Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroergonomics, 03 June 2021
Sec. Neurotechnology and Systems Neuroergonomics
Volume 2 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnrgo.2021.686596

Prefrontal Inter-brain Synchronization Reflects Convergence and Divergence of Flow Dynamics in Collaborative Learning: A Pilot Study

Takayuki Nozawa1,2* Mutsumi Kondo3 Reiko Yamamoto3 Hyeonjeong Jeong1,4 Shigeyuki Ikeda1,5 Kohei Sakaki1 Yoshihiro Miyake6 Yasushige Ishikawa3 Ryuta Kawashima1
  • 1Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
  • 2Research Institute for the Earth Inclusive Sensing, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3Department of British and American Studies, Kyoto University of Foreign Studies, Kyoto, Japan
  • 4Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
  • 5RIKEN Center for Advanced Intelligence Project, Tokyo, Japan
  • 6Department of Computer Science, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, Japan

Flow is a highly motivated and affectively positive state in which a person is deeply engaged in an activity and feeling enjoyment from it. In collaborative activities, it would be optimal if all participants were in a state of flow. However, flow states fluctuate amongst individuals due to differences in the dynamics of motivation and cognition. To explore the possibility that inter-brain synchronization can provide a quantitative measure of the convergence and divergence of collective motivational dynamics, we conducted a pilot study to investigate the relationship between inter-brain synchronization and the interpersonal similarity of flow state dynamics during the collaborative learning process. In two English as a Foreign Language (EFL) classes, students were divided into groups of three-four and seated at desks facing each other while conducting a 60-min group work. In both classes, two groups with four members were randomly selected, and their medial prefrontal neural activities were measured simultaneously using wireless functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) devices. Later the participants observed their own activities on recorded videos and retrospectively rated their subjective degree of flow state on a seven-point scale for each 2-min period. For the pairs of students whose neural activities were measured, the similarity of their flow experience dynamics was evaluated by the temporal correlation between their flow ratings. Prefrontal inter-brain synchronization of the same student pairs during group work was evaluated using wavelet transform coherence. Statistical analyses revealed that: (1) flow dynamics were significantly more similar for the student pairs within the same group compared to the pairs of students assigned across different groups; (2) prefrontal inter-brain synchronization in the relatively short time scale (9.3–13.9 s) was significantly higher for the within-group pairs than for the cross-group pairs; and (3) the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization at the same short time scale was significantly and positively correlated with the similarity of flow dynamics, even after controlling for the effects of within- vs. cross-group pair types from the two variables. These suggest that inter-brain synchronization can indeed provide a quantitative measure for converging and diverging collective motivational dynamics during collaborative learning, with higher inter-brain synchronization corresponding to a more convergent flow experience.

Introduction

In psychology, flow is a state of deeply and energetically engaging in and enjoying an activity at hand (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997). People generally experience flow when: (1) the perceived challenge of an activity is high and in balance with their own capacities or skills; (2) they receive immediate feedback about how well they are doing; and (3) the goal is clear. Flow is also linked to intrinsic motivation, which is instrumental in achieving various goals (Deci and Ryan, 2000). In education, in addition to the challenge-skill balance, the experience of flow is promoted by active participation rather than passive reception of information (Shernoff et al., 2003). Flow experience in learning can also improve a students' psychological well-being, perceived learning, satisfaction, and learning performance (Rossin et al., 2009; Steele and Fullagar, 2009; Landhäußer and Keller, 2012; Buil et al., 2019).

In group learning, such as in classrooms, teachers find it optimal when the entire class functions together in the state of flow, being in synch with one another (Kent, 2013). However, the degree of flow fluctuates from moment to moment and varies among learners. The cause of divergent flow dynamics between learners can be due to some students being distracted, not sufficiently engaged, or perceiving the task at hand to be too difficult or too easy for their capabilities. Whatever the cause, if convergence and divergence of flow experience between learners can be detected using an objective measure, it may help to achieve optimal educational situations. For example, if a detection system identifies a student in a collaborative learning project who diverges from the other group members in terms of their flow state, the teacher can be alerted to instigate countermeasures, such as guiding the student to focus more, or changing the group composition to make the members' capability levels more compatible with one another. Alternatively, giving feedback on shared flow dynamics to the learners themselves could facilitate behavioral changes that lead to better collaborations.

Recently, the hyperscanning technique, in which the brain activity of multiple people is simultaneously recorded, has been increasingly applied to objectively characterize a wide range of social interactions (Nam et al., 2020). Hyperscanning studies on natural verbal communications have shown that inter-brain synchronization between communication partners reflects various aspects of communication, such as the existence of face-to-face communication (Jiang et al., 2012; Nozawa et al., 2016), emergence of leader-follower relationships (Jiang et al., 2015), and agreement vs. disagreement (Hirsch et al., 2021). Specifically, studies on real educational classroom settings using electroencephalography (EEG) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning have shown that inter-brain synchronization is associated with higher engagement amongst students (Yamamoto et al., 2015; Dikker et al., 2017; Bevilacqua et al., 2018; Brockington et al., 2018). Based on these results, we hypothesized that the convergence and divergence of dynamically changing flow states between learners could also be associated with inter-brain synchronization between learners, with those pairs who share flow dynamics showing higher inter-brain synchronization.

