (Traub et al., 1996) and empirical data (Whittington et al., 1995). These oscillations in the cortex, for example the sensorimotor cortex (SMC), are driven largely by GABAergic interneurons (Roopun et al., 2006; Yamawaki et al., 2008). Furthermore, experimental evidence from MEG suggests that these are the same oscillations that are measurable in the MEG signal (Jensen et al., 2005; Hall et al., 2010a,b, 2011; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2012). Further support for this comes from parallel MEG and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) studies that suggest that GABA concentration corresponds with oscillatory characteristics in various regions of the cortex (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009; Gaetz et al., 2011).

However, while these aspects appear to provide a strong basis for understanding the MEG signal, MEG measurement is such that a “virtual electrode” signal integrates the activity of >2 mm of cortical tissue (Barnes et al., 2004). Therefore, cortical signals measured with MEG will be an amalgam of signals from all cortical laminae in that location. In addition, as magnetic field strength decays with distance, the relative contributions of activity from superficial and deep cortical layers are unlikely to be equal. While the distance from sensor to cortical surface is undoubtedly greater than the distance between laminae, cortical thickness can be as great as 4.5 mm (Fischl and Dale, 2000) and therefore worthy of consideration.

As discussed, the predominant mechanism for ongoing oscillations are believed to be local interneuron generated activity. However, the extensive connectivity between the cortex and other cortical and sub-cortical structures results in the potential for great complexity in the generation of oscillations. For example, lower frequency oscillations, such as the alpha rhythm (7–14 Hz) are often believed to be an emergent property of thalamo-cortical loops (Rosanova et al., 2009). Furthermore, specific oscillations are often believed to originate from specific areas. For example, in the SMC the beta rhythm (15–30 Hz) is believed to arise in the primary motor cortex (M1) (Murthy and Fetz, 1992; Baker et al., 1997), whilst the mu rhythm (10 Hz) is believed to originate in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) (Salmelin and Hari, 1994). However, it is uncertain to what extent each of the measured oscillatory signatures are inter-cortical (within cortical region) and which are intra-cortical (between regions) dependent.

In order to understand the relevance of the comparisons between fMRI and MEG, it is important to understand the relationship between the MEG signal and the underlying neuronal network activity. In this paper, we aim to contribute to the understanding of this question, using parallel in vitro and MEG measurements of M1 to characterize the relationship between the aggregate MEG signal and the underlying cortical tissue.

We focus on three simple aspects: (1) The relative contributions of oscillations arising in superficial (LIII) and deep (LV) cortical laminae to the MEG signal. (2) The comparative mechanisms of oscillations observed in vitro and with MEG. (3) The influence of cortico-cortical connectivity on the oscillations observed in vitro from M1.

Materials and Methods

Data Acquisition

In each experiment, the focus for comparison was M1. The process for localizing M1 with MEG and preparing the M1 slice in vitro was identical in each experiment; these processes are described in the following sections.

MEG

In each experiment, participants with normal or corrected to normal vision, were seated in a 275-channel MEG system (CTF Systems, Canada). MEG data were acquired at a sampling rate of 1200 Hz using a third order gradiometer configuration with a 50-Hz notch filter and a 1–300-Hz low/high pass filter. MEG data were co-registered with the individual participant’s anatomical MRI, obtained using a 3-Tesla MRI system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany), by surface matching a three-dimensional digitization of the participants scalp created using a Polhemus Isotrak system (Kaiser Aerospace Inc.). Head position was monitored throughout by matching the digitized position of three surface-mounted electromagnetic positioning coils (left and right pre-auricular and nasion), which were then monitored throughout the recording process. In each experiment, participants performed a series of 60 left and right index finger abduction movements, approximately every 6 s; finger movements were monitored using electromyography (EMG) of the first dorsal interosseus muscle. Here the left M1 cortex was localized (Figure 1A) using the synthetic aperture magnetometry (SAM) beamforming method (Vrba and Robinson, 2001; Hillebrand et al., 2005). Specifically, with time-zero defined as the offset in EMG power following movement, defined as a reduction below three standard-deviations of the baseline, the post-movement beta rebound (PMBR) was localized by comparing the change in beta (15–30 Hz) frequency power following movement termination (0.5–1.0 s) with the pre-movement beta power (−2.0 to −1.5 s), comparable to the methods described by Jurkiewicz et al. (2006). In each MEG experiment, the envelope of neuronal network activity in M1 was reconstructed during a period of inactive rest, using the virtual electrode method previously described (Hall et al., 2010a, 2011).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Recording Methods for MEG and in vitro. (A) Example of the M1 localization in a participant based upon the identification of the PMBR response following movement. (B) Schematic of a sagittal rat brain slice, showing the location of M1 and S1. Black dotted line show where cuts were made in preparation for all recordings. Solid black lines show the positioning of the microelectrodes in the slice for recording LIII and LV LFPs and the red dashed line show the position of the cut made between M1 and S1 in experiment 3.

