Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 26 March 2020
Sec. Neural Technology
This article is part of the Research Topic Closed-loop Interfaces for Neuroelectronic Devices and Assistive Robots View all 10 articles

Mechanotactile Sensory Feedback Improves Embodiment of a Prosthetic Hand During Active Use

  • 1Division of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
  • 2Glenrose Rehabilitation Hospital, Alberta Health Services, Edmonton, AB, Canada

There have been several advancements in the field of myoelectric prostheses to improve dexterity and restore hand grasp patterns for persons with upper limb loss, including robust control strategies, novel sensory feedback, and multifunction prosthetic terminal devices. Although these advancements have shown to improve prosthesis performance, a key element that may further improve acceptance is often overlooked. Embodiment, which encompasses the feeling of owning, controlling and locating the device without the need to constantly look at it, has been shown to be affected by sensory feedback. However, the specific aspects of embodiment that are influenced are not clearly understood, particularly when a prosthesis is actively controlled. In this work, we used a sensorized simulated prosthesis in able-bodied participants to investigate the contribution of sensory feedback, active motor control, and the combination of both to the components of embodiment; using a common methodology in the literature, namely the rubber hand illusion (RHI). Our results indicate that (1) the sensorized simulated prosthesis may be embodied by able-bodied users in a similar fashion as prosthetic devices embodied by persons with upper limb amputation, and (2) mechanotactile sensory feedback might not only be useful for improving certain aspects of embodiment, i.e., ownership and location, but also may have a modulating effect on other aspects, namely sense of agency, when provided asynchronously during active motor control tasks. This work may allow us to further investigate and manipulate factors contributing to the complex phenomenon of embodiment in relation to active motor control of a device, enabling future study of more precise quantitative measures of embodiment that do not rely as much on subjective perception.

Introduction

Persons with upper limb amputation face significant limitations in performing activities of daily living. Myoelectric prosthesis, controlled by electrical signals extracted from residual limb muscles, provide a potentially feasible solution (Belter et al., 2013; van der Riet et al., 2013; Geethanjali, 2016), however, dissatisfaction and rejection of the prosthesis remains high, with some studies reporting up to 75% of myoelectric prosthesis users abandoning their device (Biddiss and Chau, 2007). Although there have been several advancements to improve dexterity and restore hand grasp patterns (Gallagher, 1986; Murray, 2004; Giummarra et al., 2008), myoelectric prostheses do not provide continuous feedback to allow real-time regulation of muscle contraction. The lack of feedback poses a significant challenge to the prosthesis user; without such sensory feedback, the prosthesis needs near-constant visual attention and mental concentration to operate (Sobuh et al., 2014; Hebert et al., 2019).

Sensory feedback has thus been highlighted as a possible missing element for improving the acceptance of upper limb prosthetic devices (Ehrsson et al., 2004; Longo et al., 2008). One hypothesis is that sensory feedback will restore the feeling of ownership of the prosthesis as part of the body, by facilitating integration of the prosthesis into the body representation (Gallagher, 1986; Murray, 2004). Although ownership can be induced by providing sensory input matched to natural sensation, i.e., pressure proportionally matching touch sensation, in an expected location and orientation (Giummarra et al., 2008), embodiment is likely a more complex phenomenon. Embodiment is thought to involve sub-components of ownership (the feeling that the hand is actually a part of the body), location (the sensation that the hand is in an appropriate area and that a relationship exists between what is seen in that area and where it is felt on the hand) and agency (a feeling of control over the actions of the hand) (Ehrsson et al., 2004; Longo et al., 2008). These items interrelate and may result in a foreign object, such as the prosthetic hand, being integrated into the body schema (Gallagher and Cole, 1995), which may increase acceptance and use of the prosthesis.

Prior research has used an experimental paradigm called the rubber hand illusion (RHI) to elicit the sense of embodiment by applying synchronous stimulation to a rubber hand and the participant’s hand, demonstrating that the sense of body ownership is closely associated with cutaneous touch (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998; Ehrsson et al., 2004; Tsakiris and Haggard, 2005; Longo et al., 2008). The RHI is a robust phenomenon and appears to be sensitive to the relative strength of the tactile input (Ehrsson et al., 2008). Tactile input has been shown to induce this illusion even if it is modality mismatched (D’Alonzo and Cipriani, 2012), however, the input needs to be delivered synchronously in order to preserve the illusion (Armel and Ramachandran, 2003; Ehrsson et al., 2004; D’Alonzo and Cipriani, 2012). In persons with upper limb amputation who have undergone the targeted reinnervation procedure, the provision of direct cutaneous touch feedback to the residual limb has been reported to create a vivid illusion of ownership of a passive prosthetic hand (Marasco et al., 2011), similar to other populations with upper limb amputation when synchronous touches were applied to their residual limb and a rubber hand (Ehrsson et al., 2008) or a robotic hand (Rosen et al., 2009).

Adding active control of the hand has been shown to enhance the experience of the RHI; both able-bodied and participants with amputation have been shown to experience a sense of ownership over the robotic hand when they were remotely controlling the robotic movements (Rosen et al., 2009; Sato et al., 2018). In fact, active motor control of a congruent movement was shown to induce both ownership and agency, without a significant effect of additional brushing feedback (Sato et al., 2018). Studies in participants with amputation have further shown that embodiment responses can be positive with motor control alone as well as with sensory feedback provided by peripheral intraneural stimulation (Graczyk et al., 2018; Page et al., 2018), and that the naturalness of the tactile sensation elicited by nerve stimulation may affect embodiment responses (Valle et al., 2018). Furthermore, in other participants with wearable closed-loop control prosthetic systems, it has been shown that kinesthetic feedback enhances agency but not ownership (Marasco et al., 2018).

There is, therefore, building evidence that multisensory inputs of both sensation and motoric cues can enhance the sense of ownership (Kalckert and Ehrsson, 2014). However, there remains some inconsistency in the literature regarding the relative contribution of active motor control with or without concordant sensory stimulation to agency and ownership. The ability to further investigate these factors is limited by the small number of participants with bidirectional sensory feedback systems and the limitations of altering their sensory feedback parameters to explore the impact of feedback type.

