Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 October 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00376

Alterations of Regional Homogeneity and Functional Connectivity Following Short-Term Mindfulness Meditation in Healthy Volunteers

Qin Xiao1† Xingrong Zhao2,3† Guoli Bi1,4 Lisha Wu1 Hongjiang Zhang1,4 Ruixiang Liu5 Jingmei Zhong6 Shaoyuan Wu6* Yong Zeng7 Liqian Cui8 Yanmei Chen1 Kunhua Wu1,4* Zhuangfei Chen1,6*
  • 1Medical Faculty, Kunming University of Science and Technology, Kunming, China
  • 2Mental Health Institute, The Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Kunming Medical University, Kunming, China
  • 4Department of Magnetic Resonance Image, The First People’s Hospital of Yunnan Province, Kunming, China
  • 5Department of Clinical Psychology, The Second People’s Hospital of Yunnan Province, Kunming, China
  • 6Department of Clinical Psychology, The First People’s Hospital of Yunnan Province, Kunming, China
  • 7Department of Clinical Psychology, The Sixth Affiliated Hospital of Kunming Medical University, Yuxi, China
  • 8Department of Clinical Psychology, The First Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Mindfulness is described as the non-judgmental awareness of experiences in the present moment. The sustained practice of mindfulness may also have beneficial effects on an individual’s well-being. For instance, mindfulness meditation is an effective approach for improving emotion regulation. Specifically, the early stage of mindfulness meditation training enhances emotional monitoring systems related to attention regulation and executive function. Reduced activity in the default mode network (DMN) would probably be observed corresponding to the attenuated mind wandering. In the present study, we hypothesized that alterations in functional activity in the frontal-parietal cortex and DMN may be induced by short-term mindfulness meditation. In this study, before and after 8 weeks of weekly Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) training, healthy participants were evaluated using a mindfulness questionnaire and an affect schedule, as well as via resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Sixteen right-handed non-meditators were enrolled. Another 16 demographically matched healthy adults without any meditation experience were recruited as controls. Pre- and post-MBSR assessments were compared. Increased regional homogeneity in the right superior parietal lobule and left postcentral gyrus (PoCG), as well as altered functional connectivity in PoCG-related networks, were observed post-MBSR. The mindfulness questionnaire scores also improved and negative affect was significantly decreased after MBSR. Together with reduced involvement of the posterior brain, our results suggest a tendency toward stronger involvement of the parietal cortex in mindfulness beginners. This study provides novel evidence regarding the optimization of emotional processing with short-term mindfulness meditation.

Introduction

Mindfulness meditation has been demonstrated to improve emotion regulation (Teper et al., 2013). In the current clinical and research context, mindfulness meditation is defined as an attentive non-judgmental focus on and awareness of the present moment, particularly on internal sensations, feelings, and thoughts (Baer et al., 2006; Kong et al., 2016).

It is suggested that mindfulness acts via several underlying mechanisms (Hölzel et al., 2011), with attention and emotion regulation being two of the most regularly studied (Arch and Craske, 2006; Shapiro et al., 2006). Specifically, repeated mindfulness meditation training may bolster an individual’s self-regulation of attention and improve their emotional regulation (Lutz et al., 2015). Indeed, an improved underlying capacity for emotion regulation is precisely what mindfulness attempts to cultivate (Brown et al., 2013).

Self-regulation is a key process by which meditation fosters improved emotional functioning (Hölzel et al., 2011; Tang et al., 2015; Fox et al., 2016; Magalhaes et al., 2018). Recent neuroimaging studies focusing on attentional effects of meditation (especially, in the form of focused attention and open monitoring) revealed that improved emotion regulation was related to changes in neuronal activity in the brain (Guendelman et al., 2017). Furthermore, meditation induced changes to brain morphology and function in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, insula, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the default mode network (DMN). Those regions are functionally related to general processes of self-regulation, attention assignment, enhanced body awareness, as well as introspection and enhanced metacognitive skills. Structural differences and reduced activity of the DMN might reflect less mind wandering and reduced chaining of thoughts in long-term meditators (Brewer et al., 2011b). Additionally, brain areas of the limbic system, including the amygdala, insula, ventral anterior cingulate cortex (vACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), are also involved in emotional processing and the generation of affective responses. In fact, increased prefrontal activity and decreased activity of the limbic region have been shown during emotion regulation strategies such as cognitive reappraisal, implicit cognitive reappraisal, and attentional deployment (Magalhaes et al., 2018). Furthermore, a negative correlation between brain activations in prefrontal and right insula and mindfulness when viewing negative pictures suggests that less regulatory resources are needed in mindful individuals when facing emotional arousal (Lutz et al., 2014). Meditation training may also influence affective responses through amygdala reactivity, or connections among those regions (Herwig et al., 2018; Martelli et al., 2018; Tra et al., 2018).

By modifying emotion regulation abilities, mindfulness-related therapies, such as Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT), reduce symptoms such as stress, anxiety, and depression, and ultimately benefit well-being (Baer, 2003; Brown and Ryan, 2003; Grossman et al., 2004). Significantly, MBSR has been shown to be effective in 8 weeks and induces similar structural and functional changes to the prefrontal cortex, the sensory cortices and insula, the hippocampus, and the cingulate cortex as long-term meditation practice (Gotink et al., 2016). It is not yet clear how short-term MBSR influences spontaneous neural activity (Chambers et al., 2009). While it has been proposed that the lateral prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex are involved at the very beginning of mindfulness meditation (Farb et al., 2007; Tang et al., 2009, 2012), the ACC is mainly involved when less effort is invested (Tang et al., 2009, 2012; Tang, 2011). Therefore, it is of great interest to clarify how the initial stages of mindfulness alter brain function.

Functional imaging studies indicate that mindfulness is associated with neural activity involving various brain regions, particularly those of the DMN (Brewer et al., 2011b). The DMN is involved in the internal channels of thoughts, mindfulness awareness, and interceptive awareness, and is critical for affective control (Shaurya et al., 2013). Mind wandering has been linked to increased activation in regions of the DMN (Weissman et al., 2006; Mason et al., 2007), such as in the mPFC, precuneus, and the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). Negative emotions increase mind wandering (Jonathan et al., 2009), which in turn tends to feed negative mental states (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). These negative loops are difficult to break, especially when they occur during pathological states such as depression, which has been linked to excessive DMN activity (Connolly et al., 2013). However, mindfulness meditation can provide a meta-function for an efficient brain/mind regulation, by flexibly allocating limited resources constrained by negative emotion and mind wandering. Even a few minutes of mindfulness practice can reduce mind wandering (Mrazek et al., 2012). Thus, we speculate that short-term practitioners of mindfulness have lower aggregate neural activity in the DMN.

