Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 July 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Single-Cases in Psychology: From Practice to Theory View all 10 articles

Action Semantics at the Bottom of the Brain: Insights From Dysplastic Cerebellar Gangliocytoma

  • 1Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2Departamento de Educación Física y Salud, Instituto Superior de Educación Física, Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay
  • 3National Scientific and Technical Research Council, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 4Departamento de Lingüística y Literatura, Facultad de Humanidades, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 5Neuropsychiatry Department, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 6Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia
  • 7Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago de Chile, Chile
  • 8Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council (ARC), Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 9Faculty of Education, National University of Cuyo, Mendoza, Argentina

Recent embodied cognition research shows that access to action verbs in shallow-processing tasks becomes selectively compromised upon atrophy of the cerebellum, a critical motor region. Here we assessed whether cerebellar damage also disturbs explicit semantic processing of action pictures and its integration with ongoing motor responses. We evaluated a cognitively preserved 33-year-old man with severe dysplastic cerebellar gangliocytoma (Lhermitte-Duclos disease), encompassing most of the right cerebellum and the posterior part of the left cerebellum. The patient and eight healthy controls completed two semantic association tasks (involving pictures of objects and actions, respectively) that required motor responses. Accuracy results via Crawford’s modified t-tests revealed that the patient was selectively impaired in action association. Moreover, reaction-time analysis through Crawford’s Revised Standardized Difference Test showed that, while processing of action concepts involved slower manual responses in controls, no such effect was observed in the patient, suggesting that motor-semantic integration dynamics may be compromised following cerebellar damage. Notably, a Bayesian Test for a Deficit allowing for Covariates revealed that these patterns remained after covarying for executive performance, indicating that they were not secondary to extra-linguistic impairments. Taken together, our results extend incipient findings on the embodied functions of the cerebellum, offering unprecedented evidence of its crucial role in processing non-verbal action meanings and integrating them with concomitant movements. These findings illuminate the relatively unexplored semantic functions of this region while calling for extensions of motor cognition models.

Introduction

Motor brain networks play critical roles in grounding action meanings (verbal and non-verbal concepts related to bodily motion) and integrating them with ongoing manual movements (Buccino et al., 2005; Ibáñez et al., 2013; García and Ibáñez, 2014, 2016a). This association has been repeatedly shown for cortical regions (e.g., the primary and supplementary motor cortices) (Pulvermüller, 2005; Fischer and Zwaan, 2008; Vukovic et al., 2017) and frontostriatal networks affected by movement disorders (Cardona et al., 2013; García and Ibáñez, 2014; Kargieman et al., 2014; Melloni et al., 2015; Birba et al., 2017a). However, the contributions of the cerebellum, another key motor hub, have received comparatively lesser attention (García et al., 2016a; Guell et al., 2017). To bridge this gap, here we report the first assessment of explicit action-semantic processing and motor-semantic integration in a rare patient with bilateral cerebellar damage due to a dysplastic gangliocytoma (Lhermitte-Duclos disease).

The two domains in question are selectively or differentially compromised in patients with movement disorders (Kotz et al., 2009; Cardona et al., 2013). In Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease, atrophy of frontostriatal motor loops impairs implicit and explicit processing of words (Fernandino et al., 2013a; Bocanegra et al., 2015), sentences (Fernandino et al., 2013b; Cardona et al., 2014), and images (Ibáñez et al., 2013; Bocanegra et al., 2015; García et al., 2017b,d) evoking bodily motion, while also disrupting predictable interference or facilitation effects (García and Ibáñez, 2016a) that such stimuli produce on concomitant hand movements (Ibáñez et al., 2013; Cardona et al., 2014; Kargieman et al., 2014; Melloni et al., 2015; Buccino et al., 2018). Notably, such deficits emerge irrespective of the patients’ domain-general impairments (Ibáñez et al., 2013; Bocanegra et al., 2015, 2017; García et al., 2017b) and even in preclinical disease stages (Kargieman et al., 2014; García et al., 2017d). It follows that damage to regions implicated in motor function can markedly disrupt the embodiment of action semantics.

While the above evidence comes from frontostriatal lesion models, the same could be hypothesized regarding the cerebellum, another hub critically involved in motor control and motor learning (Fabbro, 2000; Ramnani, 2006; Glickstein and Doron, 2008). Indeed, this region has been associated with the construction of internal models, or simulations (Ito, 1993, 2008; Ramnani, 2006), and it has been argued to play critical roles in embodied cognitive evolution (Barton, 2012). More particularly, cerebellar atrophy in genetic ataxia has been associated with selective action-verb impairments in lexical decision, a task that does not require explicit semantic processing (García et al., 2016a; Guell et al., 2017).

The latter finding, in particular, suggests that the cerebellum may be critical for grounding action meanings in shallow-processing verbal tasks – i.e., when semantic access is unnecessary for task completion. However, no study has assessed whether cerebellar damage also disturbs explicit action-semantic processing in non-verbal tasks, let alone its integration with ongoing motor responses. Thus, it remains unknown whether the distinct role of the cerebellum in motor semantics holds across processing modalities (verbal vs. pictorial) and levels of semantic access (implicit vs. explicit). To bridge this gap, we assessed a patient with severe dysplastic cerebellar gangliocytoma and eight healthy controls on two picture-based semantic association tasks (one involving objects, the other involving actions) requiring hand responses. In line with previous results (García et al., 2016a) and current embodied models (García and Ibáñez, 2016a), we hypothesized that cerebellar damage could selectively impair both the semantic association of actions and motor-semantic integration. In sum, we aimed to profit from this unique case to gain new insights into the contributions of the cerebellum to embodied cognitive functions.

Materials and Methods

Case Description

Patient NA is a 35-year-old, Spanish-speaking, right-handed Argentine man with 14 years of formal education. The patient reported a family history of neurological disease (his grandfather had dementia), psychiatric disease (his grandmother suffered from depression), and an antecedent of sudden death (his older brother died 3 months after birth).

