Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 21 March 2022
Sec. Invertebrate Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Innate Immune System Guiding Physiological Plasticity in Invertebrates View all 6 articles

Host Resistance to Bacterial Infection Varies Over Time, but Is Not Affected by a Previous Exposure to the Same Pathogen

\r\nBeatriz Acua HidalgoBeatriz Acuña HidalgoSophie A. O. Armitage*\r\nSophie A. O. Armitage*
  • Institute of Biology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Immune priming describes the phenomenon whereby after a primary pathogen exposure, a host more effectively fights a lethal secondary exposure (challenge) to the same pathogen. Conflicting evidence exists for immune priming in invertebrates, potentially due to heterogeneity across studies in the pathogen species tested, the antigen preparation for the primary exposure, and the phenotypic trait used to test for priming. To explore these factors, we injected Drosophila melanogaster with one of two bacterial species, Lactococcus lactis or Providencia burhodogranariea, which had either been heat-killed or inactivated with formaldehyde, or we injected a 1:1 mixture of the two inactivation methods. Survival and resistance (the inverse of bacterial load) were assessed after a live bacterial challenge. In contrast to our predictions, none of the primary exposure treatments provided a survival benefit after challenge compared to the controls. Resistance in the acute phase, i.e., 1 day post-challenge, separated into a lower- and higher-load group, however, neither group varied according to the primary exposure. In the chronic phase, i.e., 7 days post-challenge, resistance did not separate into two groups, and it was also unaffected by the primary exposure. Our multi-angled study supports the view that immune priming may require specific circumstances to occur, rather than it being a ubiquitous aspect of insect immunity.

Introduction

Research on invertebrate immune defenses over the past few decades has changed our understanding of immune memory. The definition of immune memory has been extended beyond a phenomenon restricted to vertebrate adaptive immunity to include invertebrates, plants and bacteria (Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018). In the case of invertebrates, evidence for a memory-like phenomenon has been found across a broad range of taxa (Contreras-Garduno et al., 2016; Milutinović and Kurtz, 2016; Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018). This phenomenon, termed “immune priming” (Little and Kraaijeveld, 2004), has been described as the ability of an immune system to store or use the information on a previously encountered antigen or parasite, upon a secondary exposure (Kurtz, 2005; Milutinović and Kurtz, 2016).

There is considerable evidence supporting immune priming in invertebrates [reviewed in Contreras-Garduno et al. (2016) and Milutinović and Kurtz (2016)], with one mechanistic basis being haemocyte-mediated defences (Pham et al., 2007; Rodrigues et al., 2010). However, a number of studies testing immune priming have not found evidence to support its existence (Pham et al., 2007; Reber and Chapuisat, 2012; Longdon et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2015; Duneau et al., 2016; Patrnogic et al., 2018; Kutzer et al., 2019) [reviewed in Contreras-Garduno et al. (2016) and Milutinović and Kurtz (2016)]. It has been suggested that the inconsistent findings are due to heterogeneity in the way in which this phenomenon has been tested across studies (Milutinović and Kurtz, 2016; Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018). Although the following list is by no means exhaustive, heterogeneity has come in the form of variation in the pathogen species tested, the methods used to prepare the pathogen for the previous exposure, and the phenotypic read-out used to assess whether there is evidence for priming or not. Our experimental design encompasses testing variation in all three of these factors.

First, evidence that priming could be pathogen species dependent comes from studies where within one experiment, priming has been found against one species of pathogen but not against another, for example Pham et al. (2007) and Roth et al. (2009). The evolutionary history and ecology of the host-pathogen interaction studied might also play a role. For example, previous exposure to a gram-positive bacterium conferred Tenebrio molitor a more effective protection against infection compared to a gram-negative bacterium (Dhinaut et al., 2018). The authors suggested that since many pathogenic bacteria naturally present in the environment of T. molitor are gram-positive, immune priming might have only evolved against these bacteria as they represent a significant threat to the host (Dubuffet et al., 2015; Dhinaut et al., 2018). Here we test two species of bacteria, both isolated from the host species.

Second, Milutinović and Kurtz (2016) proposed that using different antigen preparation methods for the primary exposure might result in the antigens being recognised in contrasting ways by the host immune system, leading to inconsistent results between studies. Antigen preparations have ranged from cell components and toxins (Karp and Rheins, 1980; Rheins et al., 1980; Milutinović et al., 2014; Miyashita et al., 2014, 2015) to varying doses of live (Castro-Vargas et al., 2017; Chambers et al., 2019) or inactivated pathogens (Pham et al., 2007; Lin et al., 2013). The use of live compared to dead pathogens for the primary exposure might lead to different priming responses (Milutinović and Kurtz, 2016). A live primary infection can lead to an initial phase of host mortality, after which survivors are challenged with a secondary infection. This first exposure may act as a filter, selecting for fitter hosts. Compared to non-primed individuals, these hosts are predicted to survive the challenge better due to their higher fitness, rather than an ability to store and recall information on a previous encounter with the pathogen (Kurtz, 2005; Milutinović and Kurtz, 2016). Moreover, a live pre-exposure could lead to a persistent infection and result in differential bacterial loads across hosts (Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021) and thereby introduce heterogeneity in the immunological history of the pre-exposed flies. Therefore, in common with the majority of studies examining immune priming. We here focus on the use of inactivated pathogens.

Pathogens can be inactivated using a number of methods including heat-killing (González-Tokman et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2014; Riessberger-Gallé et al., 2015; Kutzer et al., 2019) and chemical compounds like formaldehyde (Wang et al., 2009; Zhuang et al., 2011; Dhinaut et al., 2018) and glutaraldehyde (Faulhaber and Karp, 1992; Rosengaus et al., 1999). There are a limited number of studies directly comparing whether the antigenic preparation method affects the likelihood of uncovering a priming effect (Lin et al., 2013; Miyashita et al., 2014). Lin et al. (2013) found that the immune system of the white shrimp Litopenaus vannamei is activated more quickly by heat-killed Vibrio alginotylicus, but that the response induced after challenge is stronger and induces a higher resistance to live bacteria in shrimp primed with formalin-inactivated V. alginotylicus. The authors argued that this might be due to how the inactivation methods affect the antigenicity of the bacterial cells (Lin et al., 2013). Heating bacterial cells can lead to membrane disruption (Russell, 2003), releasing lipopolysaccharides (Katsui et al., 1982; Tsuchido et al., 1985), which may act as immunostimulants for the host (Lin et al., 2013). This would lead to a fast response but cause the bacterial cells to retain less antigenicity (Lin et al., 2013). On the other hand, formaldehyde cross-links the molecules present on the surface of the cell (Fraenkel-Conrat and Olcott, 1948; Feldman, 1973) leading to formalin-inactivated bacteria to retain a high level of antigenicity (Spitznagel and Trainer, 1949; Arshadi et al., 2020).

