Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 October 2018
Sec. Psychopathology

Altered Neuronal Responses During an Affective Stroop Task in Adolescents With Conduct Disorder

  • 1Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychiatric University Clinics, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité – University Medicine Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, United States

Conduct disorder (CD) is a psychiatric disorder of childhood and adolescence which has been linked to deficient emotion processing and regulation. The behavioral and neuronal correlates targeting the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition are still under investigation. Whole-brain event-related fMRI was applied during an affective Stroop task in 39 adolescents with CD and 39 typically developing adolescents (TD). Participants were presented with an emotional stimulus (negative/neutral) followed by a Stroop task with varying cognitive load (congruent/incongruent/blank trials). fMRI analysis included standard preprocessing, region of interest analyses (amygdala, insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex) and whole-brain analyses based on a 2(group) × 2(emotion) × 3(task) full-factorial ANOVA. Adolescents with CD made significantly more errors, while reaction times did not significantly differ compared to TD. Additionally, we observed a lack of downregulation of left amygdala activity in response to incongruent trials and increased anterior insula activity for CD relative to TD during affective Stroop task processing [cluster-level family-wise error-corrected (p < 0.05)]. Even though no three-way interaction (group × emotion × task) interaction was detected, the findings presented still provide evidence for altered neuronal underpinnings of the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD. Moreover, our results may corroborate previous evidence of emotion dysregulation as a core dysfunction in CD. Future studies shall focus on investigating the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD subgroups (e.g., variations in callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity, or anxiety).

Introduction

Conduct disorder (CD) is a psychiatric disorder of childhood and adolescence marked by emotion processing and regulation deficits (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Fairchild et al., 2013). CD youths typically engage in aggressive and antisocial behavior [e.g., rule breaking, stealing, and lying (Lahey and Waldman, 2012)], present with high rates of comorbidity (Nock et al., 2006), and are at risk for academic failure, delinquency, and mental disorders in adulthood (Swanson et al., 1994; Fergusson et al., 2005; Biederman et al., 2008; Erskine et al., 2016). CD is categorized according to its age of onset (childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset), severity (mild, moderate, or severe), and the presence or absence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits (DSM-5 specifier “with limited prosocial emotions”). Moreover, adolescents with CD can exhibit either predominantly reactive/impulsive or proactive/instrumental aggression, which is associated with the levels of CU traits (Mathias et al., 2007; Freitag et al., 2018). While adolescents with reactive aggression mainly show impulsive behavior, those with proactive aggression usually present with increased CU traits. Together with oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), a milder form and developmental precursor of antisocial behavior, CD has been categorized as a disruptive behavior disorder (DBD) (Loeber et al., 2000). Antisocial youths are phenotypically characterized by a heterogeneous symptomatology, reflected in different etiological paths and variations in response to treatment (Steiner et al., 2017). Main forms of neurocognitive dysfunctions include deficient emotion processing, reduced affective empathy, and altered response inhibition (Blair et al., 2016). Indeed the mechanisms underlying deficient emotion processing and response inhibition have been hypothesized to increase the risk for antisocial behavior (Campbell et al., 2000; Davidson et al., 2000; Young et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2017). As such, a failure to inhibit negative affect and to respond appropriately to negative cues has been proposed to lead to aggressive behaviors (Davidson et al., 2000), as for example observed in CD. Consequently, adolescents with CD have been shown to be impaired when negative cues are presented prior to cognitive task performance, e.g., during response inhibition (Euler et al., 2014; Hwang et al., 2016). In other words, previous findings demonstrate an inability to adequately process distracting emotional information, which results in impaired cognitive performance. Consequently, this may lead to an inability to suppress impulsive and/or antisocial behaviors as observed in CD [similar to observations in family violence (Chan et al., 2010)].

While altered response inhibition has been observed in adolescents with DBD (Prateeksha et al., 2014; Hwang et al., 2016), results are inconsistent in regards to the direction of findings. Some studies measuring response inhibition report no differences in performance of CD or DBD youths (Banich et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 2008, 2010b). Others indicate higher error rates and/or longer reaction times (RTs) (Rubia et al., 2009a; Euler et al., 2014; Prateeksha et al., 2014; Hwang et al., 2016). Importantly, when response inhibition is preceded by emotional (both negative and/or positive) stimuli, decreases in performance are more commonly reported (Euler et al., 2014; Prateeksha et al., 2014; Hwang et al., 2016).

Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have shed light on the neuronal phenotype characteristic for CD youths. Most commonly, alterations in neural recruitment in frontal and limbic lobes (including insula, amygdala, and anterior cingulate) are reported (Stadler et al., 2007; Sterzer and Stadler, 2009; Blair, 2010; Rubia, 2011; Raschle et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016), which are likely to depend on the levels of CU traits (Blair, 2010; Baker et al., 2015). Previous studies investigating response inhibition (e.g., stop, Simon, switch, or Stroop tasks) in CD have revealed decreased and increased neuronal activity in medial prefrontal cortex, insula, cingulate gyrus, temporoparietal junction, subcortical regions, and occipital lobe (Banich et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 2008, 2009a, 2010b). To our knowledge, only one neuroimaging study has yet directly tested the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in DBD youths. In this study, Hwang et al. (2016) detected reduced ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and amygdala activity in response to negative affective stimuli and reduced insula activity with increasing cognitive load in DBD compared to typically developing (TD) youths.

The present study aims at adding to this first evidence in DBD by investigating the neuronal and behavioral correlates of the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD youths through fMRI during affective Stroop task performance. The affective Stroop task is a variation of a response inhibition task and comprises a number Stroop task with trials which vary in cognitive load. Additionally, negative and neutral images are presented prior to the Stroop trials. By combining emotional images and number Stroop trials, the affective Stroop task allows the investigation of the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition. In the present study, only adolescents with a clinical diagnosis of CD were included. This is unlike previous studies using a similar task design, but a more lenient DBD diagnosis. We used a task design adapted from Hart et al. (2010), which was previously validated in a sample of healthy young adults (Raschle et al., 2017). In contrast to Hwang et al. (2016), our task included a set of child-appropriate images, reduced task complexity, and adapted presentation times (e.g., shorter image presentation, longer Stroop task presentation). Additional efforts were made in order to develop a protocol that meets the demands of an adolescent sample (e.g., attention and time constraints, see also Raschle et al., 2009, 2012. For further specifications on the task design see section “fMRI Task: The Affective Stroop Task”). Using both region of interest and whole-brain approaches, we hypothesized (I) to observe emotion × task interactions for the Stroop effect (i.e., delayed RTs for trials with increased cognitive load and prior negative stimulation) in CD compared to TD youths, in line with previous work (Euler et al., 2014); (II) to detect reduced neuronal activity within brain regions involved in emotion processing and response inhibition (amygdala, insula, and vmPFC) during the affective Stroop task in CD relative to TD youths in line with previous findings (Hwang et al., 2016).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy-eight youths (39CD/39TD) were included in the present analyses (age range: 10.1–19.1 years, mean age: 15.7 years, 10 males in each CD/TD group were assessed in Berlin). CD was diagnosed according to DSM-5 criteria by trained PhD students. Seventeen CD youths (43.6%) additionally met DSM-5 criteria for present attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, while 20 CD youths (51.3%) additionally met diagnostic criteria for ODD. TD were included if no current psychiatric diagnosis was reported by either the participant and/or the parents/legal guardians. CD and TD groups were matched for age [t(76) = 0.87, p = 0.390] and non-verbal IQ [t(76) = -0.72, p = 0.472] (Table 1 and see section Supplementary Material – Additional Information on Participants”). Moreover, both CD and TD groups from Basel and Berlin did not differ in age [CD: t(37) = 1.04, p = 0.303; TD: t(37) = -0.92, p = 0.365] or non-verbal IQ [CD: t(37) = 1.12, p = 0.269; TD: t(37) = 1.47, p = 0.151]. Participants were recruited through referrals from child and adolescent psychiatric institutions, public schools, and the general public through the use of fliers.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Behavioral group characteristics.

Ethics Statement

All adolescents and parents/legal guardians gave written informed consent as approved by the local ethics committee ‘Ethikkommission der Nordwest- und Zentralschweiz’ and received vouchers for their participation.

