Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 08 April 2020
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00085

The Relationship Between Socioeconomic Status and Brain Volume in Children and Adolescents With Prenatal Alcohol Exposure

Kristina A. Uban1,2* Eric Kan2 Jeffrey R. Wozniak3 Sarah N. Mattson4 Claire D. Coles5 Elizabeth R. Sowell2 on behalf of Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders
  • 1Public Health, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States

The positive relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and cognitive performance is mediated, in part, by differences in brain structure in typically developing youth. Associations between brain regions that relate to SES overlap with brain regions known to be sensitive to prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE). Animal models demonstrate that PAE attenuates neural and cognitive benefits of early life enrichment. However, whether or not environmental factors related to SES are associated with brain development in youth affected by PAE remains unknown in humans.

Methods: T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans were obtained in participants with PAE and compared to age- and sex- matched Controls (n = 197, 48% with PAE, 44% girls, 6.5–17.7 years old). General linear modeling was utilized to examine associations between SES and subcortical brain volumes for youth with PAE compared to Controls.

Results: Group by SES interactions were observed within the hippocampus (HPC), nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral diencephalon (vDC) (corrected p values <0.05), where positive associations (e.g., higher SES related to larger subcortical volumes) were observed within Controls, but not youth with PAE. Post hoc analyses examined associations between SES and brain volumes within each group independently, and revealed widespread positive associations among Controls (Amyg, HPC, NAc, Pallidum, Putamen, vDC), but not youth with PAE. Across both groups, larger subcortical volumes were related to higher cognitive performance.

Conclusion: Typically developing youth exhibit increased subcortical volumes with increased SES, and surprisingly, this relationship is absent in adolescents with PAE. Findings suggest that subcortical brain volumes are neurocognitively relevant in both groups. The present results expand our understanding of the impact of PAE on the developing human brain within varying environmental contexts, and may inform novel environmental interventions that aim to improve, in part, on-going disruptions in brain development among youth with PAE. Our study highlights novel complexities in the pursuit to understand SES-brain associations, as we provide evidence that SES matters for brain outcomes among typically developing youth, and possibly not as much on an already altered brain as a result of PAE.

Introduction

Individuals affected by prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) can present with a complex profile of cognitive, behavioral, physical, and mental health problems, referred to as fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD) (Abel and Hannigan, 1995; Hellemans et al., 2010). FASD is an umbrella term that includes all individuals with a PAE-related diagnosis. FASD is completely preventable, yet is in the top 3 known causes of intellectual disability. Any amount of PAE can potentially result in FASD, with a current prevalence of 2.4–4.8% in the United States (US) (May et al., 2014). Alcohol consumption among women of childbearing age varies by geography, environment and cultural practices (May et al., 2014, 2016a,b; Bakhireva et al., 2017). For example, 2017 prevalence reports of PAE in Texas ranged significantly depending on county (0 to 17.7%) (Bakhireva et al., 2017). Despite evidence that a larger proportion of women with high SES drink during pregnancy compared to mothers from low SES (Pfinder et al., 2012; McCormack et al., 2017), more severe FASD diagnoses (e.g., FAS, partial FAS) are more frequently found among mothers within lower socioeconomic status (SES) contexts (May and Gossage, 2011; Pfinder, 2014), indicating that factors relating to SES may influence developmental outcomes of PAE.

In the United States, individuals with a diagnosis under the umbrella term of FASD tend to be adopted, experience greater early life adversity, and exhibit a higher prevalence for mental health issues later in life than those without a FASD. However, there is a lack of animal and human studies that explore the divergent effects of other key environmental versus teratological effects of PAE on cognitive and brain development that occur with FASD. Interestingly, developmental outcomes associated with low SES (Hackman et al., 2010) greatly overlap with outcomes associated with FASD (e.g., stress dysregulation, low birth weight, increased mental health problems, impaired cognitive and emotional regulation) suggesting that deficits observed among individuals with FASD may extend beyond teratological effects of prenatal substance use to include factors correlated with low SES during the perinatal period.

Socioeconomic status is multifaceted, and includes components at multiple levels (e.g., individual, home, community), which makes measuring SES challenging (Hackman and Farah, 2009). Developmental outcomes relating to SES often depend on the specific component of SES being measured (Hackman and Farah, 2009). SES is most commonly measured as a combination of parental income, education and occupation, and can greatly impact access to resources to promote healthy child and adolescent development. Experiences within the environment are necessary to shape and wire the brain during development (Hensch, 2005), and PAE alters these neural sequences during various stages of perinatal development (Maier et al., 1999). Human studies have shown that trajectories of structural and functional brain development differ between youth with PAE compared to those without PAE (Lebel et al., 2012; Gautam et al., 2014, 2015a,b). Thus, we hypothesize that a combination of lasting PAE-effects plus altered neuronal responses to SES components likely interact to influence altered brain maturation among individuals with FASD. Thus, we predict SES will relate to brain outcomes differently in youth with PAE compared to youth without PAE.

Examination of the relationship between SES and cognitive functioning has been increasing over the past 4 decades, with low SES relating to delays in language, executive function, and working memory from infancy to early adulthood (for review see Hackman and Farah, 2009). However, few studies have examined underlying neural mechanisms of this positive association between SES and IQ. Recent work has shown that the long-known negative impact of low SES on cognitive and behavioral development (Noble et al., 2005) is indeed mediated by differences in brain structure during childhood in typically developing children (Noble et al., 2005, 2012, 2015; Piccolo et al., 2016). Notably, many of the cognitive outcomes (i.e., executive function, learning and memory) that are highly correlated with SES are also altered following moderate-severe PAE (Mattson and Riley, 1999; Mattson et al., 1999; Vaurio et al., 2008). However, this important aspect of the FASD experience regarding the environmental context in which the brain develops has received little attention. Thus, we aim to address this gap in knowledge in the present study by exploring brain-SES associations in youth with PAE compared to youth without PAE.