In Vitro

Recordings were made in sagittal brain slices obtained from p18 to p22 male Wistar rats (40–60 g), 450 μm thick, containing M1 and S1 (Figure 1B). All experiments were performed in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986, European Communities Directive 1986 (86/609/EEC) and the Aston University ethical review documents. Every effort was made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering. Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and, following decapitation, the brain removed and placed in modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF). The aCSF solution was composed with the recipe (in mM): 206 sucrose, 2 KCl, 1.6 MgSO4, 25.9 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 10 glucose, 2.16 CaCl2, and indomethacin 45 nM (Pakhotin et al., 1997), saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 at pH 7.3 and 310 mOsm. Brain slices were prepared using a microslicer (Campden Instruments, Loughborough, UK) and, prior to recording, maintained at room temperature in aCSF containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 4.0 KCl, 0.5 MgSO4, 25.9 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 10 glucose, and 2 CaCl2 for at least 60 min. For recording, slices were placed in an interface recording chamber (Scientific System Design Inc., Canada) and superfused with aCSF at a flow rate of 2 ml/min maintained at 34°C. Spontaneous oscillatory activity was restored using the addition of carbachol (50 μm) and kainic acid (400 nM) (Sigma Ltd., Gillingham, UK) as previously described (Yamawaki et al., 2008). Extracellular LFP recordings were made with electrodes constructed of chlorided silver wire inserted into a borosilicate glass electrode filled with aCSF of resistance 1–3 MΩ. The glass electrodes, attached to micro-manipulators (Narashige, Japan) were inserted in LIII and LV of the M1, determined using a stereotaxic anatomical rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2006). Signals were and amplified 1000-fold, low-pass filtered at 500 Hz and notch filtered at 50 Hz (EXT 10-2F, npi electronic GmbH, Germany). Data were digitized at 10 kHz by an analog to digital converter (CED 1401, CED Ltd., UK) and signal recording made was with Spike2 software (CED Ltd., UK). The signal was then downsampled to 1 kHz and exported to MatLab (Mathworks, Inc.) for further analysis.

Experiment 1

In vitro Brain slices (n = 36) were prepared using the protocol described above and individual recordings made of spontaneous LFP activity from electrodes sited in LIII and LV (Figure 1B). These data were also integrated to produce an aggregate signal of deep and superficial M1 for comparison with MEG data. MEG Recordings were made from 13 healthy participants (7 M), with a mean age of 35.2 years, using the protocol described above. M1 was localized using SAM analysis and virtual electrode activity reconstructed during two periods of inactive rest (60 s). Data from individual LIII/LV in vitro electrode recordings and MEG virtual electrode M1 recordings were analyzed using Morlet-wavelet time-frequency analysis to identify the spontaneous oscillatory activity. The power-spectral-density (PSD) functions were determined for individual LIII and LV and the aggregate signal from in vitro M1 and compared to MEG M1. The relative contributions of deep and superficial layers were explored by weighting the ratio of LIII:LV power. The statistical difference between the spectral composition of the MEG signal and each in vitro weighting, were compared by computing the Kolmogorov–Smirnov (KS) D-statistic for each ratio using a custom script (Matlab, Mathworks, USA), as described previously (Baranauskas et al., 2012). The output of this is a measure of the statistical similarity between the oscillatory signatures of two timeseries.

