Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 05 December 2023
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 17 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1229055

Postural control deficits due to bilateral pyramidal tract lesions exemplified by hereditary spastic paraplegia (HSP) originate from increased feedback time delay and reduced long-term error corrections

  • 1Department of Neurology and Neurophysiology, Medical Faculty, University Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany

Pyramidal tract lesions determine the clinical syndrome of Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia (HSP). The clinical impairments of HSP are typically exemplified by their deficits in mobility, leading to falls and injuries. The first aim of this study was to identify the cause for postural abnormalities caused by pyramidal tract lesions in HSP. The second aim was to specify the effect of treadmill training for postural abnormalities. We examined nine HSP patients before and after treadmill training, as well as nine healthy control subjects during perturbed and unperturbed stance. We found that HSP was associated with larger sway amplitudes and velocities. Body excursions following platform tilts were larger, and upper body excursions showed a phase lead. Model-based analysis detected a greater time delay and a reduced long-term error correction of postural reactions in the center of mass. HSP patients performed significantly better in clinical assessments after treadmill training. In addition, treadmill training reduced sway amplitudes and body excursions, most likely by increasing positional and velocity error correction gain as a compensatory mechanism, while the time delay and long-term error correction gain remained largely unaffected. Moreover, the upper body’s phase lead was reduced. We conclude that HSP leads to very specific postural impairments. While postural control generally benefits from treadmill training, the effect seems to mainly rely on compensatory mechanisms, whereas the original deficits are not affected significantly.

1 Introduction

Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia (HSP) impairs full body posture leading often to falls and injuries (Nonnekes et al., 2017). The clinical presentation of HSP is mainly attributable to pyramidal tract lesions. Among different forms of the disease, mere lower limb spasticity and bladder disturbances are described as “pure” or “uncomplicated,” whereas they are classified as “complicated” when associated with other neurological signs (Salinas et al., 2008; Fink, 2013). Age of symptom onset, rate of progression, and degree of disability vary among the types of HSP, as well as within individual families with precisely the same gene mutation (Fink and Hedera, 1999; Fink, 2013).

The clinical assessment of pyramidal tract lesions is usually based on exaggerated tendon reflexes and muscle hypertonia in relaxed patients (Dietz and Sinkjaer, 2007). There is evidence, however, that these assessments correlate weakly with functional deficits of stance and gait (e.g., Burne et al., 2005; Dietz and Sinkjaer, 2007; in stroke patients, Nardone et al., 2001).

Neurophysiological correlates of pyramidal tract lesions have been described as impaired control of supraspinal drive and the use of afferent input, impaired reflex activity modulation, and abnormally prolonged motor conduction time (Jørgensen et al., 2005; Dietz and Sinkjaer, 2007). Secondary anomalies in muscle fibers might compensate for paresis and allow functional movements on a simpler organizational level (Dietz and Sinkjaer, 2007). However, the functional correlates of pyramidal tract lesions-induced postural impairments remain unknown.

Attempts to characterize pyramidal tract lesions from a more technical perspective include measuring joint torque increases or stretch reflex responses (Le Cavorzin et al., 2001; Alibiglou et al., 2008; Calota and Levin, 2009). Recent studies addressed the objective evaluation of gait in HSP patients (e.g., Bonnefoy-Mazure et al., 2013; Serrao et al., 2016). Again, the impact on postural impairments remains unclear.

Research on exercise therapies revealed that treadmill training improves gait parameters in patients with pyramidal tract lesions caused by various neurological diseases, i.e., cerebral palsy (Hodapp et al., 2009), stroke (Mehrholz et al., 2017) or spinal cord injury (Phadke et al., 2007). Pohl et al. (2002) advanced treadmill training by establishing a “Structured Speed-Dependent Treadmill Training” (STT). It proved to be superior to classic treadmill or conventional gait training to improve gait parameters, whereas effects on postural stability are still unknown.

Clinical assessments of postural control include the examination of stance and gait, usually in combination with absent or altered sensorimotor information (e.g., Romberg’s Test), but clinicians still have no standard means of measuring postural control.

Few studies have evaluated HSP patients’ postural control (Nardone et al., 2001; Nonnekes et al., 2013), and most evaluated pyramidal tract lesions in diseases affecting one limb or the limbs on one side, e.g., stroke-induced. We can assume that pyramidal tract lesions affecting both legs may lead to diverse study results as patients cannot rely on one healthy limb.

There is evidence that a perturbation-based approach enables the identification of sensorimotor parameters like response gain and phase, upper vs. lower body strategy, sensorimotor latencies, and sensory channels involved in mechanisms of postural control. It is based on the relationship between external perturbations of the body support surface and the human body’s reaction in space (Engelhart et al., 2014; Pasma et al., 2014; Wiesmeier et al., 2017).