On the basis of these previous findings, we hypothesized that (i) a short course of mindfulness practice would change neural activity in the brain regions related to attention and executive function, especially frontal-parietal regions, and (ii) a reduced recruitment of the DMN would be observed in practitioners. To gain insight about baseline neurobiological factors influencing sensory, cognitive, or behavioral processes associated with mindfulness (Mantini et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2014; Kong et al., 2016), we measured spontaneous neural activity at rest using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). To characterize the cohesiveness of resting activity in neighboring brain regions (focal connectivity), we adopted regional homogeneity (ReHo) as an analytic technique (Zang et al., 2004). ReHo supposes that voxels within a functional brain area are more temporally homogeneous when this area is involved in a specific condition. A larger ReHo value for a given voxel indicates a higher local synchronization of rs-fMRI signals among neighboring voxels. As a data-driven approach, ReHo can detect unpredicted hemodynamic responses that model-driven methods cannot. Thus, this method can help us understand the high complexity of the human brain (Zang et al., 2004). Additionally, functional connectivity was used to explore the long-range intrinsic organization of the brain function following MBSR (Han et al., 2017).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixteen right-handed non-meditators were enrolled as experimental participants and routinely attended MBSR practice for 8 weeks. An additional 16 healthy adults without any meditation experience were recruited as controls. Only individuals with no prior experience with meditation in any form (including mindfulness meditation and any other form of traditional practice like Shamatha, Satipatthana, yoga, Vipassana, Taiji) were enrolled. Demographically, the two groups were matched for age, sex, and years of education (p > 0.05) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics for all subjects.

All individuals were recruited via poster advertisements in their local community. They were interviewed by two experienced psychiatrists who used the Structured Clinical Interview of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV Axis I Disorders/Non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP) to exclude persons with a history of neuropsychiatric illness. All participants were Han Chinese and right-handed, as assessed via the Annett Handedness Scale (Annett, 1970). Persons with organic brain disorders, a history of alcohol or drug abuse, who were pregnant, or who had a history of, or present with severe physical illness were excluded.

The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Kunming University of Science and Technology (approval number: 2013JC003). All participants provided written informed consent before initiation of the experiment.

Mindfulness Meditation Training

MBSR classes were conducted weekly for 2 h for a period of 8 weeks. These weekly meetings were divided into three segments: theoretical, practical, and debriefing. In the theoretical segment, MBSR and the underlying neuroscience were introduced to the participants. Next, in the practice segment, the participants performed a sitting meditation with simple physical and breathing exercises. Participants were instructed to focus their attention on their thoughts and feelings as they experienced them without ruminating on any of them.

Participants were also assigned daily homework assignments, which were composed of both formal and informal meditation activities. Formal activities required 30 min to complete each day and included body scanning, sitting meditation, floor yoga, mountain/lake meditation, or loving kindness meditation. Each week, the participants were also asked to complete a practice and information sheet, which was tailored to that particular week’s practice.

To integrate the participants’ learning and meditation practices into their daily lives, an informal practice that primarily included simple awareness activities (i.e., bringing mindful awareness to routine activities, pleasant/unpleasant events, or communication situations) was also undertaken. Additional techniques, such as the 1-min breathing space (Ward, 2014) and the Recognition, Acceptance, Investigation and Non-identification process (Brewer et al., 2011a), were incorporated into individuals’ informal practices to draw their attention toward their automatic responses and expand their awareness. At the end of each day, participants were instructed to spend approximately 5 min reflecting on their day, using that week’s informal practice sheet as a guide. On the other hand, the controls were asked to participate in a general health education course, with the same time setting as the MBSR.

Self-Reported Measures

The Chinese versions of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) (Deng et al., 2011) and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Li and Yang, 2003) were applied both before and after the MBSR training to explore mindfulness and emotional states of all participants. The FFMQ assesses five facets of an individual’s general tendency to be mindful in daily life: observing, describing, acting with awareness, non-reactivity to one’s inner experience, and non-judgment of one’s inner experience. It consists of 39 items rated on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5 (very often or always true). The PANAS is a 20-item self-reported scale that measures positive and negative mood states in relation to the previous week. Both the negative and positive affect scales consisted of 10 corresponding adjectives that described either positive or negative emotions. Participants rated the degree to which they felt each emotion on a scale of 1 (slightly or not at all) to 5 (a lot).

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were acquired using a GE 3.0 T MR scanner (EXCLTE, General Electric, Milwaukee, WI, United States) using an eight-channel head coil. Participants were instructed to relax without engaging in any tasks and remain still with their eyes closed. All participants reported wakefulness throughout the duration of the scanning session. Functional data were collected using a contrast-gradient echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters: repetition time = 3000 ms, time to echo = 40 ms, time for inversion = 100 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 240 mm × 240 mm, matrix size = 96 × 96, slice thickness = 4 mm, voxel size = 2.5 × 2.5 × 4 mm3, and 100 volumes in total (5 min and 30 s). No participants were found to have gross structural abnormalities upon visual inspection of their scans by two senior radiologists.

Preprocessing of imaging data was performed in MATLAB 7.6 (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States) using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM81) and the Resting-State fMRI toolkit Data Processing Assistant (DPARSF v 2.22). The first ten time-points from all functional images were removed to eliminate any non-equilibrium effects of magnetization. Slice timing and head motion corrections were also applied. All functional images were then normalized to the standard EPI template and spatially resampled to a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm3. The resulting images were detrended to remove the linear trend and then temporally filtered with a Chebyshev band-pass filter (0.01–0.08 Hz). Within the settings of DPRSF, nuisance factors including head motion parameters, white matter, global mean, and cerebrospinal fluid signals were regressed out.