Experiment 2

In vitro brain slices (n = 17) were prepared using the protocol described above and individual recordings made of spontaneous LFP activity from electrodes sited in LIII and LV (Figure 1B). Ten minutes of control data were recorded, following which the GABA-A alpha-1 subunit modulator zolpidem (100 nM, Tocris Bioscience, Bristol, UK), was added to the aCSF perfusate. Following 45-min of incubation the spontaneous data were recorded from the same locations for a further 10-min.

Magnetoencephalography Recordings were made from eight healthy participants (6 M), with a mean age of 37.2 years, using the protocol described above. Participants underwent an initial control recording lasting 15-min, which included the functional M1 localizer task described above and two additional periods of inactive rest (60 s). Following the control recording, participants were administered zolpidem (0.05 mg/kg, Sanofi Aventis). Oral administration of 0.05–0.10 mg/kg produces a plasma concentration in the range 14–93 ng/ml (Olubodun et al., 2003; Greenblatt et al., 2006). As zolpidem is approximately 92% bound by plasma proteins (Salvà and Costa, 1995), these reports suggest that the effective concentration of zolpidem in vivo will be in the nanomolar range. Therefore, here we have used 100 nM as the comparative dose in our in vitro perfusion. Approximately 45-min following administration an identical recording to the control was made. Spontaneous M1 data from in vitro electrode recordings and MEG virtual electrode M1 recordings were analyzed using Morlet-wavelet time-frequency analysis and relative power change in the spontaneous oscillatory activity, in the pre and post zolpidem conditions, were visualized as normalized PSD. The power of the beta (15–30 Hz) and Mu (∼10 Hz) peaks was computed as an average of the normalized individual recordings and the statistical differences calculated using t-tests. The PSD functions were determined for individual LIII and LV and the aggregate signal from in vitro M1 and compared to MEG M1.

Experiment 3

Experiment 3 used an in vitro approach only, in which slices (n = 12) were prepared using the same process described above. Slices were placed in the recording chamber and, following the acclimatization period, electrodes inserted into LIII and LV. Control data were recorded for 10-min, following which a custom made cutting device, comprising a ceramic blade, attached to a micro-manipulator (Narashige, Japan), was used to make an incision at the border of M1 and S1 (Figure 1B). All connection between M1 and S1 was severed over the course of 2 min (∼3.75 to 6 m/s), to avoid disturbing the ongoing recording or electrode positioning. Following incision, data were recorded for a further 10-min. The 60-s of data recorded pre and post cut from LIII and LV were analyzed using Morlet-wavelet time-frequency analysis and were visualized as normalized PSD. The statistical differences in the power of LV beta (15–30 Hz) and LIII mu (∼10 Hz) frequency peaks were calculated using t-tests.

Results

Experiment 1

Time-frequency analysis using the Morlet-wavelet approach reveals that in LIII of the in vitro recording, there were constant peaks at beta and mu frequencies (Figure 2A), whereas LV exhibited a peak only at beta frequency (Figure 2B). However, the MEG signal exhibited peaks at both beta and mu frequencies (Figure 2C), as expected from recording of the SMC (Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989). Integration of the signals from LIII and LV appear to present a signal that is more comparable with the MEG in terms of mu and beta amplitude (Figure 2D). Furthermore, adjustment of the weighting of these signals toward the contribution of LIII, appears to provide a better representation of the ration of mu and beta power observed in MEG (Figure 2E). Analysis of the statistical difference between these ratios using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov reveals that as signal is weighted toward LIII, the statistical difference between the signals is reduced (Figure 2F).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Laminar Contribution to MEG M1 Oscillations. (A–C) Show normalized group Morlet-wavelet time-frequency representations of the spontaneous activity in in vitro LIII, LV, and MEG M1 signals over 30 s. (D) Shows the PSD profiles of spontaneous LIII, LV, combined in vitro LIII + LV and MEG signals. (E) Shows a comparison of the group MEG PSD and the integrated in vitro signals with various weightings of LIII and LV. (F) Shows the Kolmogorov–Smirnov statistic for the comparison between MEG data and integrated in vitro data with various weightings. The red line shows the p = 0.05 threshold, below which signals are not statistically different.