Third, the phenotypic trait that has been measured to test whether there is increased protection upon the secondary encounter, varies across studies. This protection has most frequently been tested by monitoring survival after the secondary exposure (Contreras-Garduno et al., 2016), showing an increased longevity in some (Boman et al., 1972; Faulhaber and Karp, 1992; Pham et al., 2007; Roth et al., 2009; Christofi and Apidianakis, 2013; Lin et al., 2013; Miyashita et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2014; Futo et al., 2016; Castro-Vargas et al., 2017; Lafont et al., 2017; Dhinaut et al., 2018), but not all previously exposed hosts (Boman et al., 1972; Pham et al., 2007; Kutzer et al., 2019). In the traditional sense of immune memory, it would be expected that this increased survival results from the host immune system inducing a stronger and more efficient immune response upon secondary exposure (Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018), which can be quantified at the level of the host immune effectors (Lin et al., 2013; Zhao et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014; Vargas et al., 2020). These changes in the immune response are expected to increased host resistance to the infection, which is defined as the host ability to reduce the pathogen load (Råberg et al., 2009; Graham et al., 2011). Increased resistance upon secondary exposure has been demonstrated (Boman et al., 1972; Sadd and Schmid-Hempel, 2006; Pham et al., 2007; Miyashita et al., 2014), but a primary exposure can also lead to a reduction in host resistance (Kutzer et al., 2019), potentially because a host can tolerate an infection instead of eliminating it (Kutzer and Armitage, 2016b). Despite its relevance as a phenotypic read-out for immune priming, host resistance has not frequently been assayed. Furthermore, while chronic infections can persist in insects for weeks (Haine et al., 2008; Hotson and Schneider, 2015; Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021), the effects of a primary exposure on resistance post-secondary exposure are not well-understood in the chronic infection phase (Vargas et al., 2020; but see Rodrigues et al., 2010; Contreras-Garduño et al., 2015; Kutzer et al., 2019), with some studies showing that pathogens are not always eliminated in primed hosts (Rodrigues et al., 2010; Contreras-Garduño et al., 2015; Kutzer et al., 2019).

Here, using D. melanogaster as our host, we explored the effect of pre-exposure to two bacterial species isolated from wild flies, gram-positive L. lactis (Lazzaro, 2002) and gram-negative P. burhodogranariea (Juneja and Lazzaro, 2009), which are considered opportunistic pathogens and are able to establish an infection in the fly with lethal consequences for a proportion of the infected flies (Lazzaro, 2002; Lazzaro et al., 2006; Galac and Lazzaro, 2011; Kutzer and Armitage, 2016a; Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021). We asked whether pre-exposure affords protection against each species of bacteria, and whether the inactivation method affects the level of protection. We hypothesised that flies simultaneously pre-exposed to formaldehyde-inactivated and heat-killed bacteria would benefit from both types of antigenicity and show a higher level of protection compared to a pre-exposure with only one method of inactivation. We also asked whether pre-exposure affects survival and resistance after a homologous challenge with live bacteria. By quantifying resistance as the pathogen load in the acute and chronic phases of infection (1- and 7-days post-infection), we aimed to determine the strength and duration of the immune priming response, as well as its effect on bacterial persistence (Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018).

Materials and Methods

Experimental Animals

We used an outbred population of D. melanogaster, naturally infected with the intracellular bacterium Wolbachia (gift from Élio Sucena). This population was established from 160 fertilised females collected in Azeitão, Portugal in 2007 (Martins et al., 2013). The flies were reared and maintained at a density of at least 5,000 flies inside a population cage with non-overlapping generations of 14 days on a 12:12 h light-dark cycle, at 60–80% relative humidity and a temperature of 24.8 ± 0.5°C. They were maintained on a sugar yeast agar medium [SYA medium: 970 mL water, 100 g brewer’s yeast, 50 g sugar, 15 g agar-agar, 30 mL 10% Nipagin solution and 3 mL propionic acid (Bass et al., 2007)].

Experimental flies were produced after two generations of density control. The first density-controlled generation was obtained by placing four grape juice agar plates [25 g agar-agar, 300 mL red grape juice, 21 mL 10% Nipagin solution, 550 mL water (Wensing et al., 2017)] coated with a thin layer of baker’s yeast paste, inside the population cage and letting the flies lay eggs for 24 h. Larvae were collected 24 h after the end of the oviposition period and placed in groups of 100 larvae in plastic vials (95 mm × 25 mm) containing 7 mL of SYA medium. They were left to develop for 8 days under standard conditions. The second density-controlled generation was produced by placing 4-day old adults in two embryo cages, allowing 600–800 adults per cage to oviposit on a grape juice agar plate for 24 h. Larvae were again collected 24 h later at a density of 100 larvae per vial and allowed to develop. One day after the start of eclosion, adults were collected, placed in vials in mixed sex groups of five males and five females.

Preparation of the Bacterial Solutions

In this study, we used two bacterial species isolated from wild-caught D. melanogaster (gifts from Brian Lazzaro), L. lactis (Lazzaro, 2002), and P. burhodogranaria strain B (Juneja and Lazzaro, 2009) (DSMZ; type strain: DSM-19968). Culturing of these bacteria was performed as in Kutzer and Armitage (Kutzer and Armitage, 2016a). In brief, bacteria were streaked on lysogeny broth (LB) agar directly from aliquots stored in 34.4% glycerol at −80°C. After an incubation period of 24 h at 30°C, four colony-forming units (CFUs) were added to 100 mL of sterile LB medium and incubated at 30°C and 200 rpm. Two individual bacterial cultures were incubated per bacteria. The next morning, approximately 15 h later, the liquid cultures were prepared for the primary exposure or challenge injections.

Preparation of Inactivated Bacteria for Primary Injections

After the incubation period of 15 h, the bacteria were centrifuged at 21°C and 2,880 rcf for 5 min and washed two times in Drosophila Ringer’s solution [182 mmol L–1 KCl; 46 mol L–1 NaCl; 3 mmol L–1 CaCl2; 10 mmol L–1 Tris⋅HCl (Werner et al., 2000)]. The optical density (OD) of 500 μL of each species was measured using an Ultrospec10 classic spectrophotometer (Amersham, 600 nm), and the OD values were averaged to calculate the total bacterial concentration of the overnight cultures. For each bacterial species we had pre-determined the relationship between OD and the number of live bacteria by plating serial dilutions of bacterial solutions with known ODs. The bacteria were centrifuged again at 2,880 rcf and 21°C for 10 min. The supernatant was discarded, and the remaining pellet was resuspended in sterile distilled water. No washing steps with distilled water were performed to limit the exposure of bacteria to osmotic lysis.

For formaldehyde inactivation, a solution containing 5% formaldehyde in sterile distilled water was added to the bacterial solution to achieve a final concentration of 0.5% formaldehyde (Dhinaut et al., 2018). The solution was then placed on a shaker (Biosan ES20) at 1,000 rpm at room temperature. We previously determined the inactivation time needed for each of the two bacterial species by exposing an overnight culture to 0.5% formaldehyde for 10, 30, 120 min or 24 h at room temperature, then plating the bacterial solutions on LB agar plates in triplicate for each time tested, and then verifying the absence of colonies after 24 and 48 h. Formaldehyde inactivates the bacterial cells by cross-linking proteins of the cell wall (Fraenkel-Conrat and Olcott, 1948; Feldman, 1973). Our aim was to kill the cells while preserving the conformation of the membrane as much as possible, and thus the antigenicity of the bacteria, therefore we aimed for the shortest amount of time possible. No colonies grew on the agar plates after 2 h of exposure to formaldehyde for L. lactis, and after 10 min of exposure for P. burhodogranariea. The inactivated bacterial solution was centrifuged at 21°C, at 2,880 rpm for 10 min, the supernatant was removed and the pellet was resuspended in 7 ml Ringer’s solution. This step was repeated two more times to remove the formaldehyde from the solution. To verify that the bacteria had been inactivated, we plated 100 μL of the solution onto LB agar plates and checked for the absence of bacterial colony growth after an incubation period of 24 and 48 h at 30°C. The solution was then aliquoted into 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes, snap frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80°C. Since the bacterial solution was washed in Ringer’s solution three times, we expected that a portion of the inactivated bacterial cells might have been lost during that process. Hence, we once again measured the concentration of the solutions. One tube per bacteria was defrosted at room temperature and serially diluted in Drosophila Ringer’s, and the cells were counted using a haemocytometer (Thoma, 0.02 mm deep, 0.0025 mm2).