Regional Homogeneity Analysis

Kendall’s coefficient of concordance was used to measure the voxel-wise correlation between a single given voxel’s time series and its 26 neighbors. For standardization purposes, a Fisher’s r-to-z transformation was conducted on ReHo maps of the whole brain for all participants. All maps were then smoothed by applying a 6 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel, as described previously (Yan and Zang, 2010). Voxel-wise comparisons of ReHo maps were performed to detect the inter- and intra- group differences. After fitting to the general linear model in SPM8, a two sample and a paired t-test were performed to identify the effects of meditation on ReHo between the MBSR and control (CON) group, as well as between the baseline and post-MBSR assessments, respectively.

Region of Interest-Based Network Analyses

After ReHo comparisons, regions with significant ReHo abnormalities were selected as seeds for functional connectivity (FC) analysis. Seeds were defined as a sphere with a radius of 6 mm using the Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital (MNI) stereotaxic coordinates. The mean time course of each seed was calculated by averaging the time series of all of the voxels within the seed. Voxel-wise correlation analysis was then performed between each of these selected time series and the rest. The FC maps were further transformed to z FC maps by Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to improve normality. The z FC maps in each condition were compared using paired t-tests. Additionally, eleven ROIs (defined as a sphere with a radius of 10 mm using the MNI coordinates, see Table 2) were ultimately selected for inclusion due to previous reports on their involvement in mindfulness meditation and the DMN (Allen et al., 2011; Doll et al., 2015). The averaged ReHo values for each ROI were extracted both before and after training for each participant. We tested for a main effect of MBSR training on ReHo across ROIs using a paired t-test in SPSS 13.0 (SPSS Inc., United States). Between-group ROI-based comparisons of ReHo maps were also performed between MBSR and CON groups using two sample t-tests.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regions of interest (ROIs) in DMN.

For all voxel-wise comparisons (ReHo analysis and seed-based FC analysis), results with a p-value of <0.05 at the voxel level or at the cluster level, after correcting for multiple comparisons and using the family-wise error (FWE) rate, were considered to be statistically significant. An additional extent threshold of 50 voxels was used to exclude any small clusters.

Results

Changes in Mindfulness Skills and Affective States

A paired t-test analysis of the total score and subscales (aside from the non-reactivity scores) of the FFMQ indicated a significant improvement in self-reported mindfulness after MBSR training (p < 0.05). The PANAS negative affect score also significantly decreased after training (p < 0.01) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The five facet mindfulness questionnaire (FFMQ) and positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) scores before and after meditation training.

As an exploratory step, the observing component score of the FFMQ was also found to negatively correlate with the mean ReHo value in the left ACC after MBSR training (r = −0.694, p = 0.003) (Figure 1). No correlation between the PANAS scores and the ReHo values was detected.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatter plot diagrams. The plots and fitted lines indicate the inverse correlations between the observed scores and the regional homogeneity (ReHo) values in the left anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in the Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) group.

Mindfulness and ReHo Differences

There were no significant differences in the CON group when ReHo was compared between the baseline and 8-week assessment (p > 0.05). In the MBSR group, comparison of the pre- and post-training images revealed increased ReHo values in the right superior parietal lobule (SPL.R) extending into the left supplementary motor area and mid-cingulate gyrus (MCC) (cluster size: 328 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = 33, y = −42, z = 60, t = 6.70, p < 0.01), and the left postcentral gyrus (PoCG.L) extending into the precentral gyrus (cluster size: 126 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = −33, y = −24, z = 54), t = 5.17, p = 0.01) after a cluster-level FWE correction (Figure 2). ReHo was not statistically different between the MBSR and CON group (p > 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Regional homogeneity in various regions. Regional homogeneity peaks (A) in the right superior parietal lobule, extending into the left supplementary motor area and mid-cingulate gyrus (328 voxels); (B) in the left postcentral gyrus, extending into the precentral (126 voxels). The white labels indicate the coordinate of each slice in the Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital (MNI) frame of reference (x, y, and z) (cluster level, threshold at p < 0.05, family-wise error corrected).

To estimate the regional ReHo values in distinct areas, an ROI-level ReHo analysis was also conducted. Comparison of the mean ReHo values for each ROI in the DMN (Table 2) before and after MBSR revealed significantly decreased ReHo values in the left PCC (t = 3.09, p = 0.008), bilateral precuneus (t = 2.24, p = 0.04; t = 2.32, p = 0.04) in the pdDMN and left cuneus (t = 2.26, p = 0.04) (Figure 3). No significant difference was observed pre- and post-intervention in the CON group nor between the MBSR and CON groups both in baseline and after 8 weeks (p > 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regional homogeneity values of various regions of interest. The normalized mean regional homogeneity (ReHo) values of regions of interest (ROIs) in the left posterior cingulate cortex (x = –6, y = 52, z = 28), left precuneus (x = –12, y = –67, z = 34), right cuneus (x = 9, y = –70, z = 37), and left cuneus (x = –6, y = –76, z = 34). The error bars indicate the standard error. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01.

Pre- and Post-MBSR Comparison of Functional Connectivity

Based on the voxel-wise ReHo analysis, the SPL.R and PoCG.L were selected as seeds for network analysis. No significant differences were found between the MBSR and CON group both at baseline and after training, nor within the CON group following training (p > 0.05). In the MBSR group, however, the PoCG-related network showed both increased and decreased FC with the seed region. FC in the right precuneus (cluster size: 200 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = 12, y = −51, z = 9, t = −5.57, p < 0.01) and right superior frontal medial gyrus (cluster size: 163 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = 12, y = 57, z = 33, t = −6.54, p < 0.01) decreased, while the MCC (cluster size: 126 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = −1, y = −6, z = 51, t = 8.67, p < 0.01), precentral gyrus (cluster size: 153 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = −27, y = −15, z = −54, t = 5.75, p < 0.01) and insula (cluster size: 103 voxels, peak MNI coordinates: (x = −39, y = −3, z = −6, t = 5.10, p < 0.01) in the left side showed increased FC to PoCG, after FWE correction (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Regions of functional connectivity alterations linked to PoCG.L. PoCG.L related functional connectivity with a significant decrease was observed in (A) right precuneus (200 voxels) and right superior frontal medial gyrus (163 voxels), while that in the (B) mid-cingulate gyrus (126 voxels), (C) precentral gyrus (153 voxels), and (D) insula (103 voxels) in the left side was increased.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to investigate mindfulness-related functional brain changes in meditation-naïve participants. ReHo and ROI-based functional connectivity methods were used to explore intrinsic neural changes in meditation practitioners. Increased local synchrony in the bilateral parietal lobe was detected, as was reduced synchrony at the ROI-level (per ReHo analyses) in the PCC, precuneus, and cuneus, with meditation. A network-level analysis further revealed increased functional connectivity between the left PoCG.L and the MCC, insula and precentral gyrus of the left side in practitioners.