Experiment 2

The normalized PSD measures suggest an increase in power in the mu and beta range in both the integrated in vitro (Figure 3A) and MEG (Figure 3D) M1 recordings. Analysis of the peak power revealed significant increases in the mean beta power in MEG (28%, t = 2.5, p = 0.04) and integrated in vitro (82%, t = 2.21, p = 0.04). However, the increase in mean power of the mu rhythm was not significant in either MEG (25%, t = 1.84, p = 0.11) or integrated in vitro (85%, t = 1.89, p = 0.10) signals. Interestingly, when considering the individual laminae data, as expected there is a prominent beta peak in LV (Figure 3B), which shows a significant increase following zolpidem administration (67%, t = 2.2, p = 0.043). Similarly, there is a peak at beta frequency in LIII (Figure 3C) that shows an increase following zolpidem (96%, t = 2.47, p = 0.025). However, the mu peak present in LIII, also shows a significant increase following zolpidem (86%, t = 2.13, p = 0.049). The difference in mu between the integrated in vitro M1 and independent LIII, is a reflection of the low amplitude of the LIII signal compared to LV, consistent with previous observations (Yamawaki et al., 2008).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. GABAergic Modulation of M1 Oscillations. (A–D) Shows the normalized group PSD profiles, before and after zolpidem. (A) Shows integrated layer 3 and layer 5 in vitro M1, (B) shows layer 5 only, (C) shows layer 3 only, and (D) shows MEG M1 before and after the administration of zolpidem. (E) Shows the mean increase (±SD) in power, following zolpidem, of mu and beta peaks in MEG (left), combined “In vitro (c)” layers 3 and 5 (middle) and independent “In vitro (i)” layers 3 and 5 (right).

Experiment 3

Analysis of the in vitro signal revealed that, following severance of the M1–S1 connection, there was a significant increase it the mean power of the mu rhythm in LIII (Figures 4A,C) (156%, t = 6.84, p = 0.006). However, a modest increase in the mean beta power in LV (Figures 4B,C) was non-significant (10%, t = 1.19, p = 0.26).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Influence of Cortico-cortical Connectivity on M1 Oscillations. (A,B) Show the normalized group PSD plots for layer 3 (A) and layer 5 (B) before and after the cut between M1 and S1 (see Figure 1B). (C) Shows the normalized mean group power of mu and beta peaks in M1 pre and post cut.

Discussion

These findings suggest that MEG oscillations are an amalgamation of local field activity of layers III and V, consistent with previous predictions (Hamalainen, 1992; Murakami and Okada, 2006). In the case of the motor cortex, these data suggest that the mu and beta rhythms that are characteristic of observations of activity with electroencephalography (EEG) and MEG (Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989), are both represented within M1 in the in vitro signal. Based upon assessment of the GABAergic modulation of these signals, their response consistency supports the view that their generation is analogous between modalities. There are differences in the oscillatory profiles of the different laminae, which offers a mechanistic basis for differential modulation of these signals during function. This is further supported by the observation that connectivity between M1 and S1 produces profound effects on the superficial mu rhythm, but not deep beta rhythm. This observation may reflect a physiological basis, in the form of specific cortico-cortical projections, for the observations of independent pharmacological modulation of oscillations in the mu and beta range observed in somatosensory areas previously (Roopun et al., 2006).

Composition of the Neuroimaging Signal

These experiments present evidence that the M1 exhibits multiple oscillations simultaneously. In addition to the well-characterized beta rhythm, we show for the first time, that an M1 mu rhythm also exists. These data contradict the view that these rhythms are generated by different cortical areas. Specifically, there are generators of mu in M1, which repudiates the concept of an exclusive S1 generator (Salmelin and Hari, 1994). However, it is important to consider the limitations involved with attributing MEG data to a single cortical area. Specifically, because of the close proximity of M1 and S1, it is quite possible that the virtual electrode profile of M1 will contain some contribution from S1. This is important when trying to attribute mu and beta contributions to deep and superficial M1.

