Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 30 April 2024
Sec. Children and Health
Volume 12 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2024.1369690

Cognitive performance in children and adolescents with primary hypertension and the role of body mass

  • 1Vilnius University Hospital Santaros Klinikos, Vilnius, Lithuania
  • 2Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Vilnius University, Vilnius, Lithuania

Objective: Primary hypertension has been shown to affect cognitive functions in adults but evidence in the pediatric population remain scarce and equivocal. We aimed to compare cognitive functioning between children diagnosed with primary hypertension and normotensive controls, with a focus on the role of different blood pressure (BP) parameters and body mass.

Methods: We conducted a single-center, prospective, cross-sectional study of children and adolescents (6–17 years old) with primary hypertension and age- and sex-matched normotensive controls. All participants underwent office BP, ambulatory BP monitoring (ABPM), and central BP measurements using an oscillometric device. Neurocognitive assessment consisted of evaluation of (i) intelligence quotient (IQ), (ii) categorical and phonemic fluency, (iii) verbal memory (verbal-logical story recall), and (iv) non-verbal computerized cognitive assessment.

Results: The study included a total of 59 patients with primary hypertension (14 ± 3 years) and 37 normotensive controls (14 ± 3 years). Participants in the primary hypertension group had a significantly higher body mass index z-score (BMIz: 2.1 ± 1.4 vs. 0.7 ± 0.9, p < 0.001), and 85% received antihypertensive therapy. Participants with primary hypertension showed worse performance in the domains of reaction speed, attention and processing speed, visual memory, new learning, and phonemic fluency. After adjusting for BMIz, only the differences in the reaction speed tasks remained significant. None of the BP parameters was associated with cognitive outcomes after adjustment for age, sex, and BMIz. BMIz associated with tasks of visual memory, new learning, spatial planning, and working memory, independent of age and sex.

Conclusion: Children and adolescents diagnosed with primary hypertension exhibit worse performance in the cognitive domains of reaction speed, attention, processing speed, visual memory, and new learning. These differences to healthy controls can be partially attributed to accompanying increase of body mass.

1 Introduction

The rising prevalence of primary hypertension in children and adolescents is recognized as an important global health concern. The prevalence is around four percent in those aged six years but becomes particularly high in the obese population, where almost one in six adolescents is hypertensive (1). The significance of early life blood pressure (BP) elevation is well reflected by studies reporting that childhood BP tracks into adulthood and is associated with cardiovascular outcomes, including hypertension-mediated organ damage (HMOD) (24). The latter is already apparent in childhood, including changes in left ventricular and vascular structure and function (57). Moreover, evidence suggests that pediatric primary hypertension is not a condition characterized by elevated BP alone but represents a phenotype accompanied by changes in body composition, metabolic abnormalities, immune-inflammatory alterations and increased sympathetic activity (8). Thus, the effects of primary hypertension diagnosis on other organ systems can extend beyond the isolated effects of elevated BP, as previously shown in studies of vascular alterations (5).

Brain has been described as an important target of HMOD in the adult population with relatively well-defined pathophysiologic mechanisms, including but not limited to cerebral ischemia, (micro)hemorrhages, atrophy, microvascular rarefaction and endothelial dysfunction (9). However, the evidence in the pediatric population still remains scarce (10). Exposure to elevated BP in the susceptible brain microvasculature can cause various (micro)structural injuries, particularly in the prefrontal cortex, which may affect cognitive functioning, but this remains relatively understudied (10). This may be particularly important during childhood, a critical period for cognitive development (11). The latter represents a complex and dynamic phenomenon that is also strongly influenced by socio-demographic determinants, including parental education, socioeconomic status, parenting practices, as well as cultural aspects (11, 12).

A recent systematic review indicated that the association of primary hypertension with various cognitive functioning domain deficits is already present in childhood, but the evidence is heterogenous (10). Prior studies were frequently based on indirect (proxy-reported) measurement of cognitive performance (1317) that may not reflect subtle dysfunction. Notably, none of the studies employed a comprehensive computerized cognitive assessment that would encompass wide range of cognitive domains that may be differentially affected in the state of primary hypertension (10). Finally, the role of potential hemodynamic modifiers of the effects, such as measures of central hemodynamics (central or aortic BP) that the brain is directly exposed to (18) has been only studied in one pediatric study reporting associations with indirect, proxy-reported cognitive outcomes (13). Elevated body mass and obesity that are observed in half of children and adolescents with primary hypertension (19) may have an independent effect cognitive performance in the pediatric population as previously shown in studies focusing on body mass and cognitive outcomes in childhood (20).