In this pilot study we aimed to identify underlying mechanisms of postural deficits in “uncomplicated” HSP patients as a typical example for an impaired pyramidal tract taking a perturbation-based approach. We expected that HSP patients would differ in their postural-control mechanisms from healthy subjects. Moreover, we hypothesized that postural abnormalities would be well characterized by model-based analysis of externally perturbed stance. Finally, we hypothesized that HSP patients’ postural control is modulated by STT and that beneficial effects of treadmill training might not be confined to gait parameters, but also encompass postural stability.

2 Patients and methods

2.1 Experimental design and statistical analysis

Nine HSP patients (6 male, 3 female) with a mean age of 55.2 ± 5.3 years (± SD) participated in this pilot study. They suffered from “uncomplicated” HSP with late onset (mean age of onset 43.2 ± 8.6 years; ± SD) and a mean disease duration of 12 ± 6.4 years (± SD). Six patients had a known genetic defect leading to HSP (see Table 1). The other 3 patients were considered to suffer from a sporadic form or yet unknown mutation. All patients were able to walk with different walking aids. Table 1 summarizes their clinical information.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the HSP group.

The control group consisted of nine healthy subjects who did not differ significantly from the HSP patients in terms of age, body height, body mass and body mass index (BMI, p > 0.05).

All patients and subjects gave their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Freiburg (IRB # 256/01).

2.2 Clinical assessments

Patients were examined thoroughly by an experienced neurologist (BH), including tests with the Rydal-Seiffer tuning fork. We also assessed nerve conduction time and somatosensory evoked potentials in all but two patients who were within the normal range. Patients also completed standardized questionnaires and the testing of motor skills (see below) before and after treadmill training. None of the patients suffered from ataxia, peripheral neuropathy, or parkinsonism.

Our exclusion criteria comprised the features of complicated HSP: the degenerative process affects multiple parts of the nervous system and presents clinically with additional features like neuropathy, ataxia, cognitive impairment, seizures, optic atrophy, amyotrophy, and extrapyramidal involvement. We scored the following features in particular: mental retardation, dementia, psychosis, epilepsy, visual loss, cataract, gaze evoked nystagmus, dysarthria, dysphagia, limb ataxia, gait ataxia, extrapyramidal motor signs, muscle wasting UEX, muscle wasting LEX, loss of reflexes UEX (upper extremity), loss of reflexes LEX (lower extremity), impaired touch sense, impaired pinprick sensation, impaired vibration sense, impaired joint position sense, impaired temperature discrimination, facial dysmorphism, skin abnormalities, skeletal abnormalities, Botox in the last 3 months. We had to rule out one patient with additional peripheral neuropathy.

The severity of spastic paraplegia was quantified via the Spastic Paraplegia Rating Scale (SPRS, Schüle et al., 2006). This scale rates spasticity, muscle weakness, contractures and pain as well as typical activities of daily living prone to being affected by HSP. We also employed the Barthel index to assess restrictions of daily living activities (Mahoney and Barthel, 1965).

According to the modified Ashworth scale (Bohannon and Smith, 1987; Pandyan et al., 1999), spasticity was rated for hip, knee, upper ankle joint and toes and averaged for each subject.

Motor function was further assessed with the Tinetti Balance and Gait Test (Tinetti, 1986) which is commonly used in clinical practice and addresses gait and balance in two subscales.

Patients also performed the Timed Up-and-Go Test (TUG, Podsiadlo and Richardson, 1991) used to examine the time needed to rise from a chair, walk three meters straight, turn around, walk back and sit down. We also measured the maximum walking speed and maximum distance on the treadmill during 30 min walking at the second and the last training session.

2.3 Procedures

Details of the experimental procedures are similar to Wiesmeier et al. (2015).

Subjects stood (as naturally and comfortably as possible) upright on a custom-built motion platform, stance width was adapted to shoulder width (Figure 1A). For security reasons, all subjects held two handles in their hands that were attached to ropes hanging loosely from the ceiling. The handles and ropes enabled no spatial orientation (Buettner et al., 2017). Unperturbed stance was investigated on the stationary platform. Subjects performed six trials distributed into three repetitions, each with eyes open and with eyes closed. One trial consisted of 2 minutes of quiet standing with pauses for rest in between.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup and raw traces of unperturbed and perturbed stance. (A) Subject standing on the platform. (B–D) Exemplarily illustrate the raw traces of 2D center of pressure (COP)-movements during unperturbed stance of two representative subjects, one of the control group (B) and one of the HSP group before [pre; (C)] and after [post; (D)] treadmill training while standing with closed eyes. a-p, anterior-posterior, m-l, medio-lateral, HSP, Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia. (E–G) Show the pseudo random stimulus (G) and group averages for upper body [UB; (E)] and lower body [LB; (F)] angular excursions in degrees (deg) during perturbed stance. Shown are representative subjects of each the control group (black dotted line), and the HSP group before (blue line) and after training (red dotted line). The stimulus amplitude of platform rotations was one degree, subjects stood with eyes closed (EC). Note that the average time series differ mainly at maximum body excursions.