Deficits in executive functioning and working memory are consistently documented in both human and animal studies of PAE (Mattson and Riley, 1998). Human studies have demonstrated alterations in brain regions underlying these cognitive functions, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus (HPC) (Astley et al., 2009; Dudek et al., 2014; Joseph et al., 2014; Donald et al., 2015). Mechanistically, animal models demonstrate an overall decrease in neuroplasticity following PAE within the PFC (Barr et al., 2005; Hamilton et al., 2010) and HPC (Uban et al., 2010; Brady et al., 2013; Fontaine et al., 2016). These same rodent models of PAE demonstrate that the neural outcomes of postnatal environmental manipulations differ in animals with PAE compared to those without PAE. For example, a partial rescue of PAE-related learning and memory deficits can be observed through environmental and early life manipulations such as neonatal handling, environmental enrichment, or motor training, and this improvement is sometimes reflected by neuroanatomical changes in the PAE animals despite lasting reductions in neural plasticity following PAE [reviewed in Hannigan et al. (2007)]. Recent animal models demonstrate the neural benefits of early life enrichment are observed but may be attenuated by PAE (Ignacio et al., 2014; Kajimoto et al., 2016), and the neural effects of early life stress may be enhanced by PAE (Comeau et al., 2014, 2015; Raineki et al., 2017). In humans, SES was associated with test performance on a response inhibition functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) task, but not with brain activation in youth with PAE and also in those without PAE (Ware et al., 2015). Whether or not SES is associated with brain structure in children and adolescents with PAE remains unknown.

Among recent PAE studies with adolescents, subcortical regions are perhaps more strongly implicated in a neurological signature of PAE-insult (Gautam et al., 2015a; Little and Beaulieu, 2019). Thus, here we targeted subcortical regions, with the parallel aim to extend understanding of SES-related alterations among subcortical volumes. Importantly, since the review of the present study, others have shown associations between SES factors and subcortical brain volumes among adolescents (McDermott et al., 2019; Jenkins et al., 2020; Machlin et al., 2020); therefore validating the original aim to target subcortical regions. A primary mechanism for PAE-related harm is through lasting alterations on central stress regulation (Raineki et al., 2014, 2019). With alterations in stress neurocircuitry as a potential overlapping mechanism between PAE- and SES- related effects on the developing brain (Lebel et al., 2019), we examined stress-sensitive subcortical regions: those residing within the forebrain. The present study examined differences in associations between SES and subcortical brain volume in youth with PAE compared to non-exposed youth as Controls. We hypothesize SES is positively associated with subcortical brain volumes, and that this relationship: (1) is attenuated in participants with PAE; (2) varies as a function the time from birth until adoption into permanent home; and (3) correlates with cognitive performance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

All procedures were approved by each site’s IRB and all subjects underwent a comprehensive informed consent/assent procedure both verbally and written. Participants were compensated with cash or gift cards for their time. High-resolution T1-weighted MRI data were obtained in children and adolescents with PAE, and their age- and sex- matched non-exposed Controls (Table 1; Total n = 197, 48% with PAE, 44% girls, 6.5–17.7 years old). MRI was collected from 4 imaging sites (26% Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, 31% University of Minnesota, 21% Emory, 22% San Diego State University) as part of the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (CIFASD) (Mattson et al., 2010), and data collected from Phase III by 2015 was utilized in analyses. All participants were recruited through multiple sources to increase diversity, including online and print fliers in neighborhoods and community forums with a range of SES levels, as well as word of mouth from former participants. Additionally, individuals with PAE were referred through local FASD Clinics or responded to ads with local FASD-specific caregiver groups. Positive alcohol exposure histories were confirmed via review of records or maternal report and confirmed by a licensed medical doctor (Mattson et al., 2010). Participants in the PAE group were exposed to moderate to severe levels of alcohol (≥ 13 drinks/week or >4 drinks/occasion) (Mattson et al., 2010). Adoption rates differed between participants with PAE and those serving as Controls (Table 1; Boys: 4% Control, 68%. PAE; Girls: 12.5% Control, 80% PAE). General exclusion criteria included children/adolescents: (1) who were not fluent in English and/or did not learn English by age 5; (2) whose parents did not speak English; (3) who experienced claustrophobia; (4) who had metal on or in the body that could not be removed; (5) who had a vision or hearing problem that could not be corrected; (6) who thought they may be pregnant; and (7) who endorsed any previous substance use themselves (Mattson et al., 2010). In-person interviews were conducted with the primary caregiver to obtain detailed developmental histories of adolescents (including prenatal substance exposure). Control participants were excluded if they were exposed to >1 drink per week on average or >2 drinks on a single occasion during the in utero period (Mattson et al., 2010).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptives.