To test our hypothesis, we applied fNIRS hyperscanning to real active learning activities held in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) class at a university in Japan, and investigated the relationship between the similarity of flow dynamics between learners and their medial prefrontal inter-brain synchronization. The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) has been implicated in social cognition and communication (Gilbert et al., 2006; Suda et al., 2010), and inter-brain synchronization in the mPFC has been shown to reflect different communication modes and quality (Nozawa et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2019). In addition, the mPFC is easily accessible for the wireless fNIRS recording (see Methods). We targeted a collaborative group learning setting because group dynamics and shared engagement are especially important in collaborative learning (Järvelä et al., 2010). Furthermore, the division into groups enabled us to compare within-group and cross-group learning pairs. This approach allowed us to test the following hypotheses: (1) flow dynamics are more convergent (i.e., temporally more correlated) between members of the same group who work together than between learners belonging to different groups; (2) inter-brain synchronization is higher between group members, who collaborate with each other, than between cross-group learners who share the same class but do not directly collaborate; and (3) the higher the inter-brain synchronization between a pair of learners, the more similar their dynamics of experienced flow would be; thus, inter-brain synchronization would provide an objective indicator of shared flow dynamics.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were briefed on the experimental procedure and provided written informed consent prior to participating in the experiments.

Participants

Two English as foreign language (EFL) active learning classes at the Kyoto University of Foreign Studies, Japan, participated in this study. The two classes consisted of 1st- or 2nd-grade students (27 in one class and 29 in the other). Among them, 16 students (age 19–22 years; 11 females and five males) were subjected to the fNIRS measurement and the retrospective evaluation of flow dynamics (see section fNIRS Brain Activity Measurements).

Experimental Procedures

In each class, students were divided into groups of four or three members and seated at desks facing each other (Figure 1A). In the preparation phase, students were asked to read and understand the script of a speech by Steve Jobs. They then watched a video of the actual speech given by Steve Jobs to deepen their understanding of their reading. Subsequently, they conducted a 60-min group study session, which consisted of four group work activities (GW1-4): GW1 “Connecting the Dots,” they picked out important events from Jobs' personal history and created connected dots of his life; GW2 “Reading a supplemental article,” in turn they read aloud a supplemental text on college dropout in the U.S.; GW3 “Linking Readings One and Two,” they synthesized information from the two readings and completed a worksheet together; and GW4 “Evaluative Discussion,” they discussed college dropout in Japan, comparing different opinions and making judgments. The task switch timings between the four group activities were the same across the groups in each class, but the pacing within each activity was decided by each group. The group work activities were video-recorded for the later retrospective evaluation (see section Retrospective Evaluation of Subjective Flow Dynamics).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Snapshot of a working collaborative learning group with their mPFC activities being measured with wireless fNIRS devices. (B) Illustration of student pair types. Within-group pairs and cross-group pairs are indicated in red and blue lines, respectively. For visibility, only six out of 16 cross-group pairs are illustrated.

fNIRS Brain Activity Measurements

In each of the two classes, we randomly selected two groups of four members and simultaneously recorded their brain activities throughout the group work. Gender constitution of the four groups were: four females; three females and one male; three females and one male; and one female and three females. We measured the neural activities of the mPFC in eight students using a wireless continuous-wave fNIRS device (Yamamoto et al., 2015; Nozawa et al., 2016; Ikeda et al., 2017). The fNIRS device is lightweight (100 g including battery), comfort-to-wear, and enables the simultaneous measurement of prefrontal neural activities of interacting people. The optode component of the device contains one infrared light source (wavelength 810 nm) and two light detectors at a distance of 1.0 and 3.0 cm from the light source. Using the modified Beer-Lambert law (Delpy et al., 1988; Scholkmann et al., 2014), changes in the detected light intensities were converted to the concentration changes of total hemoglobin on the optical path of the source-detector. Signals were sampled at a frequency of 10 Hz and transmitted to a host computer using the ZigBee wireless network protocol. This system enables the simultaneous measurement of up to 20 individuals. See Nozawa et al. (2016) for more details on the fNIRS device.

The placement of the fNIRS device followed that of a previous study (Nozawa et al., 2016). The center of the optode component was positioned at the center between FP1 and FP2 according to the international 10-20 system for EEG electrode placement, which covers the rostral limit of the superior frontal gyrus (Homan et al., 1987). The light source was placed on the subject's left-hand side, a light detector with a 3 cm distance from the source was on the right-hand side, and a light detector with a 1 cm distance from the source was placed between them. Any makeup on the measurement position was removed and hair brushed away to ensure good optode-skin contact. The optode component was covered with black rubber to shield it from external light. The students were instructed to adopt and maintain a relaxed posture in the seat, and to avoid rapid head movements as much as possible.

Retrospective Evaluation of Subjective Flow Dynamics

The 16 students who were subjected to the fNIRS recording conducted a retrospective evaluation of their subjective flow dynamics. They were first explained about the concept of flow, based on the description by Csikszentmihalyi (1997), with flow described as “a state in which you are so involved in the activity at hand that nothing else seems to matter. The experience itself is so enjoyable that you will continue the activity for the sheer sake of doing it. In this state, you simultaneously experience concentration, interest, and enjoyment. It is also referred to as “being in the zone.”” Then, watching the video recording of themselves performing the group work, they retrospectively rated their flow level for every 2-min segment on a seven-level scale from 1 (“very low”) through 4 (“neutral”) to 7 (“very high”). This resulted in a series of 30 time points of flow level for the 60-min group work.

Analyses of Flow Dynamics Similarity and Inter-Brain Synchronization

We calculated the similarity of flow dynamics and prefrontal inter-brain synchronization for all possible pairs of eight students in each class (Figure 1B). Thus, we had 24 pairs (6 pairs/group × 2 groups/class × 2 classes) of students in the same group (“within-group pairs”) and 32 pairs (4 × 4 pairs/class × 2 classes) of students who belonged to different groups (“cross-group pairs”).

