Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 August 2019
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00288

Beyond the Low Frequency Fluctuations: Morning and Evening Differences in Human Brain

  • 1Neuroimaging Group, Malopolska Centre of Biotechnology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
  • 2Department of Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroergonomics, Institute of Applied Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
  • 3Chair of Radiology, Medical College, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin—Oshkosh, Oshkosh, WI, United States

Human performance, alertness, and most biological functions express rhythmic fluctuations across a 24-h-period. This phenomenon is believed to originate from differences in both circadian and homeostatic sleep-wake regulatory processes. Interactions between these processes result in time-of-day modulations of behavioral performance as well as brain activity patterns. Although the basic mechanism of the 24-h clock is conserved across evolution, there are interindividual differences in the timing of sleep-wake cycles, subjective alertness and functioning throughout the day. The study of circadian typology differences has increased during the last few years, especially research on extreme chronotypes, which provide a unique way to investigate the effects of sleep-wake regulation on cerebral mechanisms. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we assessed the influence of chronotype and time-of-day on resting-state functional connectivity. Twenty-nine extreme morning- and 34 evening-type participants underwent two fMRI sessions: about 1 h after wake-up time (morning) and about 10 h after wake-up time (evening), scheduled according to their declared habitual sleep-wake pattern on a regular working day. Analysis of obtained neuroimaging data disclosed only an effect of time of day on resting-state functional connectivity; there were different patterns of functional connectivity between morning (MS) and evening (ES) sessions. The results of our study showed no differences between extreme morning-type and evening-type individuals. We demonstrate that circadian and homeostatic influences on the resting-state functional connectivity have a universal character, unaffected by circadian typology.

Introduction

Human’s performance and most biological functions express rhythmic fluctuations across a 24-h-period: an increase during the day and a decrease at night (Carrier and Monk, 2000; Persson and Persson, 2018). This phenomenon is believed to originate from homeostatic and circadian sleep-wake regulatory processes (Borbély, 1982; Roenneberg and Merrow, 2005; Schmidt et al., 2012). As it allows organisms to adjust to cyclic availabilities, it affects cognitive functions and a wide range of neurobehavioral events (Schmidt et al., 2007). Circadian variations were found in several cognitive tasks, including attention, memory, verbal, arithmetic, motor and reaction time (Kuriyama et al., 2003; Ramírez et al., 2006; Edwards et al., 2007; Tassi et al., 2012). Results of various neuropsychological tests suggest that the activity of specific brain areas also may be modulated according to the time of day (Schmidt et al., 2007; Valdez et al., 2012).

Chronotypes are relatively stable traits of the subjective diurnal rhythm of activity. Traditionally, they refer to the subjective morning vs. -evening preferences, i.e., the self-reported “feeling best” and “best performance” times of day. In evening-type people, as compared to morning-type ones, phases of physiological circadian rhythms are shifted toward later hours (e.g., Kerkhof and Van Dongen, 1996; Gibertini et al., 1999; Bailey and Heitkemper, 2001). The time course of circadian rhythms in cognitive performance differs significantly based on chronotype (Horne et al., 1980; Schmidt et al., 2007; Valdez et al., 2012). Only a handful of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies regarding circadian rhythms were performed so far. For example, Gorfine and Zisapel (2009) showed differences in brain activation patterns during night vs. afternoon hours, while Marek et al. (2010) found time-of-day variations in neural activity of the orienting attentional system. Diurnal fluctuations in performance-related brain activity were also observed in studies employing chronotype-based paradigms (e.g., Gorfine et al., 2007; Fafrowicz et al., 2009; Schmidt et al., 2009, 2012, 2015; Vandewalle et al., 2009, 2011; Peres et al., 2011).

All above-mentioned studies concerned particular neuropsychological functions examined by performing specific tasks. Resting-state networks are mostly believed to remain stable throughout the day (Biswal et al., 2010; Byrne and Murray, 2017). An opposite view was postulated by Blautzik et al. (2013) and Park et al. (2012), who argued that the level of stability varies over a period of 1 day across different resting state networks, ranging from highly rhythmic to stable. Jiang et al. (2016) found that widespread brain areas exhibit diurnal variations in resting-state. However, the study’s sample size was relatively small (16 participants), and neither the subjects’ sleep-wake pattern nor chronotypes were controlled, which could have affect the results. In our research we used the chronotype-based paradigm (Schmidt et al., 2007). Thus, the goal of this study was to investigate circadian fluctuations of functional networks using resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI).