SES Measures

All SES measures were based on the participants’ current caregiver participating in the study. SES measures reflect the primary caregiver in the home (and not combined across all caregivers), and only SES measures at the time of participation in the study (e.g., SES measures of birth parents or historical SES of current caregivers were not assessed). Two factors (educational attainment, occupational prestige) were utilized from the Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Socioeconomic Status to examine relationships between SES and brain volume. Educational attainment was assessed in the participating child/adolescent’s parent based on a 7-point scale assessing the highest level of education completed (e.g., 1 ≤ 7th grade; 7 = graduate/professional training). Raw parental education scores were multiplied by 3 to obtain weighted Educational Status Values for subsequent statistical analyses. Similarly, parental occupational status was based on a 9-point scale [1 = farm laborers, unskilled service workers, students, housewives (dependent on welfare, no regular occupation); 9 = higher executive, proprietor of large businesses, major professional]. Raw parental occupation scores were multiplied by 5 to obtain weighted Occupational Status Values for utilization in subsequent statistical analyses. Scores of “0” (e.g., not applicable or unknown) were not included in statistical analyses. A third measure of SES in the present study utilized the scaled assessment [from Noble et al. (2015)] to capture the current combined annual household parental income [1 = $0–9,999; 2 = $10,000–19,999; 3 = $20,000–29,999; 4 = $30,000–49,999; 5 = $50,000–74,999; 6 = $75,000–99,999; 7 = $100,000+].

MRI Acquisition

Acquisition protocols were based on the Pediatric Imaging Neurocognition and Genetics study (PING) (Brown et al., 2012), which harmonized parameters across imaging platforms. Whole brain high-resolution structural anatomical images were acquired in the sagittal plane using a T1 weighted MPRAGE scanning sequence (CHLA: Philips Achieva v3.2.1, echo time (TE) = 3.185 ms, repetition time (TR) = 6.795, slice thickness = 1 × 1.2 × 1 mm3 isotropic; Emory: Siemens Trio Syngo MR B17, TE = 4.33 ms, TR = 2170, slice thickness = 1 × 1.2 × 1 mm3 non-isotropic; SDSU: GE Discovery MR750 DV22.0_V02_1122, TE = 2.984, TR = 7.38, slice thickness = 1 × 1.2 × 0.9 mm3 isotropic; UMN: Siemens Trio Syngo MR B17, TE = 4.33 ms, TR = 2170, slice thickness = 1 × 1.2 × 1 mm3 non-isotropic).

MRI Preprocessing

All data were processed through FreeSurfer image analysis suite (version 5.1) recon-all automated processing pipeline, which is freely available at http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). Expert neuroanatomists reviewed each output image to detect possible errors made by the program. If errors were found, manual edits were made and the image was re-processed with corrected inputs. The recon-all processing pipeline: (1) applies linear and non-linear alignments of data to a standard MNI-space template, (2) corrects for inhomogeneities in signal, (3) removes non-brain voxels from the image (skull strip), and finally (4) applies volume labeling through probabilistic atlas. 8 ROIs were selected as from the subcortical gray matter ROIs, of which the forebrain is comprised [thalamus, caudate, putamen, pallidum, hippocampus (HPC), amygdala (Amyg), nucleus accumbens (NAc), ventral diencephalon (vDC)]. Region-of-interest (ROI)-wise subcortical volumes were averaged across the left and right hemispheres for statistical analyses.

Age at Adoption

The majority of participants with PAE lived with an adoptive family (74%; see Table 1 for details). It is possible that the associations between SES and brain measures may be attenuated among children/adolescents with PAE due to less time within the current SES context, or due to unmeasured effects of birth family SES. Thus, we utilized age (in postnatal months) upon entry into the adoptive home (e.g., 0 = not adopted, 24 = adopted at age 24 months, etc.) and age at MRI scan as covariates to begin to address this question with post hoc analyses to inform future studies.

Working Memory and Executive Functioning

A secondary aim was to examine the potential cognitive relevance of observed differences in SES-brain associations between the Control and PAE groups. To do so, two cognitive functions were selected because they are pervasively altered by moderate-heavy PAE in human adolescents (Rasmussen, 2005): working memory (implicating the hippocampus) and executive functioning (implicating the prefrontal cortex and the integrated subcortical regions of the forebrain). Standardized summary scores from two subtests of the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment (NEPSY)-II (e.g., Delayed Spatial Memory, Inhibition Switching) were utilized as metrics of working memory and executive functioning, respectively.

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were conducted with the statistical program R (v3.0.2) (R Core Team, 2013). To examine associations between SES (e.g., educational value, occupational value, income) and subcortical volumes, general linear modeling (GLM) was applied, controlling for age, sex and imaging site within youth with PAE, and then within Control youth separately. False discovery rate (FDR) was utilized for multiple comparison corrections for each SES factor. Follow-up analyses for significant results passing FDR correction were conducted to replicate GLM analyses while including age at adoption as an additional covariate. To directly assess Group (PAE vs. Control) differences in subcortical brain volumes for significant GLM results, RMANOVA was utilized with hemisphere as within-subject measure. To confirm past cognitive findings, Group (PAE vs. Control) differences in cognitive performance were assessed with ANOVA utilizing a Bonferroni test (corrected p value = 0.025). ROIs exhibiting significant group differences in SES-brain associations were examined for potential neurocognitive relevance (e.g., hippocampus, nucleus accumbens and ventral diencephalon). GLM was used to examine group differences in brain-cognition associations for each of the 3 ROIs, and for each of the two cognitive outcomes (working memory and executive functioning). Bonferroni correction was used to control for multiple comparisons for each cognitive outcome, with a corrected p value of <0.016.