The similarity of the flow dynamics of each student pair was evaluated using the Pearson correlation of their flow-level time series. To enhance the normality of the distribution of the sample correlation coefficients, Fisher z-transformation z = arctanh(r) was applied to the coefficients. Then, the flow dynamics correlation values were compared between the pair types to test hypothesis (1) that the flow dynamics are more convergent (i.e., correlation is higher) for the within-group pairs than for the cross-group pairs. Taking into account the dependence structure in the pairwise similarity data, we used a non-parametric permutation test to evaluate the significance of difference between the pair types (Pesarin and Salmaso, 2010; Winkler et al., 2015). First, all possible random permutations re-assigning the eight students into two groups within each class were generated and combined for the two classes. Then, the difference of mean similarity values between the within-group and cross-group pairs for each instance of permutation was gathered to generate the null distribution. Finally, the p-value of the observed difference of means was calculated as the proportion of permutation-based differences at least as extreme as the observed difference value. Furthermore, the correlation values were also used to test the relationship between shared flow dynamics and prefrontal inter-brain synchronization (see below).

The fNIRS signals were preprocessed using MATLAB (MathWorks Inc.). First, the drift components were removed from each signal using detrending. Second, wavelet-based motion artifact reduction (Molavi and Dumont, 2012; Brigadoi et al., 2014), implemented as “hmrMotionCorrectWavelet” function in the HomER2 toolbox (Huppert et al., 2009), was applied to correct for spike-like artifacts. Then, dual source-detector regression (Saager et al., 2011) was applied to regress out the shallow-tissue signal component, which is dominated by non-neuronal systemic and motion-related noises and can be captured by the 1 cm source-detector channel (Fukui et al., 2003; Strangman et al., 2014), from the 3 cm source-detector signal that contains both the non-neuronal shallow and brain-originated deep components, thus extracting the brain-originated component.

With the neural signals extracted through the preprocessing steps above, the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization for each pair of students was calculated using wavelet transform coherence (WTC) (Torrence and Compo, 1998; Grinsted et al., 2004). WTC evaluates a localized correlation coefficient in time-frequency space and is thus suitable for capturing transient and bi-directional synchronization, which is expected in unstructured naturalistic communications involving multiple individuals, such as collaborative learning settings. WTC has been used in many fNIRS hyperscanning studies to evaluate inter-brain synchronization (Scholkmann et al., 2013; Minagawa et al., 2018). For the computation, the cross wavelet and wavelet coherence toolbox (http://grinsted.github.io/wavelet-coherence/) was used.

For each student pair, inter-brain synchronization at each Fourier period (inverse of Fourier frequency) was temporally averaged over the entire duration of the group activities, except for the time points within the cone of influence to avoid any contaminating influences from edge effects (Torrence and Compo, 1998). Cerebral blood flow signals in the time scales of the so-called low-frequency oscillations (0.01–0.198 Hz) have been shown to produce more reliable estimates of within-brain neuronal synchronization than higher frequencies (Zuo et al., 2010). Furthermore, fNIRS signals in shorter time scales are known to be susceptible to cardiac pulsatory (0.8–2.5 Hz) and respiratory (0.15–0.3 Hz) noise components (Matthews et al., 2008; Lu et al., 2010). Thus, we focused on inter-brain synchronization in the period range of 6.7–100 s (equivalent to 0.01–0.15 Hz). The temporally averaged inter-brain synchronization values at each period in this range were subjected to the same permutation method as above to test hypothesis (2) that the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization is higher for within-group pairs than for cross-group pairs. False discovery rate (FDR) adjustment (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) was applied for multiple testing of the periods, and the test results were regarded as significant with q < 0.05.

To test hypothesis (3) that higher prefrontal inter-brain synchronization is positively associated with more similar flow dynamics, we conducted a correlation analysis. Based on the test of hypothesis (2) above, we identified the period range in which inter-brain synchronization was significantly higher for within-group than for cross-group pairs (thus sensitive to the collaborative interaction) as the period of interest. The inter-brain synchronization values in the period of interest were averaged for each pair of students and correlated with the similarity values of flow dynamics. A non-parametric permutation method was used to evaluate the significance (p-value) of the correlation. We generated 10,000 random permutations shuffling the students in each class. Each permutation of subjects produces a possible permutation of pairs. We tested the significance of the observed correlation against the null distribution of correlation values between the original inter-brain synchronization values and the flow dynamics similarities with the permutated pair labels. Furthermore, we also tested the correlation between inter-brain synchronization and flow dynamics after controlling for the effects of within- and cross-group pair types from the two variables (i.e., partial correlation).

We additionally evaluated the interpersonal synchronization of the non-neuronal shallow signals that were obtained from the 1 cm source-detector channel and subjected to the same preprocessing procedure, except for the dual source-detector regression. Then, for this interpersonal shallow signal synchronization, we repeated the same analyses corresponding to the testing of hypotheses (2) and (3) above. The comparison of the results with inter-brain synchronization and with interpersonal shallow signal synchronization helped us to check whether the obtained inter-brain synchronization results were indeed of neural origin.

Results

Subjective Flow Dynamics

Figure 2A shows an example of the flow dynamics for a group of four students. In this group, Student 2's flow dynamics were relatively divergent from the other members, with lower temporal correlation values with other members (z1,2 = 0.27, z2,3 = 0.30, z2,4 = 0.22), compared to the more convergent flow dynamics between the other three (z1,3 = 0.63, z1,4 = 0.76, z3,4 = 0.54; Figure 2B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Example of retrospectively rated flow dynamics in a group. (B) Flow dynamics similarity values between the members of the same group, evaluated by Fisher z-transformed temporal correlation.