Twenty-nine subjects with an extreme morning chronotype (M-type) and 34 subjects with an extreme evening chronotype (E-type) were evaluated during morning (MS) and evening (ES) sessions, after which we conducted the seed-to-voxel analysis using ALFF-based FC analysis, choosing the brain regions as seeds based on the study of Jiang et al. (2016).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Online advertisement on the Jagiellonian University website and Facebook that were targeted at young, healthy individuals were the primary recruitment method for the study. Five-thousand three-hundred and fifty-four volunteers participated in the first stage of selection. All volunteers were asked to complete a sleep-wake assessment (individual sleep need, ideal vs. real bedtimes and waketimes), diurnal preference as measured by the Chronotype Questionnaire (Oginska et al., 2017), night sleep quality as measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al., 1989), and daytime sleepiness as measured by the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS; Johns, 1991). Individuals reporting sleep problems or excessive daytime sleepiness were excluded from the study, as determined by the cut-off points from the PSQI (≤5 points) and ESS (≤10 points) questionnaires. Out of the 451 participants who were identified as exhibiting extreme morning or evening chronotypes, 63 healthy, young participants (38 women) were selected for the study. The low selection percentage (14%) was caused by the fact that there are very few extreme morning type people in the general population of young, healthy individuals. All participants needed to have a regular time-of-day schedule without sleep debt (length of sleep between 6 and 9 h per night). Specific selection criteria included: age between 19 and 35 years, right-handedness, no neurological or psychiatric disorders, no significant vision defects, and no MRI contraindications.

All subjects were right-handed as indicated by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). During the selection process, candidates were interviewed about drug, alcohol or nicotine dependence by a clinical psychologist. Subjects, who were dependent on any substance specified above were excluded from the study. Further exclusion criteria included shift work and having been on a flight passing more than two time zones within the past 2 months. Participants were requested to maintain a regular sleep-wake schedule 1 week prior to fMRI scanning, which was controlled using MotionWatch 8 actigraphs. The regular sleep-wake schedule matched the individuals sleep need. Participants wore actigraphs the week preceding the study as well as the days of brain imaging to control sleep length and quality. Furthermore, the night before the morning session, subjects slept in rooms located in the same building, as the MR laboratory. MRI acquisition was postponed in cases where sleep pattern was disrupted. The actigraphy data allow us to find no differences between M- and E-types in any of the parameters derived from actigraphy except for one, which was hour differences in bedtimes and waking times. Demographics, questionnaires and actigraphy results are provided in the Supplementary Material Table S1. Subjects abstained from alcohol (48 h) and caffeine (24 h) before each fMRI session. During experimental days participants could engage in non-strenuous activities. The study was conducted from November 2016 until January 2018 in MR laboratory, Malopolska Centre of Biotechnology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland and was approved by the Institute of Applied Psychology Ethics Committee of the Jagiellonian University. Informed, written consent was provided by all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Resting-state fMRI scans were acquired for MS between 8:00 AM and 9:00 AM (M = 8:21 AM; SD = 10 min) for M-type participants, and between 9:20 AM and 10:20 AM (M = 9:41 AM; SD = 14 min) for E-type participants; for ES between 5:00 PM and 6:00 PM (M = 5:19 PM; SD = 14 min) for M-type participants, and between 6:20 PM and 7:20 PM (M = 6:43 PM; SD = 20 min) for E-type participants. Session order was counterbalanced across participants.

Methods

MRI Data Acquisition

MRI data was acquired using a 3T Siemens Skyra MR System. A sagittal 3D T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence was used to obtain anatomical images. 10-min functional resting-state blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) images were acquired using a gradient-echo single-short echo planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 1,800 ms; echo time (TE) = 27 ms; field of view (FOV) = 256 mm; slice thickness = 4 mm; voxel size = 4 mm3, with no gap, using a 64-channel coil. A total of 34 interleaved transverse slices and 335 volumes were acquired. During the resting state procedure, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes open, think of nothing, and not to fall asleep. Subjects’ awakeness was monitored using an eye tracking system (Eyelink 1000, SR research, Mississauga, ON, Canada).

fMRI Data Pre-processing

The rs-fMRI data was processed using MATLAB version R2016a (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) and a statistical parametric mapping software (SPM12; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, UCL, London, UK). Scans were then slice-timed corrected, realigned by inclusion of field maps, which were created using “FieldMap Toolbox” included in SPM 12. After realignment, motion parameters were estimated for each volume of every subject. No participant needed to be excluded due to extensive movements, defined as exceeding a 3° rotation or 3 mm translation on any axis. Following motion correction, each individual’s structural T1-weighted image was co-registered and spatially normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, as well as resampled to 3 mm3 isotropic voxels using B-Spline Interpolation. Finally, the normalized volumes were smoothed using 6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.