In the light of existing evidence and uncertainties, we performed an exploratory study focusing on the effects of real-word diagnosis of primary hypertension on a wide spectrum of cognitive performance markers in children and adolescents. The primary aim was to compare cognitive performance between children and adolescents with a diagnosis of primary hypertension and normotensive controls using a comprehensive neuropsychological testing battery. As a secondary aim, we sought to investigate the potential differential effects of different BP measures (including central BP) and body mass on various cognitive domains.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Design and setting

We conducted a single-center, cross-sectional study of children and adolescents with primary hypertension and age- and sex-matched normotensive controls at a ratio of 2:1. The study enrolled a convenience sample of subjects from a tertiary care hospital (Vilnius University Hospital Santaros klinikos) from June 2021 to June 2023. The following inclusion criteria were used: (i) age 6–17 years and (ii) confirmed primary hypertension (except for normotensive controls). Patients with newly diagnosed primary hypertension following referral for elevated BP and those patients with a prior diagnosis of primary hypertension undergoing pharmacological therapy were included. All participants (including controls) with (i) clinical sensorimotor, neurologic or neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., autism spectrum disorder; including the use of medications for these disorders), (ii) pre-existing structural cardiac disease and (iii) diabetes mellitus were excluded. Primary hypertension was confirmed according to the European Society of Hypertension (ESH) guidelines and all participants in the primary hypertension group underwent examinations to exclude potential secondary causes of arterial hypertension as recommended (10). Participants were invited to take part in the study during their visits in the pediatric hypertension outpatient clinic by the treating physician. After obtaining the informed consent, all participants underwent a thorough cardiovascular and neuropsychological assessment over two days as described further.

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by Vilnius Regional Bioethics Committee (approval no. 2021/5-1348-821). The participation in the study was completely voluntarily and all participants (and their parents or legal guardians) were informed that refusing to participate will not influence their care. All parents of the participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study. Assents to participate in the study were also collected in written form from adolescents aged 12 years and older by providing them with an adapted informed assent form as required by national regulations.

2.2 Data sources and measurements

2.2.1 BMI and BP measurements

Body mass index (BMI) was determined by measuring body weight and height, and further standardized to age and sex by calculating z-scores (BMIz) using the LMS method (21) according to the World Health Organization reference data. Obesity and overweight were respectively defined as BMIz of >2 and >1, respectively (22). Office BP was measured according to ESH recommendations using an oscillometric device validated for use in children and appropriately sized cuffs (23). Office BP values were then standardized to age, sex and height according to previously published regression equations from the Fourth Report on the Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure in Children and Adolescents (24). Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring (ABPM) was performed using Spacelabs Healthcare OnTrak Intertek 315762 device. Ambulatory BP measurements were performed at intervals of 15 min during the awake period and every 30 min during sleep. The measurement was considered acceptable with at least 70% successful measurements throughout the entire 24 h period. Central systolic BP was measured using an oscillometric device (Mobil-o-Graph, IEM) that underwent validation studies in children (25, 26) and was calibrated to brachial systolic and diastolic BP as also in the reference values study (27). Both, ABPM and central systolic BP values were then standardized to age and sex by calculating z-scores using the LMS method (21) according to published reference data by Wühl et al. and Elmenhorst et al., respectively (27, 28).

2.2.2 Neurocognitive assessment

Neurocognitive assessments were conducted over two days. On the initial day, tests were administered for categorical and phonemic fluency and verbal memory. Following this, a computerised cognitive assessment utilizing Computerized cognitive assessment with the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Cambridge Cognition, Ltd) was conducted. On the second day, participants were administered the WASI–II task battery. These assessments were performed by a licensed clinical psychologist and lasted approximately 45 min each day. Each participant was assessed individually in a peaceful and quiet room to minimise distractions. Parents were requested to wait outside until the evaluations were complete. Cognitive tasks were administered during the first half of the day, and the family were informed to ensure their children had a good night's rest and breakfast before the assessments.