The observation that MBSR increased ReHo values in the bilateral parietal lobes is noteworthy. In our neuroimaging results, the largest cluster was observed in the right SPL. As a part of the attention and executive control networks, SPL is responsible for voluntary orienting of attention and coordinating of attention under competing situations (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Wadden et al., 2018). Mindfulness practice (e.g., breathing-awareness task) demands a high attentional state in which the practitioner is asked to orient his/her attention on breathing and related physical sensations such as temperature changes (Crivelli et al., 2019). Such focused attention in meditation practices fosters concentration, focusing, and self-regulation skills (Lutz et al., 2008; Lippelt et al., 2014; Hommel and Colzato, 2017). It has been shown that attention regulation is positively linked to emotion regulation (Wadlinger and Isaacowitz, 2011), and therefore the increased involvement of the superior parietal cortex may explain the MBSR group’s ability to selectively attend to their internal and external environment with awareness. A recent study similarly reported that the superior parietal cortex is causally involved in top-down allocation of attentional resources between external, ongoing stimuli and internal, prospective memory intentions; thus, it may lead to delayed intention (Cona et al., 2017). Furthermore, a recent multimodal neuroimaging study indicated that subregions of the SPL have different types of connectivity, reflecting different functions and interactions (Wang et al., 2015). The cluster we found in the right SPL may correspond to R1 (one of the anterior subregions), which has been associated with motion detection, observation, space, execution, shape detection, language orthography, sexuality, and working memory. Furthermore, SPL was found to regulate visuospatial attention and dominance of visuospatial attention (Wu et al., 2016). Meanwhile, engaging in mindfulness meditation training improves goal-directed visuospatial attention (Malinowski et al., 2017). Thus, it is of great importance to further elucidate the specific functions of SPL related to mindfulness.

Another cluster with increased ReHo value was located in the left PoCG. With the somatosensory cortex situated in it, the PoCG has a dominant role in processing sensory information from various parts of the body (Ruben et al., 2001; Eickhoff et al., 2010). In addition to processing somatosensory information, this brain area also modulates tactile attention (Burton et al., 1999), integrates somatosensory information with motor information, and processes pain (Timmermann et al., 2001). The somatosensory cortex also plays an important role in each stage of emotional processing, including identification of the emotional significance of a stimulus, the generation of empathy (Ilaria et al., 2007) and emotion (Adolphs et al., 2000; Damasio et al., 2000), as well as regulation of emotion (Satpute et al., 2015; Orenius et al., 2017).

Our result of increased ReHo values in parietal regions is consistent with data indicating that Hinduism-inspired meditation triggered a network of areas including the PoCG and the SPL (Tomasino et al., 2014). Parietal regions also support greater emotional stability in experienced meditators (Bruder et al., 1997; Broderick, 2005; Ochsner and Gross, 2008; Sheline et al., 2009). As outlined in the introduction, mindfulness improves the regulation of attention and emotion. In fact, an attention network composed of frontal-parietal regions, such as the SPL and pre-supplementary motor area, was found to be recruited by mindfulness meditation (Dickenson et al., 2013). Additionally, cognitive emotion regulation strategies may build upon more general information processing systems, such as those regulating working memory and cognitive control, to improve the functioning of these processes (Ochsner and Gross, 2008). Specifically, the SPL facilitates cognitive emotion regulation by manipulating and rearranging information in working memory stores (Koenigs et al., 2009). A previous study also found increased neural responses in attention-related parietal cortex (including SPL) when implementing attention regulation of negative self-beliefs (Goldin et al., 2013).

The intra-group analysis of ROI-based functional connectivity also revealed strengthened connectivity between PoCG and parietal as well as paralimbic regions in the left brain. In the study of Hinduism-inspired meditation, a left lateralized network was triggered. Brain areas reported to be activated in the literature overlapped with those of enhanced functional connectivity in our study, including the PoCG, the superior parietal lobe, and the mid-cingulate cortex (Tomasino et al., 2014). An increased FC was also detected between the insula and PoCG in the current study. Both MCC and insula are associated with mindfulness-related treatment (Shackman et al., 2011; Marchand, 2012; Zeidan et al., 2014) for the roles in the interoceptive processing (Craig, 2003; Vitaly et al., 2013). Besides, sensorimotor cortex, midcingulate, and insula belong to the same interoceptive network (Hayes et al., 2013). The decreased FC in the right precuneus and superior frontal medial gyrus may indicate a weakened association between attention (Nagahama et al., 1999) and somatosensory processing. However, it remains unclear how those areas work together in mindfulness-related interoceptive processing. Future studies may explore the underlying mechanism by implementing specific task-based experiments.

Taken together, our findings and prior work raise the possibility that parietal regions may subserve the improved processing of cognitive and emotional information observed with mindfulness.

At the ROI-level, the present study revealed decreased ReHo values in the PCC (precuneus, and cuneus; i.e., the posterior DMN) with mindfulness meditation. Our result is consistent with previous reports of reduced activation of DMN in experienced meditators (Brewer et al., 2011b).

The DMN encompasses a set of brain regions including the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the ACC, lateral parietal regions including the angular gyrus, and the PCC/precuneus. The DMN is normally deactivated during tasks requiring attention to external stimuli and activated during unconstrained thought, introspection, and self-related processing (Alex et al., 2012). The functions of the individual brain regions of the DMN are crucial to human mental activity (Long et al., 2008). Unsurprisingly, mindfulness, as a special form of mental activity, was also associated with functional changes in DMN-related areas.