For hypothesis (1) that flow dynamics would be more convergent for the within-group pairs than for the cross-group pairs, we compared the similarity of flow dynamics between within-group and cross-group pairs. A permutation test showed that the temporal correlation of flow time series between the student pairs within the same learning group was significantly higher than that between the pairs of students assigned across different learning groups (difference of means = 0.204, 95% CI = 0.019–0.388, p = 0.036, Cohen's d = 0.59; Figure 3). This indicates that the flow dynamics were significantly more similar for members of the same group than for those in different groups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Similarity of flow dynamics between pairs of students within the same active-learning group (within-group) vs. across different groups (cross-group). Asterisk indicates significant difference (p < 0.05) between the pair types.

Inter-brain Synchronization Within and Between Groups

For hypothesis (2) that inter-brain synchronization would be higher for within-group pairs than for cross-group pairs, we compared the medial prefrontal inter-brain synchronization between the within-group and cross-group pairs. On average, a higher inter-brain synchronization was found between the members of a same groups over a wide range of time scales (Figure 4). Permutation tests at each Fourier period indicated that the inter-brain synchronization difference was significant in the range of periods 9.3–13.9 s (FDR-adjusted q < 0.05). This indicates that inter-brain synchronization across these time scales was sensitive to the collaborative interaction. The mean inter-brain synchronization in this period of interest was used in the subsequent correlation analyses.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Prefrontal inter-brain synchronization (IBS) between pairs of students within the same active-learning group (within-group) and across different groups (cross-group). Solid lines show the mean and shaded areas the standard error of the mean for each pair type. Asterisks at the bottom indicates periods with a significant difference (q < 0.05; FDR-adjusted) between the pair types.

In addition, we compared the non-neuronal interpersonal shallow signal synchronization from the 1 cm source-detector channels between the within-group and cross-group pairs. No significant differences were found in interpersonal shallow signal synchronization in the period of interest identified above, though some significant differences were observed across longer time scales (35.0–44.1 s; Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Non-neuronal interpersonal shallow signal synchronization (ISSS) between pairs of students within the same active-learning group (within-group) and across different groups (cross-group). Solid lines show the mean and shaded areas the standard error of the mean for each pair type. Asterisks at the bottom indicates periods with a significant difference (q < 0.05; FDR-adjusted) between the pair types.

Correlation Between Flow Dynamics Similarity and Inter-brain Synchronization

Regarding hypothesis (3) that higher prefrontal inter-brain synchronization is positively associated with more similar flow dynamics, we tested the correlation between mean inter-brain synchronization in the period of interest (9.3–13.9 s) and the similarity in flow dynamics amongst pairs of students. This revealed a significant positive correlation (r = 0.36, p = 0.004; Figure 6A). Since both the similarity of flow dynamics and the inter-brain synchronization were higher for the within-group than cross-group pairs, this correlation would have been at least partially mediated by the effects of pair types (within- vs. cross-group). Therefore, we also tested a modified version of hypothesis (3), which is orthogonal to hypotheses (1) and (2) by evaluating the partial correlation between flow dynamics similarity and inter-brain synchronization after controlling for the effects of pair types. The result remained significant (r = 0.28, p = 0.022; Figure 6B), indicating a direct positive association between inter-brain synchronization and flow dynamics similarity.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. (A) Raw relationship between the mean inter-brain synchronization (IBS) in the period of interest (9.3–13.9 s) and the similarity of flow dynamics over the pairs. (B) Relationship between the mean IBS in the same period of interest and the similarity of flow dynamics after controlling for the effects of within-group vs. cross-group pair types. Solid line and shared area in each panel indicate the linear regression fit and its 95% level interval, respectively.

Correlation analyses were repeated with the non-neuronal interpersonal shallow signal synchronization. There were no significant correlations with flow dynamics similarity (Figures 7A–D), supporting the neural origin of the observed association between inter-brain synchronization and shared flow dynamics.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. (A) Raw relationship between the mean interpersonal shallow signal synchronization (ISSS) in the period of interest (9.3–13.9 s) and the similarity of flow dynamics over the pairs. (B) Relationship between the mean ISSS in the same period of interest and the similarity of flow dynamics after controlling for the effects of within-group vs. cross-group pair types. (C) Raw relationship between the mean ISSS in the period range 35.0–44.1 s where ISSS was significantly higher for within-group than cross-group pairs and the similarity of flow dynamics over the pairs. (D) Relationship between the mean ISSS in the period range 35.0–44.1 s and the similarity of flow dynamics after controlling for the effects of pair types. Solid line and shared area in each panel indicate the linear regression fit and its 95% level interval, respectively. No significant correlations were observed.

Discussion

In this study, we analyzed the similarity or sharing of fluctuating flow experiences and prefrontal inter-brain synchronization among students in real collaborative learning activities held in EFL classes in university education. In accordance with our hypotheses, we found that (1) flow dynamics were more convergent for the within-group than the cross-group pairs; (2) prefrontal inter-brain synchronization was higher among group members who directly collaborated than between cross-group learners who were just in the same class; and (3) prefrontal inter-brain synchronization was significantly positively correlated with the flow dynamics similarity, even after controlling for the effects of pair types. Below, we discuss the possible mechanisms and implications of these results, along with limitations and future directions.