Neurocognitive assessment consisted of evaluation of both verbal and non-verbal domains:

(1) Intelligence quotient (IQ) assessed using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-II (WASI-II);

(2) Categorical and phonemic fluency (the number of nouns pronounced in one minute in a particular semantic category or starting in the same letter);

(3) Verbal memory [recall of a short 24-item verbal-logical story recall (VLS) immediately after reading, after 30 min and after 24 h];

(4) This part was dedicated to evaluate the following domains (a detailed description of each test and associated variables is presented in Supplementary Table S1; dependent variables for each of the domains are provided in parentheses):

(a) Attention and processing speed (Match to Sample Visual Search, MTS: MTSPS82, MTSRCAMD);

(b) Visual memory and new learning (Paired Associates Learning, PAL: PALFAMS28, PALTEA28);

(c) Motor and mental response speed, reaction time, response accuracy and impulsivity (Reaction Time Task, RTI: RTIFMDMT, RTIFMDRT, RTISMDMT, RTISMDRT);

(d) Sustained attention (Rapid Visual Information Processing, RVP: RVPA, RVPMDL, RVPPFA);

(e) Spatial planning and working memory (Stockings of Cambridge, SOC: SOCITMD5, SOCMNM5, SOCPSMMT, SOCSTMD5);

(f) Working memory capacity (Spatial Span, SSP: SSPFSL);

(g) Working memory and strategy use (Spatial Working Memory, SWM: SWMBE4, SWMBE468, SWMBE6, SWMBE8, SWMS).

2.3 Statistical analysis

Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and Shapiro–Wilk tests were used to assess the normality of continuous data. Continuous data were described as means (standard deviations) or medians (interquartile range) depending on the normality of distribution. Categorical data were described as frequencies. Group comparisons of continuous data were performed by employing parametric (Student's t-test) or non-parametric (Mann–Whitney U) tests, whereas proportions were compared using the χ2 test. Correlations between two continuous variables were estimated using Spearman's rank correlation coefficients. Multivariable linear models were built to test the associations between exposure and outcomes, and to adjust for potential confounders. The independence of the association between the group variable (primary hypertension or control group) and cognitive variables was assessed through analysis of (co)variance [AN(C)OVA] models. The latter was also used to differentiate between different group [(i) primary hypertension and normotensive controls or (ii) normal weight and overweight/obese] effects on cognitive outcomes (as detailed in the Methods section).

A two-sided p-value of <0.05 was considered significant. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS v26.

3 Results

3.1 Participant characteristics

The study included a total of 59 patients with primary hypertension (14 ± 3 years, 71% boys) and 37 normotensive controls (14 ± 3 years, 65% boys). The groups were comparable in terms of age (p = 0.96) and sex (p = 0.5). Patients with primary hypertension had higher BMIz (2.1 ± 1.4 vs. 0.7 ± 0.9, p < 0.001) and were more frequently obese than the control group (58% vs. 5%, p < 0.001). The primary hypertension group also had higher office systolic BP z-score (SBPz), higher 24-h, daytime and nighttime BPz, and central SBPz. Fifty (85% percent) patients in the primary hypertension group were taking antihypertensive medication with the majority receiving a single agent. A more detailed description of the study population is presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

3.2 Cognitive performance comparison between primary hypertension and control groups

Participants with primary hypertension showed worse performance in several domains of the CANTAB tasks, including reaction time (RTISMDMT and RTIFMDMT), attention and processing speed (MTSRCAMD), and visual memory and new learning (PALTEA28 and PALFAMS28). Additionally, worse phonemic fluency was observed. After adjustment of between-group differences for BMIz, only the differences for reaction speed tasks (RTISMDMT and RTIFMDMT) remained significant (p = 0.03 and p = 0.009, respectively) (Figures 1, 2). There were no differences in categorical fluency, verbal memory, IQ values or other CANTAB tasks (all p > 0.05). A detailed comparison of all cognitive measures between the primary hypertension and control groups is provided in Table 2.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Reaction time (RTISMDMT) comparison between primary hypertension and control groups.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Reaction time (RTIFMDMT) comparison between primary hypertension and control groups.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of neurocognitive performance results between primary hypertension and control groups.