Reduced DMN activity is normally observed during cognitively demanding tasks. The more attentional resources are needed, the greater the activation decrease (McKiernan et al., 2003). As the brain’s dark energy, the DMN contributes to greater memory task performance when deactivated (Kim et al., 2010). Poorer task performance was associated with reduced deactivation of this background activity, which may reflect mind wandering interfering with attention to external stimuli (Weissman et al., 2006; Tom et al., 2008). Mind wandering and negative emotion could mutually reinforce each other (Jonathan et al., 2009; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010), and could form a vicious circle in certain psychopathological states. Mindfulness meditation was associated with less mind wandering (Mrazek et al., 2012), as well as decreased activation of the DMN (Brewer et al., 2011b). Our results replicate these prior findings of functional attenuation of DMN following meditation. Though no direct measurement of mind wandering was conducted in the current study, participants reported that they were less absent-minded and found it much easier to concentrate following mindfulness training. Objective measurement of mind wandering should be addressed in future studies with a strict experimental design. It has been postulated that enhanced regulation of mind wandering is an important stage in early mindfulness training that may lead to enhanced cognitive regulation and metacognition (Kerr et al., 2013). Though accumulating evidence of the benefits of mindfulness training may be erroneously interpreted as evidence that mind wandering has no benefits. However, we must also focus on the potential benefits of mind wandering, such as planning of the future and reflecting on past experience. We should examine how to achieve a balance between the pros and cons of mind wandering in the practice of mindfulness (Mrazek et al., 2012).

The PCC is associated with mind wandering; it is activated during “distraction” and deactivated during “concentration” (Brewer et al., 2013) in meditators. It is implicated in self-referential processing (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2011), including past and future thinking (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010). The PCC is also associated with various emotion and cognitive processes, including emotional processing, self-referential distortions in depression, anxiety, drug craving, and cognitive distortions in chronic pain (Brewer and Garrison, 2014). The PCC was deactivated when individuals focused on a particular task without involving self-referential evaluation (Brewer et al., 2013), which fits perfectly with the requirements of mindfulness. Similar PCC deactivation was found when individuals practiced paying attention in a non-evaluative manner. The key difference between paying attention and paying attention in a non-evaluative manner is the amount of self-referential evaluation (Brewer et al., 2013).

In a recent study in meditation-naïve subjects, the precuneus was found with increased cortical thickness and decreased amplitude of low-frequency, which correlated with the reduction of depression scores (Yang et al., 2019). Our result of decreased involvement of precuneus in the MBSR group, indicated by the reduced ReHo value, was in line with this previous report. Many fMRI studies have reliably reproduced the DMN by investigating the correlations in fMRI signals recorded from the precuneus (Peng et al., 2015). The anterior precuneus is involved in higher-order body image and attentional shifting (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), which are consistent with the practices and goals of many meditations. In a meta-analysis on the effects of meditation, structural heterogeneities in the precuneus were found in meditation practitioners (Fox et al., 2014). Furthermore, the structural connectivity between the precuneus and other areas of the attentional network such as the superior parietal lobe, may be mediated by changes in white matter after brief meditation training (Fox et al., 2014). The precuneus is crucial in the integration of mental processing with cognitive control processes such as visuo-spatial imagery, episodic memory retrieval and self-directed operations (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Fox et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2019). In the precuneus, self-referential processing is associated with increased functional activity, which decreases during mindful self-awareness. To be exact, this region may be linked to present moment-centered, body-oriented awareness cultivated in many meditation traditions, particularly Vipassana (i.e., insight meditation) (Fox et al., 2014). Functional differences of the precuneus in the MBSR group of this current study may be a result of its involvement in higher-order integration of interoceptive and exteroceptive attention at an advanced level. This process is potentially mediated by self-processing and evaluation. As advocated in many meditation traditions, a greater present-centered awareness is finally achieved by transforming the view of the self (Fox et al., 2014). The precuneus has consistently been reported to be dysregulated in depression (Li et al., 2018). The role it plays in emotion regulation should be further studied.

A reduced ReHo value was also detected in the cuneus, which belongs to the basic visual processing regions. The role of the cuneus in the cognitive domain has been rarely considered until recently. At first, the cuneus was reported to be associated with the cognitive ability of insight (Tordesillasgutierrez et al., 2018). Moreover, it has been involved in mindfulness related structural and functional change. One study found that mindfulness-based intervention in Parkinson’s disease increased gray matter volume in cuneus (Pickut et al., 2013). Another study found larger gyrification within the right cuneus in meditators. A recent study reported that individuals with borderline personality disorder showed higher deactivation in a cluster extending bilaterally from the calcarine to the cuneus and superior occipital gyri after dialectical behavioral therapy-which encompasses mindfulness as its core skill (Carmona i Farrés et al., 2019). Future studies should focus on the relationship between occipital regions and mindfulness practice both during a cognitive task and at resting state.

Enhanced mindfulness ability was observed in MBSR practitioners in our study. Our results concur with previous studies with similar findings measured using FFMQ (Kearney et al., 2011; Soler et al., 2018). Moreover, the observing component score of the FFMQ was negatively correlated with the mean ReHo value in the left ACC after MBSR training in our study. Research indicates that the ACC is involved in both cognitive control and emotional regulation, especially in the regulation and inhibition of emotion responses (Tang et al., 2015). Since we failed to find functional alteration in the ACC, this explanation should be treated cautiously.

The fMRI signal measures a relative hemodynamic change. Activation of a certain area corresponds to increased hemodynamic response (Cramer et al., 1999; Bernard et al., 2002) and vice versa (Allison et al., 2000; Hamzei et al., 2002). In ReHo analysis, Kendall’s coefficient concordance (KCC) was used to measure the similarity of the time series of a given voxel to those of its nearest neighbors in a voxel-wise way. Though the character of the hemodynamic response needs to be further investigated, it is generally accepted that a changed KCC in functional studies implies a changed hemodynamic response. Thus, the variation of ReHo value related to mindfulness in the current study was equivalent to that reported in the form of a change in activation, though no direct study evaluated the change of ReHo value in the DMN until recently. The reduction of ReHo value in the current study implies reduced involvement of the target brain regions in the resting state among mindfulness practitioners.

In the current study, there were significant differences in spontaneous brain activity and functional connectivity between the pre- and post-MBSR comparison. However, no alteration in the brain function was detected in the intergroup comparison between the MBSR and the CON group. The intra-group variability was more notable than the intergroup one. The variability might have resulted from both the small sample size and the statistical method. The two-sample t-test and paired t-test were separately used for comparison of inter- and intra-groups. The statistical efficiency of the latter would be much greater than that of the former (Rowe, 2015). The difference in statistical power would be more evident due to the small sample size. Therefore, we postulated that the sample size was not sufficient to detect the difference between the groups, but would be more sensitive to intra-group difference due to the self-control design. While the paired t-test, which, in fact is a one-sample t-test (Xu et al., 2017) that may increase the sensitivity, also raises the possibility of false positives. Therefore, further research is needed with a larger sample size, to explore MBSR-related brain change.