Results

Subcortical Volume

A summary of associations between SES and brain volumes can be found in Table 2 and illustrated in Figure 1. Between group analyses showed significant Group × SES interactions in the HPC (Employment (t(1, 197) = −2.79, p < 0.01) and Education (t(1, 197) = −2.19, p < 0.05)), NAc [Employment (t(1, 197) = −2.61, p < 0.01], Education [t(1, 197) = −2.32, p < 0.05)], and the vDC [Education value (t(1, 197) = −2.08, p < 0.05)]. No significant Group × SES interactions were observed with Income after FDR correction for multiple comparisons (corrected p values >0.24). Volumes of these subcortical regions were larger in Control compared to participants with PAE (all p’s < 0.01), except within the NAc (p = 0.78) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of all SES-brain associations.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Regions of Interest (ROIs) exhibiting significant SES × subcortical volumes associations in Controls. Plotted ROIs illustrate regions where positive associations between SES and brain volumes in Controls (corrected p values <0.05), but not youth with PAE (n.s.) were observed in post hoc analyses. Regions with significant group differences in SES-volume associations include the nucleus accumbens (NAc), hippocampus (HPC), and ventral diencephalon (vDC). Image on left provides a side view, and image on right provides a top view of ROIs. Abbreviations: A = anterior; P = posterior; S = superior; I = inferior; L = left hemisphere; R = right hemisphere.

Within-group analyses (e.g., Control only, PAE only) were utilized as post hoc examination to determine the precise nature of the significant Group × SES interactions for predicting brain volume (Figure 2). Widespread positive associations were observed in Control adolescents, where higher SES measures were associated with larger subcortical volumes with Education value [HPC: Employment value [t(102) = 2.96, p < 0.01], Education value [t(102) = 4.43, p < 0.0001]; NAc: Employment value [t(102) = 2.02, p < 0.05], Education value [t(102) = 2.88, p < 0.01]; vDC: Education value [t(102) = 2.79, p < 0.01] (Figure 2). However, within participants with PAE, no significant SES-brain associations were observed (FDR corrected p values >0.55).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatterplots illustrate SES-brain associations for each ROI using Education value as SES measure and subcortical volumes (y-axis in mm3). Raw data are plotted, thus the best-fit lines are not adjusted for the utilized covariates (site, sex, race). Statistical results demonstrate positive associations between SES and volume in Control, but not PAE. The present plots with raw data corroborate statistical findings, except within the NAc, suggesting that inclusion of covariates is necessary to observe positive associations between NAc volume and SES in Controls.

Age at Adoption

Group × SES interactions remained significant after the inclusion of “age at adoption” as a covariate in the HPC [Employment (p < 0.01) and Education (p < 0.05) value] and NAc [Employment (p = 0.055) and Education (p = 0.055) value], whereas the Group × SES interaction was no longer significant within the vDC [Education value (p = 0.47)].

Cognitive Correlates of SES on Subcortical Volume

Only brain regions exhibiting a significant SES × Group interaction were examined to see if they are related to cognitive performance. Larger HPC volumes related to better cognitive performance across both groups HPC: working memory [t(195) = 2.67, p < 0.01], executive functioning [t(1, 186) = 3.01, p < 0.01]; and a trend within the vDC [working memory (t(1, 195) = 1.64, p = 0.10), executive functioning (t(1, 186) = 2.31, p < 0.05)]. Overall, participants with PAE exhibited lower working memory [F(1,189) = 20.97, p < 0.001)] and executive functioning [F(1,180) = 24.34, p < 0.001)] scores compared to Controls, and there were no significant group differences in brain-cognition associations in any of these regions for working memory (p’s > 0.21) or executive functioning (p’s > 0.19).

Discussion

Summary

Higher SES was associated with larger subcortical volumes in Controls, but not youth with PAE, within the HPC, NAc, and vDC. After controlling for the participant’s age at time of adoption, these significant Group differences in SES-brain associations remained in the HPC and NAc, but not the vDC. Overall, larger volumes of the HPC and vDC were observed in Control compared to participants with PAE, while no Group differences were observed for NAc volumes. Results within Control youth match previous findings with the hippocampus, corroborating that SES factors are important for the typically developing brain (Noble et al., 2012, 2015). Our novel results within youth with PAE suggest an absence of the typical positive relationship between SES and regional brain volumes. The SES-brain associations observed in the present study are neurocognitively important, as higher SES related to larger subcortical volumes in Controls, and larger subcortical volumes related to better working memory and executive functioning performance for both Groups.

SES-Brain Associations Are Attenuated, or Absent, in Youth With PAE

As proposed by May et al. (2013), the effects of PAE on behavioral and cognitive outcomes are “filtered through multiple environmental conditions in the child’s formative years” (May et al., 2013). It is possible that SES-brain associations were not observed among youth with PAE as a result of reduced neuroplasticity plus increased sensitivity to stress, which could attenuate brain-SES associations. Neurobiological alterations observed in animal models of PAE demonstrate reduced capacity to benefit from protective environmental factors, as well as increased neurobiological sensitivity to harmful factors associated with low SES. Findings in the present human study corroborate these findings from animal studies.

Stress

In general, higher life stress is associated with lower SES (Goodman et al., 2005; Lantz et al., 2005). The subcortical regions that exhibit positive SES-volume associations in Controls but not children with PAE are known to be sensitive to stress [e.g., HPC, NAc, vDC (e.g., thalamus, hypothalamus), and putamen]. Animal studies demonstrate that PAE rats are more impacted by chronic stress in adolescence compared to controls, and become more sensitive to stress-induced cognitive deficits in adulthood (Comeau et al., 2015). It is possible that protective factors associated with high SES do not fully mitigate HPA sensitivity following PAE in human adolescents, and therefore underlies, in part, the attenuation of SES-brain associations. Among animal models, PAE results in a range of neurobiological disruptions that may underlie, in part, reduced neural responsivity to positive environmental factors (Hannigan et al., 2007). Interestingly, these alterations in neural plasticity often implicate interactive effects with underlying alterations in stress-hormone systems following PAE (Hannigan et al., 2007; Uban et al., 2010, 2013, 2015; Comeau et al., 2014).