Shared Flow Dynamics in Collaboration

We observed that the temporal correlation of the flow state fluctuation was higher for the within-group pairs, indicating that collaborative learning enhanced the sharing of flow dynamics. Multiple and inter-related processes can lead to shared flow dynamics in collaborating groups. First, as participants do the group work together, they face objectively easy and difficult parts at the same time. This will lead to a temporal alignment of perceived challenges, especially among group members with similar skill levels. Furthermore, collaboration leads to the assimilation of effective skills and perceived challenges between members. For example, when a task is too difficult for a student to solve alone, help from other group members can work as a type of “scaffolding” and effectively boost the student's skill level to meet the challenge. In comparison, even when the task is too easy for one member, helping and guiding a peer who is having difficulties will pose additional challenges and make the moment less boring. This kind of mutual adjustment toward the challenge-skill balance can be a function of collaborative learning (Watanabe and Swain, 2007; Storch, 2009; Walker, 2010). In addition, group members share moments of perceived achievement, and interaction can also cause emotional contagion (Barsade, 2002), both of which lead to temporally aligned affective processes. Taken together, the shared flow dynamics in collaborative learning groups could be regarded as a signature of high integration and unity between group members.

We should note that the shared flow dynamics invoked in this study are based on the existence of temporal fluctuations in individual flow states and their similarity between learners. Thus, even if the average flow level of the two learners was high, the similarity of flow dynamics could have been low if their fluctuations were not temporally aligned. Our shared flow dynamics also overlap with the concept of “group flow.” However, the definition of group flow is not limited to sharing of individual flow and is more heterogeneous, with different emphasis on various individual and collective perspectives depending on the study context (Pels et al., 2018).

Inter-brain Synchronization and Its Relation to Shared Flow Dynamics

Prefrontal inter-brain synchronization was also higher in the within-group than in the cross-group pairs. This result is consistent with previous hyperscanning studies that found both verbal and non-verbal cooperative interactions enhance prefrontal inter-brain synchronization between interacting individuals (Cui et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2016; Nozawa et al., 2016, 2019; Ikeda et al., 2017; Li et al., 2020; Sun et al., 2020). The underlying mechanisms for the enhanced inter-brain synchronization between group members may include the temporally aligned perception of challenge and achievement, which were discussed above as the likely processes underlying shared flow dynamics. In addition, temporal alignment of more fundamental cognitive processes during collaborative interactions, such as perception and understanding of each other's utterances, mentalizing each other's thoughts and intentions, and shared attention to tasks and salient events during the group activity, could also contribute to higher inter-brain synchronization within learning groups in high mutual engagement.

Among the time scales of low frequency oscillations (Zuo et al., 2010), the inter-brain synchronization differences between the within-group and cross-group pairs was significant in a relatively short time scale, overlapping with the so-called “slow-3” sub-band (0.073–0.198 Hz or 5.1–13.7 s), but not in the longer time scales. This is perhaps not surprising given that the same group work was carried out in parallel among the groups in the same class. Furthermore, all the students shared major cognitive events at longer time scales, such as the switch timings between group activities (GW1–4), general from-beginning-to-end progress pattern in each group work, and instructions and advice given by the teacher to the whole class. On the other hand, occurrences and timings of more detailed interaction events, challenges, and their solutions/achievements would have been more likely to vary among groups, leading to higher within-group inter-brain synchronization on a shorter time scale.

Finally, we observed that the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization at the time scale with sensitivity to the within-group collaborative interaction was significantly and positively correlated with the similarity of flow dynamics over the pairs. This remained true even after controlling for the mediating effects of the pair types on the two variables, and thus supports our hypothesis that convergence and divergence of flow dynamics between learners in collaborative tasks are reflected in the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization. From a neuroergonomic viewpoint, our results suggest a promising possibility of utilizing the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization for detection of an “isolated learner” in terms of their flow dynamics within a group (as exemplified in Figure 2). In addition, prefrontal inter-brain synchronization could be used for comparative and longitudinal evaluation of convergent/divergent flow dynamics on a group level. Such information could help teachers in deciding to take specific actions to facilitate flow, as well as learners to improve their manner of collaboration.

Additional analysis revealed that the interpersonal synchronization of the non-neuronal shallow signals was significantly higher for the within-group than for the cross-group pairs in the period range of 35.0–44.1 s. This time scale was longer than the period of interest (9.3–13.9 s) in which the significantly enhanced inter-brain synchronization for the within group was observed. A previous study on the frequency content of skin blood flow (Söderström et al., 2003) showed that sympathetic nerve control is involved in oscillations of skin blood flow signals in the overlapping time scale (“interval II”; 0.02–0.05 Hz or 20–50 s). Consistently, another study (Kirilina et al., 2013) identified a significant contribution of the scalp blood flow fluctuations in the forehead at the corresponding time scale (“A-band”; 0.02–0.04 Hz or 25–50 s) to the forehead fNIRS signals, and called for caution on the potential influence of sympathetic control. These suggest that the collaborative interaction induced interpersonal synchronization in sympathetic nerve regulation. On the other hand, the interpersonal shallow signal synchronization in either the shorter or longer time scales revealed no significant correlation with flow dynamics similarity. These results indicate that the sharing of flow dynamics is better captured by the interpersonal synchronization of the prefrontal cortical activities rather than that of the autonomic blood flow regulation processes.