While the results of the present study offer some profound and important conclusions, there are also some limitations that must be taken into consideration. First, our sample size was moderately small, which limited our ability to detect small differences between conditions and inflated our chances of revealing positive findings, even when a correction for multiple-tests was used. As to the technical parameters for images, only 100 volumes were collected for each scan, which could be limited in reflecting the intrinsic state of the brain. Additionally, this study was limited by the lack of an active comparison or clinical intervention group, which would have provided a basis for making stronger inferences regarding how MBSR modifies the behavioral and neural bases of different emotion regulation strategies. Further studies should employ larger participant populations and clinically oriented observations with a focus on emotional pathways. Future work should also include additional assessment modalities, such as cognitive batteries and electrophysiology, in order to deepen our understanding of the mechanisms that underpin the effects of mindfulness on emotion regulation.

Conclusion

The present study illustrated that individuals who undergo mindfulness training exhibit significant differences in functional connectivity between regions of the DMN. Short-term mindfulness training was also associated with strengthened mindfulness abilities. These results shed light on the preferential role of the parietal cortex and related associated areas in mindfulness beginners, in particular. Considering the acceptability and cost of short-term mindfulness training, the current findings indicate the potential value of mindfulness training on clinical and public health in emotion regulation.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the guidelines for scientific research on human subjects of the Ethics Committee of the Kunming University of Science and Technology with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Kunming University of Science and Technology (the ethical approval number: 2013JC003).

Author Contributions

QX and XZ: data collection, data processing, data analysis, statistical analysis, original manuscript drafting, and manuscript editing. GB and HZ: data processing and data analysis. LW, RL, and YC: data collection, data processing, and data analysis. JZ, SW, YZ, and LC: project conception and manuscript revision. ZC and KW: project conception, research design, and manuscript revision.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC) (Nos. 31760281 and 31300926 to ZC; 81760251 to YC).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Xize Jia for the technical assistance as well as Fang Zhou for helping out with the data collection.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
  2. ^ http://rfmri.org/DPARSF

References

Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., and Damasio, A. R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J. Neurosci. 20, 2683–2690. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.20-07-02683.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alex, F., Harrison, B. J., Andrew, Z., and Simons, J. S. (2012). Competitive and cooperative dynamics of large-scale brain functional networks supporting recollection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 12788–12793. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1204185109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, E., Erhardt, E., Damaraju, E., Gruner, W., Segall, J., Silva, R., et al. (2011). A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, J. D., Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Figueroa, R. E., and Wright, J. C. (2000). Functional MRI cerebral activation and deactivation during finger movement. Neurology 54:135. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.1.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br. J. Psychol. 61, 303–321. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01248.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arch, J. J., and Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1849–1858. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin. Psychol. 10, 125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, R. A., Goran, D. A., Sakai, S. T., Carr, T. H., Mcfarlane, D., Nordell, B., et al. (2002). Cortical activation during rhythmic hand movements performed under three types of control: an fMRI study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 271–281. doi: 10.3758/cabn.2.3.271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., and Garrison, K. A. (2014). The posterior cingulate cortex as a plausible mechanistic target of meditation: findings from neuroimaging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1307, 19–27. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., Garrison, K. A., and Whitfieldgabrieli, S. (2013). What about the “Self” is processed in the posterior cingulate cortex? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:647. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., et al. (2011a). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 119, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y., Weber, J., and Kober, H. (2011b). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20254–20259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broderick, P. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cogn. Ther. Res. 29, 501–510. doi: 10.1007/s10608-005-3888-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., and Inzlicht, M. (2013). Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8:93. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Fong, R., Tenke, C. E., Leite, P., Towey, J. P., Stewart, J. E., et al. (1997). Regional brain asymmetries in major depression with or without an anxiety disorder: a quantitative electroencephalographic study. Biol. Psychiatry 41, 939–948. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00260-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, H., Abend, N. S., Macleod, A. M. K., Sinclair, R. J., Snyder, A. Z., and Raichle, M. E. (1999). Tactile attention tasks enhance activation in somatosensory regions of parietal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb. Cortex 9:662. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.7.662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmona i Farrés, C., Elices, M., Soler, J., Domínguez-Clavé, E., Pomarol-Clotet, E., Salvador, R., et al. (2019). Effects of mindfulness training on borderline personality disorder: impulsivity versus emotional dysregulation. Mindfulness 10, 1243–1254. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1071-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanna, A., and Trimble, M. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, R., Gullone, E., and Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: an integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 560–572. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cona, G., Marino, G., and Bisiacchi, P. S. (2017). Superior parietal cortex and the attention to delayed intention: an rTMS study. Neuropsychologia 95, 130–135. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, C. G., Jing, W., Ho, T. C., Fumiko, H., Owen, W., Stuart, E., et al. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity of subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in depressed adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 898–907. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3:201. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 500–505. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, S. C., Finklestein, S. P., Schaechter, J. D., Bush, G., and Rosen, B. R. (1999). Activation of distinct motor cortex regions during ipsilateral and contralateral finger movements. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 383–387. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.1.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crivelli, D., Fronda, G., and Balconi, M. (2019). Neurocognitive enhancement effects of combined mindfulness-neurofeedback training in sport. Neuroscience 412, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049. doi: 10.1038/79871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y. Q., Liu, X. H., Rodriguez, M. A., and Xia, C. Y. (2011). The five facet mindfulness questionnaire: psychometric properties of the chinese version. Mindfulness 2, 123–128. doi: 10.1177/1073191113485121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickenson, J., Berkman, E. T., Arch, J., and Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Neural correlates of focused attention during a brief mindfulness induction. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 40–47. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doll, A., Hölzel, B., Boucard, C., Wohlschläger, A., and Sorg, C. (2015). Mindfulness is associated with intrinsic functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:461. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Saad, J., Svenja, C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Karl, Z., et al. (2010). Anatomical and functional connectivity of cytoarchitectonic areas within the human parietal operculum. J. Neurosci. 30, 6409–6421. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5664-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farb, N., Segal, Z., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 313–322. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M., Floman, J., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S., et al. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? a systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 48–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. C., Dixon, M. L., Nijeboer, S., Girn, M., Floman, J. L., Lifshitz, M., et al. (2016). Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: a review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 65, 208–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin, P., Ziv, M., Jazaieri, H., Hahn, K., and Gross, J. (2013). MBSR vs aerobic exercise in social anxiety: fMRI of emotion regulation of negative self-beliefs. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 65–72. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotink, R. A., Meijboom, R., Vernooij, M. W., Smits, M., and Hunink, M. G. M. (2016). 8-week mindfulness based stress reduction induces brain changes similar to traditional long-term meditation practice – A systematic review. Brain Cogn. 108, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., and Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: a meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 35–43.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Guendelman, S., Medeiros, S., and Rampes, H. (2017). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: insights from neurobiological, psychological, and clinical studies. Front. Psychol. 8:220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamzei, F., Dettmers, C., Rzanny, R., Liepert, J., Büchel, C., and Weiller, C. (2002). Reduction of excitability (“Inhibition”) in the ipsilateral primary motor cortex is mirrored by fMRI signal decreases. Neuroimage 17, 490–496. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, W., Qian, S., Jiang, Q., Liu, K., Li, B., and Sun, G. (2017). Regional and long-range neural synchronization abnormality during passive hyperthermia. Behav. Brain Res. 341:9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, D. J., Duncan, N. W., Christine, W., Karin, P., Pengmin, Q., Stefan, L., et al. (2013). GABAA receptors predict aversion-related brain responses: an fMRI-PET investigation in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacol. Official Public. Am. College Neuropsychopharmacol. 38, 1438–1450. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herwig, U., Opialla, S., Cattapan, K., Wetter, T. C., Jäncke, L., and Brühl, A. B. (2018). Emotion introspection and regulation in depression. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimag. 277:S0925492717303396.