Recent combined EEG and fMRI studies suggest that an inverse correlation between post-central BOLD amplitude and EEG rolandic mu, indicates that the generator of this rhythm lies in S1 (Ritter et al., 2009). While this multimodal approach offers a powerful combination for associating the functional coincidence of observations, it also highlights the difficulty in drawing conclusions from such parallel measurements. Specifically, we observe that connectivity between S1 and M1 has a profound effect on the amplitude of the mu rhythm (Figure 4A). Therefore, it is absolutely plausible that changes in neuronal network activity in S1, would modify the strength of connectivity between S1 and M1 and therefore mu power. Therefore, modulation of the BOLD signal by large metabolic changes in S1 could correspond to mu changes arising in M1. Furthermore, it is entirely possible that spontaneous mu rhythms are generated at multiple locations throughout the SMC, as is the case for beta rhythms (Roopun et al., 2006; Yamawaki et al., 2008). However, these data demonstrate that while various cortical rhythms may be under the influence of connectivity with other areas, these M1 beta and mu rhythms are not emergent properties of thalamo-cortical loops as suggested elsewhere (Rosanova et al., 2009).

Mechanisms of Neuroimaging Signals

The data described here, suggest that the signals observed with MEG are a good reflection of the underlying activity in the cortex as measured in vitro. However, they also demonstrate that the composition of that signal is dependent upon the balance of contribution from the different laminae (Figure 2E), with an importance on the weighting toward superficial layers (Figure 2F), consistent with the principles of magnetic field decay in MEG (Hamalainen, 1992). This raises some important questions about the comparison of MEG with fMRI, as the BOLD signal is not subject to the same weighting of signals; all laminae have equal potential to contribute to the signal. This has important connotations for the attribution of BOLD changes to spatially coincident oscillatory changes. For example, where the tuning characteristics of signals in MEG are not consistent with those of BOLD (Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2008, 2009), this may be explained by differential laminar responses and the effect of signal weighting in MEG.

The comparable effects of GABAergic modulation in both models here (Figure 3), goes some way to increasing confidence in the analogous mechanisms of oscillatory generation. Specifically, this supports the view that motor cortex beta rhythms are under the control of previously described phasic entrainment by GABAergic interneurons (Whittington et al. (1995); Traub et al., 1996; Roopun et al., 2006; Yamawaki et al., 2008). This is consistent with previous MEG studies that describe a relationship between GABA concentration, or GABAergic modulation and beta in the motor cortex (Jensen et al., 2005; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009, 2012; Hall et al., 2010a,b, 2011; Gaetz et al., 2011). However, there are questions regarding the extent to which the sensorimotor mu rhythm is controlled by GABAergic mechanisms, refuted in previous MEG studies (Hall et al., 2010a). Given that recent studies demonstrate a direct relationship between GABA concentration and the characteristics of the hemodynamic response (Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2009; Donahue et al., 2010), an understanding of the proportion and profile of neural oscillations under GABAergic, and other, control is vital in order to obtain a comprehensive model of fMRI signals. However, while the concentration used in the in vitro experiment (100 nM) is believed to be of comparable physiological concentration to the 0.05-mg/kg dose used in the MEG experiment. It is important to bear in mind the variability of pharmacokinetics and drug metabolism in human participants are a limitation to the precise comparisons here.

Cortical Connectivity and Neuroimaging Signals

The data described here, demonstrate that the presence of oscillations in the motor cortex is not dependant upon connection with other cortical or sub-cortical locations. This suggests that neither beta nor mu rhythms per se are emergent properties of connectivity with locations such as the thalamus. However, the observation of increased power at mu frequency demonstrates that functional modulation of the power of an intrinsic oscillation can be the result of modulated connectivity with other areas. With regard to the mu rhythm, the data suggest that a reduction in the input to M1 produces an increase in mu power, from which it follows that an increase in the input to M1 may result in a reduction in mu power. A number of studies have demonstrated changes in the power of the mu rhythm during movement preparation, performance and recovery (Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989; Pfurtscheller et al., 1997; Leocani et al., 2001). These data would suggest that these phases are indicative of reduced connectivity between M1 and S1 or other areas during movement phases. Recent neuroimaging studies suggest that elevated mu prior to movement is a predictor of error in movement tasks (Mazaheri et al., 2009); which may be the result of a reduced connectivity during this phase. With regard to beta oscillatory power, connectivity with S1 did not influence the power in M1. This suggests that reductions in power during the preparation and execution phases of movement (Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989), are more likely the result of drive from cortical or sub-cortical areas other than S1; pre-motor areas appear to be of importance in this respect.