On December 20, 2015, at age 33, NA experienced vertigo, low pressure, and generalized body weakness. Four days later, he manifested progressive dysarthria. At the end of January 2016, he suffered from sudden loss of consciousness but resumed normal activities after a few days. Throughout the following month, persistent signs of dysarthria were accompanied by reduced right-hand agility and progressive gait instability – mainly due to right-leg abnormalities. FLAIR and T2 MRI sequences revealed mild hyperintensity on the cerebellum without contrast enhancement, alongside thickened folia, small cysts, and sparing of the fourth ventricle. A posterior biopsy, together with histological and immunohistochemical studies, confirmed the diagnosis of dysplastic cerebellar gangliocytoma (Lhermitte-Duclos disease) as WHO stage IV.

In March 2016, NA started pharmacological treatment, shifting between Valcas (250 mg qd), Logical (200 mg tid), and Gabapentin (100 and 200 mg tid). That same year, on September 27, NA was hospitalized after experiencing aggravated vertigo, oscillopsia, and ataxia. Motor-system impairment was variously documented. In addition to right-sided horizontal gaze nystagmus (grade 2) and hearing deficits (negative Rinne’s test on the right side and left-lateralized Weber’s test), neurological examination revealed mild dysarthria, loss of balance (positive Rhomberg’s test), right-dominant muscular hypotonia with preserved force, motor nerve disturbances (positive Hoffman’s test on the right side), and ataxic gait. A follow-up MRI revealed an expansive right cortical-subcortical cerebellar lesion, characterized by hypointensity in T1 and corresponding hyperintensity in T2 and FLAIR, weighted signals with pseudocystic formations and no contrast enhancement, perilesional edema with mass effect on adjacent structures and the fourth ventricle, a right cerebellar nodular mass corresponding to a primary neo-proliferative lesion, and a discrete intensity change on the left cerebellum. No other signs of atrophy or malformations were observed, and lesions were essentially restricted to cerebellar structures. Given the rarity of Lhermitte-Duclos disease – with roughly 220 cases reported by 2006 (Robinson and Cohen, 2006)–, alongside its highly focal compromise of the cerebellum and its pervasive impact on motor function (Marcus et al., 1996; Nowak and Trost, 2002), this case offers a unique opportunity to test our hypothesis.

Control Participants

The patient’s performance on the experimental tasks was compared with that of eight right-handed healthy men with no history of neurological or psychiatric disease. This control group matched the patient in terms of age (31.6 ± 5.53, p = 0.82) and education level (15.6 ± 2.7, p = 0.58). The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO, now a host institution of the Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience), with written informed consent from all subjects. All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and written informed consent was obtained from patient NA for the publication of this case report. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of INECO.

Neuroimaging: Lesion Localization

Following previous procedures (García-Cordero et al., 2015, 2016; Melloni et al., 2016), an expert neurologist (JF) manually traced the patient’s lesion in native space. A T1 scan shows the extension of damage, comprising various parts of the cerebellum. Coordinates obtained from the Automated Anatomical Labeling software (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) indicated that cerebellar damage included most of the right anterior and posterior hemisphere (comprising lobules VII, VIIB, VI, and IX, as well as crus I and II regions), part of the left posterior lobe (lobules VII, VIIB, IX, X, and crus II), and a few areas from the vermis (regions VII, VIII, IX, and X) (Figure 1, panels A1–A3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Neuroanatomical and behavioral evidence from patient NA. (A) Extent of the tumor highlighted in red on the original T1 MRI scan of the patient’s brain: (A1) axial view; (A2) coronal view; (A3) sagittal section – right (up) left (down). (B) Behavioral performance of patient and controls on the Pyramids and Palm Trees (PPT) test and the Kissing and Dancing Test (KDT). (B1) The patient showed significantly reduced accuracy on the KDT (p < 0.001) but not on the PPT test (p = 0.93). The patient’s deficit on the KDT remained after covarying for global scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) battery, and the State version of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S) – all p-values < 0.001. (B2) Subtraction of PPT from KDT accuracy scores in each subject showed a difference of 28 answers for the patient and an average difference of –0.3 in controls; this difference was significant (p = 0.001) and it remained so after covarying for MoCA (p = 0.005), IFS (p = 0.01), and STAI-S (p = 0.004) scores. (B3) Reaction times for the patient were similar to those of controls on both the PPT test (p = 0.59) and the KDT (p = 0.32). (B4) However, subtraction of reaction times on the PPT test from those of the KDT in each subject revealed notably longer latencies (∼800 ms more) for the latter in controls, and virtually null differences in the patient; such a difference between the patient and controls was significant (p = 0.005) and it remained so after covarying for MoCA (p < 0.001), IFS (p = 0.01), and STAI-S (p = 0.004) scores. The asterisk () indicates significant differences.

Instruments

Neuropsychological and Psychiatric Evaluation

The assessment protocol included instruments tapping overall cognitive status, executive functions, and anxiety levels.

The participants’ overall cognitive state was assessed with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), a sensitive tool for detecting cognitive dysfunction in populations with atrophy of motor regions, such as the basal ganglia (Gluhm et al., 2013; Bocanegra et al., 2015, 2017) and the cerebellum (Mercadillo et al., 2014; Dogan et al., 2016; García et al., 2016a). The MoCA covers eight cognitive domains, namely: visuospatial/executive skills, naming, memory, attention, language, abstraction, delayed recall, and orientation. It has a maximum of 30 points, and its total score is corrected for the participant’s years of education.

In addition, executive functions were assessed through the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) battery (Torralva et al., 2009). This tool has proved sensitive for population with damage to motor regions (Cardona et al., 2014; Bocanegra et al., 2015; García et al., 2016a, 2017a). The IFS taps domains such as motor programming, conflict resolution, inhibitory control, and working memory. This battery comprises 20 items, and its maximum score is 30.

Anxiety levels during the cognitive tasks were assessed via the state version of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S; Spielberger et al., 1970). The psychometric properties of this instrument attest to its reliability and validity to detect situational anxiety (Spielberger et al., 1980) in the general population (Barnes et al., 2002). It has also been used in patients with damage in motor regions, such as the basal ganglia (Leritz et al., 2004; Tinaz et al., 2011) and the cerebellum (Zawacki et al., 2002; Orsi et al., 2011). The STAI-S comprises 20 self-report items inquiring on the examinee’s current feelings. Each item rates severity on a 1–4 scale, so that the total score ranges from 20 to 80.