A common technique used to study myoelectric prosthesis function is the use of simulated devices on able-bodied research participants as an approximation to prostheses used by persons with upper-limb amputation (Panarese et al., 2009; Amsuess et al., 2016; Johansen et al., 2016; Clemente et al., 2017; White et al., 2017). We designed such a device to allow the manipulation of sensory feedback during motor control (Kuus et al., 2017), to investigate the factors of ownership, location, and agency in a wearable prosthesis. The objective of the current work was to assess the embodiment responses of participants using a wearable simulated prosthesis platform providing mechanotactile feedback during active motor control. In order to ascertain the validity of this approach in comparison to the classic RHI, we first had to determine the influence of type of feedback (mechanotactile tapping versus brushing) in a passive condition with the worn prosthesis simulator (the “passive prosthesis test”), followed by investigation of the contributions of active motor control to the embodiment phenomenon (the “active prosthesis test”).

Materials and Methods

In this study, we used a simulated prosthesis that allows the integration of sensors and mechanotactile feedback tactors (Kuus et al., 2017). The study was divided into two test phases – passive prosthesis test and active prosthesis test.

Study Participants

Twenty-one able-bodied individuals were recruited to participate in this study [12 females; age: 31.9 ± 9.3 (mean ± SD); 3 left-hand dominant]. All participants were over 18 years old with no upper limb dysfunction (no muscular or neurological dysfunction, no sensory deficit in the hand), normal or corrected to normal vision, and no upper limb surgery in the past year. Only 1 participant had previous experience operating a simulated prosthesis. Written informed consent according to the University of Alberta Health Research Ethics Board was obtained from all participants before conducting the experiment (Pro00057340).

Experimental Setup

Participants sat comfortably on a chair in front of a table and facing the experimenter. The height of the chair was adjusted to ensure that the participant’s arm was resting on the table. A black sheet was placed over the participant’s shoulder to ensure that their arm was completely obscured. Noise-canceling headphones were placed over the participant’s ears during testing but were removed during instruction phases and setup.

Device Setup and Control Parameters

The participant’s right arm was secured in an adjustable brace that comfortably restricted wrist movement. A prosthetic hand was secured to the brace such that it was oriented beneath the participant’s right hand, similar to the specifications outlined by Kuus et al. (2017) with adjustments of the strapping to allow access to the participant’s hand. The prosthetic hand was controlled using two-cite proportional myoelectric control (Battye et al., 1955) with one of the electromyography (EMG) sensors placed on the wrist extensor muscle and the other placed on the wrist flexor muscle. Muscle contractions at these sites were mapped to the velocity of the opening and closing of the single degree of freedom prosthetic hand. EMG sensor gains were adjusted to ensure easy and reliable control of the prosthetic hand. The participant was free to move around with the brace attached during training to use the device, but the testing occurred in a seated position resting the device on the table.

Tactor Setup

Three mechanotactile tactors integrated into the brace were aligned to stimulate the thumb, index, and middle fingers of the participant to relay tactile feedback to participants. These tactors pushed on the participant’s fingers by converting rotational motion from a motor using rack and pinion gears to linear motion (Figure 6a in Schoepp et al., 2018). The linear displacement of these actuators on the fingertips was mapped proportionally to the force sensed using force-sensitive resistor sensors that were placed on the corresponding thumb, index, and middle fingers of the prosthetic hand (Saunders and Vijayakumar, 2011). This system had an average delay in response of 92 ± 16 ms, which is below the recommended threshold to evoke embodiment (Ismail and Shimada, 2016).

Experimental Protocol

In the first portion of this study, we investigated the effect of receiving two types of feedback (brushing and tapping) with and without delay (asynchronous and synchronous, respectively) on the sense of embodiment of the prosthetic hand while wearing the device during a Passive Prosthesis Test. We then investigated the effect of providing no feedback, synchronous tapping feedback, or asynchronous (delayed) tapping feedback during an Active Prosthesis Test. After each test and for each feedback condition, participants were asked to perform an assessment of proprioceptive drift followed by filling out a questionnaire. An overview of the experimental protocol is provided in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of experimental protocol. (A) A representative randomization sequence for the Passive Prosthesis Test. There were 4 feedback conditions tested: Synchronous Brushing (SB), Asynchronous Brushing (AB), Synchronous Tapping (ST), and Asynchronous Tapping (AT). A single trial block consisted of the experimenter applying one of the feedback conditions, followed by the participant performing the drift test, and then filling out the questionnaire (Q). Participants were randomized into one of ten predetermined randomization sequences, each consisting of 2 repetitions of each of the 4 conditions, presented in a random order for a total of 8 trial blocks. (B) A representative randomization sequence for the Active Prosthesis Test. For this test, the participant actively controlled the prosthesis in 3 feedback conditions: Synchronous Tapping (ST), Asynchronous Tapping (AT), and no feedback (Nil), for which the tactors were turned off. A trial block consisted of the participant actively using the prosthesis for grasp activities with one of the feedback conditions, followed by an assessment of proprioceptive drift, and then filling out the questionnaire. Participants were randomized to one of ten randomized sequences, consisting of 2 repetitions of each of the 3 conditions, presented in a random order for a total of 6 trial blocks.

Passive Prosthesis Test

A box that was accessible from both the front and back sides was placed on the table in front of the participant. This box was placed over the participant’s right arm between the brace and the prosthetic hand (Figure 2). The black sheet that was covering the participant’s arm and shoulder was adjusted if needed. In this manner, the participant could see only the prosthetic hand and forearm section, but not their real hand or forearm. The experimenter administered various conditions of feedback stimuli to the participant’s obscured right hand and to the visible prosthetic hand.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experimental setup for Passive Prosthesis Test. (A) Brushing feedback condition. The brushing stimulation was administered by the experimenter to the prosthetic hand and to the participant’s hand with two paintbrushes. (B) Tapping feedback condition. Three mechanotactile tactors placed on the thumb, index and middle fingers of the participant delivered a mechanotactile stimulus when the experimenter applied pressure to the corresponding sensors on the fingertips of the prosthetic hand.