In addition, it is important to note that in the in vitro slice preparation, even before severing the connections between M1 and S1, no connections exist between this cortical area and other nodes of the motor network such as the basal ganglia. This highlights the important point, that the beta rhythm observed in motor cortex is not simply an emergent property of its connection with these structures. Conversely, it is unclear to what extent the nature of these oscillations is altered by the loss of connection with these structures. These questions could be addressed more comprehensively by the addition of in vivo measurements to those presented here.

Conclusion

The present study demonstrates the commonalities between in vitro and MEG signals and provides strong support for the postulated source and mechanisms of signals recorded non-invasively in humans. However, it raises a number of important considerations that are particularly important when attempting to infer mechanisms of indirect measures of neuronal function, such as the fMRI BOLD response. Firstly, the fact that multiple oscillatory signatures arise within the same cortical area. Secondly, that these rhythms have independent mechanisms of generation and may, therefore, be modulated independently of one another. Finally, the role of connectivity between areas in the generation of oscillatory activity, should not be underestimated when interpreting the neural basis of non-invasively measured signals. In summary, while strong overlaps exist between MEG and fMRI signals, the subtleties of the intra- and inter-cortical constitution will influence this relationship.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) and Parkinson’s UK for supporting these projects. Also, ARCHA for supporting the in vitro electrophysiology laboratory and the Dr. Hadwen Trust for supporting the MEG laboratory.

References

Adjamian, P., Hadjipapas, A., Barnes, G. R., Hillebrand, A., and Holliday, I. E. (2008). Induced Gamma activity in primary visual cortex is related to luminance and not color contrast: an MEG study. J. Vis. 8, 4–7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adjamian, P., Holliday, I. E., Barnes, G. R., Hillebrand, A., Hadjipapas, A., and Singh, K. D. (2004). Induced visual illusions and gamma oscillations in human primary visual cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 587–592.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ahlfors, S. P., Simpson, G. V., Dale, A. M., Belliveau, J. W., Liu, A. K., Korvenoja, A., et al. (1999). Spatiotemporal activity of a cortical network for processing visual motion revealed by MEG and fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 2545–2555.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Baker, S. N., Olivier, E., and Lemon, R. N. (1997). Coherent oscillations in monkey motor cortex and hand muscle EMG show task-dependent modulation. J. Physiol. 501(Pt 1), 225–241.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baranauskas, G., Maggiolini, E., Vato, A., Angotzi, G., Bonfanti, A., Zambra, G., et al. (2012). Origins of 1/f2 scaling in the power spectrum of intracortical local field potential. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 984–994.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barnes, G. R., Hillebrand, A., Fawcett, I. P., and Singh, K. D. (2004). Realistic spatial sampling for MEG beamformer images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 23, 120–127.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boorman, L., Kennerley, A. J., Johnston, D., Jones, M., Zheng, Y., Redgrave, P., et al. (2010). Negative blood oxygen level dependence in the rat: a model for investigating the role of suppression in neurovascular coupling. J. Neurosci. 30, 4285–4294.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brookes, M. J., Gibson, A. M., Hall, S. D., Furlong, P. L., Barnes, G. R., Hillebrand, A., et al. (2005). GLM-beamformer method demonstrates stationary field, alpha ERD and gamma ERS co-localisation with fMRI BOLD response in visual cortex. Neuroimage 26, 302–308.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brookes, M. J., Woolrich, M., Luckhoo, H., Price, D., Hale, J. R., Stephenson, M. C., et al. (2011). Investigating the electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using magnetoencephalography. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 16783–16788.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dale, A. M., and Halgren, E. (2001). Spatiotemporal mapping of brain activity by integration of multiple imaging modalities. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 202–208.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D., et al. (2000). Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Donahue, M. J., Near, J., Blicher, J. U., and Jezzard, P. (2010). Baseline GABA concentration and fMRI response. Neuroimage 53, 392–398.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fischl, B., and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 11050–11055.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaetz, W., Edgar, J. C., Wang, D. J., and Roberts, T. P. (2011). Relating MEG measured motor cortical oscillations to resting gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration. Neuroimage 55, 616–621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greenblatt, D. J., Legangneux, E., Harmatz, J. S., Weinling, E., Freeman, J., Rice, K., et al. (2006). Dynamics and kinetics of a modified-release formulation of zolpidem: comparison with immediate-release standard zolpidem and placebo. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 46, 1469–1480.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, S. D., Barnes, G. R., Furlong, P. L., Seri, S., and Hillebrand, A. (2010a). Neuronal network pharmacodynamics of GABAergic modulation in the human cortex determined using pharmaco-magnetoencephalography. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 581–594.