Semantic Association of Objects and Actions

Semantic association of objects and actions was assessed through the Pyramids and Palm Trees (PPT) test (Howard and Patterson, 1992), and the Kissing and Dancing Test (KDT) (Bak and Hodges, 2003), respectively. In both tests, participants must choose which of two pictures is most closely related to a cue picture. Each test comprises 52 trials, yielding a maximum score of 52. These instruments have revealed specific deficits in patients with damage to motor regions, such as the basal ganglia (Cardona et al., 2014; Bocanegra et al., 2015; García et al., 2017b,d), and other less focused injuries that include cerebellar atrophy (Baez et al., 2013).

The patient and all controls were tested individually in a dimly illuminated room. They sat comfortably at a desk, in front of an Intel Core i5-3470 PC equipped with a monitor (Lenovo 15.6” 16:9 HD LED backlight display) and a QWERTY keyboard (gx gaming).

For this study we implemented a computerized version of both tasks, designed and run on DMDX software1, as to automatically record accuracy and reaction-time (RT) data and thus assess the impact of action-semantic processing on concurrent manual actions (i.e., responses made by pressing of predefined keyboard buttons). In each trial, the cue picture was located at the top of the screen and the two option pictures appeared at the bottom, in a pyramid-like arrangement. All three images were presented simultaneously and they remained on the screen for 5 s. If no response was made within this time interval, a blank screen was shown for a maximum of 5 s before the next trial appeared.

Participants were instructed to respond as fast and accurately as possible by pressing the left or the right arrow of the keyboard with two fingers of the dominant hand, indicating their choice of the picture at the corresponding location. Each key press was logged to compute accuracy and RT, and it triggered a 1-s blank screen followed by the next trial. Prior to each task, the instructions were recapped on-screen and four additional items were presented for practice purposes. The patient performed the PPT test first and then the KDT, whereas both tasks were counterbalanced across control participants.

Statistical Analysis

Demographic, neuropsychological, psychiatric, and behavioral data were compared between the patient and controls via two-tailed Crawford’s modified t-tests (Crawford and Howell, 1998; Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002), as done in previous studies (Couto et al., 2013a,b; Sedeño et al., 2014; García et al., 2016a,b; Birba et al., 2017b). This test allows comparing test scores from one or more individuals with norms derived from small samples. It has been proved to be robust for non-normal distributions and it presents low rates of type-I error (Crawford and Howell, 1998; Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002, 2012; Crawford et al., 2009, 2011).

Also, to assess performance differences between the PPT test and the KDT, RT data from the patient and the controls were analyzed via the Revised Standardized Difference Test (RSDT) (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2005). This test examines the difference between a subject’s scores on two tasks relative to the difference observed in the control group, considering the distribution of values in the latter and its correlation between tasks. As the modified t-test, the RSDT controls for the type-I error rate in the case of small control samples (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2005). In RT analyses, all trials exceeding 2 SDs from the subject’s mean were removed as outliers (less than 10% of the trials in all cases).

Finally, to determine whether hypothesized differences between the patient and controls were influenced by the former’s cognitive status, executive skills, or anxiety levels, all analyses were repeated with global MoCA, IFS, and STAI-S scores as independent covariates. These analyses were based on a Bayesian Test for a Deficit allowing for Covariates (BTD-Cov) when Crawford’s t-test was applied, and a Bayesian Standardized Difference Test allowing for Covariates (BSDT-Cov) when the RSDT was applied (Crawford et al., 2011). Effect sizes obtained from these methods are reported as point estimates (ZCCC and ZDCCC for covaried results from the modified t-test and RSDT, respectively), as suggested in a previous study (Crawford et al., 2010). In all analyses, alpha levels were set at p < 0.05.

Results

Cognitive Status, Executive Functions, and State Anxiety Level

No significant differences emerged between the patient and controls in the MoCA, the IFS battery, or the STAI-S (Table 1). Therefore, the patient showed no cognitive deficits or abnormal anxiety levels.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Overall cognitive profile and anxiety levels.

Semantic Performance: Accuracy Results

Compared to controls, the patient exhibited normal accuracy on the PPT test (t = −0.09, ZCC = −0.09, p = 0.93), with a marked impairment on the KDT (t = −8.41, ZCC = −8.94, p < 0.001). This differential pattern remained after covarying for MoCA (PPT: ZCCC = −0.1, p = 0.94; KDT: ZCCC = −10.18, p < 0.001), IFS (PPT: ZCCC = −0.41, p = 0.78; KDT: ZCCC = −8.84, p < 0.001). and STAI-S (PPT: ZCCC = −0.19, p = 0.88; KDT: ZCCC = −9.74, p < 0.001) scores (Figure 1, panel B1). Also, a comparison of between-task differences (KDT minus PPT) highlighted the markedly differential outcome for the patient in the KDT (t = 5.29, ZDCC = 6.51, p = 0.001) and corroborated their independence from general cognitive skills (MoCA: ZDCCC = 7.5, p = 0.005), executive functions (IFS: ZDCCC = 6.44, p = 0.01), and state anxiety (STAI-S: ZDCCC = 7.71, p = 0.004) (Figure 1, panel B2).

Semantic Performance: RT Results

Moreover, although RTs revealed no significant differences between the patient and controls on either the PPT test (t = 0.58, ZCC = 0.62, p = 0.59) or the KDT (t = −1.08, ZCC = −1.15, p = 0.32) (Figure 1, panel B3), analysis of between-task differences via Crawford’s RSDT revealed a specific abnormality: whereas controls responded more slowly on the KDT than on the PPT test, no such difference was observed in the patient (t = 4.08, ZDCC = 5.58, p = 0.005). This result, too, was uninfluenced by general cognitive skills (MoCA: ZDCCC = 13.47, p < 0.001), executive functions (IFS: ZDCCC = 6.32, p = 0.01), and state anxiety (STAI-S: ZDCCC = 7.44, p = 0.004) (Figure 1, panel B4).