Google Scholar

Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., and Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Persp. Psychol. Sci. 6, 537–559. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B., and Colzato, L. S. (2017). Meditation and metacontrol. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 1, 115–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilaria, B., Taryn, A., Alessio, A., Francesco, D. R., and Salvatore Maria, A. (2007). Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2553–2561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonathan, S., Annamay, F., Miles, L. K., and Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion 9, 271–276. doi: 10.1037/a0014855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kearney, D. J., Mcdermott, K., Martinez, M., and Simpson, T. L. (2011). Association of participation in a mindfulness programme with bowel symptoms, gastrointestinal symptom-specific anxiety and quality of life. Alimentary Pharmacol. Ther. 34, 363–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2036.2011.04731.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., and Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killingsworth, M. A., and Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330, 932–932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Daselaar, S. M., and Cabeza, R. (2010). Overlapping brain activity between episodic memory encoding and retrieval: roles of the task-positive and task-negative networks. Neuroimage 49, 1045–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenigs, M., Barbey, A. K., Postle, B. R., and Grafman, J. (2009). Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory. J. Neurosci. Official J. Soc. Neurosci. 29, 14980–14986. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3706-09.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., Wang, X., Song, Y., and Liu, J. (2016). Brain regions involved in dispositional mindfulness during resting state and their relation with well-being. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 331–343. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1092469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, G., Rossbach, K., Zhang, A., Liu, P., and Zhang, K. (2018). Resting-state functional changes in the precuneus within first-episode drug-naive patients with MDD. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 14, 1991–1998. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S168060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., and Yang, T. A. (2003). Applicability of the positive and negative affect scale in chinese. Chin. Men. Health J. 17, 54–56.

Google Scholar

Lippelt, D. P., Hommel, B., and Colzato, L. S. (2014). Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity - a review. Front. Psychol. 5:1083. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, X., Zuo, X., Kiviniemi, V., Yang, Y., Zou, Q., Zhu, C., et al. (2008). Default mode network as revealed with multiple methods for resting-state functional MRI analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 171, 349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, J., Brühl, A. B., Doerig, N., Scheerer, H., Achermann, R., Weibel, A., et al. (2015). Altered processing of self-related emotional stimuli in mindfulness meditators. Neuroimage 124, 958–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, J., Herwig, U., Opialla, S., Hittmeyer, A., Jäncke, L., Rufer, M., et al. (2014). Mindfulness and emotion regulation–an fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9:776. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magalhaes, A. A., Oliveira, L., Pereira, M. G., and Menezes, C. B. (2018). Does meditation alter brain responses to negative stimuli? a systematic review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:448. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malinowski, P., Moore, A. W., Mead, B. R., and Gruber, T. (2017). Mindful aging: the effects of regular brief mindfulness practice on electrophysiological markers of cognitive and affective processing in older adults. Mindfulness 8, 78–94. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0482-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Perrucci, M. G., Gratta, C., Del, Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170–13175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700668104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchand, W. R. (2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, and Zen meditation for depression, anxiety, pain, and psychological distress. J. Psychiatr. Pract. 18:233. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000416014.53215.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martelli, A. M., Chester, D. S., Warren Brown, K., Eisenberger, N. I., and Dewall, C. N. (2018). When less is more: mindfulness predicts adaptive affective responding to rejection via reduced prefrontal recruitment. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 648–655. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Horn, J. D., Van, Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., et al. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKiernan, K., Kaufman, J., Kucera-Thompson, J., and Binder, J. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 394–408. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion 12, 442–448. doi: 10.1037/a0026678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagahama, Y., Okada, T., Katsumi, Y., Hayashi, T., Yamauchi, H., Sawamoto, N., et al. (1999). Transient neural activity in the medial superior frontal gyrus and precuneus time locked with attention shift between object features. NeuroImage 10, 193–199. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 153–158.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Orenius, T. I, Raij, T. T., Nuortimo, A., NãÃTãNen, P., Lipsanen, J., and Karlsson, H. (2017). The interaction of emotion and pain in the insula and secondary somatosensory cortex. Neuroscience 349, 185–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, D., Liddle, E., Iwabuchi, S., Zhang, C., Wu, Z., Liu, J., et al. (2015). Dissociated large-scale functional connectivity networks of the precuneus in medication-naïve first-episode depression. Psychiatry Res. 232, 250–256. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickut, B., Van Hecke, W., Kerckhofs, E., Mariën, P., Vanneste, S., Cras, P., et al. (2013). Mindfulness based intervention in parkinson’s disease leads to structural brain changes on MRI: a randomized controlled longitudinal trial. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 115, 2419–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, P. (2015). The paired t-test. Essential Statistics for the Pharmaceutical Sciences. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Google Scholar