ALFF-Based FC Analysis

Pre-processed data underwent connectivity analyses using the CONN: Functional Connectivity Toolbox (CONN v17.f). Confounding effects of white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and six motion parameters obtained from realignment pre-processing steps were removed by linear regression. BOLD data was then band-pass filtered (0.008–0.09 Hz) to remove high-frequency noise, linear detrended, and despiked. First level functional connectivity analysis was performed using seed-to-voxel whole brain correlations at an uncorrected level (p < 0.001). Bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients between the time series of seeds and the rest of the voxels in the brain were extracted and then transformed into Fisher Z-scores. Regions of Interest (ROIs) were chosen based on a previous report by Jiang et al. (2016) using an ALFF-based FC approach (Tadayonnejad et al., 2015). All ROIs were derived from the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL)-90 atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Description of the selected ROIs and their MNI coordinates are provided in Supplementary Material Table S2.

Statistical Analyses

First-level, subject-specific connectivity maps for each seed were used in a second-level analysis. We used three different statistical approaches to verify our research hypotheses:

(1) a two-sided paired t-test implemented in CONN was performed, in order to investigate differences in seed-to-voxel connectivity between MS and ES for each ROI and the rest of the brain.

(2) two sample t-test implemented in CONN was used to evaluate possible differences between M-type and E-type participants in seed-to-voxel connectivity between each ROI and the rest of the brain.

(3) whole-brain voxel-wise 2 (between-subject factor: morning and evening chronotype) × 2 (within-subject factor: morning and evening session) repeated measures ANOVA was performed to evaluate the time of the day × chronotype effect on FC between selected ROIs and the rest of the voxels in the brain.

Results were then exported to SPM in order to identify significant clusters. Based on previous studies (Ichesco et al., 2012; Fallon et al., 2016; Zhu et al., 2017), voxelwise statistics throughout the whole brain were performed at an uncorrected (p < 0.001) level, and clusters which survived a false discovery rate (FDR) correction at a cluster level of p < 0.05, were reported.

Results

Seed-to-Voxel Analysis in Relation to Time of the Day

Compared to the ES, the MS showed significant (p < 0.05; FDR corrected on cluster level) differences in multiple brain regions—see Figures 1, 2.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Results of two-sample t-test on functional connectivity maps with MS > ES contrast. Yellow areas depict increased functional connectivity in the morning session (MS) compared to the evening session [ES; p < 0.05; false discovery rate (FDR) corrected on cluster level]. (B) Table representing seeds with the presence of at least one positively correlated cluster with significant (p < 0.05; FDR corrected on cluster level) differences between morning and evening sessions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Results of two-sample t-test on functional connectivity maps with ES > MS contrast. Blue areas depict increased functional connectivity in the evening session compared to the morning session (p < 0.05; FDR corrected on cluster level). (B) Table representing seeds with the presence of at least one positively correlated cluster with significant (p < 0.05; FDR corrected on cluster level) differences between evening and morning sessions.

Seed-to-Voxel Analysis in Relation to Chronotype

We did not find any significant differences (p > 0.05) between extreme M-type and E-type in seed-to-voxel connectivity between each ROI and the rest of the brain.