The combination of two different feedback types (brushing and tapping) and two different feedback timing (synchronous and asynchronous) yielded four different conditions of feedback stimuli. These conditions were: Synchronous Brushing (SB), Asynchronous Brushing (AB), Synchronous Tapping (ST), and Asynchronous Tapping (AT) (described in Table 1). A single trial block for each condition consisted of the experimenter applying the feedback stimulus, followed by the participant performing an assessment of proprioceptive drift and then filling out a questionnaire (Appendix A).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Feedback stimuli provided to the participant in random order during the passive prosthesis test.

Participants were randomized into one of ten predetermined randomization sequences (Figure 1A). Each of these sequences consisted of 2 repetitions of each of the 4 conditions, presented in a random order for a total of 8 trial blocks (2 × 4).

Active Prosthesis Test

Participants had active motor control of the prosthetic hand, as per device setup 2.2.1. The participants were provided with a period of functional task training, wherein they used the simulated prosthesis to grasp and move objects with the prosthetic hand in a structured environment. They were encouraged to grasp and release a variety of objects of different sizes and densities (soft balls, blocks, plastic cups) to ensure adequate control and to experience the sensory feedback. Objects were then presented in a predetermined order, and participants were instructed to move the object to different positions or to stack them. Participants were asked to be as precise as possible and told that the time taken for each manipulation was not going to be considered, so that they would focus on the sensory experience and control rather than speed of moving the objects. Once they completed the object manipulation sequence, the participant would immediately rest their arm and device on the table.

For this test, three feedback conditions were tested, namely ST, AT, and no feedback. For all conditions, the participants had the same active motor control of the prosthetic hand. A trial block consisted of the participant actively using the prosthesis for grasp activities with one of the feedback conditions, followed by an assessment of proprioceptive drift, and then filling out the questionnaire. Participants were randomized to one of ten randomized sequences (Figure 1B). Each of these sequences consisted of two repetitions of each of the six conditions, presented in a random order for a total of six trial blocks.

During ST condition block, participants felt the mechanotactile tactor push on their right-hand thumb, index, and middle fingers when using the prosthetic hand to grasp objects. Ismail and Shimada (Ismail and Shimada, 2016) showed that participants felt significant weaker sense of agency with temporal delays of 240–490 ms; therefore, for the AT condition block, the mechanotactile tactors were delayed by 500 ms, and for the no feedback condition the tactors were switched off. For all conditions, the participants had the same active motor control of the prosthetic hand.

Outcome Measures

Following each testing condition, participants were asked to (a) with vision obscured, point with their left index finger on the board where they thought the tip of their actual index finger was (to measure proprioceptive drift) and (b) rate their agreement with 10 questions in the embodiment survey using a visual analog scale, adapted from previous work in this area (Ehrsson et al., 2008; Longo et al., 2008).

Proprioceptive Drift

The proprioceptive drift, outlined by Tsakiris et al. (2005), was calculated by measuring the difference between the points at which the participant indicated the position of their index finger pre- and post-test. With eyes closed, participants were instructed to point with their left index finger where they thought the tip of their finger was before and after a test. A positive result (positive drift) was indicative of the participant locating their hand toward the prosthetic hand. A negative result (negative drift) indicated that the participant had located their hand further away from the prosthetic hand.

Embodiment Questionnaire

Ten questions (five control and five related to embodiment) were adapted from Ehrsson et al. (2008) and Longo et al. (2008) (see Supplementary Table S1) (Ehrsson et al., 2004; Longo et al., 2008), similar to modifications used by prior authors for closed loop prosthetic control (Marasco et al., 2011; Graczyk et al., 2018; Page et al., 2018; Valle et al., 2018). The control statements were included to assess suggestibility and the embodiment statements were included to assess perception of key components of embodiment which are location, ownership, and agency. Additional questions on agency and “loss of hand” were included as potential components of the RHI, modified from Longo et al. (2008). Four versions of this questionnaire with a randomized order of the questions were developed, and a randomly selected version of the questionnaire was administered after each condition. The participant was asked to rate the strength of their agreement or disagreement for each question by pointing on the Visual Analog Scale with their left index finger. The scale was graded from 0 mm (strongly disagree) to 100 mm (strongly agree), and the distance was measured in millimeters. Higher grades on the embodiment questions indicated a greater sense of embodiment.

Statistical Analysis

Normality was assessed using Levene’s test. A paired sampled t-test was conducted to assess if there was a difference between the means of embodiment questions (Q1–Q5) and control questions (Q6–Q10) for each condition, to determine suggestibility.

A repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni correction was used on the average score of the embodiment questions. The factors for the ANOVA were Feedback Type and Feedback Condition, and levels of the factors were Brushing/Tapping and Synchronous/Asynchronous, respectively. The F-test of significance was used to assess the effects of the different independent variables. If significance was found, pairwise comparisons were made to assess where the differences lie. An α of 0.05 was used for all comparisons, and Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was utilized.

If a significant difference was detected between conditions on the average embodiment scores, then repeated measures ANOVA was run on the VAS response to each embodiment question (Q1–5) across conditions to determine if we could further detect differences among the responses to individual questions.

Results

Passive Prosthesis Embodiment

Responses to the questionnaire show that there was a statistically significant difference between the responses to embodiment items (Q1–Q5) and control items (Q6–Q10) for synchronous brushing feedback condition, as determined by paired sample t-test [t(20) = 5.1, p < 0.001]. Conversely, there was no statistically significant difference between participant’s responses to embodiment items and control items for the asynchronous brushing feedback condition; paired sample t-test [t(20) = 1.8, p = 0.095; Supplementary Table S2]. Both of these findings confirm that participants were not suggestible.