Clinical Testing and Questionnaires

Conduct disorder youths and their legal guardians completed the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children – Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., 1997) in order to assess CD criteria and comorbid disorders according to the DSM-5 (Table 1). CD and TD participants completed the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI, Andershed et al., 2007) and the matrix reasoning subtest of the WISC-IV (ages ≤ 16 years; Petermann, 2011) and the WAIS-III (ages ≥ 17 years; Petermann, 1997) measuring non-verbal IQ. For 10 participants (9 CD and 1 TD), only a composite IQ score was obtained. CD and TD legal guardians moreover completed a socioeconomic status (SES) questionnaire (see section “Supplementary Material – Psychometric Testing: Socioeconomic Status”). Participants were asked to report any medication administered prior to the MRI session (see section “Supplementary Material – Medication of Adolescents With Conduct Disorder (N = 35) and Typically Developing Controls (N = 39) at MRI Session”).

fMRI Task: The Affective Stroop Task

We applied an affective number Stroop task as previously described in Raschle et al. (2017) (Figure 1). Each trial started with an emotional stimulus, i.e., a negative (Neg) or neutral (Neu) stimulus (150 ms), followed by a task trial (congruent/incongruent/neutral Stroop trial or a blank screen) and finally a relaxation period, i.e., blank screen (350 ms). All pictures were selected from a child-appropriate image system [Developmental Affective Photo System (DAPS); Cordon et al., 2013]. During task trials, participants were presented with an array of 1 to 4 digits or a blank screen and were asked to press a button corresponding to the number of items displayed. The number of items was either congruent (C; e.g., number 3 in an array of 3) or incongruent (IC; e.g., number 1 in an array of 2) with the digits presented. Star shaped stimuli (S; as a neutral baseline counting condition) and blank trials (B; no response expected from participants) were used as control conditions (for further details see Raschle et al., 2017). Trial order and interstimulus intervals (which were 350–1850 ms) were randomized using Optseq1 and kept constant across participants. A total of 300 task and 100 blank trials were administered (100 for C/IC/S trials, 50 with preceding negative images, 50 with neutral images, in 2 runs), with a total scan time of about 16 min (7.59 min each run).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. fMRI task design including three exemplary affective Stroop trials. (A) Negative-congruent trial; (B) neutral-incongruent trial; (C) negative-blank trial.

Behavioral Measures: In-Scanner Performance

All participants scored > 60% correct responses per task condition and run. RTs and task accuracy (raw scores) were analyzed using 2x2x2 full-factorial ANOVAs with the between-subject factor group (CD and TD) and within-subject factors emotion (negative and neutral) and task (congruent and incongruent) for RTs and accuracy separately using SPSS, version 24. Data was unavailable for a minority of responses because of technical difficulties with the response box (for a detailed description see section “Supplementary Material – Button Box Responses”).

fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis

Acquisition Parameter

In Basel, whole-brain blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI data and structural T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo imaging images were acquired on a Siemens 3T Prisma MRI system using a 20-channel phased-array radio frequency head coil. In Berlin, a Siemens 3T TimTRIO MRI system equipped with a 12-channel head coil was used. At both sites a T2-weighted EPI (echo-planar imaging) sequence with TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30.0 ms, FOV = 192 mm, image matrix = 64 mm × 64 mm, voxel size = 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm, and number of slices = 37 was used. We further acquired high-resolution T1-weighted structural images for coregistration during fMRI preprocessing using the following specifications: TR = 1900.0 ms, TE = 3.42 ms, FOV = 256 mm, image matrix = 256 × 256, voxel size = 1 mm.

fMRI Analysis

Functional MRI data were analyzed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping software, version 12 (SPM122). Preprocessing of the data included realignment, co-registration to the structural image, segmentation, normalization to the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) standard brain, and spatial smoothing using an 8 mm Full Width at Half Maximum Gaussian kernel.

Single-subject fMRI data was analyzed using the general linear model. The model comprised eight task regressors [each combining a negative or neutral stimulus with congruent, incongruent, or neutral (stars/blank) Stroop trials, namely negative-congruent (NegC), negative-stars (NegS), negative-incongruent (NegIC), negative-blank (NegB), neutral-congruent (NeuC), neutral-stars (NeuS), neutral-incongruent (NeuIC), neutral-blank (NeuB)], one regressor for incorrect/missed responses, and six motion regressors. The task regressors were modeled as stick functions convolved with the hemodynamic response function as implemented in SPM12. Stars trials were not considered in further analyses.