For heat-killing, the bacterial solution was serially diluted to achieve double of the aimed concentration, i.e., 2 × 108 CFUs/mL. The solution was pipetted into several 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes and placed on a heat-block (Eppendorf ThermoMixer® C) at 90°C and 1,000 rpm. Prolonged exposure to heat can lead to protein denaturation, and thus reduce recognition of the antigens present in the solution by the host immune system. Therefore, we tested for the shortest amount of heat-killing time that would lead to the inactivation of the bacterial cells. The time needed to kill each of the two bacteria was previously tested by exposing them to this treatment for 5, 10, and 20 min. The bacterial solutions were then plated in triplicates on LB agar plates to verify the absence of colonies after 24 h, and then again after 48 h. No L. lactis colonies grew on the plates after 10 min of heating, and no P. burhodogranariea colonies grew after 5 min of heating. The bacterial solutions were then aliquoted into 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes. The final solutions for the injections were made by adding double concentrated Ringer’s solution to the tubes in a 1:1 volume ratio, diluting the concentration to 1 × 108 CFUs/mL. Subsequently, 100 μL per tube were plated onto LB agar and checked for the absence of bacterial colony growth after 24 h of incubation at 30°C. The tubes containing the inactivated bacteria were frozen in aliquots in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80 C until use.

Before injection of the primary exposure, the inactivated bacterial aliquots were allowed to defrost at room temperature. For the formaldehyde-inactivated bacteria, three serial dilutions per bacteria were performed and pooled together to adjust the solution to a concentration of 1 × 108 CFUs/mL. For the combination treatment, an equal volume of the formaldehyde-inactivated and heat-killed bacteria were pooled together. A volume of 50 μL of each solution was plated onto LB agar to confirm the absence of bacterial colony growth.

Bacterial Preparation for Challenge Injections

The experiment was performed in five independent experimental replicates, and for each experimental replicate, the overnight bacterial cultures were produced following the same protocol as described above. After an incubation period of 15 h, the liquid cultures were centrifuged at 2,880 rcf and 4°C for 10 min. The supernatant was removed, and the cultures were washed twice with Drosophila Ringer’s solution. The concentration of the bacterial solution was estimated measuring the optical density of 500 μL of the bacterial solution after serial dilution. We aimed to infect the flies with a dose that caused an intermediate mortality, i.e., 50–60% of dead flies by day seven, therefore the concentration was adjusted to 5 × 106 CFUs/mL for L. lactis and to 5 × 107 CFUs/mL for P. burhodogranariea (Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021). To verify these concentrations, we performed three serial dilutions of the bacterial solution from 1:1 to 1:104, plated 5 μL of the solution onto agar eight times and counted the number of CFUs that grew after incubation for 20 h at 30°C.

Previous Exposure and Challenge Injections

Of the five independent experimental replicates, three replicates assessed the effect of pre-exposure on survival and bacterial load, and two replicates assessed only the effect on survival. Four days after having been placed in vials with five males and five females, females were exposed to a pre-exposure injection, and then to a challenge injection after 7 days (Figure 1). The previous exposure injections were performed in a randomised block design. Flies were anesthetised with CO2 for a maximum of 5 min in groups of 10 flies. A total volume of 18.4 nL of the primary exposure solution containing 1 × 108 CFUs/mL [resulting in around ∼1,840 CFUs injected per fly (Kutzer et al., 2019)] was injected on the right side of the thorax using a fine glass capillary (Ø 0.5 mm, Drummond), pulled to a fine tip with a Narishige PC-10, and connected to a Nanoject II™ injector (Drummond). Flies were injected with one of the three previous exposure treatments per bacteria, i.e., formaldehyde-inactivated bacteria (F), heat-killed bacteria (HK) or a solution containing equal volumes of the two types of inactivated bacterial solutions (F + HK). Control flies were injected with 18.4 nL of Drosophila Ringer’s solution (R). In total, per each pre-exposure treatment with dead bacteria, 260 flies were injected (40-60 flies per experimental repeat), and 460 flies were given a control injection with Ringer’s (80–100 flies per repeat). Flies were then transferred to vials containing 7 mL of fresh SYA medium, kept in groups of 10 at 25°C and 70% relative humidity and flipped into new food vials every 2–4 days. For each group of 10 flies, one aliquot containing the injection solution was used. At the end of the injections, the remaining volume of each aliquot was plated onto LB agar and incubated at 30°C for 15 h to confirm that there was no contamination. No CFUs grew on any the incubated plates.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design. (A) Timeline of the experiment with essential steps and assaying timepoints. Emergence refers to the time at which the adults eclosed. (B) Previous exposure and challenge treatment combinations used in the experiment. The primary exposure was either to Drosophila Ringer’s solution (R), Lactococcus lactis (Ll) or Providencia burhodogranariea (Pb). The primary bacterial exposure was either formaldehyde inactivated (F), a mixture of formaldehyde-inactivated and heat-killed bacteria (F/HK), or heat-killed bacteria (HK). For each primary exposure-challenge combination treatment flies were challenged with live bacteria; either 92 colony forming units (CFUs) of L. lactis or 920 CFUs of P. burhodogranariea. Figure adapted from Kutzer et al. (2019).

The secondary exposure to live bacteria (challenge injections) was carried out 7 days after the previous exposure (Pham et al., 2007; Kutzer et al., 2019). Before the injections, the survival of pre-exposed flies was assessed. For the injections, flies were anesthetised and injected on the left side of the thorax with a volume of 18.4 nL of live bacterial solution or Drosophila Ringer’s solution. Therefore, flies injected with L. lactis were given a dose of approximately 92 CFUs and those injected with P. burhodogranariea were given a dose of approximately 920 CFUs (Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021). Across experimental repeats, 138 flies per primary exposure treatment were injected with either live L. lactis or P. burhodogranariea (24–30 flies per repeat), and 78 were injected with Ringer’s solution (12–18 flies per repeat). After the challenge injections, flies were placed in vials containing fresh SYA medium in groups of six flies (Kutzer et al., 2018) and flipped into new food vials every 3–4 days. A single aliquot of bacterial solution or Ringer’s solution was used for each group of six flies and each of them was plated at the end of the injections to check for potential contamination, which we did not find. Additionally, to verify the dose of bacteria that had been injected we prepared three serial dilutions from 1:1 to 1:104 for L. lactis and 1:1 to 105 for P. burhodogranariea. Eight droplets of 5 μL per dilution were plated for the three highest dilutions before and after the challenge injections, and counted after 20 h of incubation at 30°C. From these counts we estimated that the injected doses were thereby on average 136 ± 5.22 CFUs for L. lactis and 1,168 ± 37.60 CFUs for P. burhodogranariea.

Survival and Bacterial Load Assays

For three of the five experimental replicates, a portion of the vials from each replicate were randomly allocated to survival, which was monitored daily for 7 days for a total of 96 bacteria-infected flies per primary exposure and challenge treatment (18–30 flies per experimental repeat), and for a total of 60 Ringer’s injected flies (12–18 flies per experimental repeat). The remaining vials from each replicate were randomly allocated to bacterial load measures. For the two other replicates, we monitored only survival as described above.