Perturbed stance was assessed by the subjects’ postural responses to transient tilts of the moving platform in the anterior-posterior direction (a-p, sagittal plane). Figure 1G shows the time course of the tilt stimulus, which relies on a pseudorandom ternary sequence of numbers (PRTS). One stimulus cycle lasted 20 s and was repeated six times (during each 2 min-trial). Peak-to-peak amplitude was either 0.5 or 1°. During the six trials (three repetitions per amplitude), subjects kept their eyes closed. The HSP group performed the platform experiments before and after 10 days of intensive treadmill training, whereas the control group underwent the platform experiments only once.

2.4 Data acquisition and analysis

During unperturbed stance, we recorded the two-dimensional center of pressure (COP) sway path with the help of a force transducing system (Kistler platform type 9286, Winterthur, Switzerland®). Figures 1B–D shows the COP sway path of one representative control subject and the same HSP patient before and after training. Root mean square (RMS; sway amplitude), Mean Velocity (MV; sway velocity) and 50%-frequency of body sway (F50; frequency content of sway) were extracted from COP measures using custom-made software programmed in MATLAB® (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). All values were calculated for single subjects and subsequently summarized in a group mean.

Furthermore, we measured the position of the body segments in space using an optoelectronic device with markers placed at the subjects’ shoulder and hip and on the platform (Optotrak® 3020, Waterloo, ON, Canada). Optotrak and Kistler outputs as well as stimulus signals were recorded with software programmed in LabView® (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA).

We obtained upper body (UB) and lower body (LB) excursions in space from 3-D translational and angular positions of each marker. Stimulus-response data on perturbed stance was calculated using a discrete Fourier transformation (DFT) in MATLAB. From Fourier coefficients of stimulus and response time series, we calculated transfer functions, i.e., gain, phase and coherence values across five cycles in the stimulus presentation. The first cycle was discarded due to transient posture adjustments before reaching a steady state (Figures 1E, F). Gain values relate the body’s response (postural reaction; upper and lower body angle) to the stimulus (external perturbation; platform angle). For example, a gain value more than one indicates amplified external perturbation. Phase is the relative delay between the stimulus and body’s reaction. For example, phase advance is indicated by more positive phase values. Coherence helps to estimate the linear relationship between stimulus and response. A coherence value of zero implies no linear relationship, whereas a coherence value of one implies a perfect linear relationship. Gain, phase and coherence depend on frequency (see Peterka, 2002).

2.5 Parameter identification technique

Transfer functions were implemented into an established biomechanical model of human stance (e.g., Maurer and Peterka, 2005; Engelhart et al., 2014). The human body is modeled as an inverted pendulum with the center of rotation at the ankle joint. Deviations from an upright position are fed back into the model via vestibular and proprioceptive sensors. A weighting mechanism determines the individual importance of the information from the proprioceptive system (Wp). A time delay (Td) takes into account the human sensory-motor-conduction and central processing time. The inverted pendulum is stabilized via a PID controller mimicking active stiffness (Kp), damping (Kd), and integral properties (Ki) of the body. Moreover, passive stiffness (Ppas) and passive damping (Dpas) are included in the model. Using a MATLAB-based script, the model’s settings are optimized with the “fmincon” function until the mean standard error (mse) between measured and modeled transfer functions is minimal.

2.6 Treadmill training

Our study’s HSP group performed a “Structured Speed-Dependent Treadmill Training” (STT) according to Pohl et al. (2002) lasting 30 min every day over a 2-week period. Adapted to each patient’s walking ability, 5 patients received 20% body weight support (BWS) during treadmill training. Four patients with BWS needed intermittent help with foot placement too.