Timing of Brain Development

Animal models of PAE have demonstrated a persistent reduction in neurons, microglia and oligodendrocytes. Subcortical structures were smaller in youth with PAE compared to Controls, suggesting that PAE-related alterations persist well beyond the perinatal period. Subcortical structures typically mature early relative to more prolonged maturation of lateral cortical brain regions. This relatively earlier timeline may underlie, in part, increased susceptible to prenatal, and decreased susceptibility to postnatal, factors. The present study spans a large period of brain development when many dynamic processes are occurring, including rising pubertal hormone levels. Thus, it is possible that the relationship between SES and brain volumes also exhibits a dynamic developmental trajectory, and deserves future investigation. Regardless of PAE status, larger subcortical volumes related to better cognitive performance; thus, subcortical structures may be important for future research on environmental interventions to optimize later cognitive performance in all youth.

Impact of SES of Birth Versus Adoptive Parents

The present study was unable to disentangle the association between SES of birth parents versus adoptive parents on brain measures. Past research shows that IQ is higher among children adopted by families with high SES compared to those adopted into low SES, and IQ is higher among children born into families of high SES compared to low SES among typically developing individuals (Capron and Duyme, 1989): suggesting unique contributions from birth- versus adoptive- family SES on cognition. Among typically developing individuals, almost half of the variability in IQ was explained by the SES of the adoptive, and not the birth, family (Capron and Duyme, 1989). However, poverty occurring earlier in life was a better predictor of cognitive performance compared to poverty experienced later in life (Duncan et al., 1998). Further, heritability of IQ is modified by SES, with higher SES contexts amplifying genetic influences (Turkheimer et al., 2003), particularly in nations where quality of education and health care vary as a function of SES like the United States (Tucker-Drob and Bates, 2016). Given that the majority of participants with PAE in the study were adopted (and very few adoptions within the Control group), this is a major limitation for interpretation of SES-brain results. Thus, whether or not the absence of SES-brain associations in the present study among youth with PAE is related to SES during perinatal development remains unknown and warrants future investigation.

Risk Versus Protective Factors of SES

When considering the underlying mechanisms of SES-brain associations in typically developing participants, it is important to note that SES is a highly complex phenomenon, and should be viewed as an approximation of clustered health-behaviors (Pfinder, 2014). It is the differential environmental factors that are afforded across different income levels that impact brain development, rather than income level itself, per se. Specifically, risk (e.g., environmental toxins, stress) and protective (e.g., environmental enrichment, nutrition) factors known to be important for neural development often vary as a function of SES, where children from low SES families are generally exposed to higher levels of environmental toxins and stress/adversity, and children from high SES families are typically exposed to more environmental enrichment (Lupien et al., 2000; Morello-Frosch and Shenassa, 2006; Hackman et al., 2010; McEwen and Gianaros, 2010; Noble et al., 2012). Thus, it is possible that higher exposures to known risk factors for brain development may result in smaller subcortical brain volumes, and poorer cognitive performance, whereas higher exposures to known protective factors for brain development may result in larger subcortical brain volumes, and better cognitive performance. Given the complex nature of SES, both risk and protective factors contribute simultaneously to SES-related differences in brain development. The timing of exposure to risk or protective factors is likely unique to each family or wider community. Future research is needed to explore specific, and perhaps opposing, contributions of risk versus protective factors on brain development. The presence of cognitive differences between children from families with low compared to high SES prior to school-age suggests that SES impacts cognitive and brain development during perinatal years [reviewed in Hackman and Farah (2009)]. Further, PAE is a teratogen, which may enhance sensitivity to other environmental toxin exposures that generally occur at a higher level among low SES neighborhoods. Thus, it is possible that future studies exploring SES-brain associations that involve other toxin exposures (e.g., BPA, phthalates, air pollution, lead) may find an exaggerated, and not attenuated, relationship between SES and brain measures compared to non-exposed Controls.

Unique Associations Among SES Factors

Interestingly, within Controls, higher educational and employment values were associated with larger volumes of several subcortical regions, but not income in the present study. Examination of different SES factors, such as education and income, might impart different risk or resilience because they may represent distinct resources that have different roles in a child’s development. Family income may provide access to material resources such as good nutrition, health care and good educational environments, whereas higher parental education in the presence or absence high family income may contribute to positive child-parent interactions (Duncan and Magnuson, 2012). Thus, it is entirely plausible that the individual factors that comprise our measurement of SES show unique, and interactive, associations with specific subcortical brain regions. Experimental manipulation of social status within the laboratory environment of a past study, elicits changes in activity within the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate and thalamus, indicating that social status may influence development of brain regions important for processing emotional and social stimuli (Zink et al., 2008).