For bacterial load measures, at one- and 7-days post-challenge, flies from randomly allocated vials were homogenised. A total of 21 flies per previous exposure and challenge treatment (seven flies for each of the three experimental repeats) were allocated to each timepoint. For homogenisation, flies were anesthetised with CO2, removed from their vial, and transferred into a 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tube containing 100 μL of LB media and one stainless steel bead (Ø 3 mm, Retsch) and immediately placed on ice. The tubes were placed in a Retsch Mill (MM300) inside holders that had been previously chilled for 30 min at 4°C. The flies were homogenised at a frequency of 20 Hz for 45 s. The tubes were subsequently centrifuged at 420 rcf for 1 min at 4°C. The homogenate was re-suspended and 80 μL were placed in a 96-well plate, and one serial dilution from 1:10 to 1:105 was performed for each sample. For each of the six dilutions, three droplets of 5 μL per fly were placed onto LB agar and incubated at 30°C for approximately 20 h. The number of CFUs per droplet were counted for the dilutions with droplets containing between approximately 10–60 CFUs. The bacterial load per fly was estimated by averaging the counts for the three droplets and back-calculating the number of CFUs in each fly based on the number of dilutions. D. melanogaster microbiota does not easily grow under the above culturing conditions (e.g., Hanson et al., 2019; Kutzer et al., 2019). Nonetheless we homogenised flies that had been challenged with Ringer’s as a control. Of the 145 Ringer’s-injected flies, four flies had more than 2 CFUs in the 1:1 dilution. Of the remaining 439 bacteria-challenged flies, 11 flies (six challenged with L. lactis and five challenged with P. burhodogranariea) had more than 2 CFUs and were excluded from the analyses. One of the L. lactis-injected flies had too many CFUs to count in the highest dilution factor (1:105), therefore, its bacterial load was replaced by the highest bacterial load from the same bacteria, experimental replicate and day post-challenge, i.e., 3,133,333 CFUs.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using R studio (R version 3.6.3). Figures were created using plyr (Wickham, 2011), dplyr (Wickham et al., 2020), and ggplot2 (Wickham, 2009). For each model, the effects of the explanatory variables and interactions on the response variable were tested using a Wald test (Bolker et al., 2009). As explanatory variables, all the models included the previous exposure treatment and the experimental repeat, as well as the interaction between these two variables unless stated otherwise. For all the analyses, each model was tested independently for each bacterial species, and the same group of control flies, i.e., injection with Ringer’s was used as the control.

We tested the effect of the previous exposure treatment on survival 7 days after the pre-exposure, by comparing the survival of dead bacteria-injected flies to Ringer’s injected flies. We used a generalised linear models glm binomial errors. Using the function cbind, the number of flies that died and the number of flies that survived per vial was combined into a vector, which we used as a response variable. Previous exposure treatment (F: formaldehyde inactivation, HK: heat-killing, F + HK: equal parts of bacterial cells inactivated with heat-killing or formaldehyde), experimental repeat, and their interaction, were used as factors. Model 1a tested the survival after pre-exposure for L. lactis, and model 1b for P. burhodogranariea:

Models 1a,b:Survivalpost-primingL.lactis,P.burhodogranarieaPreviousexposure×Repeat

The effect of previous exposure on survival post-challenge was tested by comparing the survival of flies infected with either L. lactis or P. burhodogranriea that had been previously exposed to Ringer’s solution, formaldehyde inactivated bacteria, heat-killed bacteria or the pre-exposure treatment containing both types of inactivated bacteria. We tested for differences in survival using coxme in the survival package (Therneau and Grambsch, 2000; Therneau, 2020). Previous exposure treatment and experimental repeat, and their interaction, were included as factors. The identification number of the vial that the flies had been kept in for the survival assay was included as a random effect. The variable tested was a survival object constructed for each individual fly with the function Surv in the survival package. This vector contained two variables: a binary censor variable that indicates whether the fly was dead (1) or alive (0), and the day at which the fly died, or in the case of censored flies (i.e., that were still alive at the end of the assay) the last survival check day (7 days post-infection). Models 2a and 2b tested survival after a challenge with L. lactis and P. burhodogranariea, correspondingly:

Models 2a,b:Survivalpost-challengeL.lactis,P.burhodogranarieaPreviousexposure×Repeat+(1|VialID)

For both bacterial species, visual inspection of the log10 transformed bacterial load + 1 suggested that the data distribution on day one post-challenge was not unimodal. This was statistically confirmed using a Hartigan’s Dip test for unimodality (Hartigan and Hartigan, 1985) with the dip.test function from the diptest package (Maechler, 2016) by simulating 5,000 p-values (see Results Section). The k-means clustering method (Forgy, 1965; MacQueen, 1967; Hartigan and Wong, 1979; Lloyd, 1982), suggested that the data is bimodal. We therefore sub-set the bacterial load data for day one post-challenge into two groups. We determined the cut-off point between these groups as the local minima in the interval between the highest values for both modes. For L. lactis the cut-off value was 18268.63 CFUs and for P. burhodogranariea it was 14383.78 CFUs. We divided the data into two subsets comprised of flies with a “low” (i.e., below the cut-off point) or “high” (above the cut-off point) bacterial load. Both subsets were analysed separately for each bacterial species. The effect of the previous exposure treatment and experimental repeat on bacterial load was tested with a linear model on a log10 transformation of the bacterial load + 1 using the lm function. Models 3a and 3b were for the low and high subsets of flies infected with L. lactis, respectively, and models 3c and 3d for the low and high subsets infected with P. burhodogranariea, respectively. Additionally, we detected three data points in model 3c, which could potentially have been influential (i.e., they were either around or above 0.5 Cook’s distance). We therefore additionally analysed the low subset of data without these three data points (model 3e) as a generalised linear model with quasipoisson distribution. Both models 3c and 3e gave qualitatively similar results (see Supplementary Table 2 for the results from model 3e). We did not include the identity of the vial in which flies had been kept as a random variable, because the flies were sampled at random from the vials. We did not include the interaction between previous exposure and repeat because some combinations of previous exposure and experimental repeat contained only one individual.

Models 3a,b,c,d:Log10(Bacterialloadday 1L.lactisor P.burhodogranariea+1)Previousexposure+Repeat
Models 3e:Bacterialloadday 1P.burhodogranarieaPreviousexposure+Repeat

Given the bimodal distributions described above, we reasoned that in addition to the bacterial load per se, the primary exposure could also affect the proportion of flies in the high and low groups. Therefore, for each replicate, using the function cbind we created a response vector, y, containing the number of flies in the low group, and the number of flies in the low group subtracted from the total number of flies in the low and the high group. This was entered into a glm with binomial error for the L. lactis load. We used a quasibinomial error for the P. burhodogranariea load to account for over dispersion of the data. Previous exposure treatment was given as the factor.

Models 4a,b:yL.lactisorP.burhodogranarieaPreviousexposure

Bacterial load data 7-days post-challenge was found not to differ significantly from a unimodal distribution using the Hartigan’s Dip test for unimodality as described above. The effect of previous exposure treatment on the bacterial load 7 days post-challenge was tested using a linear model with log10 transformed bacterial load + 1 (Model 4a for a challenge with L. lactis, and 4b for P. burhodogranariea).

Models 5a,b:Log(Bacterialloadday 7L.lactisor P.burhodogranariea+1)Previousexposure×Repeat

Results

Survival After a Previous Exposure to Inactivated Bacteria

Fly survival directly before the live bacterial challenge was higher than 96% across all treatments and experimental repeats (Supplementary Figure 1). There was no significant effect of the previous exposure treatment or experimental repeat for either bacterial species, and there was no interaction between these two factors (Supplementary Table 1).

Survival After a Live Bacterial Challenge

As expected, fly survival was high 7 days after challenge with Drosophila Ringer’s, and it was unaffected by the previous exposure injection: across all seven control groups there was 98.95% survival (three out of 287 flies died). Contrary to our expectations, we did not find any significant differences in survival between flies injected with the different pre-exposure treatments, whether they were challenged with L. lactis or P. burhodogranariea (Table 1 and Figure 2), meaning that there were no survival benefits to any of the primary bacterial exposure treatments compared to the Ringer’s primary control exposure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The effects of previous exposure and experimental repeat on fly survival for the 7 days post-challenge.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effect of the previous exposure on survival 7 days post-challenge. Flies were challenged with either (A) 92 colony forming units (CFUs) of Lactococcus lactis, or (B) 920 of Providencia burhodogranariea. Previous exposure treatments are Drosophila Ringer’s solution (R), formaldehyde-inactivated bacteria (F), a mixture of formaldehyde-inactivated and heat-killed bacteria (F + HK), and heat-killed bacteria (HK). Survival did not differ significantly according to previous exposure treatment. For statistics, see Table 1.