2.7 Statistical analyses

We performed statistical analyses with Microsoft Excel and JMP® (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). First, we tested the normal distribution with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Statistical significance between HSP patients before treadmill training and the healthy control group was then examined applying analyses of variance (ANOVA) with the between-subjects variable “group” (HSP patients, control subjects). For unperturbed stance, we chose visual condition (eyes open, eyes closed) and sway direction (mediolateral, anteroposterior) as within-subjects variables. For externally perturbed stance we applied stimulus amplitude (0.5 and 1°) and body segment (hip, shoulder) as within-subjects variables. We also tested the effect of treadmill training on postural control of HSP patients via multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) with “time” as the repeated measure variable. Statistical significance was assumed at p ≤ 0.05. To estimate the effect of treadmill training on clinical assessments, we adjusted the significance levels for multiple comparisons.

3 Results

3.1 Clinical assessments

Table 2 shows an overview of our clinical assessment findings. Prior to treadmill training (pre) the average Spastic Paraplegia Rating Scale (SPRS) amounted to 21.1 ± 8.2 (mean ± SD; 52 points = maximal impairment), after treadmill training (post) the SPRS was significantly reduced (17.8 ± 7.1; F = 7.6, p = 0.03). Treadmill training significantly reduced the average Ashworth scale score of 1.8 ± 0.9 before training to 1.5 ± 0.8 (both “slight increase in muscle tone”; F = 10.3, p = 0.01). Treadmill training also significantly increased the maximum walking distance (pre 1104 ± 279 m, post 1669 ± 665 m; F = 6.3, p = 0.04) and maximum walking speed (pre 2.7 ± 0.8 m/s, post 3.8 ± 1.3 m/s, F = 15.6, p = 0.006), whereas it only slightly reduced the time needed to do the Timed Up and Go Test (TUG, pre 21.7 ± 12.8 s, post 19.4 + /10.7 s; F = 0.5, p = 0.5). Moreover, it ameliorated slightly the performance of patients in the Tinetti Balance and Gait Test, thereby lowering the risk of falls (total score pre 17.3 ± 5.3, “high risk for falls,” post 19.8 ± 4.5, “risk for falls”; F = 4.8, p = 0.06; Table 2). Seven of the nine patients scored 100 in the Barthel index, one 95 and one 90. At the end of treadmill training, the number of patients needing body weight support (BWS) was reduced from five to two. All patients were able to walk independently; help with foot placement was no longer necessary.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clinical assessments.

3.2 Unperturbed stance

Results of unperturbed stance are presented for the center of pressure (COP; Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Parameters of unperturbed stance (mean ± 95% confidence interval). Root Mean Square (RMS), Mean Velocity (MV) and 50%-frequency of body sway (F50) of the control group (hCon) and the HSP group before (pre) and after (post) treadmill training with eyes closed (ec) and eyes open (eo) in anterior-posterior and medio-lateral sway direction. Hz, Hertz.

Sway amplitude was quantified by calculating Root mean square (RMS). RMS of the HSP group was significantly larger compared to the control group (hCon; HSP 0.61 cm, hCon 0.40 cm, F = 20.7, p < 0.0001). Likewise, Mean Velocity (sway velocity, MV) was significantly larger in the HSP than in the control group (HSP 0.94 cm/s, hCon 0.67 cm/s, F = 16.5, p = 0.0001). Sway amplitude and velocity depended on visual condition with larger and faster sway upon eye closure (RMS: F = 7.0, p = 0.01; MV: F = 23.2, p < 0.0001). Group differences were significantly more pronounced during trials with eyes closed (RMS: F = 4.1, p = 0.048; MV: F = 4.2, p = 0.04). Sway amplitude and velocity were also more pronounced in anterior-posterior (a-p) than in medio-lateral (m-l) direction (RMS: F = 29.1, p < 0.0001; MV: F = 41.6, p < 0.0001). We observed no interaction between groups or sway directions.

Two weeks of daily treadmill training reduced the HSP group’s sway amplitudes significantly (RMS: pre 0.63 cm, post 0.55 cm, F = 8.3, p = 0.008). Patients still failed to achieve the values of healthy controls. In contrast, MV did not significantly change due to training (MV: pre 0.97 m/s, post 0.90 m/s, F = 3.1, p = 0.09). Treadmill training did not significantly affect visual conditions or directions of sway.

3.3 Externally perturbed stance

Our HSP group’s GAIN values were significantly higher than the control group’s (HSP 3.0, hCon 2.4, F = 24.4, p < 0.0001). We detected a significant interaction between group and body segments, with more pronounced group differences at the upper body (F = 19.2, p < 0.0001; Figure 3). In contrast, stimulus amplitudes did not interact significantly with group. GAIN as function of stimulus frequency rose up to 0.2–0.4 Hz and fell rapidly at higher frequencies. At approximately 1 Hz, body angle and stimulus amplitude were equal (GAIN = 1). Treadmill training significantly reduced GAIN (pre 2.94, post 2.74, F = 4.3, p = 0.04) without reaching the values of control subjects. Treadmill training did not significantly influence GAIN with respect to body segments, frequencies or stimulus amplitudes (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Parameters of perturbed stance. Group averages for GAIN, PHASE, and COHERENCE (mean ± 95% confidence interval) of the lower (LB) and upper (UB) body across both stimulus amplitudes (0.5°, 1°) and visual conditions (eyes open, eyes closed). The groups shown are the control group (hCon), and the HSP group before (pre) and after (post) treadmill training. F, frequency; Hz, Hertz.