Limitations of Present Study Influencing SES-Brain Associations

Several indices of SES exist that were not available for investigation in the present study, and may prove to be valuable for further elucidation of how PAE alters SES-brain associations in future studies. These include change between prenatal and adolescent SES, income-to-needs ratio (same income may afford different levels of resources across imaging sites), and other school-community based factors related to SES. Significantly more participants affected by PAE were living in adoptive homes (74%) compared to Control youth (8%); therefore SES of the adoptive family was analyzed in youth with PAE, but SES of the birth family was analyzed in Control youth. It is possible that birth family SES may interact with adoptive family SES for brain development in youth with PAE, as low SES has been previously shown to related to other known risk health-behaviors in biological mothers during pregnancy, such as greater incidence of cigarette smoking or poorer nutrition (Pfinder, 2014). Information on physical, psychological or nutritional trauma experienced between birth entry into a permanent adoptive home was not readily available in the present study, and may explain a lack of SES-brain associations among youth with PAE. A caveat to the present findings is that Figure 2 illustrates raw data and graphical best-fit lines that do not take important covariates into account (e g., sex, site, race). All regions demonstrating positive associations with SES and volume in Controls are observable in the raw data figures, except within the NAc, where the association in Controls appears to be neutral, and negative in PAE group when plotting raw data. Thus thee present findings within this region are likely dependent on incorporation of covariates, and may be difficult to replicate in future studies in the absence of these covariates. The present study focused on subcortical volumes, however, existing studies demonstrate associations between cortical measures and SES in typically developing individuals (Noble et al., 2012), and deserve further investigation among individuals with PAE.

Conclusion

SES-brain associations in typically developing youth corroborate findings from previous research. The present study expands this understanding by revealing an absence of typical SES-brain associations following PAE. This novel finding in humans integrate well with animal literature demonstrating that PAE alters the impact of environmental factors on neurobehavioral outcomes. It is possible that reduced neural plasticity, increased stress sensitivity, prenatal SES (not measured in present study) and/or higher rates of early life adversity common to FASD may all contribute to the absence of typical SES-brain associations, warranting future investigation. The present findings expand our understanding of how PAE impacts brain outcomes in humans as a function of SES-factors. Future research on the PAE brain should investigate differential impact of risk versus protective factors that differ as a function of SES to inform interventions that aim to improve cognitive functioning among youth with FASD.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study will be made publicly available upon request to CIFASD. The datasets are publicly available, and all requests should be made through the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (see https://cifasd.org/data-sharing/).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, SanDiego State University, University of Minnesota and Emory’s Internal Review Boards. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

KU helped design specific analytic plan, collect and analyze data, interpret and write up results, manuscript preparation and submission. EK helped collect and analyze data. JW, CC, SM, and ES helped design overall consortium under which data was collected, and provided feedback on manuscript preparation.

Funding

The neuroimaging research was funded by grants from National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) through the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (CIFASD) U01 AA017122 and U01 AA014834 to ES, JW, SM, and CC. KU was funded by K01AA026889.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the families for their participation, and the research assistants for executing data collection at CHLA (Max Orozco, Alexy Andrade, Trinh Luu, Jordan Barlam) and UMN (Julia Tang, Daniel Keefe, Timothy Hendrickson), Emory (Zhihao Li, Sharron Paige). We would also like to thank Dr. Bryon Mueller and Dr. Christopher Boys for their assistance with UMN MRI data and participant acquisition, respectively. This work analyzed the data collected as part of the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (CIFASD; E. Riley, San Diego State University, Principal Investigator). The data collection sites and associated investigators described in this manuscript are: Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles (ES), University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (JW), San Diego State University, San Diego (SM), and Emory, Atlanta (CC). Additional information about CIFASD can be found at www.cifasd.org.

References

Abel, E. L., and Hannigan, J. H. (1995). Maternal risk factors in fetal alcohol syndrome: provocative and permissive influences. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 17, 445–462. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(95)98055-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astley, S. J., Aylward, E. H., Olson, H. C., Kerns, K., Brooks, A., Coggins, T. E., et al. (2009). Magnetic resonance imaging outcomes from a comprehensive magnetic resonance study of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 1671–1689. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01004.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakhireva, L. N., Sharkis, J., Shrestha, S., Miranda-Sohrabji, T. J., Williams, S., and Miranda, R. C. (2017). Prevalence of prenatal alcohol exposure in the state of texas as assessed by phosphatidylethanol in newborn dried blood spot specimens. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 1004–1011. doi: 10.1111/acer.13375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, A. M., Hofmann, C. E., Phillips, A. G., Weinberg, J., and Honer, W. G. (2005). Prenatal ethanol exposure in rats decreases levels of complexin proteins in the frontal cortex. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 1915–1920. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000187806.68957.0a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, M. L., Diaz, M. R., Iuso, A., Everett, J. C., Valenzuela, C. F., and Caldwell, K. K. (2013). Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure reduces plasticity and alters NMDA receptor subunit composition in the dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 33, 1062–1067. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1217-12.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M., Chung, Y., Erhart, M., McCabe, C., and Hagler, D. J. Jr., et al. (2012). Neuroanatomical assessment of biological maturity. Curr. Biol. 22, 1693–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capron, C., and Duyme, M. (1989). Assessment of effects of socio-economis status on IQ in a full cross-fostering study. Nature 340, 552–554. doi: 10.1038/340552a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comeau, W. L., Lee, K., Anderson, K., and Weinberg, J. (2015). Prenatal alcohol exposure and adolescent stress increase sensitivity to stress and gonadal hormone influences on cognition in adult female rats. Physiol. Behav. 148, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comeau, W. L., Winstanley, C. A., and Weinberg, J. (2014). Prenatal alcohol exposure and adolescent stress - unmasking persistent attentional deficits in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 3078–3095. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donald, K. A., Eastman, E., Howells, F. M., Adnams, C., Riley, E. P., Woods, R. P., et al. (2015). Neuroimaging effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the developing human brain: a magnetic resonance imaging review. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 27, 251–269. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudek, J., Skocic, J., Sheard, E., and Rovet, J. (2014). Hippocampal abnormalities in youth with alcohol-related neurodevelopmental disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 181–191. doi: 10.1017/s1355617713001343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Yeunh, W. J., and Smith, J. R. (1998). How much does childhood poverty affect the life chances of children? Am. Sociol. Rev. 63, 406–423.

Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., and Magnuson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: Moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdiscipl. Rev. Cognit. Sci. 3, 377–386. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontaine, C. J., Patten, A. R., Sickmann, H. M., Helfer, J. L., and Christie, B. R. (2016). Effects of pre-natal alcohol exposure on hippocampal synaptic plasticity: sex, age and methodological considerations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 64, 12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, P., Lebel, C., Narr, K. L., Mattson, S. N., May, P. A., Adnams, C. M., et al. (2015a). Volume changes and brain-behavior relationships in white matter and subcortical gray matter in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2318–2329. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, P., Nunez, S. C., Narr, K. L., Mattson, S. N., May, P. A., Adnams, C. M., et al. (2015b). Developmental trajectories for visuo-spatial attention are altered by prenatal alcohol exposure: a longitudinal FMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4761–4771. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, P., Nunez, S. C., Narr, K. L., Kan, E. C., and Sowell, E. R. (2014). Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the development of white matter volume and change in executive function. Neuroimage Clin. 5, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, E., McEwen, B. S., Dolan, L. M., Schafer-Kalkhoff, T., and Adler, N. E. (2005). Social disadvantage and adolescent stress. J. Adolesc. Health 37, 484–492. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.11.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., and Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cognir. Sci. 13, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., and Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, D. A., Candelaria-Cook, F. T., Akers, K. G., Rice, J. P., Maes, L. I., Rosenberg, M., et al. (2010). Patterns of social-experience-related c-fos and Arc expression in the frontal cortices of rats exposed to saccharin or moderate levels of ethanol during prenatal brain development. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannigan, J. H., O’Leary-Moore, S. K., and Berman, R. F. (2007). Postnatal environmental or experiential amelioration of neurobehavioral effects of perinatal alcohol exposure in rats. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 202–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellemans, K. G., Sliwowska, J. H., Verma, P., and Weinberg, J. (2010). Prenatal alcohol exposure: fetal programming and later life vulnerability to stress, depression and anxiety disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 791–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hensch, T. K. (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ignacio, C., Mooney, S. M., and Middleton, F. A. (2014). Effects of acute prenatal exposure to ethanol on microRNA expression are ameliorated by social enrichment. Front. Pediatr. 2:103. doi: 10.3389/fped.2014.00103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, L. M., Chiang, J. J., Vause, K., Hoffer, L., Alpert, K., Parrish, T. B., et al. (2020). Subcortical structural variations associated with low socioeconomic status in adolescents. Human Brain Mapp. 41, 162–171. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, J., Warton, C., Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., Molteno, C. D., Eicher, A., et al. (2014). Three-dimensional surface deformation-based shape analysis of hippocampus and caudate nucleus in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 659–672. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kajimoto, K., Valenzuela, C. F., Allan, A. M., Ge, S., Gu, Y., and Cunningham, L. A. (2016). Prenatal alcohol exposure alters synaptic activity of adult hippocampal dentate granule cells under conditions of enriched environment. Hippocampus 26, 1078–1087. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lantz, P. M., House, J. S., Mero, R. P., and Williams, D. R. (2005). Stress, life events, and socioeconomic disparities in health: results from the Americans’, Changing Lives Study. J. Health Soc. Behav. 46, 274–288. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jones, K. L., Adnams, C. M., May, P. A., et al. (2012). A longitudinal study of the long-term consequences of drinking during pregnancy: heavy in utero alcohol exposure disrupts the normal processes of brain development. J. Neurosci. 32, 15243–15251. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1161-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C. A., McMorris, C. A., Kar, P., Ritter, C., Andre, Q., Tortorelli, C., et al. (2019). Characterizing adverse prenatal and postnatal experiences in children. Birth Defects Res. 111, 848–858. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, G., and Beaulieu, C. (2019). Multivariate models of brain volume for identification of children and adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp 4, 1181–1194.

Google Scholar

Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., and McEwen, B. S. (2000). Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 976–980. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00965-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machlin, L., McLaughlin, K. A., and Sheridan, M. A. (2020). Brain structure mediates the association between socioeconomic status and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Sci. 23, e12844.

Google Scholar

Maier, S. E., Miller, J. A., Blackwell, J. M., and West, J. R. (1999). Fetal alcohol exposure and temporal vulnerability: regional differences in cell loss as a function of the timing of binge-like alcohol exposure during brain development. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 726–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04176.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Foroud, T., Sowell, E. R., Jones, K. L., Coles, C. D., Fagerlund, A., et al. (2010). Collaborative initiative on fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: methodology of clinical projects. Alcohol 44, 635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Goodman, A. M., Caine, C., Delis, D. C., and Riley, E. P. (1999). Executive functioning in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 1808–1815. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04077.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., and Riley, E. P. (1998). A review of the neurobehavioral deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome or prenatal exposure to alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 22, 279–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03651.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., and Riley, E. P. (1999). Implicit and explicit memory functioning in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 462–471. doi: 10.1017/s1355617799555082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Baete, A., Russo, J., Elliott, A. J., Blankenship, J., Kalberg, W. O., et al. (2014). Prevalence and characteristics of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 134, 855–866.