Resistance After a Live Bacterial Challenge

Host resistance, i.e., the inverse of bacterial load was assessed on days one and seven after challenge (Figure 3). We found that eleven flies across both days cleared the infection: five out of 157 flies had no L. lactis CFUs, and six out of 156 flies had no P. burhodogranariea CFUs.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Bacterial load of individual flies 1 and 7 days after a homologous challenge with (A,C) 92 colony forming units (CFUs) of Lactococcus lactis, or (B,D) 920 CFUs of Providencia burhodogranariea. Bacterial load on the y-axis was quantified as the number of colony-forming units per fly. Here, we present a log transformation of the CFU (+1) for ease of interpretation. On the x-axis, previous exposure treatments are presented as Drosophila Ringer’s solution (R), formaldehyde-inactivated bacteria (F), a mixture of formaldehyde-inactivated and heat-killed bacteria (F+HK), and heat-killed bacteria (HK). Bacterial load at day one is in the left-hand column, and the load at day seven is in the right-hand column. Black lines show the geometric mean of the bacterial load per treatment, and per subset for bacterial load 1-day post-challenge. The grey dotted lines represent the cut-off points dividing the low and high bacterial load subsets, which were analysed separately. We did not find any effect of the previous exposure on bacterial load for either of the 2 days assayed. For statistics, see Table 2.

On day one post-challenge, regardless of treatment and bacterial species, bacteria-infected flies showed large variation in their bacterial load (Figures 3A,B). The data did not follow a unimodal distribution (L. lactis: D = 0.071, p = 0.0026; P. burhodogranariea: D = 0.072, p = 0.0026), with some flies showing a high bacterial load while most flies had a lower bacterial load. Therefore, by calculating the local minima between the highest values for each group of flies, a cut-off point was determined to split the data into two subsets. The data were analysed separately for flies belonging to the low (below the cut-off point) or high (above the cut-off point) subsets (Figures 3A,B) for both bacterial species. We found for both subsets and bacterial species that the pre-exposure treatment did not have a significant effect on the mean bacterial load on day one post-challenge (Table 2). For P. burhodogranariea, experimental repeat had a significant effect on the bacterial load of the low subset (Table 2). This effect was mainly driven by the presence of a replicate with two flies pre-exposed with heat-killed bacteria that cleared the infection, as clearance was not found in any other treatment for this bacterial species and day post-challenge. We found that previous exposure treatment did not affect the proportion of flies in the high and the low load groups for either flies infected with L. lactis (Chi square = 0.37, df = 3, p = 0.95) or P. burhodogranariea (Chi square = 4.00, df = 3, p = 0.26).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The effects of previous exposure and experimental repeat on bacterial load on day one post-challenge.

On day seven post-challenge, the bacterial load for the two bacterial species did not differ significantly from a unimodal distribution (L. lactis: D = 0.040, p = 0.48; P. burhodogranariea: D = 0.026, p = 0.99) (Figures 3C,D). We did not find any significant effect of the priming treatment on the bacterial load 7 days after challenge (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The effects of previous exposure and experimental repeat on bacterial load on day seven post-challenge.

Discussion

Our study addresses whether pre-exposure to two bacterial species inactivated with different methods, affects subsequent host survival and resistance against a secondary challenge. We found no enhanced host survival or resistance after a primary exposure to dead bacteria, which was consistent across inactivation treatments and bacterial species. Our results highlight the dynamic nature of host resistance over the infection course, and they raise questions as to whether immune priming is a universal trait of insect immunity.

Pre-exposure Treatment Does Not Affect Survival

As predicted, we found that a primary injection with inactivated bacteria resulted in high survival (>96%) and similar mortality compared to a primary injection with Ringer’s solution. We used dead bacteria for the primary exposure, which for priming experiments has potential advantages over live bacteria: first there is usually minimal mortality after injection with dead bacteria meaning that unlike after the injection of live bacteria, there is no self-selection for a sub-group of fitter flies that survive until challenge; in the case of live bacterial injection, these latter flies may themselves then be predicted to have increased survival after a second infection. Second, a primary exposure with live bacteria will likely reach varying densities across flies by the time of the secondary challenge or even be cleared (Duneau et al., 2017; Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021); this will result in heterogeneity in the immunological history of the population of flies that are to be challenged. Bacterial infections in insects have been shown to be highly persistent and to lead to sustained antimicrobial responses in the host (Haine et al., 2008; Chambers et al., 2019; Acuña Hidalgo et al., 2021). Using live bacteria for the pre-exposure can lead to persistent infections inside the host, as well as the maintenance of a high level of immune activity, in turn advantaging the host when fighting a secondary bacterial infection (Chambers et al., 2019). However, it is important to note that immune priming responses to inactivated bacteria can persist over time, e.g., antimicrobial responses to heat-killed S. aureus can be sustained in T. molitor for at least 21 days (Makarova et al., 2016).

Pre-exposed Flies Have Neither Increased Survival nor Resistance, but Resistance Varies Over the Course of Infection

An advantage of a pre-exposure to fighting a secondary bacterial challenge has most frequently been measured in terms of increased survival to the secondary infection (Boman et al., 1972; Faulhaber and Karp, 1992; Pham et al., 2007; Roth et al., 2009; Christofi and Apidianakis, 2013; Lin et al., 2013; Miyashita et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2014; Futo et al., 2016; Castro-Vargas et al., 2017; Lafont et al., 2017; Dhinaut et al., 2018). Contrary to our expectations, we did not find that pre-exposed flies survived the bacterial challenge better than non-exposed flies. Although less commonly tested in the context of immune priming, host resistance, as measured by pathogen load, has been shown to be increased in hosts previously exposed to pathogens (Boman et al., 1972; Sadd and Schmid-Hempel, 2006; Pham et al., 2007; Miyashita et al., 2014). However, we did not find pre-exposed hosts to be more resistant to a live bacterial challenge in the acute (1 day post-challenge) or chronic (day seven post-challenge) phases of infection. While our results contrast with some pathogen infections in D. melanogaster (Boman et al., 1972; Pham et al., 2007), they are consistent with those of a recent study by Kutzer et al. (2019) which showed that four inbred fly genotypes pre-exposed to heat-killed L. lactis did not have a higher survival in the 28 days post-homologous challenge, and they did not have increased resistance one and 28 days post-challenge (Kutzer et al., 2019). Despite using a lower challenge dose in our current study, an outbred fly population, and different antigen production methods, the results of the two studies are consistent in that pre-exposure does not offer any significant advantages.

While resistance did not differ between pre-exposure treatments, bacterial load varied over the course of the challenge infection. One day post-challenge, bacterial load followed a bimodal distribution, consistent with previous data on the dynamics of bacterial infections (Duneau et al., 2017). Duneau et al. (2017) showed that the early dynamics of bacterial load follow a bimodal distribution for intermediately virulent bacterial species, with different predicted outcomes of infection for each of the modes. Hosts with high pathogen burden are not able to control the infection and will die during the acute phase of infection. Meanwhile, other hosts will manage to control the pathogen growth and will survive, entering a phase of chronic infection with a constant pathogen load, the set point bacterial load (Duneau et al., 2017). We expected that, if the primary exposure affected acute phase resistance, it would be apparent in the proportion of flies in the high versus low sub-group, or it would be apparent in the resistance of the flies in the lower subgroup. However, primary exposure affected neither of these responses. Seven days after infection, we observed that clearance of the bacteria was rare, and bacterial load was unimodally distributed. Our results highlight the importance of measuring bacterial load as a measure of resistance at more than one point during the infection.