Limitations and Future Directions

The limitations of this study and possible future research directions should be noted. First, this study used a relatively small sample size and a specific activity design for collaborative learning. It is left for future studies to replicate the current results in a wider range of learning situations. In addition, we conducted the analyses using the student pair or dyad as a unit, because it is the basic unit whereby the sharing of flow dynamics and inter-brain synchronization can be defined. (Note that the statistical dependence structure in the dyadic data has been controlled for by the subject-based permutation procedures.) On the other hand, investigating the relationship between the inter-brain synchronization and shared flow dynamics at the group level would be interesting since students work as groups on collaborative learning. For this purpose, a larger sample with more groups or a repetition of multiple classroom sessions will be needed. Second, the fNIRS recording site in this study was limited to the mPFC. It will be meaningful to shed light on the relationship between the shared flow dynamics and the inter-brain synchronization in other cortical areas involved in social cognitions and interaction, such as the temporoparietal junction, superior temporal gyrus/sulcus, inferior frontal gyrus and so on. In addition, it would be more convincing to demonstrate that some areas contribute to the shared experience, whereas other areas do not. Next, although the degree of subjective experience sharing and inter-brain synchronization were correlated, this does not prove that inter-brain synchronization is the mechanism behind a shared flow experience. To understand the dynamic process of sharing flow and to determine how much significant role inter-brain synchronization plays in the process as “hidden variables” that cannot be observed externally (Kingsbury and Hong, 2020), simultaneous measurement of behavioral interactions between learners as well as individual systemic physiological activities could help. This can lead to the further endeavor of exploring how informative those peripheral signals could be in predicting flow and its sharing, and how such information could be overlapping with or independent from the information conveyed by inter-brain synchronization. (The video recordings for retrospective rating of flow dynamics in this study were limited and uneven in terms of the view coverage and directions for the target group members, thus not adequate for such behavioral analysis.) Another remaining issue is the impact of shared flow on various outcomes in group learning. Recent studies have begun to elucidate how inter-brain synchronization captured by EEG and fNIRS hyperscanning correlates with better team performance in several settings, including visual search tasks (Szymanski et al., 2017), collaborative problem solving (Antonenko et al., 2019; Reinero et al., 2021), and learning success in teacher-student settings (Holper et al., 2013; Pan et al., 2018, 2020; Liu et al., 2019). However, it remains to be confirmed whether shared experiences with peers, as marked by the prefrontal inter-brain synchronization, can inform success or failure in a learner achieving their goals.

Despite the above limitations, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that prefrontal inter-brain synchronization during collaborative learning can be an objective marker for the convergence and divergence of flow dynamics between learners. In the future, prefrontal inter-brain synchronization could be used to evaluate the convergent engagement of students in active group learning tasks, with proper feedback provided to the teacher or the students themselves to facilitate further collaboration. The technology may also be applicable to the detection of left-behind students in lecture-style learning settings as well as in the promotion of a wider range of collaborative activities. We are hoping to extend this study toward such practical neuroergonomic applications in the field of education. This pursuit will be benefited from and contribute to the wide effort to overcome the grand challenges for neuroergonomics in general, such as the improvement and combining of sensors, incorporation of advanced analytical methods and artificial intelligence approaches, development of design principles and techniques for better user experience, and consensus building for privacy and ethical issues (Dehais et al., 2020; Fairclough and Lotte, 2020).

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TN contributed to the study design, experimentation, analysis, and manuscript preparation. MK contributed to the study design, experimentation, organization, and instruction in the EFL classes. RY and YI contributed to the study design, experimentation, and preparation of the EFL classes. HJ, SI, and KS contributed to the study design and experimentation. YM contributed to the interpretation of the results. RK contributed to the supervision and provided useful discussions throughout the project. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was partially supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP16K13274, JP15H01771, JP17H01753, JP20H03553, and also by JST-COI Grant Number of JPMJCE1309 from the Japan Science and Technology Agency.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Kyoto University of Foreign Studies and all the students in the measured classes for their kind cooperation.

References

Antonenko, P. D., Davis, R., Wang, J., and Celepkolu, M. (2019). On the same wavelength: exploring team neurosynchrony in undergraduate dyads solving a cyberlearning problem with collaborative scripts. Mind Brain Educ. 13, 4–13. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effect: emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Adm. Sci. Q. 47, 644–675. doi: 10.2307/3094912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bevilacqua, D., Davidesco, I., Wan, L., Oostrik, M., Chaloner, K., Rowland, J., et al. (2018). Brain-to-brain synchrony and learning outcomes vary by student–teacher dynamics: evidence from a real-world classroom electroencephalography study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 401–411. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brigadoi, S., Ceccherini, L., Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., Selb, J., et al. (2014). Motion artifacts in functional near-infrared spectroscopy: a comparison of motion correction techniques applied to real cognitive data. Neuroimage 85(Pt 1), 181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brockington, G., Balardin, J. B., Zimeo Morais, G. A., Malheiros, A., Lent, R., Moura, L. M., et al. (2018). From the laboratory to the classroom: the potential of functional near-infrared spectroscopy in educational neuroscience. Front. Psychol. 9:1840. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil, I., Catalán, S., and Martínez, E. (2019). The influence of flow on learning outcomes: an empirical study on the use of clickers. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 50, 428–439. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding Flow: The Psychology of Engagement With Everyday Life. New York, NY: Basic Books. Available online at: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1997-08434-000 (accessed September 17, 2017).