Google Scholar

May, P. A., Blankenship, J., Marais, A. S., Gossage, J. P., Kalberg, W. O., Joubert, B., et al. (2013). Maternal alcohol consumption producing fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD): quantity, frequency, and timing of drinking. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133, 502–512. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., de Vries, M. M., Marais, A. S., Kalberg, W. O., Adnams, C. M., Hasken, J. M., et al. (2016a). The continuum of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in four rural communities in South Africa: prevalence and characteristics. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 159, 207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.12.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Marais, A. S., de Vries, M. M., Kalberg, W. O., Buckley, D., Hasken, J. M., et al. (2016b). The continuum of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in a community in South Africa: Prevalence and characteristics in a fifth sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 168, 274–286. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.09.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., and Gossage, J. P. (2011). Maternal risk factors for fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: not as simple as it might seem. Alcohol. Res Health 34, 15–26.

Google Scholar

McCormack, C., Hutchinson, D., Burns, L., Wilson, J., Elliott, E., Allsop, S., et al. (2017). Prenatal alcohol consumption between conception and recognition of pregnancy. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 369–378. doi: 10.1111/acer.13305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, C. L., Seidlitz, J., Nadig, A., Liu, S., Clasen, L. S., Blumenthal, J. D., et al. (2019). Longitudinally mapping childhood socioeconomic status associations with cortical and subcortical morphology. J. Neurosci. 39, 1365–1373. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1808-18.2018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S., and Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1186, 190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morello-Frosch, R., and Shenassa, E. D. (2006). The environmental “riskscape” and social inequality: implications for explaining maternal and child health disparities. Environ. Health Perspect. 114, 1150–1153. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Engelhardt, L. E., Brito, N. H., Mack, L. J., Nail, E. J., Angal, J., et al. (2015). Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive development in the first two years of life. Dev. Psychobiol. 57, 535–551. doi: 10.1002/dev.21303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., and Sowell, E. R. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 15, 516–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., and Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 8, 74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfinder, M. (2014). Anthropometric and health-related behavioral factors in the explanation of social inequalities in low birth weight in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 11, 849–865. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110100849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfinder, M., Liebig, S., and Feldmann, R. (2012). Explanation of social inequalities in hyperactivity/inattention in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Klin Padiatr. 224, 303–308. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1314841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccolo, L. R., Merz, E. C., He, X., Sowell, E. R., Noble, K. G., and Pediatric Imaging, N. G. S. (2016). Age-related differences in cortical thickness vary by socioeconomic status. PLoS ONE 11:e0162511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 2016-2017).

Google Scholar

Raineki, C., Bodnar, T. S., Holman, P. J., Baglot, S. L., Lan, N., and Weinberg, J. (2017). Effects of early-life adversity on immune function are mediated by prenatal environment: Role of prenatal alcohol exposure. Brain Behav. Immun. 66, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2017.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raineki, C., Hellemans, K. G., Bodnar, T., Lavigne, K. M., Ellis, L., Woodward, T. S., et al. (2014). Neurocircuitry underlying stress and emotional regulation in animals prenatally exposed to alcohol and subjected to chronic mild stress in adulthood. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 5:5. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raineki, C., Morgan, E. J., Ellis, L., and Weinberg, J. (2019). Glucocorticoid receptor expression in the stress-limbic circuitry is differentially affected by prenatal alcohol exposure and adolescent stress. Brain Res. 1718, 242–251. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, C. (2005). Executive functioning and working memory in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 1359–1367. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000175040.91007.d0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker-Drob, E. M., and Bates, T. C. (2016). Large cross-national differences in gene x socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 27, 138–149. doi: 10.1177/0956797615612727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., and Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol. Sci. 14, 623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uban, K. A., Comeau, W. L., Bodnar, T., Yu, W. K., Weinberg, J., and Galea, L. A. (2015). Amphetamine sensitization and cross-sensitization with acute restraint stress: impact of prenatal alcohol exposure in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology 232, 1705–1716. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3804-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uban, K. A., Comeau, W. L., Ellis, L. A., Galea, L. A., and Weinberg, J. (2013). Basal regulation of HPA and dopamine systems is altered differentially in males and females by prenatal alcohol exposure and chronic variable stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1953–1966. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uban, K. A., Sliwowska, J. H., Lieblich, S., Ellis, L. A., Yu, W. K., Weinberg, J., et al. (2010). Prenatal alcohol exposure reduces the proportion of newly produced neurons and glia in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in female rats. Horm. Behav. 58, 835–843. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaurio, L., Riley, E. P., and Mattson, S. N. (2008). Differences in executive functioning in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 119–129. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ware, A. L., Infante, M. A., O’Brien, J. W., Tapert, S. F., Jones, K. L., Riley, E. P., et al. (2015). An fMRI study of behavioral response inhibition in adolescents with and without histories of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Behav. Brain Res. 278, 137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., Bassett, D. S., Stein, J. L., and Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2008). Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron 58, 273–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain volume, subcortical, brain development, adoption, collaborative initiative

Citation: Uban KA, Kan E, Wozniak JR, Mattson SN, Coles CD and Sowell ER (2020) The Relationship Between Socioeconomic Status and Brain Volume in Children and Adolescents With Prenatal Alcohol Exposure. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:85. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00085

Received: 17 September 2019; Accepted: 27 February 2020;
Published: 08 April 2020.

Edited by:

Aaron J. Newman, Dalhousie University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Todd L. Richards, University of Washington, United States
Christian Beaulieu, University of Alberta, Canada

Copyright © 2020 Uban, Kan, Wozniak, Mattson, Coles and Sowell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kristina A. Uban, kuban@uci.edu

Download