Ruben, J., Schwiemann, J., Deuchert, M., Meyer, R., Krause, T., Curio, G., et al. (2001). Somatotopic organization of human secondary somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 463–473. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.5.463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satpute, A. B., Kang, J., Bickart, K. C., Yardley, H., Wager, T. D., and Barrett, L. F. (2015). Involvement of sensory regions in affective experience: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 6:1860. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackman, A., Salomons, T., Slagter, H., Fox, A., Winter, J., and Davidson, R. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., and Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 373–386.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shaurya, P. R., De Leon, A. A., Klatt, M., Malarkey, W., and Patterson, B. (2013). Mindfulness disposition and default-mode network connectivity in older adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 112–117. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2009). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1942–1947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812686106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soler, J., Elices, M., Dominguez-Clavé, E., Pascual, J. C., Feilding, A., Navarro-Gil, M., et al. (2018). Four weekly ayahuasca sessions lead to increases in “acceptance” capacities: a comparison study with a standard 8-week mindfulness training program. Front. Pharmacol. 9:224. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y. (2011). Mechanism of integrative body-mind training. Neurosci. Bull. 27, 383–388. doi: 10.1007/s12264-011-1141-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y. H., Fan, Y. X., Feng, H. B., Wang, J. H., Feng, S. G., et al. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8865–8870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2012). Neural correlates of establishing, maintaining, and switching brain states. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 330–337. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, V. A., Daneault, V., Grant, J., Scavone, G., Breton, E., Roffe-Vidal, S., et al. (2013). Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8:4. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teper, R., Segal, Z. V., and Inzlicht, M. (2013). Inside the mindful mind: how mindfulness enhances emotion regulation through improvements in executive control. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 22, 449–454. doi: 10.1177/0963721413495869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmermann, L., Ploner, M., Haucke, K., Schmitz, F., Baltissen, R., and Schnitzler, A. (2001). Differential coding of pain intensity in the human primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1499–1503. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.3.1499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tom, E., Stefan, D., Calhoun, V. D., Karsten, S., Engel, A. K., and Kenneth, H. (2008). Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 6173–6178. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708965105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Chiesa, A., and Fabbro, F. (2014). Disentangling the neural mechanisms involved in Hinduism- and Buddhism-related meditations. Brain Cogn. 90, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tordesillasgutierrez, D., Ayesaarriola, R., Delgadoalvarado, M., Robinson, J. L., Lopezmorinigo, J., Pujol, J., et al. (2018). The right occipital lobe and poor insight in first-episode psychosis. Plos One 13:e0197715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tra, K., Schuyler, B. S., Mumford, J. A., Rosenkranz, M. A., Lutz, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2018). Impact of short- and long-term mindfulness meditation training on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. Neuroimage 181, 301–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitaly, N., Sheehan, J. D., Jieun, K., Lacount, L. T., Kyungmo, P., Kaptchuk, T. J., et al. (2013). The brain circuitry underlying the temporal evolution of nausea in humans. Cereb. Cortex 23, 806–813. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadden, K., Snow, N., Sande, P., Slawson, S., Waller, T., and Boyd, L. (2018). Yoga practitioners uniquely activate the superior parietal lobule and supramarginal gyrus during emotion regulation. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 12:60. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadlinger, H. A., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Fixing our focus: training attention to regulate emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 15, 75–102. doi: 10.1177/1088868310365565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Yang, Y., Fan, L., Xu, J., Li, C., Liu, Y., et al. (2015). Convergent functional architecture of the superior parietal lobule unraveled with multimodal neuroimaging approaches. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 238–257. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Xu, M., Song, Y., Li, X., Zhen, Z., Yang, Z., et al. (2014). The network property of the thalamus in the default mode network is correlated with trait mindfulness. Neuroscience 278, 291–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, S. (2014). The Mindful Trader and Investor. TraderMind: Get a Mindful Edge in the Markets. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Google Scholar

Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., and Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978. doi: 10.1038/nn1727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Moran, J. M., Nieto-Castañón, A., Triantafyllou, C., Saxe, R., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2011). Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage 55, 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y., Wang, J., Zhang, Y., Zheng, D., Zhang, J., Rong, M., et al. (2016). The neuroanatomical basis for posterior superior parietal lobule control lateralization of visuospatial attention. Front. Neuroanatom. 10:32. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, M., Fralick, D., Zheng, J. Z., Wang, B., Tu, X. M., and Feng, C. (2017). The differences and similarities between two-sample -test and paired -test. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 29, 184–188. doi: 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.217070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, C. G., and Zang, Y. F. (2010). DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, C., Barrós-Loscertales, A., Li, M., Pinazo, D., Borchardt, V., Ávila, C., et al. (2019). Alterations in brain structure and amplitude of low-frequency after 8 weeks of mindfulness meditation training in meditation-naïve subjects. Sci. Rep. 9:10977.

Google Scholar

Zang, Y., Jiang, T., Lu, Y., He, Y., and Tian, L. (2004). Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 22, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidan, F., Martucci, K., Kraft, R., McHaffie, J., and Coghill, R. (2014). Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 751–759. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion regulation, functional connectivity, mindfulness, regional homogeneity, default mode network, functional magnetic resonance imaging

Citation: Xiao Q, Zhao X, Bi G, Wu L, Zhang H, Liu R, Zhong J, Wu S, Zeng Y, Cui L, Chen Y, Wu K and Chen Z (2019) Alterations of Regional Homogeneity and Functional Connectivity Following Short-Term Mindfulness Meditation in Healthy Volunteers. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:376. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00376

Received: 27 February 2019; Accepted: 03 October 2019;
Published: 18 October 2019.

Edited by:

Felix Scholkmann, University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Emmanuel Mellet, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France
Jiaojian Wang, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Copyright © 2019 Xiao, Zhao, Bi, Wu, Zhang, Liu, Zhong, Wu, Zeng, Cui, Chen, Wu and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shaoyuan Wu, wushaoyuan777@163.com; Kunhua Wu, wukunhua@hotmail.com; Zhuangfei Chen, chen.zhf@outlook.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work as joint first authors

Download