We observed significant group differences between PHASE of the HSP and control group (HSP −96.1°, hCon −121.8°, F = 11.2, p < 0.001). As with GAIN, differences were more pronounced at the upper body with a significant interaction between groups and body segments (F = 22.4, p < 0.0001; Figure 3). Group also interacted significantly with stimulus amplitudes (F = 12.4, p < 0.001) which is due to the fact that the main group differences are observed in trials with 0.5° peak-to-peak amplitude. After 2 weeks of treadmill training, The HSP group’s PHASE values approached the control group’s values significantly (pre −91°, post −130°, F = 19.7, p < 0.0001). The training effect interacted significantly with the body segments (F = 10.1, p = 0.002), and stimulus amplitudes (F = 11.2, p = 0.0009; Figure 3). PHASE lag of the upper body increased from −78.8° to −130.8°, whereas the lower body’s PHASE lag increased from −103.3 to −114.6°.

The HSP group’s COHERENCE was significantly smaller than the control group’s (HSP 0.36, hCon 0.46, F = 109.0, p < 0.0001) reflecting the smaller signal-to-noise ratio due to the former’s larger unperturbed stance. Moreover, we found a significant interaction of group with body segment (F = 18.4, p < 0.0001) displaying larger COHERENCE differences between groups at the upper body. There was no interaction between groups and stimulus amplitudes. Treadmill training significantly increased the HSP group’s COHERENCE (pre 0.36, post 0.43, F = 119.0, p < 0.0001). It significantly affected the COHERENCE of hip and shoulder (body segments, F = 9.4, p = 0.002), but did not change the reaction to different stimulus amplitudes (F = 0.0, p = 1.0).

GAIN, PHASE, and COHERENCE were frequency-dependent in both the HSP and control group. However, we found no significant interaction between group and frequency.

3.4 Model-based analysis

The integral gain (Ki) of the HSP group was significantly smaller than the control group’s Ki (HSP 59.8, hCon 70.7, F = 8.03, p = 0.008). Time delay (Td) was significantly larger in the HSP group (0.21 s, hCon 0.18 s; F = 12.9, p = 0.001). The proportional (Kp) and derivative (Kd) gain, the proprioceptive weight (Wp) and passive gains Kpas and Bpas did not differ significantly between the HSP and control group (Kp: F = 0.9, p = 0.35; Kd: F = 3.0, p = 0.09; Wp: F = 0.002, p = 0.96, Kpas: F = 2.5, p = 0.13; Bpas: F = 2.6, p = 0.12).

Treadmill training seemed to have no significant effect on the model parameters (Ki: F = 0.05, p = 0.8; Kp: F = 0.4, p = 0.6; Kd: F = 0.001, p = 0.9; Wp: F = 1.0, p = 0.3; Td: F = 0.07, p = 0.8; Kpas: F = 0.3, p = 0.6; Bpas: F = 0.5, p = 0.5; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Parameters obtained from the model-based analysis. Kp (Nm ⋅ rad-1), Kd (Nm ⋅ s ⋅ rad-1), Ki (Nm ⋅ s-1 ⋅ rad-1), Wp, Td (seconds), Kpas, Bpas of the control group (hCon), and the HSP group before (pre) and after (post) treadmill training across all stimulus amplitudes with eyes closed.

4 Discussion

In this study we compared the postural control mechanisms of patients with Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia (HSP) to a healthy control group taking a perturbation-based approach. Through a model-based analysis we aimed to identify functional correlates of postural impairments caused by pyramidal tract lesions. We also evaluated the effect of a “Structured Speed-Dependent Treadmill Training” (STT) protocol (on postural control).

Our ratings of SPRS and Ashworth scale indicate disease severities resembling those reported in studies by Schüle et al. (2006) and Martinuzzi et al. (2016) (SPRS 23.37 ± 10.7, and 20.0 ± 10.2, respectively; Ashworth Scale 2.04 ± 0.98). The values of the Timed Up and Go Test (TUG) in our study resemble the values of Heetla et al. (2015) who describe in an HSP patient’s case report that a continuous intrathecal Baclofen infusion reduced TUG values from 32 to 17 s. In our study, TUG values did not differ significantly, but maximum walking distance and speed improved significantly after treadmill training.