Participants’ responses to embodiment questions (Q1–Q5) were statistically significantly different between all tested conditions as determined by repeated measures ANOVA [F(3, 60) = 9.8, p < 0.001]; post hoc analysis indicated all comparisons were significantly different, except for SB vs. ST (Supplementary Figure S1 and Supplementary Table S3). Further analysis of the responses to individual questions indicated that providing users with synchronous brushing feedback prompted a significantly higher sense of embodiment on 4 out 5 of the embodiment questions than when providing users with asynchronous brushing feedback (Q 1, 2, 3, and 5, Bonferroni post hoc test, p < 0.05). The only question that did not evoke a significantly stronger response with synchronous brushing was the agency question (Q4) (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Passive Prosthesis Test: Questionnaire results for synchronous and asynchronous brushing. The questionnaire included these 10 statements, presented randomly. Statements 1–5 were used to describe aspects of the embodiment phenomena (Longo et al., 2008). Subjects indicated their response on a visual analog scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Red points indicate mean responses for synchronous brushing condition and blue points indicate mean responses for asynchronous brushing condition. Bars extending from these points indicate standard error of the mean (SEM) response. Horizontal black lines indicate statistically significant tendency to evoke affirmative responses (p < 0.05).

Results from the proprioceptive drift test showed a significant difference between testing conditions as determined by repeated measures ANOVA [F(3, 60) = 3.02, p = 0.036; Supplementary Table S3]. Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that participants had statistically significantly higher drift toward the prosthesis after receiving synchronous brushing feedback (14 ± 5.8 mm) or synchronous tapping feedback (6 ± 5.5 mm) compared to asynchronous tapping feedback (-13 ± 5.9 mm) (p = 0.014 and p = 0.018, respectively; Figure 4). Temporal manipulation of the brushing feedback (AB) did not result in a statistically significant difference in proprioceptive drift compared to the synchronous conditions, although the trend was to drift away from the prosthesis (−4 ± 6.0 mm).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Passive Prosthesis Test: Mean proprioceptive drifts toward the prosthetic hand for each of the conditions. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Black horizontal lines indicate statistical significance at p < 0.05. Red blocks indicate synchronous feedback, and blue blocks indicate asynchronous feedback.

To ensure that the mechanotactile system described in this work would have similar positive effects on prosthesis embodiment to brushing feedback, we also compared individual questionnaire responses for mechanotactile ST feedback to SB feedback (Figure 5). There was no statistically significant difference in responses to the embodiment statements between synchronous brushing and synchronous tapping conditions as determined by paired sample t-test [t(4) = 2.78, p = 0.26]. A Pearson product-moment correlation was performed to determine the relationship between responses to the questionnaire after receiving SB feedback and after receiving ST feedback in the passive prosthesis test. There was a strong, positive correlation between SB and ST, which was statistically significant (r = 0.919, n = 10, p = 0.00017).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Passive Prosthesis Test: Questionnaire results for synchronous feedback conditions. Participants’ responses on statements 1–4 for synchronous brushing were higher than their corresponding responses for synchronous tapping. No statistically significant difference was found between both feedback types. Red points indicate mean responses for synchronous brushing condition and red with black outline dots indicate mean responses for synchronous tapping condition. Bars extending from these points indicate standard error of the mean (SEM) response.

It is worth noting that, although not statistically significant, brushing feedback tended to evoke more positive responses on the first three embodiment statements than the tapping feedback (Figure 5). Similarly, participants had a trend toward greater proprioceptive drift toward the prosthetic hand when provided with synchronous brushing feedback than when provided with synchronous tapping feedback (see Figure 4), however this was not statistically significant.

Similar to the brushing condition, comparing the embodiment items to the control items for each tapping condition confirmed that participants were not suggestible, with a significant difference for the synchronous tapping condition [t(20) = 3.8, p = 0.001], but not for asynchronous tapping [t(20) = 2.0, p = 0.06]. The average of the embodiment question scores were statistically significantly different between synchronous and asynchronous conditions of the tapping feedback on Bonferroni post hoc analysis (p = 0.02; Supplementary Table S3). When examining individual questions, there was a trend for the temporal delay of the mechanotactile tapping feedback (AT) to negatively affect participants’ responses to embodiment statements in the questionnaire (Figure 6), although no statistically significant differences were found in responses to individual questions (p > 0.05).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Passive Prosthesis Test: Questionnaire results for synchronous and asynchronous tapping feedback conditions. Subjects indicated their response on a visual analog scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Red with black outline points indicate mean responses for synchronous tapping condition and blue with black outline points indicate mean responses for asynchronous tapping condition. Bars extending from these points indicate standard error of the mean (SEM) response.

Active Prosthesis Embodiment

Having determined that tapping feedback using mechanotactile tactors promotes the embodiment of the prosthesis in a passive condition similar to brushing with the hand and forearm constrained in the brace, we next compared participants’ responses after actively controlling the prosthetic device with synchronous mechanotactile tapping feedback, delayed tapping feedback, and without tapping feedback. Responses to the questionnaire show that there was a statistically significant difference between the responses to embodiment items (Q1–Q5) and control items (Q6–Q10) within each condition, as determined by paired sample t-test [ST: t(18) = 5.5, p < 0.001; AT: t(18) = 4.0, p = 0.001; no feedback: t(18) = 2.6, p = 0.02; Supplementary Table S4].

There was a significant difference in embodiment responses during the active prosthesis test with synchronous mechanotactile tapping feedback, delayed feedback, and no feedback as determined by repeated measures ANOVA [F(2, 36) = 7.2, p = 0.002]; Supplementary Figure S2 and Supplementary Table S5. Bonferroni Post hoc analysis showed that providing synchronous mechanotactile tapping feedback to participants while controlling the simulated prosthesis promoted statistically significant higher average responses to the embodiment questions than either asynchronous (p = 0.003) or no feedback (p = 0.003). When examining responses to individual embodiment questions, there was a significantly higher response to embodiment statement 1 with synchronous feedback compared to the response to the same statement when provided with no feedback (p = 0.004) (Figure 7). In contrast to the passive prosthesis experiment, high responses were seen on the agency question (Q4) for both the synchronous and no feedback conditions, with asynchronous tapping showing a non-significant trend of negatively affecting agency.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Active Prosthesis Test: Questionnaire results for controlling a prosthesis while receiving three feedback conditions. Participants’ responses on statements 1–5 for synchronous tapping were higher than responses for no feedback condition. Red with black outline points indicate mean responses for synchronous tapping condition, blue with black outline dots indicate mean responses for asynchronous tapping condition, and green dots indicate mean responses for no feedback condition. Bars extending from these points indicate standard error of the mean (SEM) response. Horizontal black line indicates statistically significant tendency to evoke affirmative responses (p < 0.05).