3.3 Associations between different BP parameters and cognitive performance in the overall group

In the overall group, office SBPz did not correlate with any of the cognitive outcomes, while office diastolic BPz correlated with better spatial working memory results [ρ = 0.25, p < 0.02 (SWMS)]. Central SBPz correlated with worse phonemic fluency (ρ = −0.25; p = 0.03), worse verbal memory (VLS at all three time points: ρ = −0.28, ρ = −0.27 and ρ = −0.29; p = 0.01, p = 0.02 and p = 0.01, respectively), worse sustained attention [ρ = −0.23, p = 0.04 (RVPA)], better spatial planning and working memory tasks [ρ = −0.24, p = 0.04 (SOCITMD5)] and worse working memory capacity [ρ = −0.30, p = 0.007 (SSPFSL)]. Of the 24-h ABPM parameters, 24-h SBPz correlated with better verbal memory (VLS at 24-h: r = 0.23, p = 0.04), while 24-h diastolic BPz correlated with better sustained attention [r = −0.25, p = 0.02 (RVPPFA)] and 24-h mean arterial BPz also correlated with better sustained attention [r = −0.213, p = 0.01 (RVPA)]. None of the associations remained after adjusting for age, sex and BMIz in multiple linear regression models.

3.4 Associations between BMIz and cognitive performance in the overall group

Higher BMIz correlated with worse phonemic fluency (ρ = −0.22, p = 0.03), worse visual memory and new learning [ρ = −0.28, p = 0.008 (PALFAMS28), ρ = 0.23, p = 0.03 (PALTEA28)] and worse performance in spatial planning and working memory tasks [ρ = 0.28, p = 0.007 (SOCMNM5), ρ = −0.22, p = 0.04 (SOCPSMMT), ρ = 0.27, p = 0.01 (SOCSTMD5)]. After adjusting for age and sex in linear regression models, this relationship remained statistically significant for visual memory and new learning [β = −0.28, p = 0.008 (PALFAMS28)], spatial planning and working memory [β = 0.29, p = 0.007 (SOCMNM5), β = −0.22, p = 0.03 (SOCPSMMT)], but not for other measures (phonemic fluency, PALTEA28 and SOCSTMD5; all p > 0.05).

The main effect of belonging to the overweight or obese group, but not the primary hypertension/control group, was statistically significant in the univariable ANOVA with PALFAMS28 (measure of visual memory and new learning) as the dependent variable (p = 0.02). The effect of the primary hypertension/control group was statistically significant in analogical ANOVA models with reaction time measures as dependent variables [p = 0.04 (RTISMDMT) and p = 0.008 (RTIFMDMT)]. No significant group (normal weight/obese or overweight) by group (primary hypertension/control) interactions were detected in the latter models.

4 Discussion

In the present study we explored cognitive performance of children and adolescents diagnosed with primary hypertension and normotensive controls using a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery. We hypothesized that children and adolescents with primary hypertension may exhibit subtle differential impairments in cognitive performance measures that relate to the diagnosis of primary hypertension and due to its complexity to other associated alterations (i.e., elevated body mass). Compared to age- and sex-matched normotensive peers, children and adolescents with primary hypertension exhibited worse performance in computerized tasks addressing reaction time, attention and processing speed, visual memory and new learning, and phonemic fluency. However, only differences in reaction time remained significant after adjusting for BMI, suggesting it as an important potential effect modifier. Indeed, BMI itself associated with the tasks of visual memory and new learning, while none of the cognitive domains were associated with parameters of BP. Notably, between-group differences were evident irrespective of the majority of participants in the primary hypertension group receiving antihypertensive therapy.

Several prior studies have reported differences in relatively wide-range of neurocognitive domains in children and adolescents with primary hypertension compared with normotensive controls (10). The negative effects of elevated BP on the cognition in children and adolescents are further emphasized by the data from the Young Finns Study that demonstrated associations between childhood-onset BP elevation with cognitive functioning in mid-life (29). A recent systematic review on neurocognition in pediatric primary hypertension highlighted the scarcity of studies compared to adult population. The authors also noted gaps in the existing literature, including the heterogeneity of findings, lack of studies employing direct assessment of neurocognitive functions, role of potential confounders and lack of studies assessing the effects of treatment or central hemodynamics (10).