Seed-to-Voxel Analysis in Relation to Time of the Day × Chronotype

There were no significant chronotype × time of the day interactions at any cluster (p > 0.05; FDR corrected on cluster level) at neither morning nor evening session.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first depicting functional connectivity in subjects with extreme chronotypes using ALFF-based FC analysis. The resting state functional connectivity patterns were evaluated in a limited number of previous studies which provided inconclusive results. In our article, we have chosen to concentrate on clusters which were identified in a article of Jiang et al. (2016), reported as having both increased and decreased ALFF values. We have focused on ALFF instead of ReHo analysis due to the distinct nature of those two separate analyses. While REHO reflects the local synchronization of spontaneous neural activity between neighboring voxels, ALFF measures the fluctuation amplitude of spontaneous neural activity at the single-voxel level (global), being more suitable for a seed-to-voxel approach (An et al., 2013). Our results showed that diurnal differences in amplitudes of low frequency fluctuations are also involved in seed-to-voxel functional connectivity related to the time of day. Previous study by Hodkinson et al. (2014) that, focused on the functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow confirmed that the default mode network (DMN) decreases its integration in the afternoon compared to in the morning. Blautzik et al. (2013) revealed that daily modulation of resting-state connectivity patterns fluctuates from highly rhythmic (DMN and sensorimotor regions) to stable (frontal-cortical areas). Only one recently published article by Horne and Norbury (2018) explored the impact of chronotype on functional connectivity in the brain. Horne et al.’s study results suggest that evening-oriented participants showed reduced connectivity within the DMN network. On the contrary, our results state that the influence of the time of day is stronger than that of the chronotype on functional organization of the brain. No diurnal differences between extreme chronotypes were found in the areas which are a part of DMN. Therefore, our results are not in line with the outcomes of Horne and Norbury (2018). Our seed-to-voxel analysis revealed functional connectivity differences between morning and evening sessions, between regions involved in important resting-state networks. Namely, in the morning compared to the evening session, we found increased connectivity between regions which are part of visual and sensorimotor networks and the thalamus. The thalamus is a critical structure for the integration and processing of sensory and motor information. It is a relay station for visual, auditory and sensorimotor pathways on the way to the cerebral cortex (Huguenard and McCormick, 2007). The thalamus also plays a role in homeostatic sleep pressure, sleep regulation and wakefulness, due to its many nonphotic and photic inputs into the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN; Jan et al., 2008). Schmidt et al. (2009) showed that global alertness in the evening was associated with a larger thalamus response in M-type individuals compared to E-type individuals. In addition, our work showed increased connectivity between the left postcentral gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex and parts of the cerebellum during the MS as compared to the ES. It is well known, that the cerebellum is involved not only in motor activities, but also in a variety of cognitive functions (Buckner, 2013). The increased functional connectivity we found between the cerebellum and postcentral gyrus may indicate greater involvement of the sensorimotor system early in the morning. A previous study by Tüshaus et al. (2017) showed decreased connectivity of the sensorimotor network as the amount of time awake increases, which is in line with our results.

It may be concluded that the time of day effect is stronger than the chronotype effect on resting-state functional connectivity in the brain. The results of our study showed that circadian and homeostatic influences on resting-state functional connectivity have a universal character and it is unaffected by circadian typology.

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the Supplementary Files.

Ethics Statement

Human Subject Research

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Jagiellonian University Ethic’s Commission. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MF was the study coordinator and contributed to the study idea, fMRI data acquisition, discussion of analysis and results, as well as writing and revision of the manuscript. BB contributed to the scripting of data preprocessing, data analysis, discussion of analysis and results, as well as writing and revision of the manuscript. AC contributed to the analysis of demographics, questionnaires and actigraphy results, discussion of analysis and results, as well as writing and revision of the manuscript. MC, KL, BS-W, and AB contributed to fMRI data acquisition and revision of the manuscript. HO contributed to the analysis of demographics, questionnaires and actigraphy results, as well as revision of the manuscript. JO contributed to the revision of the manuscript. TM contributed to the study idea, the discussion of analysis and results, and revision of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Polish National Science Centre through grant Harmonia 2013/08/M/HS6/00042.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Prof. Patricia Reuter-Lorenz for her constructive suggestions during the planning and development of the Harmonia project and for her valuable support. We also thank Piotr Faba for his technical support on this project and help in data acquisition, Aleksandra Zyrkowska for help in process of participants selection, Magdalena Debowska for help in collecting actigraphy data and Anna Holubecki for wide support during manuscript preparation process.

Abbreviations

AAL, Automated Anatomical Labeling; BOLD, blood-oxygenation-level-dependent; DMN, default mode network; ES, evening session; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; E-type, extreme evening chronotype; FDR, false discovery rate; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; FOV, field of view; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; MS, morning session; M-type, extreme morning chronotype; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; ROI, region of interest; rs-fMRI, resting-state fMRI; SCN, suprachiasmatic nucleus; TE, echo time; TR, repetition time.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00288/full#supplementary-material