Results from the proprioceptive drift task in the Active Prosthesis Test showed all conditions resulted in some shift toward the prosthetic hand with a trend to higher proprioceptive drift for the synchronous tapping condition (shown in Figure 8), although not statistically significant (Supplementary Table S5).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Active Prosthesis Test: Mean proprioceptive drifts toward the prosthetic hand after actively controlling it while receiving three types of feedback. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Red with black outline bar indicates synchronous tapping feedback, blue with black outline bar indicates asynchronous tapping feedback, and green bar indicates no feedback condition.

Discussion

Simulated upper-limb prosthesis systems are commonly used as an approximation to prostheses used by persons with upper-limb amputation, as a method of allowing able-bodied participants to actively control a prosthetic hand in a situation more similar to actual prosthesis use. Researchers have used various versions of simulated prostheses to investigate the performance of commercial prosthetic hands (Kyberd, 2011), performance of novel control strategies (Johansen et al., 2016; Shehata et al., 2018a), kinematic movement trajectories when using prosthetic hands (Williams et al., 2019), and, recently, the effect of providing sensory feedback to users on performance in functional tasks (Wilson et al., 2017; Engels et al., 2019). In this work, we used a sensorized simulated prosthesis to investigate the contribution of sensory feedback to the embodiment phenomenon during active motor control of the prosthesis, utilizing a common methodology in the literature, namely the RHI (Longo et al., 2008).

Passive RHI

Wearing a simulated prosthesis in a passive situation evoked similar embodiment responses to prior work with the RHI. Specifically, we found that similar to other studies (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998; Tsakiris et al., 2005), participants tended to embody the prosthesis in the passive synchronous brushing condition as indicated by their positive responses to location and ownership statements on the questionnaire (particularly Q1–Q3) and high proprioceptive drift toward the device (Tsakiris et al., 2005). This finding was important, as the tactile contact of the brace on the skin of the intact forearm and hand may have presented a distracting stimulus that could alter the embodiment experience (Parmentier et al., 2011). However, brushing stimulation is not common in prosthesis user applications. In previous work (Marasco et al., 2011; Hebert et al., 2014), researchers relayed tactile feedback to persons with upper-limb loss by placing tactors on reinnervated skin areas; therefore, returning physiologically appropriate touch and pressure feedback from the prosthesis to the user through skin indentation. Implantable peripheral nerve interface approaches also most commonly report and utilize touch and pressure feedback corresponding to the digits (Wendelken et al., 2017; Schiefer et al., 2018; George et al., 2019). We, therefore, investigated the effect of using a mechanotactile stimulation of touch and pressure to provide the sensory information (Schoepp et al., 2018), to investigate its effect on simulated prosthesis embodiment. We found that synchronous touch and pressure stimulation evoked similar embodiment responses as brushing (no significant differences and strong positive correlation), although responses were blunted, consistent with prior literature (D’Alonzo and Cipriani, 2012). This finding was not surprising given that stroking a brush is known to evoke higher emotional affective responses (Crucianelli et al., 2013), and affect has also been considered an influential component of embodiment (Longo et al., 2008). The proprioceptive drift results confirmed that the synchronous stimulation conditions evoked higher displacement toward the prosthetic hand compared to the asynchronous tapping condition.

Similar to work in participants with amputation (Ehrsson et al., 2008; Marasco et al., 2011; Schmalzl et al., 2014), in our study, ownership and location aspects of embodiment were affected by the synchronicity of the feedback in the passive condition, however, the agency question was not affected by either synchronous or asynchronous brushing and tapping. This would be expected since participants did not have any control over the device during the first testing phase and therefore did not develop any sense of agency over the device. Our results for the passive (no voluntary control) test, therefore, showed that wearing a sensorized simulated prosthesis with integrated mechanotactile feedback can drive the perceptual shift of certain aspects of embodiment, namely ownership and location.

Active Task

For the active prosthesis experiments, we found that active motor control induced a form of agency, reflected in the agency question and the proprioceptive drift, even for the no feedback condition. These results were consistent with prior findings in able-bodied subjects (Dummer et al., 2009) and those with limb amputation (Ehrsson et al., 2008; Sato et al., 2018). Having control over visualized movements of a robotic hand has been theorized to allow embodiment due to implicit knowledge of a kinesthetic sense, which contributes to making the experience personal (Dummer et al., 2009). The prosthesis user study by Marasco et al. (2018) examined the effect of restoring the kinesthetic sense of hand movement during an active grasping task on embodiment of a prosthetic hand. Results showed that providing kinesthetic feedback conferred a significantly greater sense of agency, but did not affect statements of limb ownership. Our findings support that the sense of agency can be induced by the use of the inherent kinesthetic sense associated with muscle contraction matched to active robotic control in our intact able-bodied participants (Kalckert and Ehrsson, 2012; Marotta et al., 2017), consistent with the restored kinesthesia and sense of agency in those with limb amputation (Marasco et al., 2018).

Adding synchronous tactile feedback to the active task enhanced embodiment responses compared to asynchronous or no feedback, when the average score of all embodiment questions were considered. Examining responses to individual questions revealed that synchronous tapping tended to result in the highest embodiment responses particularly for the first three questions, with no feedback resulting in the least amount of embodiment (see Figure 7). These findings are consistent with Graczyk et al. (2018), who reported positive responses to embodiment questions in participants with implantable neural interfaces after a sensory-enabled take-home trial; although, this embodiment did persist to the subsequent non-sensory enabled trial. Page et al. (2018) also found that providing sensory feedback to their participants with a neural interface in a passive condition significantly induced embodiment compared to the motor control only condition, however, there was no additional advantage of closed-loop control in enhancing the embodiment response. However, these studies did not conduct a deeper investigation of the sense of agency, which may, in fact, potentiate embodiment (Sato et al., 2018).

In our active prosthesis control experiment, although the addition of synchronous feedback added to specific aspects of embodiment, i.e., ownership and location (as represented by the first three questions), it did not affect agency. Agency was high with active control and not changed with adding synchronous feedback; however, asynchronous feedback tended to result in the lowest response to the agency question. This possible influence of asynchronous tapping raises an intriguing possibility for a potential mechanism to separate agency from ownership. Given that this finding was based on a single question, additional and more sensitive measures of agency would be helpful to test this hypothesis in future studies.