Resistance Is Not Influenced by the Inactivation Method

Heat-killing (Pham et al., 2007; González-Tokman et al., 2010; Longdon et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014; Riessberger-Gallé et al., 2015; Kutzer et al., 2019) and formaldehyde-inactivation (Wang et al., 2009; Zhuang et al., 2011; Dhinaut et al., 2018) are two of the most frequently used methods to inactivate pathogens in priming studies. Based on Lin et al. (2013) we had reason to hypothesise that host responses would vary according to the inactivation protocol and to our knowledge, a combination of these two methods has not been tested before. Based on the properties of both types of antigenic preparations, we predicted that combining bacterial cells inactivated with both treatments would result in a synergistic effect in which hosts would benefit from the high antigenicity of formaldehyde-inactivated bacteria, and a fast trigger of the immune response caused by the lipopolysaccharides freed upon cell membrane disruption during heat-killing (Lin et al., 2013). However, our results showed that the method used to inactivate the bacteria for the pre-exposure did not influence host resistance. It could be that these treatments still induce differential immune responses in terms of strength, speed and duration (Pradeu and Du Pasquier, 2018) but lead to similar outcomes in terms of bacterial load, however we did not test this. Interestingly host survival in the Lin et al. (2013), study was not different between hosts pre-exposed to different antigen preparations despite the differences measured in the immune response to both types of inactivated bacteria (Lin et al., 2013).

Can We Consider Priming as a Ubiquitous Aspect of Innate Insect Immunity?

Our study offers a multi-angled evaluation of the effects of pre-exposure on a secondary challenge. Despite this, we did not find any advantage of previous exposure against a bacterial infection across any pre-exposure treatments. Other studies have identified a priming response in D. melanogaster (Boman et al., 1972; Pham et al., 2007) but similar to our study, priming is not always found (Pham et al., 2007; Reber and Chapuisat, 2012; Kutzer et al., 2019). In addition, many experimental parameters can be explored to achieve priming, including the pre-exposure and challenge doses and bacterial species. While L. lactis and P. burhodogranariea were isolated from D. melanogaster (Lazzaro, 2002; Juneja and Lazzaro, 2009), and can cause intermediate virulence and persistent infections (Acuña-Hidalgo, Silva & Armitage, personal observation), it could be that pre-exposure against other pathogens with different infection dynamics might result in other outcomes. For example, Kutzer et al. (2019) found that pre-exposure with heat-killed Pseudomonas entomophila, a more virulent bacterium than the two bacteria tested in this study resulted in a lower resistance across genotypes (Kutzer et al., 2019). A theoretical consideration of immune priming suggested that virulence plays a role in how a pre-exposed host will respond to the infection (Best et al., 2013). Tolerance is another host defence strategy that quantifies the ability of the host to maintain its fitness in the face of an infection (Råberg et al., 2009), and which has been rarely explored in priming studies (but see Kutzer et al., 2019). In the case of fecundity as a measure for fitness, Kutzer et al. (2019) found no effect of previous exposure on fecundity-tolerance, and although we did not explicitly test it here, the fact that survival and bacterial load did not differ across treatments suggests no effect of survival tolerance under these experimental conditions.

Finally, as mentioned above, while our study and several others did not find support for priming, it might be that this phenomenon only occurs only under certain circumstances, such as specific host-pathogen combinations (Roth et al., 2009; Pope et al., 2011). For instance, Pope et al. (2011) found that white shrimp can be primed using the bacteria Vibrio harveyi but not Bacillus subtilis. They argued that shrimp pre-exposed to V. harveyi might have an advantage against a live challenge since this bacterium is a known pathogen present in the host natural environment, to which the host may have evolved priming defences, while B. subtilis is not naturally present in this environment (Pope et al., 2011). Because it allows the host to reduce or avoid the negative effects of an infection on host fitness, immune priming might be expected to be subjected to a strong selection pressure (Best et al., 2013). However, if it is the case that priming is only elicited in specific experimental circumstances, one could argue about the adaptive value of this phenomenon. Immune priming might then not be a general trait of the innate immune system, but rather a defence trait specific to populations where it gives a significant evolutionary advantage against pathogens.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are publicly available: http://dx.doi.org/10.17169/refubium-33781.

Author Contributions

SA conceived the idea and designed the experiments together with BA. BA collected the data, conducted the statistical analyses with advice from SA, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Both authors contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication.

Funding

We thank the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) for funding to SA through grant number AR 872/3-1 and for a Heisenberg Fellowship grant numbers AR 872/4-1 and AR 872/7-1.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Alexandro-Rodríguez Rojas for his advice on the bacterial preparation methods, as well as Karolin Hublitz and Seulkee Yang for their technical support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2022.860875/full#supplementary-material

References

Acuña Hidalgo, B., Silva, L. M., Franz, M., Regoes, R. R., and Armitage, S. A. O. (2021). Decomposing virulence to understand bacterial clearance in persistent infections. bioRxiv [preprint] doi: 10.1101/2021.03.29.437521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arshadi, N., Mousavi, S. L., Amani, J., and Nazarian, S. (2020). Immunogenic potency of formalin and heat inactivated E. coli O157:H7 in mouse model administered by different routes. Avicenna J. Med. Biotechnol. 12, 194–200.

Google Scholar

Bass, T. M., Grandison, R. C., Wong, R., Martinez, P., Partridge, L., and Piper, M. D. W. (2007). Optimization of dietary restriction protocols in Drosophila. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 62, 1071–1081.

Google Scholar

Best, A., Tidbury, H., White, A., and Boots, M. (2013). The evolutionary dynamics of within-generation immune priming in invertebrate hosts. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20120887. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.0887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolker, B. M., Brooks, M. E., Clark, C. J., Geange, S. W., Poulsen, J. R., Henry, M., et al. (2009). Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 127–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boman, H. G., Nilsson, I., and Rasmuson, B. (1972). Inducible antibacterial defence system in Drosophila. Nature 237, 232–235.

Google Scholar

Castro-Vargas, C., Linares-López, C., Ópez-Torres, A. L., Wrobel, K., Torres-Guzmán, J. C., Hernández, G. A. G., et al. (2017). Methylation on RNA: a potential mechanism related to immune priming within but not across generations. Front. Microbiol. 8:473. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2017.00473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, M. C., Jacobson, E., Khalil, S., and Lazzaro, B. P. (2019). Consequences of chronic bacterial infection in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One 14:e0224440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christofi, T., and Apidianakis, Y. (2013). Drosophila immune priming against Pseudomonas aeruginosa is short-lasting and depends on cellular and humoral immunity. F1000Res. 2:76. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.2-76.v1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Garduno, J., Lanz-Mendoza, H., Franco, B., Nava, A., Pedraza-Reyes, M., and Canales-Lazcano, J. (2016). Insect immune priming: ecology and experimental evidences. Ecol. Entomol. 41, 351–366. doi: 10.1111/een.12300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Garduño, J., Rodríguez, M. C., Hernández-Martínez, S., Martínez-Barnetche, J., Alvarado-Delgado, A., Izquierdo, J., et al. (2015). Plasmodium berghei induced priming in Anopheles albimanus independently of bacterial co-infection. Dev. Comp. Immunol. 52, 172–181. doi: 10.1016/j.dci.2015.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhinaut, J., Chogne, M., and Moret, Y. (2018). Immune priming specificity within and across generations reveals the range of pathogens affecting evolution of immunity in an insect. J. Anim. Ecol. 87, 448–463. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubuffet, A., Zanchi, C., Boutet, G., Moreau, J., Teixeira, M., and Moret, Y. (2015). Trans-generational immune priming protects the eggs only against gram-positive bacteria in the mealworm beetle. PLoS Pathog. 11:e1005178. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1005178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duneau, D., Ebert, D., and Pasquier, L. D. (2016). Infections by Pasteuria do not protect its natural host Daphnia magna from subsequent infections. Dev. Comp. Immunol. 57, 120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.dci.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duneau, D., Ferdy, J.-B., Revah, J., Kondolf, H., Ortiz, G. A., Lazzaro, B. P., et al. (2017). Stochastic variation in the initial phase of bacterial infection predicts the probability of survival in D. melanogaster. ELife 6:e28298. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faulhaber, L. M., and Karp, R. D. (1992). A diphasic immune against bacteria in the American cockroach. Immunology 75, 378–381.