Prior to treadmill training, our HSP group’s body sway in terms of sway amplitude (RMS) and sway velocity (MV) was significantly greater than our control group’s. Our finding of increased RMS and MV stand in contrast to results of Nardone et al. (2001) who described a normal sway area in paraparetic patients. This discrepancy might be explained by differences in patient groups between the study of Nardone et al., and ours, as Nardone et al. (2001) included paraparetic patients with different diseases (HSP, idiopathic spastic paraparesis, sequela of resected meningioma). Large RMS and MV may be explained by an abnormally large sensorimotor feedback time delay and reduced long-term correction gain as explained in more detail below (see also Collins et al., 1995; Maurer and Peterka, 2005). Treadmill training induced less sway amplitude.

Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia patient’s postural adjustments, especially in the upper body after small platform tilts are more advanced in a timely manner than those of healthy control subjects. In addition, HSP patients react with larger body reactions and more random sway movements to platform tilts. Altogether, these findings might be interpreted as an affectation of intersegmental coordination.

It is well known that HSP mainly affects the largest and fastest conducting motor axons (e.g., McDermott et al., 2000; Martinuzzi et al., 2016). Accordingly, nearly all patients reveal increased motor conduction time (Jørgensen et al., 2005; Sartucci et al., 2007). With the help of our model-based analysis, we identified an increased time delay (Td) and reduced long-term error correction (Ki) in postural reactions of the center of mass in HSP patients. They may represent additional functional correlates of impairments due to pyramidal tract lesions. Treadmill training did not influence time delay, or long-term error correction of the center of mass. We suppose that the increased time delay is a functional correlate of the increased motor conduction time and thus may not be modifiable by any intervention. However, the changes of GAIN and PHASE values toward a normal range indicate that HSP patients learn to compensate for their functional deficits by adjusting parameters that are changeable. Moreover, the upper body’s phase lead was reduced.

Until now, the benefit of physical exercises such as treadmill training for patients suffering from HSP and other forms of pyramidal tract lesions seemed to be unclear. Adams and Hicks (2011) reported a beneficial effect of treadmill training on spasticity in patients with chronic spinal cord injury. In addition, robotic gait training significantly improved clinical assessments of gait and balance as well as quality of life in HSP patients, even after a 2-month follow-up period (Bertolucci et al., 2015). On the other hand, systematic reviews decry the lack of high-quality trials addressing the effect of physical exercise on spasticity in different neurological diseases (Ashworth et al., 2012; Khan et al., 2019). Mehrholz et al. (2017) found no evidence for a beneficial effect of treadmill training in stroke patients in their overall ability to walk. However, walking speed and endurance seemed to improve temporarily, especially in people able to walk (Mehrholz et al., 2017).

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that HSP clearly damages postural stability, specifically motor reactions that are more fragile and delayed. The long-term error correction of movements is also impaired. While treadmill training did not modify increased time delay and reduced long-term error correction gain of the center of mass, probably because of the disease’s unaltered anatomical correlate, i.e., pyramidal tract lesions, the specific profile of training benefits indicates that HSP patients’ postural control was ameliorated by compensatory mechanisms. We hold that our model-based analysis of sensorimotor behavior is capable of differentiating between functional correlates of a disease’s anatomical substrate and the parameter changes due to therapeutic interventions.