In the active prosthesis test, in addition to the high positive responses to the sense of agency statement on the questionnaire, participants demonstrated proprioceptive drift toward the simulated prosthesis for all conditions. This finding was unexpected as we hypothesized asynchronous tapping would cause a drift away from the prosthesis, as evidenced in the passive experiment. A possible influencing factor within our set up was that the asynchronous stimulation was provided at a fixed time delay, and the participant may have learned to incorporate that feedback, even though delayed (Blustein et al., 2018). Exploring the effect of timing of delayed feedback on feedback incorporation and real-time control may be an important area of future study.

Others have also noted a discrepancy between drift and subjective ownership responses in passive conditions of synchronous and asynchronous stroking (Rohde et al., 2011). In our work, participants were controlling a simulated prosthesis that was attached to their forearms to grasp and move objects. We propose that our participants utilized the kinesthetic sense of contracting their muscles to control the prosthesis to achieve the sense of agency over hand grasp. The proprioceptive senses associated with more proximal intact sensory organs around the shoulder and elbow could have affected the observed proprioceptive drift (Proske and Gandevia, 2012). There is also evidence that proprioceptive drift and agency may respond similarly (Tajima et al., 2015) and be task dependent (Shibuya et al., 2017). Proprioceptive drift may, therefore, be expected to differ between passive and active conditions, such that there is a stronger influence of motor control on this measurement of embodiment specifically.

Limitations

There are limited opportunities to access participants with bidirectional sensory feedback systems and further limitations in manipulating sensory experiences to explore the impact of feedback type. We, therefore, chose to use able-bodied participants using a simulated prosthesis to determine if their embodiment responses would be similar to those with limb amputation, and potentially modifiable. However, it must be kept in mind that the inherent neurophysiological structures have not been interrupted as in those with limb amputation (Knecht et al., 1996), therefore, these participants are a proxy at best. These preliminary results suggest that the use of a sensory simulated prosthesis can induce embodiment responses (ownership and location) and may separately affect the construct of agency, even with the limited subjective measures employed. This approach opens an avenue for more in-depth exploration of this phenomenon that may then be applied to the sample of individuals with sensory-enabled upper limb prosthesis systems.

We used three traditional embodiment statements commonly cited in the literature, and included additional questions on agency and loss of own hand. The questions, originally derived from Botvinick and Cohen (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998) and modified by other authors (Ehrsson et al., 2008; Longo et al., 2008) were based on experiments designed for a passive experience rather than an active control situation, and may need to be further refined and validated for new experimental paradigms (Gonzalez-Franco and Peck, 2018). Other authors have similarly used analysis of three modified embodiment statements (Graczyk et al., 2018; Page et al., 2018; Valle et al., 2018), as well as answers to individual questions (Marasco et al., 2011; Valle et al., 2018) to interpret embodiment in close-loop prosthetic control conditions. A lack of multiple quantitative measures to assess embodiment and the related phenomenon (such as agency, location, and proprioception) is a clear limitation of this work. The use of subjective questionnaire ratings generally limits the interpretation of the findings and highlights the crucial lack of quantitative measures to address outstanding questions on the components of embodiment such as agency. More recent work on quantitative measures of agency and ownership, including intentional binding paradigms, incorporation measures, and internal model (Haggard et al., 2002; Moore and Obhi, 2012; Kühn et al., 2013; Blustein et al., 2018; Shehata et al., 2018b) may allow future work to more adequately parse out the contributions of sensory feedback and active motor control in an active prosthesis control situation.

Conclusion

We have shown that a simulated prosthesis actively used for functional control activities may be embodied by able-bodied users (Ehrsson et al., 2008; Marasco et al., 2011; Schmalzl et al., 2014). In addition, we verified that mechanotactile sensory feedback might not only be useful for improving sense of ownership and location but also may have a modulating effect on the sense of agency when provided asynchronously during active motor control tasks. The simulated sensory-motor prosthesis system may allow us to manipulate the factors contributing to the complex phenomenon of embodiment, enabling future study of more precise quantitative measures of embodiment that do not rely as much on subjective perception, which will be crucial to advancing knowledge in this field.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available upon reasonable request, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

Written informed consent according to the University of Alberta Health Research Ethics Board was obtained from all participants before conducting the experiments (Pro00057340). This work is not a clinical trial. All experiments were conducted with able-bodied participants in a lab setting and not in a clinic.

Author Contributions

JSH and ZEJ conceived the idea and designed the study. ZEJ conducted the experiments, summarized the data, and contributed to the manuscript draft. MR and AWS analyzed the data. AWS, MR, and JSH wrote and contributed to manuscript revisions. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

Open access publication fees supported by the University of Alberta (JSH).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the volunteer participants in this study, the staff, and students at the Bionic Limbs for Improved Natural Control (BLINC) Lab for their help and support, and Tarvo Kuus for assistance with experimental setup and data collection.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2020.00263/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

RHI, rubber hand illusion; SB, synchronous brushing; AB, asynchronous brushing; ST, synchronous tapping; AT, asynchronous tapping.