Previous studies in the field have reported deficits in short-term and working memory, attention, fine motor dexterity and verbal fluency (30, 31). In addition, studies employing indirect (proxy-reported) evaluations have reported potential disparities in cognitive functioning and a higher prevalence of internalizing behaviors (17). In our study, we found evidence for potential deficits in visual memory and new learning, attention, processing speed and phonemic fluency among children and adolescents diagnosed with primary hypertension. These findings are in line with those of prior studies and strengthen the evidence for domain-specific injury (10). On the other hand, we identified differences in the tasks assessing reaction time, a finding that has not been previously shown in pediatric studies, but has been observed in the elderly hypertensive population (32). It is important to note that proxy-reported rating scales (such as previously applied parental questionnaires) provide a general assessment of everyday functioning, as assessed mostly through observation of behavior in a real-life setting. Whereas, direct performance assessments as employed in our study may be more accurate in detecting subtle changes in a structured environment (33) that may be more important in the context of understanding the effects of primary hypertension in childhood and more relevant for HMOD assessment.

One of the major findings of our study was the apparent effect of BMI on cognitive performance in children and adolescents with primary hypertension. This is important as elevated BP in childhood primary hypertension is frequently accompanied by increased BMI (19, 34). In our study, the prevalence of obesity (58 percent) in the primary hypertension group was comparable to that reported in prior studies in referral settings (19) and 10-fold higher than that in normotensive controls. BMI in our study population showed a negative association with visual memory and new learning, and spatial planning and working memory. Higher BMI in childhood has been linked to worse executive functions and prior studies in the pediatric primary hypertension population have suggested that BMI might be a mediating factor for cognitive dysfunction, but both studies used indirect (parental) assessments (15, 17). Collectively, this suggests that worse cognitive performance in children and adults with primary hypertension is at least in part determined by the increased BMI. Notably, the effects of BMI may be itself mediated by the associations with sleep apnea and disordered breathing (35, 36) and empirical evidence suggests negative associations of disturbed sleep with cognitive performance (37). Several longitudinal and cross-sectional studies revealed that externalising (3840) and internalizing (38, 39) difficulties could also be related to lower task-based cognitive performance in community-dwelling children and adolescents. Thus, it is important to acknowledge that correlates of psycho-emotional wellbeing that have been out of the scope of our study may have had a mediating effect on cognitive performance.

Finally, we analyzed different measures of BP, including office BP, ABPM and central BP but were unable to find associations with cognitive performance domains after adjusting for age and sex. Dose-dependent association of BP level and cognitive functions has been reported in populational studies (30) and the hypertensive population (14, 15, 41) and it has been shown that ABPM may be superior to office BP in discriminating those with worse cognitive performance (15, 41). Considering the direct exposure of brain circulation to central BP and known disparities between brachial and central BP, particularly in young subjects, central hemodynamics could be a superior risk factor for cognitive dysfunction (18). Central BP has only been addressed in one study of children with primary hypertension and hypertension secondary to kidney disease which used a proxy-reported cognitive assessment and reported that poorer cognitive functioning was associated with higher central BP (13).

Our study population well represents the previously reported phenotype of pediatric primary hypertension, i.e., predominantly consisting of adolescents, higher frequency of males and high prevalence of obesity (1, 8, 19). However, it can also be noted to include a high proportion of participants with antihypertensive therapy that relates to the tertiary care and referral setting of our center where patients with more severe hypertension are typically followed. Although BP values among participants with primary hypertension were still higher than normotensive controls, the use of antihypertensive medications may still have blunted the associations between the BP parameters and cognitive performance. It is nevertheless worth noting that cognitive function remains worse even among children and adolescents on antihypertensive treatment compared to normotensive peers. Although it is difficult to infer the effects of antihypertensive therapy on cognitive performance due to exploratory nature of this study, these findings may also suggest that the observed differences are not solely determined by BP levels alone. Importantly, two previous longitudinal studies have reported conflicting results on the effects of antihypertensive treatment. While one study demonstrated improvements in parent-reported cognitive functioning, another study directly assessing performance showed no significant improvement after 12 months of antihypertensive treatment compared with normotensive controls (42, 43).