Discussion

This is the first study examining explicit processing of action-related meanings and their integration with ongoing actions in a patient with extensive cerebellar damage. The patient exhibited a selective impairment of action semantics, relative to object semantics, together with a probable alteration of the predictable motor-semantic integration pattern observed in controls. Furthermore, both patterns remained after covarying for executive skills. Below we discuss these findings in turn, addressing their theoretical and clinical relevance.

Cerebellar Damage and Action Semantics

First, the patient showed selective deficits in processing action (as opposed to object) semantics. This highlights the role of the cerebellum in grounding movement-related meanings, arguably because of its critical role in motor function (Ramnani, 2006; Manto et al., 2012). In healthy subjects, action semantics is differentially related to activity in the primary motor and premotor cortices (Jirak et al., 2010; Vigliocco et al., 2011; García and Ibáñez, 2016a). Moreover, this domain is selectively affected by damage to those regions (Neininger and Pulvermüller, 2003) or to frontostriatal motor loops (García et al., 2016b, 2017a,b; Birba et al., 2017a). Our results extend these findings, showing that damage to the cerebellum, another critical motor hub, can also lead to selective deficits in action-semantic processing.

A previous report on action-verb processing in cerebellar ataxia revealed selective deficits in this category, even though more demanding lexical classes, such as abstract verbs, were preserved (García et al., 2016a). Of note, the latter study found this impairment through a lexical decision task, involving implicit semantic access. Our study shows that cerebellar damage can lead to action semantic deficits even in explicit picture-based tasks, suggesting that the cerebellum plays a crucial role in grounding action-related meanings irrespective of presentation modality (verbal vs. pictorial) or mode of access (implicit vs. explicit), as previously observed for other motor regions (Pulvermüller, 2005; Jirak et al., 2010; Birba et al., 2017a). Taken together, this finding supports the view that the cerebellum may play a transmodal role in the embodiment of action-related meanings (García and Ibáñez, 2016a; Birba et al., 2017a), alongside more general contributions to semantic processing at large (Booth et al., 2007; De Smet et al., 2007; Murdoch, 2010; Barton, 2012; Mariën et al., 2014).

Compatibly, in fact, a feasible interpretation of our RT results is that the patient was also impaired in motor-semantic integration, another relevant embodied domain (García and Ibáñez, 2014, 2016a). In healthy subjects, processing of effector-specific action meanings can predictably interfere with contiguous hand movements (as indexed by increased RTs). This has been shown in multiple experimental settings, including semantic decision via single-key presses (e.g., Dalla Volta et al., 2014), semantic congruency judgment paradigms (e.g., Bernardis and Gentilucci, 2006; Barbieri et al., 2009), and keyboard-based verb-copying tasks (García and Ibáñez, 2016b). However, in disorders characterized by motor-network atrophy, such as Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease, systematic motor-semantic integration effects are abolished (Ibáñez et al., 2013; Cardona et al., 2014; Kargieman et al., 2014; Buccino et al., 2018), in the context of abnormal task-specific neural signatures (Melloni et al., 2015), and even before the onset of overt motor symptomatology (Kargieman et al., 2014). For example, in these populations, contrary to healthy controls, congruency between response-hand shape and the hand-position denoted by action verbs fails to significantly modulate RTs (Ibáñez et al., 2013; Cardona et al., 2014; Kargieman et al., 2014). Likewise, healthy individuals respond more slowly to stimuli involving motor affordances (i.e., pictures and words depicting graspable, as opposed to non-graspable, objects), but no such selective delay is observed in PD patients (Buccino et al., 2018). As proposed by (García and Ibáñez, 2018), these findings would show that motor-network atrophy disturbs the integration of manual movements with processing of action-related stimuli, due to a disruption of embodied mechanisms.

In line with this claim and its supporting evidence, our study offers the first indication that similar patterns could emerge upon damage to the cerebellum. Indeed, whereas manual responses in controls were slower for action than for object stimuli, no such interference was observed in the patient. Given that the same cerebellar regions are engaged by the execution and the observation of actions (Gazzola and Keysers, 2009), we surmise that joint recruitment of motor and action-semantic processes led to a competition for resources in controls, while such natural integrative dynamics became disturbed upon cerebellar damage in the patient. In fact, motor-network disruptions have been shown to result in the recruitment of alternative non-motor pathways during processing of action-related stimuli (Abrevaya et al., 2017), which warrants the possibility that similar abnormal grounding effects could be triggered by cerebellar compromise. In this sense, the cerebellum seems critical not only for the processing of action semantics per se, but also for the integration of action meanings with ongoing motoric behavior.

Admittedly, although the results support the proposed interpretation, other factors could be contributing to the observed patterns. First, larger RTs in the KDT than the PPT test in controls could be partially driven by differential stimulus-related demands in each task. Although both instruments are similar in structure and overall difficulty (Bak and Hodges, 2003), their respective images are not matched for fine-grained variables which could impact behavioral outcomes, such as visual complexity, familiarity, or age of acquisition – for examples from normative picture-based studies, see (Cycowicz et al., 1997; Manoiloff et al., 2010). Therefore, subtle differences in such variables may have partly contributed to the behavioral differences observed in controls. Second, the patient’s pattern of more errors and faster RTs on the PPT test could also be influenced by a trade-off between speed and accuracy: should this task prove harder than the KDT, instruction-induced time pressure could have led the patient to respond more quickly than his actual processing speed requires, resulting in a greater propensity to errors. While these factors cannot be fully ruled out as partial contributors to the results, their impact is only speculative and could well run in parallel to (rather than in lieu of) the argued abolished action-interference effect, which has been previously reported in patients with motor-network damage (Buccino et al., 2018). Further research would be necessary to establish the relative role of these factors on the observed effects.

Of note, the two patterns of deficit exhibited by the patient emerged despite otherwise normal cognitive performance, and they survived after covariation with MoCA, IFS, and STAI-S scores. This indicates that both forms of embodied disturbances were not dependent on the patient’s overall cognitive status, executive functioning, or state anxiety levels. Similar patterns have been observed in previous studies showing distinctive action-semantic deficits in Parkinson’s (Bocanegra et al., 2015, 2017) and Huntington’s (García et al., 2017b) disease, indicating that such impairments were sui generis (i.e., not secondary to domain-general dysfunctions). By the same token, our study suggests that action-semantic and motor-semantic-integration difficulties may emerge as primary manifestations not only following early damage to frontostriatal motor networks, but also to posterior motor hubs (in particular, the bilateral cerebellum).