References

Amsuess, S., Vujaklija, I., Goebel, P., Roche, A. D., Graimann, B., Aszmann, O. C., et al. (2016). Context-dependent upper limb prosthesis control for natural and robust use. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 24, 744–753. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2015.2454240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armel, K. C., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 1499–1506. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battye, C. K., Nightingale, A., and Whillis, J. (1955). The use of myo-electric currents in the operation of prostheses. J. Bone Joint Surg. Br. 37-B, 506–510. doi: 10.1302/0301-620X.37B3.506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belter, J. T., Segil, J. L., Dollar, A. M., and Weir, R. F. (2013). Mechanical design and performance specifications of anthropomorphic prosthetic hands: a review. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 50, 599–618.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Biddiss, E., and Chau, T. (2007). Upper limb prosthesis use and abandonment: a survey of the last 25 years. Prosthet. Orthot. Int. 31, 236–257. doi: 10.1080/03093640600994581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blustein, D., Wilson, A., and Sensinger, J. (2018). Assessing the quality of supplementary sensory feedback using the crossmodal congruency task. Sci. Rep. 8:6203. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24560-24563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756. doi: 10.1038/35784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemente, F., Dosen, S., Lonini, L., Markovic, M., Farina, D., and Cipriani, C. (2017). Humans can integrate augmented reality feedback in their sensorimotor control of a robotic hand. IEEE Trans. Hum. Machine Syst. 47, 583–589. doi: 10.1109/THMS.2016.2611998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crucianelli, L., Metcalf, N., Fotopoulou, A. K., and Jenkinson, P. (2013). Bodily pleasure matters: velocity of touch modulates body ownership during the rubber hand illusion. Front. Psychol. 4:703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Alonzo, M., and Cipriani, C. (2012). Vibrotactile sensory substitution elicits feeling of ownership of an alien hand. PLoS One 7:e50756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dummer, T., Picot-Annand, A., Neal, T., and Moore, C. (2009). Movement and the rubber hand illusion. Perception 38, 271–280. doi: 10.1068/p5921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Rosen, B., Stockselius, A., Ragno, C., Kohler, P., and Lundborg, G. (2008). Upper limb amputees can be induced to experience a rubber hand as their own. Brain 131, 3443–3452. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., and Passingham, R. E. (2004). That’s my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305, 875–877. doi: 10.1126/science.1097011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engels, L. F., Shehata, A. W., Scheme, E. J., Sensinger, J. W., and Cipriani, C. (2019). When less is more – discrete tactile feedback dominates continuous audio biofeedback in the integrated percept while controlling a myoelectric prosthetic hand. Front. Neurosci. 13:578. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (1986). Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. J. Mind Behav. 7, 541–554.

Google Scholar

Gallagher, S., and Cole, J. (1995). Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject. J. Mind Behav. 16, 369–389.

Google Scholar

Geethanjali, P. (2016). Myoelectric control of prosthetic hands: state-of-the-art review. Med. Devices 9, 247–255. doi: 10.2147/MDER.S91102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, J. A., Kluger, D. T., Davis, T. S., Wendelken, S. M., Okorokova, E. V., He, Q., et al. (2019). Biomimetic sensory feedback through peripheral nerve stimulation improves dexterous use of a bionic hand. Sci. Robot. 4:eaax2352. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.aax2352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giummarra, M. J., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2008). Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 143–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Franco, M., and Peck, T. C. (2018). Avatar embodiment. towards a standardized questionnaire. Front. Robot. AI 5:74. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2018.00074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graczyk, E. L., Resnik, L., Schiefer, M. A., Schmitt, M. S., and Tyler, D. J. (2018). Home use of a neural-connected sensory prosthesis provides the functional and psychosocial experience of having a hand again. Sci. Rep. 8:9866. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26952-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P., Clark, S., and Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 382–385. doi: 10.1038/nn827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebert, J., Boser, Q., Valevicius, A., Tanikawa, H., Lavoie, E., Vette, A., et al. (2019). Quantitative gaze and movement differences in visuomotor adaptations of upper extremity prosthesis users to varying task demands. JAMA Netw. Open 2:e1911197. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.11197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebert, J. S., Olson, J. L., Morhart, M. J., Dawson, M. R., Marasco, P. D., Kuiken, T. A., et al. (2014). Novel targeted sensory reinnervation technique to restore functional hand sensation after transhumeral amputation. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 22, 765–773. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2013.2294907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ismail, M. A. F., and Shimada, S. (2016). ‘Robot’ hand illusion under delayed visual feedback: relationship between the senses of ownership and agency. PLoS One 11:e0159619. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansen, D., Cipriani, C., Popovic, D. B., and Struijk, L. N. S. A. (2016). Control of a robotic hand using a tongue control system-a prosthesis application. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 63, 1368–1376. doi: 10.1109/tbme.2016.2517742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalckert, A., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: a dissociation of ownership and agency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:40. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalckert, A., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Conscious. Cogn. 26, 117–132. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knecht, S., Henningsen, H., Elbert, T., Flor, H., Hohling, C., Pantev, C., et al. (1996). Reorganizational and perceptional changes after amputation. Brain 119(Pt 4), 1213–1219. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühn, S., Brass, M., and Haggard, P. (2013). Feeling in control: neural correlates of experience of agency. Cortex 49, 1935–1942. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuus, T., Dawson, M. R., Schoepp, K., Carey, J. P., and Hebert, J. S. (2017). “Development of a simulated sensory motor prosthesis: a device to research prosthetic sensory feedback using able-bodied individuals,” in Proceedings of the Myoelectric Controls Symposium, Fredericton.