Hall, S. D., Yamawaki, N., Fisher, A. E., Clauss, R. P., Woodhall, G. L., and Stanford, I. M. (2010b). GABA(A) alpha-1 subunit mediated desynchronization of elevated low frequency oscillations alleviates specific dysfunction in stroke – a case report. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 549–555.

CrossRef Full Text

Hall, S. D., Holliday, I. E., Hillebrand, A., Singh, K. D., Furlong, P. L., Hadjipapas, A., et al. (2005). The missing link: analogous human and primate cortical gamma oscillations. Neuroimage 26, 13–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, S. D., Stanford, I. M., Yamawaki, N., McAllister, C. J., Ronnqvist, K. C., Woodhall, G. L., et al. (2011). The role of GABAergic modulation in motor function related neuronal network activity. Neuroimage 56, 1506–1510.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamalainen, M. S. (1992). Magnetoencephalography: a tool for functional brain imaging. Brain Topogr. 5, 95–102.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hillebrand, A., Singh, K. D., Holliday, I. E., Furlong, P. L., and Barnes, G. R. (2005). A new approach to neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 199–211.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jensen, O., Goel, P., Kopell, N., Pohja, M., Hari, R., and Ermentrout, B. (2005). On the human sensorimotor-cortex beta rhythm: sources and modeling. Neuroimage 26, 347–355.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jurkiewicz, M. T., Gaetz, W. C., Bostan, A. C., and Cheyne, D. (2006). Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: evidence from neuromagnetic recordings. Neuroimage, 32, 1281–1289.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leocani, L., Toro, C., Zhuang, P., Gerloff, C., and Hallett, M. (2001). Event-related desynchronization in reaction time paradigms: a comparison with event-related potentials and corticospinal excitability. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 923–930.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu, A. K., Belliveau, J. W., and Dale, A. M. (1998). Spatiotemporal imaging of human brain activity using functional MRI constrained magnetoencephalography data: Monte Carlo simulations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 8945–8950.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., and Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazaheri, A., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., van, D. H., and Jensen, O. (2009). Prestimulus alpha and mu activity predicts failure to inhibit motor responses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1791–1800.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mullen, K. T., Dumoulin, S. O., McMahon, K. L., de Zubicaray, G. I., and Hess, R. F. (2007). Selectivity of human retinotopic visual cortex to S-cone-opponent, L/M-cone-opponent and achromatic stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 491–502.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murakami, S., and Okada, Y. (2006). Contributions of principal neocortical neurons to magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography signals. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 575, 925–936.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murthy, V. N., and Fetz, E. E. (1992). Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 5670–5674.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Edden, R. A., Jones, D. K., Swettenham, J. B., and Singh, K. D. (2009). Resting GABA concentration predicts peak gamma frequency and fMRI amplitude in response to visual stimulation in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8356–8361.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Myers, J. F., Wilson, S. J., Nutt, D. J., Lingford-Hughes, A., Singh, K. D., et al. (2012). The effects of elevated endogenous GABA levels on movement-related network oscillations. Neuroimage 66C, 36–41.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., and Singh, K. D. (2008). Spatiotemporal frequency tuning of BOLD and gamma band MEG responses compared in primary visual cortex. Neuroimage 40, 1552–1560.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., and Singh, K. D. (2009). Functional decoupling of BOLD and gamma-band amplitudes in human primary visual cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2000–2007.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nir, Y., Fisch, L., Mukamel, R., Gelbard-Sagiv, H., Arieli, A., Fried, I., et al. (2007). Coupling between neuronal firing rate, gamma LFP, and BOLD fMRI is related to interneuronal correlations. Curr. Biol. 17, 1275–1285.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olubodun, J. O., Ochs, H. R., von Moltke, L. L., Roubenoff, R., Hesse, L. M., Harmatz, J. S., et al. (2003). Pharmacokinetic properties of zolpidem in elderly and young adults: possible modulation by testosterone in men. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 56, 297–304.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pakhotin, P. I., Pakhotina, I. D., and Andreev, A. A. (1997). Functional stability of hippocampal slices after treatment with cyclooxygenase inhibitors. Neuroreport 8, 1755–1759.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (2006). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates: Hard Cover Edition. Burlington: Academic press.