References

An, L., Cao, Q. J., Sui, M. Q., Sun, L., Zou, Q. H., Zang, Y. F., et al. (2013). Local synchronization and amplitude of the fluctuation of spontaneous brain activity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a resting-state fMRI study. Neurosci. Bull. 29, 603–613. doi: 10.1007/s12264-013-1353-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, S. L., and Heitkemper, M. M. (2001). Circadian rhythmicity of cortisol and body temperature. Morningness-eveningness effects. Chronobiol. Int. 18, 249–261. doi: 10.1081/cbi-100103189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X.-N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S. M., et al. (2010). Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 4734–4739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911855107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blautzik, J., Vetter, C., Peres, I., Gutyrchik, E., Keeser, D., Berman, A., et al. (2013). Classifying fMRI-derived resting-state connectivity patterns according to their daily rhythmicity. Neuroimage 71, 298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbély, A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1, 195–204.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F. III, Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, J. E. M., and Murray, G. (2017). The sleep and circadian modulation of neural reward pathways: a protocol for a pair of systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 6:237. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0631-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrier, J., and Monk, T. H. (2000). Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiol. Int. 17, 719–732.

Google Scholar

Edwards, B., Waterhouse, J., and Reilly, T. (2007). The effects of circadian rhythmicity and time-awake on a simple motor task. Chronobiol. Int. 24, 1109–1124. doi: 10.1080/07420520701795316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fafrowicz, M., Golonka, K., Marek, T., Mojsa-Kaja, J., Tucholska, K., Oginska, H., et al. (2009). Diurnal variability of human operator attention disengagement and chronotype: an fMRI-based case study. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 10, 545–557. doi: 10.1080/14639220902992001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallon, N., Chiu, Y., Nurmikko, T., and Stancak, A. (2016). Functional connectivity with the default mode network is altered in fibromyalgia patients. PLoS One 11:e0159198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibertini, M., Graham, C., and Cook, M. R. (1999). Self-report of circadian type reflects the foxphase of the melatonin rhythm. Biol. Psychol. 50, 19–33. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(98)00049-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorfine, T., and Zisapel, N. (2009). Late evening brain activation patterns and their relation to the internal biological time, melatonin, and homeostatic sleep debt. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 541–552. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorfine, T., Yeshurun, Y., and Zisapel, N. (2007). Nap and melatonin-induced changes in hippocampal activation and their role in verbal memory consolidation. J. Pineal Res. 43, 336–342. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(98)00049-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodkinson, D. J., O’Daly, O., Zunszain, P. A., Pariante, C. M., Lazurenko, V., Zelaya, F. O., et al. (2014). Circadian and homeostatic modulation of functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow in humans under normal entrained conditions. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 34, 1493–1499. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, J. A., Brass, C. G., and Pettitt, A. N. (1980). Circadian performance differences between morning and evening “types”. Ergonomics 23, 29–36. doi: 10.1080/00140138008924715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, C. M., and Norbury, R. (2018). Altered resting-state connectivity within default mode network associated with late chronotype. J. Psychiatr. Res. 102, 223–229. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huguenard, J. R., and McCormick, D. A. (2007). Thalamic synchrony and dynamic regulation of global forebrain oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 350–356. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ichesco, E., Quintero, A., Clauw, D. J., Peltier, S., Sundgren, P. M., Gerstner, G. E., et al. (2012). Altered functional connectivity between the insula and the cingulate cortex in patients with temporomandibular disorder: a pilot study. Headache 52, 441–454. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2011.01998.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jan, J. E., Reiter, R. J., Wasdell, M. B., and Bax, M. (2008). The role of the thalamus in sleep, pineal melatonin production, and circadian rhythm sleep disorders. J. Pineal Res. 46, 1–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-079x.2008.00628.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, C., Yi, L., Su, S., Shi, C., Long, X., Xie, G., et al. (2016). Diurnal variations in neural activity of healthy human brain decoded with resting-state blood oxygen level dependent fMRI. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:634. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 14, 540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerkhof, G. A., and Van Dongen, H. P. (1996). Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator. Neurosci. Lett. 218, 153–156. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(96)13140-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuriyama, K., Uchiyama, M., Suzuki, H., Tagaya, H., Ozaki, A., Aritake, S., et al. (2003). Circadian fluctuation of time perception in healthy human subjects. Neurosci. Res. 46, 23–31. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(03)00025-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marek, T., Fafrowicz, M., Golonka, K., Mojsa-Kaja, J., Oginska, H., Tucholska, K., et al. (2010). Diurnal patterns of activity of the orienting and executive attention neuronal networks in subjects performing a Stroop-like task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Chronobiol. Int. 27, 945–958. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2010.489400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oginska, H., Mojsa-Kaja, J., and Mairesse, O. (2017). Chronotype description: in search of a solid subjective amplitude scale. Chronobiol. Int. 34, 1388–1400. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2017.1372469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, B., Kim, J. I., Lee, D., Jeong, S. O., Lee, J. D., and Park, H. J. (2012). Are brain networks stable during a 24-hour period? Neuroimage 59, 456–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peres, I., Vetter, C., Blautzik, J., Reiser, M., Pöppel, E., Meindl, T., et al. (2011). Chronotype predicts activity patterns in the neural underpinnings of the motor system during the day. Chronobiol. Int. 28, 883–889. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.619084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persson, P. B., and Persson, A. B. (2018). Light and darkness in circadian rhythm. Acta Physiol. 222:e13036. doi: 10.1111/apha.13036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramírez, C., Talamantes, J., Garcia, A., Morales, M., Valdez, P., and Menna-Barreto, L. (2006). Circadian rhythms in phonological and visuospatial storage components of working memory. Biol. Rhythm Res. 37, 433–441. doi: 10.1080/09291010600870404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roenneberg, T., and Merrow, M. (2005). Circadian clocks—the fall and rise of physiology. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 6, 965–971. doi: 10.1038/nrm1766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., and Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24, 755–789. doi: 10.1080/02643290701754158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Leclercq, Y., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Berthomier, P., et al. (2009). Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area. Science 324, 516–519. doi: 10.1126/science.1167337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Reichert, C. F., Maire, M., Vandewalle, G., Peigneux, P., et al. (2015). Pushing the limits: chronotype and time of day modulate working memory-dependent cerebral activity. Front. Neurol. 6:199. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., Peigneux, P., Leclercq, Y., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Phillips, C., et al. (2012). Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One 7:e29658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tadayonnejad, R., Yang, S., Kumar, A., and Ajilore, O. (2015). Clinical, cognitive, and functional connectivity correlations of resting-state intrinsic brain activity alterations in unmedicated depression. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 241–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tassi, P., Pellerin, N., Moessinger, M., Eschenlauer, R., and Muzet, A. (2012). Variation of visual detection over the 24-hour period in humans. Chronobiol. Int. 17, 795–805. doi: 10.1081/cbi-100102115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tüshaus, L., Balsters, J. H., Schläpfer, A., Brandeis, D., O’Gorman Tuura, R., and Achermann, P. (2017). Resisting sleep pressure: impact on resting state functional network connectivity. Brain Topogr. 30, 757–773. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0575-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdez, P., Ramírez, C., and García, A. (2012). Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: implications for neuropsychological assessment. Chronophysiol. Ther. 2, 81–92. doi: 10.2147/cpt.s32586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G., Archer, S. N., Wuillaume, C., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (2009). Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J. Neurosci. 29, 7948–7956. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0229-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G., Archer, S. N., Wuillaume, C., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (2011). Effects of light on cognitive brain responses depend on circadian phase and sleep homeostasis. J. Biol. Rhythms 26, 249–259. doi: 10.1177/0748730411401736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Helpman, L., Papini, S., Schneier, F., Markowitz, J. C., Van Meter, P. E., et al. (2017). Altered resting state functional connectivity of fear and reward circuitry in comorbid PTSD and major depression. Depress. Anxiety 34, 641–650. doi: 10.1002/da.22594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resting state-fMRI, diurnal (circadian) rhythm, morning and evening chronotypes, ALFF/fALFF, functional connectivity

Citation: Fafrowicz M, Bohaterewicz B, Ceglarek A, Cichocka M, Lewandowska K, Sikora-Wachowicz B, Oginska H, Beres A, Olszewska J and Marek T (2019) Beyond the Low Frequency Fluctuations: Morning and Evening Differences in Human Brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:288. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00288

Received: 06 June 2019; Accepted: 05 August 2019;
Published: 27 August 2019.

Edited by:

Jodie Reanna Gawryluk, University of Victoria, Canada

Reviewed by:

Timo Partonen, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Finland
Michal Ramot, National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), United States

Copyright © 2019 Fafrowicz, Bohaterewicz, Ceglarek, Cichocka, Lewandowska, Sikora-Wachowicz, Oginska, Beres, Olszewska and Marek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Magdalena Fafrowicz, magda.fafrowicz@uj.edu.pl; Bartosz Bohaterewicz, bohaterewicz@gmail.com

Download