At the second level, hypothesis-based ROI and whole-brain analyses were performed. A priori defined anatomical ROIs included bilateral amygdala, insula, and vmPFC based on previous literature (Rubia et al., 2008, 2009a, 2010a; Rubia, 2011; Raschle et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016) and derived from the automated anatomical labeling atlas (aal; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Mean parameter estimates were extracted from each ROI using the marsbar toolbox (Brett et al., 2002). A repeated measures ANOVA with the factors group (CD and TD), emotion (negative and neutral), and task (blank, congruent, and incongruent) and follow-up pairwise comparisons applying a Bonferroni multiple comparisons correction in order to account for the number of ROIs were then computed within SPSS, version 24.

For whole-brain analyses, beta images resulting from first-level model estimation for each regressor and run were submitted to a group-level random-effects analysis using a 2x2x3 full-factorial ANOVA with the between-subject factor group (CD and TD) and within-subject factors emotion (negative and neutral) and task (blank, congruent, and incongruent).

Quality control was performed throughout the analyses in order to control for effects of motion. Besides including six additional motion regressors during single-subject analysis, each analysis mask was visually inspected for head motion. An F-test performed on the six motion regressors revealed no significant differences in quantitative motion between groups [F(5,72) = 0.34, p = 0.889]. Qualitative motion was evaluated using Artifact Detection Tools (Whitfield-Gabrieli, 2009), where excessive motion was defined by the number of scans with a movement threshold of >2 mm and a rotation threshold of >0.05 mm. A two-sample t-test was then conducted, resulting in no significant differences in qualitative motion across groups [t(74) = 0.49, p = 0.626]. For all analyses, site (Basel, Berlin) was added as an additional factor of no interest. Brain activation was assessed for the main effects of group, emotion, and task, and all possible interactions thereof are reported at a cluster-extent family-wise error (FWE) rate of p < 0.05 (cluster building threshold of p < 0.001). Significant clusters of main effects and interactions were followed up with masked post hoc t-tests.

The F-maps and t-maps of the whole-brain analyses are available at https://neurovault.org/collections/XEYAPOGU/ (Gorgolewski et al., 2015) and the mean parameter estimates from each ROI are available in the Supplementary Material.

Results

Questionnaires

Psychometric assessments are reported in Table 1. CD scored significantly higher than TD in the callous-unemotional (i.e., affective) and impulsive-irresponsible (i.e., behavioral) dimensions and the total score of the YPI (all p < 0.01; YPI, Andershed et al., 2007). Nevertheless, psychopathic traits in our CD group were overall low [YPI total score: M = 11.16 (maximum score: 20.00), SD = 2.38, CU dimension: M = 11.23 (maximum score: 20.00), SD = 3.08; both within one standard deviation of a representative school sample (N = 480), see also Stadlin et al., 2016].

Behavioral Results: In-Scanner Performance

Analysis of RTs revealed a significant main effect of emotion [Neu > Neg, F(1,76) = 5.74, p < 0.05], and a main effect of task [IC > C, F(1,76) = 615.48, p < 0.001]. There was no main effect of group and no interaction effects for RTs. For accuracy, we found a significant main effect of emotion [Neu > Neg, F(1,76) = 8.29, p < 0.01], a main effect of task [C > IC, F(1,76) = 118.42, p < 0.001], and a main effect of group [CD < TD, F(1,76) = 6.77, p < 0.05]. There were no significant interaction effects for accuracy [see section “Supplementary Material – In-Scanner Performance (Accuracy and Reaction Times) for Adolescents With Conduct Disorder (N = 39) and Typically Developing Controls (N = 39)].

Functional MRI Results

ROI Results

ROI analyses in relevant regions of interest (bilateral amygdala, insula, and vmPFC) revealed significant group × task and emotion × task interactions, as well as main effects of emotion and task (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Bar graphs displaying mean values of parameter estimates (mean centered) in predefined regions of interest [amygdala, insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)] for the main effect of emotion and group × task interaction (left amygdala; A,B), emotion × task interaction (right amygdala and insula; C,D), and main effect of task (left and right vmPFC; E,F). B, blank trial; C, congruent trial; IC, incongruent trial; Neg, negative trial; Neu, neutral trial; CD, conduct disorder; TD, typically developing adolescents, p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001, two-tailed t-test; all other t-tests non-significant at threshold p = 0.05.