Implications

Our work has theoretical and clinical implications. In the last decades, the cerebellum has been acknowledged as a key hub for adaptive control functions, including the modeling, prediction, and organization of motor, cognitive, and emotional behaviors (Schmahmann, 1991; Barton, 2012; Koziol et al., 2014; Guell et al., 2017). More particularly, sparse evidence has hinted to its role in semantic processing, through coarse-grained tasks such as word selection (Silveri and Misciagna, 2000; Murdoch, 2010) and story comprehension (Mar, 2011). However, as established in a recent consensus paper, the contributions of the cerebellum to semantic and other higher-order domains represent an “ongoing enigma” (Mariën et al., 2014). In light of our results, and considering current theoretical proposals (Barton, 2012), we propose that important theoretical breakthroughs can be made by studying cerebellar function from an embodied perspective.

Anticipatory control loops in the cerebellum have been implicated in the mental rehearsal and imagination of actions, as well as in the prediction of their distal and abstract consequences (Koziol et al., 2014). Among other things, these processes would support the simulation (Jeannerod, 2001; Hesslow, 2002) and emulation (Grush, 2004) of bodily states, as tacitly assumed by recent models characterizing the prediction and understanding of external events (Schubotz, 2007). These general findings broadly support the view that the cerebellum plays a distinct role in grounding action-specific meanings. However, no specific proposals have been advanced in such a direction, arguably because there is no consensus on the role of motor and non-motor cerebellar regions in processing action-related rules (Balsters and Ramnani, 2008; Koziol et al., 2014).

In light of our results, we propose that the cerebellum could constitute a key hub in the vast motor-preferential network supporting the embodiment of action meanings (García and Ibáñez, 2016a; García et al., 2016a). Notably, most neuroanatomical models of action semantics (Pulvermüller, 2005, 2018; Garagnani and Pulvermüller, 2016) emphasize the putative role of cortical motor regions, failing to acknowledge the contributions of subcortical and cerebellar motor hubs. This may largely be the case because several relevant studies have found cerebellar activity but failed to include it in their discussions or accompanying summary diagrams (Decety and Grèzes, 1999; Shapiro et al., 2005; Lauro et al., 2013), and because relevant lesion models of damage to subcortical motor regions have been overlooked in the field (Birba et al., 2017a). However, as shown by present results, previous evidence of action-language deficits following cerebellar atrophy (García et al., 2016a), and even some imaging studies of action semantics including results from the cerebellum (Saccuman et al., 2006; Rüschemeyer et al., 2007; Boulenger et al., 2009), the embodied foundations of action semantics may span across any and all regions subserving motor function.

This proposal also entails clinical implications. On the assumption that the contributions of the cerebellum to higher-order processes were restricted to managing novel situations, organizing responses or creating linguistic strategies (Copland et al., 2000), various authors have proposed that deficits triggered by cerebellar damage could be more accurately detected and characterized through assessments of complex domain-general processes (Murdoch, 2010). While such approaches are certainly useful, here we propose that more fine-grained examinations targeting specific semantic categories (in particular, those alluding to bodily motion) could represent a novel clinical alternative. Indeed, the very combination of tasks used in this study has revealed deficits in early (Bocanegra et al., 2015) and even preclinical (García et al., 2017b) stages of motor disorders characterized by subcortical motor-network atrophy.

Building on the notion that deficits in both action semantics and motor-language coupling could constitute sensitive biomarkers of frontostriatal motor loops (Ibáñez et al., 2013; García and Ibáñez, 2014; Birba et al., 2017a), our findings suggest that relevant tasks could also reveal subtle and primary signatures of cerebellar damage. In this sense, embodied semantic tasks could emerge as robust transdiagnostic tools for detecting early motor-network disruptions, irrespective of lesion site or etiology. However, replication and normative studies are needed to directly test this possibility.

Limitations and Suggestions for Further Research

Two main limitations must be recognized in this work. First, the control sample had a modest size. However, our tests are considered robust with small control groups (∼5 subjects) (Crawford and Howell, 1998), as attested by previous single-case studies yielding robust findings with similar or even smaller control-sample sizes (Bak et al., 2006; García et al., 2016a, 2017c; Birba et al., 2017b), or even in the absence of control groups (Caramazza and Hillis, 1991; Silveri et al., 1998; Berndt and Haendiges, 2000). Second, our assessment was restricted to a single cognitive task, whereas the explored domains manifest in multiple ways. Notwithstanding, note that the KDT and the PPT test have shown good sensitivity for revealing differential and selective deficits in other pathologies (Bak and Hodges, 2004; Kargieman et al., 2014; Bocanegra et al., 2015; Tsermentseli et al., 2016; García et al., 2017b,d), which attests to their empirical relevance.

Looking forward, although single cases are crucial to determine potential links between cognitive impairments and neuroanatomical injuries (Dubois and Adolphs, 2016), future studies should test the replicability of our results in a broad population of cerebellar patients. In particular, regression models could be implemented between semantic performance and structural or functional neural correlates, with a view to identifying specific cerebellar regions implicated in action semantics. This could illuminate the controversy regarding the contributions of motor and non-motor portions of the cerebellum to processing of action-related information (Balsters and Ramnani, 2008; Koziol et al., 2014). Also, given that the cerebellum and the basal ganglia have profuse connections with classical language areas (Booth et al., 2007), assessment of structural and functional connectivity in patients with cerebellar damage could reveal the putative basis of potential embodied deficits, as revealed by relevant behavioral tasks. Finally, a comparison of action-semantic processing between cerebellar and non-cerebellar models of motor-network lesions could reveal informative dissociations and inform fine-grained models of embodied cognition.