Pfurtscheller, G., and Berghold, A. (1989). Patterns of cortical activation during planning of voluntary movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 72, 250–258.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., Andrew, C., and Edlinger, G. (1997). Foot and hand area mu rhythms. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 26, 121–135.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ritter, P., Moosmann, M., and Villringer, A. (2009). Rolandic alpha and beta EEG rhythms’ strengths are inversely related to fMRI-BOLD signal in primary somatosensory and motor cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1168–1187.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roopun, A. K., Middleton, S. J., Cunningham, M. O., Lebeau, F. E., Bibbig, A., Whittington, M. A., et al. (2006). A beta2-frequency (20–30 Hz) oscillation in nonsynaptic networks of somatosensory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 15646–15650.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosanova, M., Casali, A., Bellina, V., Resta, F., Mariotti, M., and Massimini, M. (2009). Natural frequencies of human corticothalamic circuits. J. Neurosci. 29, 7679–7685.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salmelin, R., and Hari, R. (1994). Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement. Neuroscience 60, 537–550.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salvà, P., and Costa, J. (1995). Clinical pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of zolpidem. Therapeutic implications. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 29, 142–153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singh, K. D., Barnes, G. R., Hillebrand, A., Forde, E. M., and Williams, A. L. (2002). Task-related changes in cortical synchronization are spatially coincident with the hemodynamic response. Neuroimage 16, 103–114.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singh, K. D. (2012). Which “neural activity” do you mean? fMRI, MEG, oscillations and neurotransmitters. Neuroimage 62, 1121–1130.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Traub, R. D., Whittington, M. A., Stanford, I. M., and Jefferys, J. G. (1996). A mechanism for generation of long-range synchronous fast oscillations in the cortex. Nature 383, 621–624.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vrba, J., and Robinson, S. E. (2001). Signal processing in magnetoencephalography. Methods 25, 249–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whittington, M. A., Traub, R. D., and Jefferys, J. G. (1995). Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation. Nature 373, 612–615.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yamawaki, N., Stanford, I. M., Hall, S. D., and Woodhall, G. L. (2008). Pharmacologically induced and stimulus evoked rhythmic neuronal oscillatory activity in the primary motor cortex in vitro. Neuroscience 151, 386–395.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zumer, J. M., Brookes, M. J., Stevenson, C. M., Francis, S. T., and Morris, P. G. (2010). Relating BOLD fMRI and neural oscillations through convolution and optimal linear weighting. Neuroimage 49, 1479–1489.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: motor cortex, magnetoencephalography, local field potential, oscillation, gamma-aminobutyric acid, beta rhythm, mu rhythm, BOLD fMRI

Citation: Ronnqvist KC, McAllister CJ, Woodhall GL, Stanford IM and Hall SD (2013) A multimodal perspective on the composition of cortical oscillations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:132. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00132

Received: 17 January 2013; Accepted: 25 March 2013;
Published online: 10 April 2013.

Edited by:

Matthew Jon Brookes, University of Nottingham, UK

Reviewed by:

Peter Liddle, University of Nottingham, UK
Suresh Muthukumaraswamy, Cardiff University, UK

Copyright: © 2013 Ronnqvist, McAllister, Woodhall, Stanford and Hall. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Stephen D. Hall, School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Drake Circus, Plymouth, Devon PL4 8AA, UK. e-mail: hallsd@aston.ac.uk

Download