Our study is subject to several limitations. First, the majority of the children and adolescents in the primary hypertension group were receiving antihypertensive medications, which may have attenuated the potential associations between BP parameters and cognitive outcomes. We were unable to address this issue by performing subgroup analyses due to the low number of patients without antihypertensive treatment. Second, despite good matching for age and sex, the control group had a lower BMI. In addition, owing to the inherent nature of observational cross-sectional studies, it is impossible to control for unknown confounders and infer causality. It should also be acknowledged that we did not assess the symptoms of internalising (e.g., anxiety, depression) or externalising (e.g., hyperactivity, conduct disorders) difficulties, and sleep disruptions that may have had and independent effect on cognitive performance. Nevertheless, the comprehensive assessment of neurocognitive functioning, including paper and pencil tests, computerized battery and IQ testing represent important strength of our study. Finally, the present state of the tested cohorts (including normotensive controls) also better corresponds to real-life settings, and allows the derivation of the effects of primary hypertension diagnosis as opposed to elevation of BP alone.

5 Conclusions

Overall, our findings suggest that primary hypertension in childhood and adolescence is associated with changes in cognitive performance that can be largely attributed to increased BMI characteristics of this population but also the diagnosis of primary hypertension itself. Both of these factors appear to exert differential effects on the impairment of different cognitive functioning domains. Our results may be in line with the hypothesis that primary hypertension in childhood is not a disease of elevated BP but represents a state of neuro-immuno-metabolic dysfunction (8). Apparent deficits in cognitive performance, including reaction time, attention and processing speed, visual memory, new learning, and phonemic fluency, adds to the evidence of the burden associated with primary hypertension and obesity during childhood. The presence of those impairments during a critical period of development implies the need to include comprehensive neurocognitive outcomes in longitudinal and interventional studies that address childhood obesity and primary hypertension.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Vilnius Regional Bioethics Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

KA: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. KP: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. OK: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. DM: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. RM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AJ: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The authors declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

This project was supported by the Research Council of Lithuania (LMTLT) (grant no P-MIP-21-127).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all participants of the present study for their contribution and the nurse of Vilnius University Hospital Santaros klinikos Daiva Goriakiniene for her contribution to study coordination.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2024.1369690/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Song P, Zhang Y, Yu J, Zha M, Zhu Y, Rahimi K, et al. Global prevalence of hypertension in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. (2019) 173:1154. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Yang L, Magnussen CG, Yang L, Bovet P, Xi B. Elevated blood pressure in childhood or adolescence and cardiovascular outcomes in adulthood: a systematic review. Hypertension. (2020) 75:948–55. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.119.14168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Meng Y, Sharman JE, Koskinen JS, Juonala M, Viikari JSA, Buscot M-J, et al. Blood pressure at different life stages over the early life course and intima-media thickness. JAMA Pediatr. (2023) 178(2):133–41. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2023.5351

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Azegami T, Uchida K, Tokumura M, Mori M. Blood pressure tracking from childhood to adulthood. Front Pediatr. (2021) 9:785356. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.785356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Azukaitis K, Sinha MD, Obrycki Ł, Pac M, Bjelakovic B, Jankauskiene A, et al. Disparities between determinants of impaired vascular structure and function in young people with primary hypertension: a systematic review. J Hypertens. (2022) 40:1369–79. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000003155

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sinha MD, Azukaitis K, Sladowska-Kozłowska J, Bårdsen T, Merkevicius K, Karlsen Sletten IS, et al. Prevalence of left ventricular hypertrophy in children and young people with primary hypertension: meta-analysis and meta-regression. Front Cardiovasc Med. (2022) 9:993513. doi: 10.3389/fcvm.2022.993513

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rus RR, Pac M, Obrycki Ł, Sağsak E, Azukaitis K, Sinha MD, et al. Systolic and diastolic left ventricular function in children with primary hypertension: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Hypertens. (2023) 41:51–62. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000003298

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Litwin M, Feber J. Origins of primary hypertension in children: early vascular or biological aging? Hypertension. (2020) 76:1400–9. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.120.14586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Canavan M, O’Donnell MJ. Hypertension and cognitive impairment: a review of mechanisms and key concepts. Front Neurol. (2022) 13:821135. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.821135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lucas I, Puteikis K, Sinha MD, Litwin M, Merkevicius K, Azukaitis K, et al. Knowledge gaps and future directions in cognitive functions in children and adolescents with primary arterial hypertension: a systematic review. Front Cardiovasc Med. (2022) 9:973793. doi: 10.3389/fcvm.2022.973793