Cui, X., Bryant, D. M., and Reiss, A. L. (2012). NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. Neuroimage 59, 2430–2437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehais, F., Karwowski, W., and Ayaz, H. (2020). Brain at work and in everyday life as the next frontier: grand field challenges for neuroergonomics. Front. Neuroergon. 1:583733. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2020.583733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delpy, D. T., Cope, M., Zee, P., van der Arridge, S., Wray, S., and Wyatt, J. (1988). Estimation of optical pathlength through tissue from direct time of flight measurement. Phys. Med. Biol. 33, 1433–1442. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/33/12/008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., et al. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 27, 1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairclough, S. H., and Lotte, F. (2020). Grand challenges in neurotechnology and system neuroergonomics. Front. Neuroergon. 1:602504. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2020.602504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukui, Y., Ajichi, Y., and Okada, E. (2003). Monte Carlo prediction of near-infrared light propagation in realistic adult and neonatal head models. Appl. Opt. 42, 2881–2887. doi: 10.1364/AO.42.002881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., et al. (2006). Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 932–948. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grinsted, A., Moore, J. C., and Jevrejeva, S. (2004). Application of the cross wavelet transform and wavelet coherence to geophysical time series. Nonlinear Process. Geophys. 11, 561–566. doi: 10.5194/npg-11-561-2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, J., Tiede, M., Zhang, X., Noah, J. A., Salama-Manteau, A., and Biriotti, M. (2021). Interpersonal agreement and disagreement during face-to-face dialogue: an fNIRS investigation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:606397. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.606397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holper, L., Goldin, A. P., Shalóm, D. E., Battro, A. M., Wolf, M., and Sigman, M. (2013). The teaching and the learning brain: a cortical hemodynamic marker of teacher–student interactions in the Socratic dialog. Int. J. Educ. Res. 59, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2013.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Homan, R. W., Herman, J., and Purdy, P. (1987). Cerebral location of international 10-20 system electrode placement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 66, 376–382. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90206-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., and Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 48, D280–D298. doi: 10.1364/AO.48.00D280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikeda, S., Nozawa, T., Yokoyama, R., Miyazaki, A., Sasaki, Y., Sakaki, K., et al. (2017). Steady beat sound facilitates both coordinated group walking and inter-subject neural synchrony. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:147. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Järvelä, S., Volet, S., and Järvenoja, H. (2010). Research on motivation in collaborative learning: moving beyond the cognitive–situative divide and combining individual and social processes. Educ. Psychol. 45, 15–27. doi: 10.1080/00461520903433539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Chen, C., Dai, B., Shi, G., Ding, G., Liu, L., et al. (2015). Leader emergence through interpersonal neural synchronization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 4274–4279. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422930112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Dai, B., Peng, D., Zhu, C., Liu, L., and Lu, C. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. J. Neurosci. 32, 16064–16069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kent, A. (2013). Synchronization as a classroom dynamic: a practitioner's perspective. Mind Brain Educ. 7, 13–18. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kingsbury, L., and Hong, W. (2020). A multi-brain framework for social interaction. Trends Neurosci. 43, 651–666. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirilina, E., Yu, N., Jelzow, A., Wabnitz, H., Jacobs, A. M., and Tachtsidis, I. (2013). Identifying and quantifying main components of physiological noise in functional near infrared spectroscopy on the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:864. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landhäußer, A., and Keller, J. (2012). “Flow and its affective, cognitive, and performance-related consequences,” in Advances in Flow Research, ed S. Engeser (New York, NY: Springer), 65–85.

Google Scholar

Li, L., Wang, H., Luo, H., Zhang, X., Zhang, R., and Li, X. (2020). Interpersonal Neural synchronization during cooperative behavior of basketball players: a fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:169. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Zhang, R., Geng, B., Zhang, T., Yuan, D., Otani, S., et al. (2019). Interplay between prior knowledge and communication mode on teaching effectiveness: interpersonal neural synchronization as a neural marker. NeuroImage 193, 93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, N., Mok, C., Witt, E. E., Pradhan, A. H., Chen, J. E., and Reiss, A. L. (2016). NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals inter-brain neural synchronization during cooperative jenga game with face-to-face communication. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:82. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, C.-M., Zhang, Y.-J., Biswal, B. B., Zang, Y.-F., Peng, D.-L., and Zhu, C.-Z. (2010). Use of fNIRS to assess resting state functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. Methods 186, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, F., Pearlmutter, B. A., Ward, T. E., Soraghan, C., and Markham, C. (2008). Hemodynamics for brain-computer interfaces: optical correlates of control signals. IEEE Signal Process. Mag. 25, 87–94. doi: 10.1109/MSP.2008.4408445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minagawa, Y., Xu, M., and Morimoto, S. (2018). Toward interactive social neuroscience: neuroimaging real-world interactions in various populations. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 60, 196–224. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molavi, B., and Dumont, G. A. (2012). Wavelet-based motion artifact removal for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Physiol. Meas. 33, 259–270. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/33/2/259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nam, C. S., Choo, S., Huang, J., and Park, J. (2020). Brain-to-brain neural synchrony during social interactions: a systematic review on hyperscanning studies. Appl. Sci. 10:6669. doi: 10.3390/app10196669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozawa, T., Sakaki, K., Ikeda, S., Jeong, H., Yamazaki, S., Santos Kawata, K. H., et al. (2019). Prior physical synchrony enhances rapport and inter-brain synchronization during subsequent educational communication. Sci. Rep. 9:12747. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49257-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozawa, T., Sasaki, Y., Sakaki, K., Yokoyama, R., and Kawashima, R. (2016). Interpersonal frontopolar neural synchronization in group communication: an exploration toward fNIRS hyperscanning of natural interactions. Neuroimage 133, 484–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Y., Dikker, S., Goldstein, P., Zhu, Y., Yang, C., and Hu, Y. (2020). Instructor-learner brain coupling discriminates between instructional approaches and predicts learning. NeuroImage 211:116657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Y., Novembre, G., Song, B., Li, X., and Hu, Y. (2018). Interpersonal synchronization of inferior frontal cortices tracks social interactive learning of a song. Neuroimage 183, 280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pels, F., Kleinert, J., and Mennigen, F. (2018). Group flow: a scoping review of definitions, theoretical approaches, measures, and findings. PLoS ONE 13:e0210117. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinero, D. A., Dikker, S., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2021). Inter-brain synchrony in teams predicts collective performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 43–57. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossin, D., Ro, Y., Klein, B., and Guo, Y. (2009). The effects of flow on learning outcomes in an online information management cou. J. Inf. Syst. Educ. 20, 87–98. Available online at: https://aisel.aisnet.org/jise/vol20/iss1/9/