Google Scholar

Feldman, M. Y. (1973). Reactions of nucleic acids and nucleoproteins with formaIdehyde. Prog. Nucleic Acid Res. Mol. Biol. 13, 1–49.

Google Scholar

Forgy, C. (1965). Cluster analysis of multivariate data: efficiency versus interpretability of classifications. Biometrics 21, 768–769.

Google Scholar

Fraenkel-Conrat, H., and Olcott, H. S. (1948). The reaction of formaldehyde with proteins. V. Cross-linking between amino and primary amide or guanidyl groups. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 70, 2673–2684.

Google Scholar

Futo, M., Armitage, S. A. O., and Kurtz, J. (2016). Microbiota plays a role in oral immune priming in Tribolium castaneum. Front. Microbiol. 6:1383. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2015.01383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galac, M. R., and Lazzaro, B. P. (2011). Comparative pathology of bacteria in the genus Providencia to a natural host, Drosophila melanogaster. Microb. Infect. 13, 673–683.

Google Scholar

González-Tokman, D. M., González-Santoyo, I., Lanz-Mendoza, H., and Aguilar, A. C. (2010). Territorial damselflies do not show immunological priming in the wild. Physiol. Entomol. 35, 364–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.2010.00752.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, A. L., Shuker, D. M., Pollitt, L. C., Auld, S. K. J. R., Wilson, A. J., and Little, T. J. (2011). Fitness consequences of immune responses: strengthening the empirical framework for ecoimmunology. Funct. Ecol. 25, 5–17.

Google Scholar

Haine, E. R., Moret, Y., Siva-Jothy, M. T., and Rolff, J. (2008). Antimicrobial defense and persistent infection in insects. Science 322, 1257–1259. doi: 10.1126/science.1165265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, M. A., Dostálová, A., Ceroni, C., Poidevin, M., Kondo, S., and Lemaitre, B. (2019). Synergy and remarkable specificity of antimicrobial peptides in vivo using a systematic knockout approach. ELife 8:e44341.

Google Scholar

Hartigan, J. A., and Wong, M. A. (1979). Algorithm AS 136: a k-means clustering algorithm. J. R. Stat. Soc. 28, 100–108.

Google Scholar

Hartigan, P. M., and Hartigan, J. A. (1985). The dip test of unimodality. Ann. Stat. 13, 70–84.

Google Scholar

Hotson, A. G., and Schneider, D. S. (2015). Drosophila melanogaster natural variation affects growth dynamics of infecting Listeria monocytogenes. G3 Genes Genomes Genet. 5, 2593–2600. doi: 10.1534/g3.115.022558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juneja, P., and Lazzaro, B. P. (2009). Providencia sneebia sp. nov. and Providencia burhodogranariea sp. nov., isolated from wild Drosophila melanogaster. Int. J. Syst. Evol. Microbiol. 59, 1108–1111. doi: 10.1099/ijs.0.000117-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karp, R. D., and Rheins, L. A. (1980). Induction of specific humoral immunity to soluble proteins in the American cockroach (Periplaneta americana). II. Nature of the secondary response. Dev. Comp. Immunol. 4, 629–639. doi: 10.1016/s0145-305x(80)80065-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsui, N., Tsuchido, T., Hiramatsu, R., Fujikawa, S., Takano, M., and Shibasaki, I. (1982). Heat-induced blebbing and vesiculation of the outer membrane of Escherichia coli. J. Bacteriol. 151, 1523–1531. doi: 10.1128/jb.151.3.1523-1531.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtz, J. (2005). Specific memory within innate immune systems. Trends Immunol. 26, 186–192. doi: 10.1016/j.it.2005.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutzer, M. A. M., and Armitage, S. A. O. (2016b). Maximising fitness in the face of parasites: a review of host tolerance. Zoology 119, 281–289. doi: 10.1016/j.zool.2016.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutzer, M. A. M., and Armitage, S. A. O. (2016a). The effect of diet and time after bacterial infection on fecundity, resistance, and tolerance in Drosophila melanogaster. Ecol. Evol. 6, 4229–4242. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutzer, M. A. M., Kurtz, J., and Armitage, S. A. O. (2018). Genotype and diet affect resistance, survival, and fecundity but not fecundity tolerance. J. Evol. Biol. 31, 159–171. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutzer, M. A. M., Kurtz, J., and Armitage, S. A. O. (2019). A multi-faceted approach testing the effects of previous bacterial exposure on resistance and tolerance. J. Anim. Ecol. 88, 566–578. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafont, M., Petton, B., Vergnes, A., Pauletto, M., Segarra, A., Gourbal, B., et al. (2017). Long-lasting antiviral innate immune priming in the Lophotrochozoan Pacific oyster, Crassostrea gigas. Sci. Rep. 7:13143. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13564-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazzaro, B. P. (2002). A Population and Quantitative Genetic Analysis of the Drosophila melanogaster Antibacterial Immune Response. State College, PA: The Pennsylvania State University.

Google Scholar

Lazzaro, B. P., Sackton, T. B., and Clark, A. G. (2006). Genetic variation in Drosophila melanogaster resistance to infection: a comparison across bacteria. Genetics 174, 1539–1554. doi: 10.1534/genetics.105.054593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y. C., Chen, J. C., Morni, W. Z. W., Putra, D. F., Huang, C. L., Li, C. C., et al. (2013). Vaccination enhances early immune responses in white shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei after secondary exposure to Vibrio alginolyticus. PLoS One 8:e69722. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, T. J., and Kraaijeveld, A. R. (2004). Ecological and evolutionary implications of immunological priming in invertebrates. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 58–60. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2003.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, S. (1982). Least squares quantization in PCM. IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory 28, 129–137. doi: 10.1109/tit.1982.1056489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longdon, B., Cao, C., Martinez, J., and Jiggins, F. M. (2013). Previous exposure to an RNA virus does not protect against subsequent infection in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One 8:e73833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacQueen, J. (1967). “Some methods for classification and analysis of multivariate observations,” in Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability, Vol. 1, (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press), 281–297.

Google Scholar

Maechler, M. (2016). diptest: Hartigan’s dip Test Statistic for Unimodality – Corrected.

Google Scholar

Makarova, O., Rodriguez-Rojas, A., Eravci, M., Weise, C., Dobson, A., Johnston, P., et al. (2016). Antimicrobial defence and persistent infection in insects revisited. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150296.