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1641–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Santos DN, Assis AMO, Bastos ACS, Santos LM, Santos CAS, Strina A, et al. Determinants of cognitive function in childhood: a cohort study in a middle income context. BMC Public Health. (2008) 8:202. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-202

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Stabouli S, Chrysaidou K, Kotsis V, Chainoglou N, Chatzipapa N, Gidaris D, et al. Central SBP and executive function in children and adolescents with primary and secondary hypertension. J Hypertens. (2020) 38:2176–84. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000002551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stabouli S, Chrysaidou K, Chainoglou A, Gidaris D, Kotsis V, Zafeiriou D. Uric acid associates with executive function in children and adolescents with hypertension. Hypertension. (2021) 77:1737–44. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.120.16761

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Chrysaidou K, Kotsis V, Chainoglou A, Tzovaras F, Gidaris D, Chatzipapa N, et al. Impact of ambulatory SBP and overweight on executive function performance in children and adolescents. J Hypertens. (2020) 38:1123–30. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000002371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ostrovskaya MA, Rojas M, Kupferman JC, Lande MB, Paterno K, Brosgol Y, et al. Executive function and cerebrovascular reactivity in pediatric hypertension. J Child Neurol. (2015) 30:543–6. doi: 10.1177/0883073813494264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lande MB, Adams H, Falkner B, Waldstein SR, Schwartz GJ, Szilagyi PG, et al. Parental assessments of internalizing and externalizing behavior and executive function in children with primary hypertension. J Pediatr. (2009) 154:207–12.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.08.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. McEniery CM, Cockcroft JR, Roman MJ, Franklin SS, Wilkinson IB. Central blood pressure: current evidence and clinical importance. Eur Heart J. (2014) 35:1719–25. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/eht565

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Flynn JT, Alderman MH. Characteristics of children with primary hypertension seen at a referral center. Pediatr Nephrol. (2005) 20:961–6. doi: 10.1007/s00467-005-1855-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Reinert KRS, Po’e EK, Barkin SL. The relationship between executive function and obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. J Obes. (2013) 2013:1–10. doi: 10.1155/2013/820956

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cole TJ, Green PJ. Smoothing reference centile curves: the LMS method and penalized likelihood. Stat Med. (1992) 11:1305–19. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780111005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. De Onis M. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull World Health Organ. (2007) 85:660–7. doi: 10.2471/BLT.07.043497

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lurbe E, Agabiti-Rosei E, Cruickshank JK, Dominiczak A, Erdine S, Hirth A, et al. 2016 European society of hypertension guidelines for the management of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. J Hypertens. (2016) 34:1887–920. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000001039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. National High Blood Pressure Education Program Working Group on High Blood Pressure in Children and Adolescents. The fourth report on the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. (2004) 114:555–76. doi: 10.1542/peds.114.S2.555

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Mynard JP, Goldsmith G, Springall G, Eastaugh L, Lane GK, Zannino D, et al. Central aortic blood pressure estimation in children and adolescents: results of the KidCoreBP study. J Hypertens. (2020) 38:821–8. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000002338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Shiraishi M, Murakami T, Higashi K. The accuracy of central blood pressure obtained by oscillometric noninvasive method using Mobil-O-Graph in children and adolescents. J Hypertens. (2020) 38:813–20. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0000000000002360