Study limitations are our small sample size and the lack of a control intervention. Hence, we cannot rule out that the beneficial effects on postural impairments are attributable to a regular and supervised exercise protocol rather than treadmill training per se. In addition, we did not assess the long-term effects of treadmill training. Further research with larger cohorts and follow-up examinations is needed.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Freiburg (IRB # 256/01). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DD, BH, CW, and CM contributed to conception and design of the study. DD organized data acquisition and analysis. DD and CM wrote the first draft. IW and CM contributed to data analysis and further draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was partially supported by the European Union FP7 (EMBALANCE: Grant Agreement no 610454) and H2020-SC1–2017-CNECT-1 (HOLOBALANCE: Project ID 769574; HOLOgrams for personalized virtual coaching and motivation in an ageing population with BALANCE disorders) and by the Brainlinks-Braintools Cluster of Excellence funded by the German Research foundation (DFG, grant no ADV139). We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Freiburg.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Maike Hodapp for her support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adams, M. M., and Hicks, A. L. (2011). Comparison of the effects of body-weight-supported treadmill training and tilt-table standing on spasticity in individuals with chronic spinal cord injury. J. Spinal Cord Med. 34, 488–494. doi: 10.1179/2045772311Y.0000000028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alibiglou, L., Rymer, W. Z., Harvey, R. L., and Mirbagheri, M. M. (2008). The relation between Ashworth scores and neuromechanical measurements of spasticity following stroke. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 5:18. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-5-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashworth, N. L., Satkunam, L. E., and Deforge, D. (2012). Treatment for spasticity in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis/motor neuron disease. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2012, CD004156. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004156.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertolucci, F., Di Martino, S., Orsucci, D., Ienco, E. C., Siciliano, G., Rossi, B., et al. (2015). Robotic gait training improves motor skills and quality of life in hereditary spastic paraplegia. NeuroRehabilitation 36, 93–99. doi: 10.3233/NRE-141196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohannon, R. W., and Smith, M. B. (1987). Interrater reliability of a modified Ashworth scale of muscle spasticity. Phys. Ther. 67, 206–207. doi: 10.1093/ptj/67.2.206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnefoy-Mazure, A., Turcot, K., Kaelin, A., De Coulon, G., and Armand, S. (2013). Full body gait analysis may improve diagnostic discrimination between hereditary spastic paraplegia and spastic diplegia: a preliminary study. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 495–504. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buettner, D., Dalin, D., Wiesmeier, I. K., and Maurer, C. (2017). Virtual balancing for studying and training postural control. Front. Neurosci. 11:531. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burne, J. A., Carleton, V. L., and O’Dwyer, N. J. (2005). The spasticity paradox: Movement disorder or disorder of resting limbs? J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 47–54. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.034785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calota, A., and Levin, M. F. (2009). Tonic stretch reflex threshold as a measure of spasticity: Implications for clinical practice. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 16, 177–188. doi: 10.1310/tsr1603-177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, J. J., De Luca, C. J., Burrows, A., and Lipsitz, L. A. (1995). Age-related changes in open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms. Exp Brain Res 104, 480–492. doi: 10.1007/BF00231982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, V., and Sinkjaer, T. (2007). Spastic movement disorder: impaired reflex function and altered muscle mechanics. Lancet Neurol. 6, 725–733. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70193-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelhart, D., Pasma, J. H., Schouten, A. C., Meskers, C. G. M., Maier, A. B., Mergner, T., et al. (2014). Impaired standing balance in elderly: a new engineering method helps to unravel causes and effects. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 15, e1–e6. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2013.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, J. K. (2013). Hereditary spastic paraplegia: clinico-pathologic features and emerging molecular mechanisms. Acta Neuropathol. 126, 307–328. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1115-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, J. K., and Hedera, P. (1999). Hereditary spastic paraplegia: genetic heterogeneity and genotype-phenotype correlation. Semin. Neurol. 19, 301–309. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1040846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heetla, H. W., Halbertsma, J. P., Dekker, R., Staal, M. J., and van Laar, T. (2015). Improved gait performance in a patient with hereditary spastic paraplegia after a continuous intrathecal baclofen test infusion and subsequent pump implantation: a case report. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 96, 1166–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2015.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodapp, M., Vry, J., Mall, V., and Faist, M. (2009). Changes in soleus H-reflex modulation after treadmill training in children with cerebral palsy. Brain J. Neurol. 132, 37–44. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jørgensen, L. M., Nielsen, J. E., and Ravnborg, M. (2005). MEP recruitment curves in multiple sclerosis and hereditary spastic paraplegia. J. Neurol. Sci. 237, 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, F., Amatya, B., Bensmail, D., and Yelnik, A. (2019). Non-pharmacological interventions for spasticity in adults: An overview of systematic reviews. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 62, 265–273. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Cavorzin, P., Poudens, S. A., Chagneau, F., Carrault, G., Allain, H., and Rochcongar, P. (2001). A comprehensive model of spastic hypertonia derived from the pendulum test of the leg. Muscle Nerve 24, 1612–1621. doi: 10.1002/mus.1196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahoney, F., and Barthel, D. (1965). Functional evaluation: The Barthel Index. Md. State Med. J. 14, 61–65. doi: 10.1037/t02366-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinuzzi, A., Montanaro, D., Vavla, M., Paparella, G., Bonanni, P., Musumeci, O., et al. (2016). Clinical and paraclinical indicators of motor system impairment in hereditary spastic paraplegia: A pilot study. PLoS One 11:e0153283. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, C., and Peterka, R. J. (2005). A new interpretation of spontaneous sway measures based on a simple model of human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 189–200. doi: 10.1152/jn.00221.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, C., White, K., Bushby, K., and Shaw, P. (2000). Hereditary spastic paraparesis: a review of new developments. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 69, 150–160. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.69.2.150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehrholz, J., Pohl, M., and Elsner, B. (2017). Treadmill training and body weight support for walking after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 8:CD002840. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002840.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nardone, A., Galante, M., Lucas, B., and Schieppati, M. (2001). Stance control is not affected by paresis and reflex hyperexcitability: the case of spastic patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 70, 635–643. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.5.635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nonnekes, J., de Niet, M., Oude Nijhuis, L. B., de Bot, S. T., van de Warrenburg, B. P. C., Bloem, B. R., et al. (2013). Mechanisms of postural instability in hereditary spastic paraplegia. J. Neurol. 260, 2387–2395. doi: 10.1007/s00415-013-7002-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nonnekes, J., van Lith, B., van de Warrenburg, B. P., Weerdesteyn, V., and Geurts, A. C. H. (2017). Pathophysiology, diagnostic work-up and management of balance impairments and falls in patients with hereditary spastic paraplegia. J. Rehabil. Med. 49, 369–377. doi: 10.2340/16501977-2227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandyan, A. D., Johnson, G. R., Price, C. I., Curless, R. H., Barnes, M. P., and Rodgers, H. (1999). A review of the properties and limitations of the Ashworth and modified Ashworth Scales as measures of spasticity. Clin. Rehabil. 13, 373–383. doi: 10.1191/026921599677595404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasma, J. H., Engelhart, D., Schouten, A. C., van der Kooij, H., Maier, A. B., and Meskers, C. G. M. (2014). Impaired standing balance: the clinical need for closing the loop. Neuroscience 267, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterka, R. J. (2002). Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1097–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phadke, C. P., Wu, S. S., Thompson, F. J., and Behrman, A. L. (2007). Comparison of soleus H-reflex modulation after incomplete spinal cord injury in 2 walking environments: treadmill with body weight support and overground. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 1606–1613. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsiadlo, D., and Richardson, S. (1991). The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 39, 142–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohl, M., Mehrholz, J., Ritschel, C., and Rückriem, S. (2002). Speed-dependent treadmill training in ambulatory hemiparetic stroke patients: A randomized controlled trial. Stroke 33, 553–558. doi: 10.1161/hs0202.102365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salinas, S., Proukakis, C., Crosby, A., and Warner, T. (2008). Hereditary spastic paraplegia: Clinical features and pathogenetic mechanisms. Lancet Neurol. 7, 1127–1138. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70258-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sartucci, F., Tovani, S., Murri, L., and Sagliocco, L. (2007). Motor and somatosensory evoked potentials in Autosomal Dominant Hereditary Spastic Paraparesis (ADHSP) linked to chromosome 2p. SPG4. Brain Res. Bull. 74, 243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schüle, R., Holland-Letz, T., Klimpe, S., Kassubek, J., Klopstock, T., Mall, V., et al. (2006). The Spastic Paraplegia Rating Scale (SPRS): A reliable and valid measure of disease severity. Neurology 67, 430–434. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228242.53336.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrao, M., Rinaldi, M., Ranavolo, A., Lacquaniti, F., Martino, G., Leonardi, L., et al. (2016). Gait patterns in patients with hereditary spastic paraparesis. PLoS One 11:e0164623. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinetti, M. E. (1986). Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly patients. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 34, 119–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1986.tb05480.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiesmeier, I. K., Dalin, D., and Maurer, C. (2015). Elderly use proprioception rather than visual and vestibular cues for postural motor control. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:97. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiesmeier, I. K., Dalin, D., Wehrle, A., Granacher, U., Muehlbauer, T., Dietterle, J., et al. (2017). Balance training enhances vestibular function and reduces overactive proprioceptive feedback in elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:273. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: postural control, pyramidal tract, sensorimotor system, hereditary spastic paraplegia, model, exercise

Citation: Dalin D, Wiesmeier IK, Heimbach B, Weiller C and Maurer C (2023) Postural control deficits due to bilateral pyramidal tract lesions exemplified by hereditary spastic paraplegia (HSP) originate from increased feedback time delay and reduced long-term error corrections. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1229055. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1229055

Received: 25 May 2023; Accepted: 13 November 2023;
Published: 05 December 2023.

Edited by:

Jia Han, Shanghai University of Medicine and Health Sciences, China

Reviewed by:

Alice Barbara Schindler, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NIH), United States
Marcondes C. França Jr, State University of Campinas, Brazil

Copyright © 2023 Dalin, Wiesmeier, Heimbach, Weiller and Maurer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christoph Maurer, christoph.maurer@uniklinik-freiburg.de

Download