Google Scholar

Saager, R. B., Telleri, N. L., and Berger, A. J. (2011). Two-detector Corrected Near Infrared Spectroscopy (C-NIRS) detects hemodynamic activation responses more robustly than single-detector NIRS. Neuroimage 55, 1679–1685. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Holper, L., Wolf, U., and Wolf, M. (2013). A new methodical approach in neuroscience: assessing inter-personal brain coupling using functional near-infrared imaging (fNIRI) hyperscanning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:813. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Kleiser, S., Metz, A. J., Zimmermann, R., Mata Pavia, J., Wolf, U., et al. (2014). A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. Neuroimage 85(Pt 1), 6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Shneider, B., and Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. Sch. Psychol. Q. 18, 158–176. doi: 10.1521/scpq.18.2.158.21860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Söderström, T., Stefanovska, A., Veber, M., and Svensson, H. (2003). Involvement of sympathetic nerve activity in skin blood flow oscillations in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 284, H1638–H1646. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00826.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, J. P., and Fullagar, C. J. (2009). Facilitators and outcomes of student engagement in a college setting. J. Psychol. 143, 5–27. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.143.1.5-27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storch, N. (2009). The Nature of Pair Interaction: Learners' Interaction in an ESL Class: Its Nature and Impact on Grammatical Development. Saarbrücken: VDM Publishing. Available online at: https://www.amazon.com/nature-pair-interaction-grammatical-development/dp/3639143418 (accessed March 18, 2021).

Strangman, G. E., Zhang, Q., and Li, Z. (2014). Scalp and skull influence on near infrared photon propagation in the Colin27 brain template. Neuroimage 85(Pt 1), 136–149. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suda, M., Takei, Y., Aoyama, Y., Narita, K., Sato, T., Fukuda, M., et al. (2010). Frontopolar activation during face-to-face conversation: an in situ study using near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychologia 48, 441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, B., Xiao, W., Feng, X., Shao, Y., Zhang, W., and Li, W. (2020). Behavioral and brain synchronization differences between expert and novice teachers when collaborating with students. Brain Cogn. 139:105513. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.105513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymanski, C., Pesquita, A., Brennan, A. A., Perdikis, D., Enns, J. T., Brick, T. R., et al. (2017). Teams on the same wavelength perform better: inter-brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation. Neuroimage 152, 425–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrence, C., and Compo, G. P. (1998). A practical guide to wavelet analysis. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 79, 61–78.

Google Scholar

Walker, C. J. (2010). Experiencing flow: is doing it together better than doing it alone? J. Posit. Psychol. 5, 3–11. doi: 10.1080/17439760903271116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, Y., and Swain, M. (2007). Effects of proficiency differences and patterns of pair interaction on second language learning: collaborative dialogue between adult ESL learners. Lang. Teach. Res. 11, 121–142. doi: 10.1177/136216880607074599

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Webster, M. A., Vidaurre, D., Nichols, T. E., and Smith, S. M. (2015). Multi-Level block permutation. Neuroimage 123, 253–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, R., Nozawa, T., Jeong, H., Ikeda, S., Sakaki, K., Kawashima, R., et al. (2015). “An investigation of brain synchrony between students and their teacher during a secondary school EFL lesson,” in Breaking Theory: New Directions in Applied Linguistics, 201 (London). Available online at: http://martinedwardes.webplus.net/scitsiugnil/proceedings_2015.pdf#page=211 (accessed February 16, 2017).

Google Scholar

Zuo, X.-N., Martino, A. D., Kelly, C., Shehzad, Z. E., Gee, D. G., Klein, D. F., et al. (2010). The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 49, 1432–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: shared flow, inter-brain synchronization, fNIRS hyperscanning, collaborative learning, engagement, dynamics

Citation: Nozawa T, Kondo M, Yamamoto R, Jeong H, Ikeda S, Sakaki K, Miyake Y, Ishikawa Y and Kawashima R (2021) Prefrontal Inter-brain Synchronization Reflects Convergence and Divergence of Flow Dynamics in Collaborative Learning: A Pilot Study. Front. Neuroergon. 2:686596. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2021.686596

Received: 27 March 2021; Accepted: 11 May 2021;
Published: 03 June 2021.

Edited by:

Domen Novak, University of Wyoming, United States

Reviewed by:

Jack Adam Noah, Yale University, United States
Frederic Dehais, Institut Supérieur de l'Aéronautique et de l'Espace (ISAE-SUPAERO), France
Suzanne Dikker, New York University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Nozawa, Kondo, Yamamoto, Jeong, Ikeda, Sakaki, Miyake, Ishikawa and Kawashima. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Takayuki Nozawa, nozawa.t.ac@m.titech.ac.jp

Download