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Elmenhorst J, Hulpke-Wette M, Barta C, Dalla Pozza R, Springer S, Oberhoffer R. Percentiles for central blood pressure and pulse wave velocity in children and adolescents recorded with an oscillometric device. Atherosclerosis. (2015) 238:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2014.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wühl E, Witte K, Soergel M, Mehls O, Schaefer F. Distribution of 24-h ambulatory blood pressure in children: normalized reference values and role of body dimensions. J Hypertens. (2002) 20:1995–2007. doi: 10.1097/00004872-200210000-00019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Rovio SP, Pahkala K, Nevalainen J, Juonala M, Salo P, Kähönen M, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors from childhood and midlife cognitive performance. J Am Coll Cardiol. (2017) 69:2279–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2017.02.060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lande MB, Kaczorowski JM, Auinger P, Schwartz GJ, Weitzman M. Elevated blood pressure and decreased cognitive function among school-age children and adolescents in the United States. J Pediatr. (2003) 143:720–4. doi: 10.1067/S0022-3476(03)00412-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lande MB, Batisky DL, Kupferman JC, Samuels J, Hooper SR, Falkner B, et al. Neurocognitive function in children with primary hypertension. J Pediatr. (2017) 180:148–55.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.08.076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Harrington F, Saxby BK, McKeith IG, Wesnes K, Ford GA. Cognitive performance in hypertensive and normotensive older subjects. Hypertension. (2000) 36:1079–82. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.36.6.1079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Souissi S, Chamari K, Bellaj T. Assessment of executive functions in school-aged children: a narrative review. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:991699. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.991699

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Brady TM. Obesity-related hypertension in children. Front Pediatr. (2017) 5:197. doi: 10.3389/fped.2017.00197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lande MB, Hooper SR, Batisky DL, Kupferman JC, Szilagyi PG, Samuels JA, et al. Sleep disordered breathing as measured by SRBD-PSQ and neurocognition in children with hypertension. Am J Hypertens. (2015) 28:552–8. doi: 10.1093/ajh/hpu180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Madaeva I, Berdina O, Polyakov V, Kolesnikov S. Obstructive sleep apnea and hypertension in adolescents: effect on neurobehavioral and cognitive functioning. Can Respir J. (2016) 2016:1–6. doi: 10.1155/2016/3950914

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kuula L, Pesonen A-K, Martikainen S, Kajantie E, Lahti J, Strandberg T, et al. Poor sleep and neurocognitive function in early adolescence. Sleep Med. (2015) 16:1207–12. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.06.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Blanken LME, White T, Mous SE, Basten M, Muetzel RL, Jaddoe VWV, et al. Cognitive functioning in children with internalising, externalising and dysregulation problems: a population-based study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 26:445–56. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0903-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Blok E, Schuurmans IK, Tijburg AJ, Hillegers M, Koopman-Verhoeff ME, Muetzel RL, et al. Cognitive performance in children and adolescents with psychopathology traits: a cross-sectional multicohort study in the general population. Dev Psychopathol. (2023) 35:926–40. doi: 10.1017/S0954579422000165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Flouri E, Papachristou E, Midouhas E, Ploubidis GB, Lewis G, Joshi H. Developmental cascades of internalising symptoms, externalising problems and cognitive ability from early childhood to middle adolescence. Eur Psychiatr. (2019) 57:61–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.12.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kupferman JC, Batisky DL, Samuels J, Adams HR, Hooper SR, Wang H, et al. Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring and neurocognitive function in children with primary hypertension. Pediatr Nephrol. (2018) 33:1765–71. doi: 10.1007/s00467-018-3954-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lande MB, Adams H, Falkner B, Waldstein SR, Schwartz GJ, Szilagyi PG, et al. Parental assessment of executive function and internalizing and externalizing behavior in primary hypertension after anti-hypertensive therapy. J Pediatr. (2010) 157:114–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.12.053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lande MB, Batisky DL, Kupferman JC, Samuels J, Hooper SR, Falkner B, et al. Neurocognitive function in children with primary hypertension after initiation of antihypertensive therapy. J Pediatr. (2018) 195:85–94.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.12.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children, adolescents, primary hypertension, body mass index, executive functions, cognition

Citation: Azukaitis K, Puteikis K, Kinciniene O, Mikucionyte D, Mameniskiene R and Jankauskiene A (2024) Cognitive performance in children and adolescents with primary hypertension and the role of body mass. Front. Pediatr. 12:1369690. doi: 10.3389/fped.2024.1369690

Received: 15 January 2024; Accepted: 17 April 2024;
Published: 30 April 2024.

Edited by:

Tim S. Nawrot, University of Hasselt, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Antonio G. Lentoor, Sefako Makgatho Health Sciences University, South Africa
Athanasia Chainoglou, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece

© 2024 Azukaitis, Puteikis, Kinciniene, Mikucionyte, Mameniskiene and Jankauskiene. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karolis Azukaitis karolis.azukaitis@mf.vu.lt

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download