Conclusion

This study offers unprecedented evidence that cerebellar damage could alter explicit processing of action-related meanings and their integration with ongoing actions. These findings illuminate the relatively unexplored semantic functions of this region while calling for extensions of motor cognition models. Moreover, as previously shown in other movement disorders, embodied semantic tasks also offer promising alternatives for detecting early motor-network disruptions upon cerebellar damage. In this sense, the study of cerebellar contributions to action-semantic processing may afford a fruitful overarching framework for future basic and applied research in cognitive neuroscience.

Author Contributions

AG, AI, and LS conceived the study. SA, MMC, and GK collected the data. SC, EM, JF, and AG analyzed the data. SA and SC designed Figure 1. SC and AG wrote the manuscript. AI and LS provided critical revisions on the successive drafts. All authors approved the manuscript in its final form.

Funding

This work was supported by CONICET; CONICYT/ FONDECYT (Regular 1170010); Programa Interdisciplinario de Investigación Experimental en Comunicación y Cognición (PIIECC), Facultad de Humanidades, USACH; Proyecto Basal USACH USA1398_ME112214; FONDAP (15150012); and the INECO Foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.u.arizona.edu/~kforster/dmdx/dmdx.htm

References

Abrevaya, S., Sedeño, L., Fitipaldi, S., Pineda, D., Lopera, F., Buritica, O., et al. (2017). The road less traveled: alternative pathways for action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55, 1429–1435. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baez, S., Couto, B., Herrera, E., Bocanegra, Y., Trujillo-Orrego, N., Madrigal-Zapata, L., et al. (2013). Tracking the cognitive, social, and neuroanatomical profile in early neurodegeneration: type III Cockayne syndrome. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:80. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). Kissing and dancing—a test to distinguish the lexical and conceptual contributions to noun/verb and action/object dissociation. Preliminary results in patients with frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurolinguist. 16, 169–181. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., and Hodges, J. R. (2004). The effects of motor neurone disease on language: further evidence. Brain Lang. 89, 354–361. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00357-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., Yancopoulou, D., Nestor, P. J., Xuereb, J. H., Spillantini, M. G., Pulvermüller, F., et al. (2006). Clinical, imaging and pathological correlates of a hereditary deficit in verb and action processing. Brain 129, 321–332. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsters, J. H., and Ramnani, N. (2008). Symbolic representations of action in the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 43, 388–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbieri, F., Buonocore, A., Volta, R. D., and Gentilucci, M. (2009). How symbolic gestures and words interact with each other. Brain Lang. 110, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, L. L., Harp, D., and Jung, W. S. (2002). Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 62, 603–618. doi: 10.1177/0013164402062004005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, R. A. (2012). Embodied cognitive evolution and the cerebellum. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 2097–2107. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardis, P., and Gentilucci, M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia 44, 178–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berndt, R. S., and Haendiges, A. N. (2000). Grammatical class in word and sentence production: evidence from an aphasic patient. J. Mem. Lang. 43, 249–273. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2726

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birba, A., García-Cordero, I., Kozono, G., Legaz, A., Ibáñez, A., Sedeño, L., et al. (2017a). Losing ground: frontostriatal atrophy disrupts language embodiment in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 673–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birba, A., Hesse, E., Sedeño, L., Mikulan, E., García, M., Ávalos, J., et al. (2017b). Enhanced working memory binding by direct electrical stimulation of the parietal cortex. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:178. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocanegra, Y., García, A. M., Lopera, F., Pineda, D., Baena, A., Ospina, P., et al. (2017). Unspeakable motion: selective action-verb impairments in Parkinson’s disease patients without mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang. 168, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocanegra, Y., García, A. M., Pineda, D., Buriticá, O., Villegas, A., Lopera, F., et al. (2015). Syntax, action verbs, action semantics, and object semantics in Parkinson’s disease: dissociability, progression, and executive influences. Cortex 69, 237–254. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Wood, L., Lu, D., Houk, J. C., and Bitan, T. (2007). The role of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in language processing. Brain Res. 1133, 136–144. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., and Pulvermüller, F. (2009). Grasping ideas with the motor system: semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1905–1914. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Dalla Volta, R., Arabia, G., and Morelli, M. (2018). Processing graspable object images and their nouns is impaired in Parkinson’s disease patients. Cortex 100, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Brain Res. Cogn. 24, 355–363.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., and Hillis, A. E. (1991). Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain. Nature 349, 788–790.

Google Scholar

Cardona, J. F., Gershanik, O., Gelormini-Lezama, C., Houck, A. L., Cardona, S., Kargieman, L., et al. (2013). Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor-language coupling. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 1355–1373. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0510-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardona, J. F., Kargieman, L., Sinay, V., Gershanik, O., Gelormini, C., Amoruso, L., et al. (2014). How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases. Cognition 131, 311–322. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Copland, D. A., Chenery, H. J., and Murdoch, B. E. (2000). Persistent deficits in complex language function following dominant nonthalamic subcortical lesions. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 8, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Couto, B., Salles, A., Sedeño, L., Peradejordi, M., Barttfeld, P., Canales-Johnson, A., et al. (2013a). The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1253–1260. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couto, B., Sedeño, L., Sposato, L. A., Sigman, M., Riccio, P. M., Salles, A., et al. (2013b). Insular networks for emotional processing and social cognition: comparison of two case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement. Cortex 49, 1420–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia 40, 1196–1208. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00224-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2005). Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: evaluation of alternatives using Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. Neuropsychology 19, 318–331. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2012). Single-case research in neuropsychology: a comparison of five forms of t-test for comparing a case to controls. Cortex 48, 1009–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., and Howell, D. C. (2009). On comparing a single case with a control sample: an alternative perspective. Neuropsychologia 47, 2690–2695. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., and Porter, S. (2010). Point and interval estimates of effect sizes for the case-controls design in neuropsychology: rationale, methods, implementations, and proposed reporting standards. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 27, 245–260. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2010.513967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., and Ryan, K. (2011). Comparing a single case to a control sample: testing for neuropsychological deficits and dissociations in the presence of covariates. Cortex 47, 1166–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Howell, D. C. (1998). Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. Clin. Neuropsychol. 12, 482–486.