Google Scholar

Kyberd, P. J. (2011). The influence of control format and hand design in single axis myoelectric hands: assessment of functionality of prosthetic hands using the Southampton hand assessment procedure. Prosthet. Orthot. Int. 35, 285–293. doi: 10.1177/0309364611418554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Schuur, F., Kammers, M. P. M., Tsakiris, M., and Haggard, P. (2008). What is embodiment? a psychometric approach. Cognition 107, 978–998. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marasco, P. D., Hebert, J. S., Sensinger, J. W., Shell, C. E., Schofield, J. S., Thumser, Z. C., et al. (2018). Illusory movement perception improves motor control for prosthetic hands. Sci. Transl. Med. 10:eaao6990. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aao6990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marasco, P. D., Kim, K., Colgate, J. E., Peshkin, M. A., and Kuiken, T. A. (2011). Robotic touch shifts perception of embodiment to a prosthesis in targeted reinnervation amputees. Brain 134, 747–758. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marotta, A., Bombieri, F., Zampini, M., Schena, F., Dallocchio, C., Fiorio, M., et al. (2017). The moving rubber hand illusion reveals that explicit sense of agency for tapping movements is preserved in functional movement disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:291. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. W., and Obhi, S. S. (2012). Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 546–561. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, C. D. (2004). An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the embodiment of artificial limbs. Disabil. Rehabil. 26, 963–973. doi: 10.1080/09638280410001696764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, D. M., George, J. A., Kluger, D. T., Duncan, C., Wendelken, S., Davis, T., et al. (2018). Motor control and sensory feedback enhance prosthesis embodiment and reduce phantom pain after long-term hand amputation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:352. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panarese, A., Edin, B. B., Vecchi, F., Carrozza, M. C., and Johansson, R. S. (2009). Humans can integrate force feedback to toes in their sensorimotor control of a robotic hand. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 17, 560–567. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2009.2021689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parmentier, F. B. R., Ljungberg, J. K., Elsley, J. V., and Lindkvist, M. (2011). A behavioral study of distraction by vibrotactile novelty. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 37, 1134–1139. doi: 10.1037/a0021931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proske, U., and Gandevia, S. C. (2012). The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol. Rev. 92, 1651–1697. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohde, M., Di Luca, M., and Ernst, M. O. (2011). The rubber hand illusion: feeling of ownership and proprioceptive drift do not go hand in hand. PLoS One 6:e21659. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, B., Ehrsson, H. H., Antfolk, C., Cipriani, C., Sebelius, F., and Lundborg, G. (2009). Referral of sensation to an advanced humanoid robotic hand prosthesis. Scand. J. Plast. Reconstr. Surg. hand Surg. 43, 260–266. doi: 10.3109/02844310903113107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, Y., Kawase, T., Takano, K., Spence, C., and Kansaku, K. (2018). Body ownership and agency altered by an electromyographically controlled robotic arm. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5:172170. doi: 10.1098/rsos.172170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, I., and Vijayakumar, S. (2011). The role of feed-forward and feedback processes for closed-loop prosthesis control. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 8:60. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-8-60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiefer, M. A., Graczyk, E. L., Sidik, S. M., Tan, D. W., and Tyler, D. J. (2018). Artificial tactile and proprioceptive feedback improves performance and confidence on object identification tasks. PLoS One 13:e0207659. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmalzl, L., Kalckert, A., Ragnö, C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). Neural correlates of the rubber hand illusion in amputees: A report of two cases. Neurocase 20, 407–420. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2013.791861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoepp, K. R., Dawson, M. R., Schofield, J. S., Carey, J. P., and Hebert, J. S. (2018). Design and integration of an inexpensive wearable mechanotactile feedback system for myoelectric prostheses. IEEE J. Transl. Eng. Heal. Med. 6:2100711. doi: 10.1109/JTEHM.2018.2866105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shehata, A. W., Engels, L. F., Controzzi, M., Cipriani, C., Scheme, E. J., and Sensinger, J. W. (2018a). Improving internal model strength and performance of prosthetic hands using augmented feedback. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 15:70. doi: 10.1186/s12984-018-0417-414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shehata, A. W., Scheme, E. J., and Sensinger, J. W. (2018b). Evaluating internal model strength and performance of myoelectric prosthesis control strategies. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 26, 1046–1055. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2018.2826981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shibuya, S., Unenaka, S., and Ohki, Y. (2017). Body ownership and agency: task-dependent effects of the virtual hand illusion on proprioceptive drift. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 121–134. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4777-4773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobuh, M. M. D., Kenney, L. P. J., Galpin, A. J., Thies, S. B., McLaughlin, J., Kulkarni, J., et al. (2014). Visuomotor behaviours when using a myoelectric prosthesis. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11:72. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajima, D., Mizuno, T., Kume, Y., and Yoshida, T. (2015). The mirror illusion: does proprioceptive drift go hand in hand with sense of agency? Front. Psychol. 6:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., and Haggard, P. (2005). The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 80–91. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., Haggard, P., Franck, N., Mainy, N., and Sirigu, A. (2005). A specific role for efferent information in self-recognition. Cognition 96, 215–231. doi: 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2004.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valle, G., Mazzoni, A., Iberite, F., D’Anna, E., Strauss, I., Granata, G., et al. (2018). Biomimetic intraneural sensory feedback enhances sensation naturalness, tactile sensitivity, and manual dexterity in a bidirectional prosthesis. Neuron 100, 37.e–45.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Riet, D., Stopforth, R., Bright, G., and Diegel, O. (2013). “An overview and comparison of upper limb prosthetics,” in Proceedings of the IEEE AFRICON Conference, Mauritius.

Google Scholar

Wendelken, S., Page, D. M., Davis, T., Wark, H. A. C., Kluger, D. T., Duncan, C., et al. (2017). Restoration of motor control and proprioceptive and cutaneous sensation in humans with prior upper-limb amputation via multiple Utah Slanted Electrode Arrays (USEAs) implanted in residual peripheral arm nerves. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14:121. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0320-324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M. M., Zhang, W., Winslow, A. T., Zahabi, M., Zhang, F., Huang, H., et al. (2017). Usability comparison of conventional direct control versus pattern recognition control of transradial prostheses. IEEE Trans. Hum. Machine Syst. 47, 1146–1157. doi: 10.1109/THMS.2017.2759762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, H. E., Boser, Q. A., Pilarski, P. M., Chapman, C. S., Vette, A. H., and Hebert, J. S. (2019). Hand function kinematics when using a simulated myoelectric prosthesis. IEEE Int. Conf. Rehabil. Robot. 2019, 169–174. doi: 10.1109/ICORR.2019.8779443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A. W., Blustein, D. H., and Sensinger, J. W. (2017). A third arm – design of a bypass prosthesis enabling incorporation. IEEE Int. Conf. Rehabil. Robot. 2017, 1381–1386. doi: 10.1109/ICORR.2017.8009441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: rubber hand illusion, prosthetics, sensory feedback, embodiment, motor learning, electromyography, simulated prosthesis

Citation: Shehata AW, Rehani M, Jassat ZE and Hebert JS (2020) Mechanotactile Sensory Feedback Improves Embodiment of a Prosthetic Hand During Active Use. Front. Neurosci. 14:263. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00263

Received: 20 November 2019; Accepted: 09 March 2020;
Published: 26 March 2020.

Edited by:

Max Ortiz-Catalan, Chalmers University of Technology, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Giovanni Di Pino, Campus Bio-Medico University, Italy
Francesco Clemente, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Shehata, Rehani, Jassat and Hebert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jacqueline S. Hebert, jhebert@ualberta.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.