Google Scholar

Martins, N. E., Faria, V. G., Teixeira, L., Magalhães, S., and Sucena, É (2013). Host adaptation is contingent upon the infection route taken by pathogens. PLoS Pathog. 9:e1003601. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1003601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milutinović, B., and Kurtz, J. (2016). Immune memory in invertebrates. Semin. Immunol. 28, 328–342. doi: 10.1016/j.smim.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milutinović, B., Fritzlar, S., and Kurtz, J. (2014). Increased survival in the red flour beetle after oral priming with bacteria-conditioned media. J. Innate Immun. 6, 306–314. doi: 10.1159/000355211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyashita, A., Kizaki, H., Kawasaki, K., Sekimizu, K., and Kaito, C. (2014). Primed immune responses to gram-negative peptidoglycans confer infection resistance in silkworms. J. Biol. Chem. 289, 14412–14421. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M113.525139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyashita, A., Takahashi, S., Ishii, K., Sekimizu, K., and Kaito, C. (2015). Primed immune responses triggered by ingested bacteria lead to systemic infection tolerance in silkworms. PLoS One 10:e0130486. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrnogic, J., Castillo, J. C., Shokal, U., Yadav, S., Kenney, E., Heryanto, C., et al. (2018). Pre-exposure to non-pathogenic bacteria does not protect Drosophila against the entomopathogenic bacterium Photorhabdus. PLoS One 13:e0205256. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pham, L. N., Dionne, M. S., Shirasu-Hiza, M., and Schneider, D. S. (2007). A specific primed immune response in Drosophila is dependent on phagocytes. PLoS Pathog. 3:e26. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.0030026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pope, E. C., Powell, A., Roberts, E. C., Shields, R. J., Wardle, R., and Rowley, A. F. (2011). Enhanced cellular immunity in shrimp (Litopenaeus vannamei) after ‘vaccination’. PLoS One 6:e20960. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pradeu, T., and Du Pasquier, L. (2018). Immunological memory: what’s in a name? Immunol. Rev. 283, 7–20. doi: 10.1111/imr.12652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Råberg, L., Graham, A. L., and Read, A. F. (2009). Decomposing health: tolerance and resistance to parasites in animals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 37–49. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, A., and Chapuisat, M. (2012). No evidence for immune priming in ants exposed to a fungal pathogen. PLoS One 7:e35372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rheins, L. A., Karp, R. D., and Butz, A. (1980). Induction of specific humoral immunity to soluble proteins in the American cockroach (Periplaneta americana). I. Nature of the primary response. Dev. Comp. Immunol. 4, 447–458. doi: 10.1016/s0145-305x(80)80047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riessberger-Gallé, U., Hernández López, J., Schuehly, W., Crockett, S., Krainer, S., and Crailsheim, K. (2015). Immune responses of honeybees and their fitness costs as compared to bumblebees. Apidologie 46, 238–249. doi: 10.1007/s13592-014-0318-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, J., Brayner, F. A., Alves, L. C., Dixit, R., and Barillas-Mury, C. (2010). Hemocyte differentiation mediates innate immune memory in Anopheles gambiae mosquitoes. Science 329, 1353–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1190689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosengaus, R. B., Traniello, J. F. A., Chen, T., Brown, J. J., and Karp, R. D. (1999). Immunity in a social insect. Naturwissenschaften 86, 588–591.

Google Scholar

Roth, O., Sadd, B. M., Schmid-Hempel, P., and Kurtz, J. (2009). Strain-specific priming of resistance in the red flour beetle, Tribolium castaneum. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 276, 145–151. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, A. D. (2003). Lethal effects of heat on bacterial physiology and structure. Sci. Prog. 86, 115–137. doi: 10.3184/003685003783238699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadd, B. M., and Schmid-Hempel, P. (2006). Insect immunity shows specificity in protection upon secondary pathogen exposure. Curr. Biol. 16, 1206–1210. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitznagel, J. K., and Trainer, R. T. (1949). Quantitative studies on induction of active immunity to smooth, v form Salmonella typhosa: a comparison of antigenicity of heat-killed and formalin-killed bacterial suspensions. J. Immunol. 62, 229–235.

Google Scholar

Therneau, T. M. (2020). A Package for Survival Analysis in R’.

Google Scholar

Therneau, T. M., and Grambsch, P. M. (2000). Modeling Survival Data: Extending the Cox Model. New York: Springer.

Google Scholar

Tsuchido, T., Katsui, N., Takeuchi, A., Takano, M., and Shibasaki, I. (1985). Destruction of the outer membrane permeability barrier of Escherichia coli by heat treatment. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 50, 298–303. doi: 10.1128/aem.50.2.298-303.1985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, V., Cime-Castillo, J., and Lanz-Mendoza, H. (2020). Immune priming with inactive dengue virus during the larval stage of Aedes aegypti protects against the infection in adult mosquitoes. Sci. Rep. 10:6723. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63402-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G., Zhang, S., and Wang, Z. (2009). Responses of alternative complement expression to challenge with different combinations of Vibrio anguillarum, Escherichia coli and Staphylococcus aureus: evidence for specific immune priming in amphioxus Branchiostoma belcheri. Fish Shellfish Immunol. 26, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.fsi.2008.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wensing, K. U., Koppik, M., and Fricke, C. (2017). Precopulatory but not postcopulatory male reproductive traits diverge in response to mating system manipulation in Drosophila melanogaster. Ecol. Evol. 7, 10361–10378. doi: 10.1002/ece3.3542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, T., Liu, G., Kang, D., Ekengren, S., Steiner, H., and Hultmark, D. (2000). A family of peptidoglycan recognition proteins in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 13772–13777. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.25.13772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 40, 1–29.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., and Müller, K. (2020). dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation’.

Google Scholar

Wu, G. Q., Zhao, Z. Y., Liu, C. L., and Qiu, L. H. (2014). Priming Galleria mellonella (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae) larvae with heat-killed bacterial cells induced an enhanced immune protection against Photorhabdus luminescens TT01 and the role of innate immunity in the process. J. Econ. Entomol. 107, 559–569. doi: 10.1603/ec13455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, G., Yi, Y., Sun, J., Li, M., and Qiu, L. (2015). No evidence for priming response in Galleria mellonella larvae exposed to toxin protein PirA2B2 from Photorhabdus luminescens TT01: an association with the inhibition of the host cellular immunity. Vaccine 33, 6307–6313. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Z., Wu, G., Wang, J., Liu, C., and Qiu, L. (2013). Next-generation sequencing-based transcriptome analysis of Helicoverpa armigera larvae immune-primed with Photorhabdus luminescens TT01. PLoS One 8:e80146. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuang, Z.-H., Li, H., Yang, J.-B., Liu, X., Gao, Y.-Y., Li, Q.-F., et al. (2011). Gut SCP is an immune-relevant molecule involved in the primary immunological memory or pattern recognition in the amphioxus Branchiostoma belcheri. Fish Shellfish Immunol. 30, 700–705. doi: 10.1016/j.fsi.2010.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bacterial pathogen, Drosophila melanogaster, heat-killed bacteria, formaldehyde inactivated bacteria, immune priming, resistance, survival, innate immunity

Citation: Acuña Hidalgo B and Armitage SAO (2022) Host Resistance to Bacterial Infection Varies Over Time, but Is Not Affected by a Previous Exposure to the Same Pathogen. Front. Physiol. 13:860875. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.860875

Received: 23 January 2022; Accepted: 07 February 2022;
Published: 21 March 2022.

Edited by:

Bharat Bhusan Patnaik, Fakir Mohan University, India

Reviewed by:

Nolwenn M. Dheilly, Laboratoire de Ploufragan-Plouzané, Agence Nationale de Sécurité Sanitaire de l’Alimentation, de l’Environnement et du Travail (ANSES), France
Ulrich Theopold, Stockholm University, Sweden

Copyright © 2022 Acuña Hidalgo and Armitage. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sophie A. O. Armitage, sophie.armitage@fu-berlin.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.