Google Scholar

Cycowicz, Y. M., Friedman, D., Rothstein, M., and Snodgrass, J. G. (1997). Picture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 65, 171–237. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.2356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Volta, R., Fabbri-Destro, M., Gentilucci, M., and Avanzini, P. (2014). Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 61, 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Smet, H. J., Baillieux, H., De Deyn, P. P., Mariën, P., and Paquier, P. (2007). The cerebellum and language: the story so far. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 59, 165–170.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Grèzes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 172–178. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01312-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dogan, I., Tinnemann, E., Romanzetti, S., Mirzazade, S., Costa, A. S., Werner, C. J., et al. (2016). Cognition in Friedreich’s ataxia: a behavioral and multimodal imaging study. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 3, 572–587. doi: 10.1002/acn3.315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, J., and Adolphs, R. (2016). Building a science of individual differences from fMRI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 425–443. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabbro, F. (2000). Introduction to language and cerebellum. J. Neurolinguist. 13, 83–94. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00005-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandino, L., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Blindauer, K., Hiner, B., Spangler, K., et al. (2013a). Parkinson’s disease disrupts both automatic and controlled processing of action verbs. Brain Lang. 127, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandino, L., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Blindauer, K., Hiner, B., Spangler, K., et al. (2013b). Where is the action? Action sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 51, 1510–1517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H., and Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 825–850. doi: 10.1080/17470210701623605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garagnani, M., and Pulvermüller, F. (2016). Conceptual grounding of language in action and perception: a neurocomputational model of the emergence of category specificity and semantic hubs. Eur. J. Neurosci. 43, 721–737. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Abrevaya, S., Kozono, G., Cordero, I. G., Córdoba, M., Kauffman, M. A., et al. (2016a). The cerebellum and embodied semantics: evidence from a case of genetic ataxia due to STUB1 mutations. J. Med. Genet. 54, 114–124. doi: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2016-104148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Bocanegra, Y., Herrera, E., Moreno, L., Carmona, J., Baena, A., et al. (2017a). Parkinson’s disease compromises the appraisal of action meanings evoked by naturalistic texts. Cortex 100, 111–126. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Bocanegra, Y., Herrera, E., Pino, M., Muñoz, E., Sedeño, L., et al. (2017b). Action-semantic and syntactic deficits in subjects at risk for Huntington’s disease. J. Neuropsychol. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12120 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Carrillo, F., Orozco-Arroyave, J. R., Trujillo, N., Vargas Bonilla, J. F., Fittipaldi, S., et al. (2016b). How language flows when movements don’t: an automated analysis of spontaneous discourse in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Lang. 162, 19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2014). Words in motion: motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease. Transl. Neurosci. 5, 152–159. doi: 10.2478/s13380-014-0218-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2016a). A touch with words: dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 59–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2016b). Hands typing what hands do: action-semantic integration dynamics throughout written verb production. Cognition 149, 56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2018). When embodiment breaks down: language deficits as novel avenues into movement disorders. Cortex 100, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Sedeño, L., Herrera Murcia, E., Couto, B., and Ibáñez, A. (2017c). A lesion-proof brain? Multidimensional sensorimotor, cognitive, and socio-affective preservation despite extensive damage in a stroke patient. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:335. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Sedeño, L., Trujillo, N., Bocanegra, Y., Gomez, D., Pineda, D., et al. (2017d). Language deficits as a preclinical window into Parkinson’s disease: evidence from asymptomatic Parkin and Dardarin mutation carriers. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 150–158. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Cordero, I., Sedeño, L., de la Fuente, L., Slachevsky, A., Forno, G., Klein, F., et al. (2016). Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371:20160006. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Cordero, I., Sedeño, L., Fraiman, D., Craiem, D., de la Fuente, L. A., Salamone, P., et al. (2015). Stroke and neurodegeneration induce different connectivity aberrations in the insula. Stroke 46, 2673–2677. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.009598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzola, V., and Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1239–1255. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glickstein, M., and Doron, K. (2008). Cerebellum: connections and functions. Cerebellum 7, 589–594. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0074-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluhm, S., Goldstein, J., Brown, D., Van Liew, C., Gilbert, P. E., and Corey-Bloom, J. (2013). Usefulness of the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) in Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 28, 1744–1747. doi: 10.1002/mds.25578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 377–396. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guell, X., Gabrieli, J. D., and Schmahmann, J. D. (2017). Embodied cognition and the cerebellum: perspectives from the dysmetria of thought and the universal cerebellar transform theories. Cortex 100, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesslow, G. (2002). Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 242–247. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01913-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, D., and Patterson, K. E. (1992). The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test: A Test of Semantic Access from Words and Pictures. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Google Scholar

Ibáñez, A., Cardona, J. F., Dos Santos, Y. V., Blenkmann, A., Aravena, P., Roca, M., et al. (2013). Motor-language coupling: direct evidence from early Parkinson’s disease and intracranial cortical recordings. Cortex 49, 968–984. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, M. (1993). Movement and thought: identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 16, 448–450.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 304–313. doi: 10.1038/nrn2332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14, S103–S109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language–a short story on embodiment. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 711–720. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kargieman, L., Herrera, E., Baez, S., García, A. M., Dottori, M., Gelormini, C., et al. (2014). Motor–language coupling in Huntington’s disease families. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:122. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotz, S. A., Schwartze, M., and Schmidt-Kassow, M. (2009). Non-motor basal ganglia functions: a review and proposal for a model of sensory predictability in auditory language perception. Cortex 45, 982–990. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koziol, L. F., Budding, D., Andreasen, N., D’Arrigo, S., Bulgheroni, S., Imamizu, H., et al. (2014). Consensus paper: the cerebellum’s role in movement and cognition. Cerebellum 13, 151–177. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0511-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauro, L. J. R., Mattavelli, G., Papagno, C., and Tettamanti, M. (2013). She runs, the road runs, my mind runs, bad blood runs between us: literal and figurative motion verbs: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 83, 361–371. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leritz, E., Loftis, C., Crucian, G., Friedman, W., and Bowers, D. (2004). Self-awareness of deficits in Parkinson disease. Clin. Neuropsychol. 18, 352–361. doi: 10.1080/1385404049052412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manoiloff, L., Artstein, M., Canavoso, M. B., Fernandez, L., and Segui, J. (2010). Expanded norms for 400 experimental pictures in an Argentinean Spanish-speaking population. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 452–460. doi: 10.3758/brm.42.2.452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manto, M., Bower, J. M., Conforto, A. B., Delgado-García, J. M., da Guarda, S. N. F., Gerwig, M., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control—the diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. Cerebellum 11, 457–487. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0331-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 103–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, C. D., Galeon, M., Peruzzi, P., Bazin, A., Bernard, M. H., Pluot, M., et al. (1996). Lhermitte-Duclos disease associated with syringomyelia. Neuroradiology 38, 529–531. doi: 10.1007/BF00626089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariën, P., Ackermann, H., Adamaszek, M., Barwood, C. H., Beaton, A., Desmond, J., et al. (2014). Consensus paper: language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum 13, 386–410. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0540-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., De la Fuente, L., Forno, G., et al. (2016). Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 3022–3040. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melloni, M., Sedeño, L., Hesse, E., García-Cordero, I., Mikulan, E., Plastino, A., et al. (2015). Cortical dynamics and subcortical signatures of motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease. Sci. Rep. 5:11899. doi: 10.1038/srep11899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercadillo, R. E., Galvez, V., Díaz, R., Hernández-Castillo, C. R., Campos-Romo, A., Boll, M. C., et al. (2014). Parahippocampal gray matter alterations in Spinocerebellar Ataxia Type 2 identified by voxel based morphometry. J. Neurol. Sci. 347, 50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdoch, B. E. (2010). The cerebellum and language: historical perspective and review. Cortex 46, 858–868. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Ger. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neininger, B., and Pulvermüller, F. (2003). Word-category specific deficits after lesions in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 41, 53–70. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00126-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowak, D. A., and Trost, H. A. (2002). Lhermitte–Duclos disease (dysplastic cerebellar gangliocytoma): a malformation, hamartoma or neoplasm? Acta Neurol. Scand. 105, 137–145. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.1r127.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orsi, L., D’Agata, F., Caroppo, P., Franco, A., Caglio, M. M., Avidano, F., et al. (2011). Neuropsychological picture of 33 spinocerebellar ataxia cases. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 33, 315–325. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.518139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Prog. Neurobiol. 160, 1–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramnani, N. (2006). The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 511–522. doi: 10.1038/nrn1953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S., and Cohen, A. R. (2006). Cowden disease and Lhermitte–Duclos disease: an update: case report and review of the literature. Neurosurg. focus 20, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Rüschemeyer, S. A., Brass, M., and Friederici, A. D. (2007). Comprehending prehending: neural correlates of processing verbs with motor stems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 855–865. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saccuman, M. C., Cappa, S. F., Bates, E. A., Arevalo, A., Della Rosa, P., Danna, M., et al. (2006). The impact of semantic reference on word class: an fMRI study of action and object naming. Neuroimage 32, 1865–1878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D. (1991). An emerging concept: the cerebellar contribution to higher function. Arch. Neurol. 48, 1178–1187. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530230086029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Melloni, M., Canales-Johnson, A., Yoris, A., Baez, S., et al. (2014). How do you feel when you can’t feel your body? Interoception, functional connectivity and emotional processing in depersonalization-derealization disorder. PLoS One 9:e98769. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. A., Mottaghy, F. M., Schiller, N. O., Poeppel, T. D., Flüß, M. O., Müller, H. W., et al. (2005). Dissociating neural correlates for nouns and verbs. Neuroimage 24, 1058–1067. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silveri, M. C., Di Betta, A. M., Filippini, V., Leggio, M. G., and Molinari, M. (1998). Verbal short-term store-rehearsal system and the cerebellum. Evidence from a patient with a right cerebellar lesion. Brain 121, 2175–2187. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.11.2175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silveri, M. C., and Misciagna, S. (2000). Language, memory, and the cerebellum. J. Neurolinguist. 13, 129–143. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00008-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., and Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the Stait-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychological Press.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Vagg, P. R., Barker, L. R., Donham, G. W., and Westberry, L. G. (1980). The factor structure of the state-trait anxiety inventory. Stress Anxiety 7, 95–109.

Google Scholar

Tinaz, S., Courtney, M. G., and Stern, C. E. (2011). Focal cortical and subcortical atrophy in early Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 436–441. doi: 10.1002/mds.23453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., LOPez, P., and Manes, F. (2009). INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia–CORRECTED VERSION. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 777–786. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsermentseli, S., Leigh, P. N., Taylor, L. J., Radunovic, A., Catani, M., and Goldstein, L. H. (2016). Syntactic processing as a marker for cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Amyotroph. Lateral Scler. Frontotemporal Degener. 17, 69–76. doi: 10.3109/21678421.2015.1071397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289.

Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Druks, J., Barber, H., and Cappa, S. F. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 407–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vukovic, N., Feurra, M., Shpektor, A., Myachykov, A., and Shtyrov, Y. (2017). Primary motor cortex functionally contributes to language comprehension: an online rTMS study. Neuropsychologia 96, 222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zawacki, T. M., Grace, J., Friedman, J. H., and Sudarsky, L. (2002). Executive and emotional dysfunction in Machado-Joseph disease. Mov. Disord. 17, 1004–1010. doi: 10.1002/mds.10033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cerebellar atrophy, neurodegeneration, action pictures, object pictures, embodied cognition

Citation: Cervetto S, Abrevaya S, Martorell Caro M, Kozono G, Muñoz E, Ferrari J, Sedeño L, Ibáñez A and García AM (2018) Action Semantics at the Bottom of the Brain: Insights From Dysplastic Cerebellar Gangliocytoma. Front. Psychol. 9:1194. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01194

Received: 03 January 2018; Accepted: 20 June 2018;
Published: 12 July 2018.

Edited by:

Gianluca Castelnuovo, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Reviewed by:

Silvia Clausi, Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), Italy
Rutvik H. Desai, University of South Carolina, United States

Copyright © 2018 Cervetto, Abrevaya, Martorell Caro, Kozono, Muñoz, Ferrari, Sedeño, Ibáñez and García. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adolfo M. García, adolfomartingarcia@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.