Group × task interaction

A group × task interaction was observed in left amygdala [F(2,73) = 4.83, p < 0.05], reflecting significantly decreased activity for incongruent compared to blank trials in TD (IC < B, p < 0.05), but not CD (all p > 0.227). This effect was independent of emotion [no significant group × emotion × task interaction; F(2,73) = 0.73, p = 0.485]. After exclusion of participants with medication or substance/alcohol abuse or dependence the group × task interaction in left amygdala (main finding) remained significant [F(2,57) = 3.54, p < 0.05]. Similarly, inclusion of the YPI CU dimension as a covariate did not change the significance levels of the group × task interaction in left amygdala [F(2,71) = 4.52, p < 0.05].

In order to examine the relationship between psychopathic traits (YPI total score) and left amygdala activity during IC–C (group × task interaction), follow-up bivariate correlations were computed for CD and TD separately. Results revealed no significant relationships between left amygdala activation and psychopathic traits for CD or TD.

Emotion × task interaction

A significant emotion × task interaction effect was found in right amygdala [F(2,73) = 4.77, p < 0.05]. Across all subjects we observed relatively increased right amygdala activity for blank trials with a prior negative compared to neutral emotion (NegB > NeuB, p < 0.05), but relatively decreased activity in the right amygdala during congruent trials with a prior negative versus neutral emotion (NegC < NeuC, p < 0.05). In addition we observed decreased activity in the right amygdala for congruent relative to blank trials following negative stimuli (NegC < NegB, p < 0.01). Within right insula, a significant emotion × task effect was observed [F(2,73) = 5.40, p < 0.01]. This effect reflected increased activity during congruent trials following negative compared to neutral stimuli (NegC > NeuC, p < 0.005) and increased activity for incongruent compared to congruent trials after negative stimuli (NegIC > NegC, p = 0.001). Decreased right insula activity was moreover detected for congruent compared to blank trials following negative stimuli (NegC < NegB, p < 0.001).

Main effect of emotion

A significant main effect of emotion [F(1,74) = 7.12, p < 0.01] was detected in left amygdala, driven by increased neuronal activity for negative compared to neutral trials (Neg > Neu, p < 0.01).

Main effect of task

A significant main effect of task was detected in right [F(2,73) = 5.40, p < 0.01] and left vmPFC [F(2,73) = 9.36, p < 0.001], resulting from relatively decreased activation for incongruent compared to both blank and congruent trials (IC < B, p < 0.001; IC < C, p < 0.005) for the left hemisphere and incongruent compared to blank trials for the right hemisphere (IC < B, p < 0.005).

Whole-Brain Results

Whole-brain analysis of brain activation during affective Stroop task processing revealed significant main effects of group and task (Table 2), but no significant main effect of emotion. There were no significant two-way or three-way interaction effects. Whole-brain findings did not overlap with the ROI results. All images are neurologically displayed using the Multi-image Analysis GUI as available at http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/mango.html.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) coordinates, cluster size and Z-scores for whole-brain results using a FWE cluster level correction of p < 0.05 (cluster building threshold of p < 0.001) for the main effect of group and main effect of task during the affective Stroop task.

Main effect of group

A main effect of group (Figure 3) was detected for regions including bilateral parietal and middle/inferior temporal and occipital lobes, bilateral precentral and inferior orbitofrontal areas extending into anterior insula and striatum, frontal cortices, and anterior/middle cingulate cortex (CD > TD) and regions including right middle frontal and supramarginal gyri (extending into posterior insula), left postcentral gyrus, right middle/superior temporal cortex, and bilateral thalamus (CD < TD).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Statistical parametric maps depicting the main effect of group (masked post hoc t-tests for group differences in brain activation between conduct disorder (CD) and typically developing (TD) adolescents, 39CD/39TD; hypoactivations in CD in red-yellow, hyperactivations in CD in green-yellow) (p < 0.05, FWE).

Main effect of task

Regions showing a differential BOLD response in response to task included bilateral parietal and frontal lobes, supramarginal gyri, occipital, temporal, and cerebellar regions, right middle cingulate cortex, left precuneus, and left amygdala. Bilateral supramarginal, superior frontoparietal, and occipital areas exhibited increased activity for congruent and incongruent relative to blank trials (IC/C > B). Left amygdala and inferior frontal areas exhibited increased activity for incongruent compared to blank trials (IC > B). In contrast, decreased left inferior parietal lobe, right middle frontal and cingulate cortices, and left precuneus activity was detected for congruent and incongruent relative to blank trials (C/IC < B). Decreased activity in left inferior temporal and right middle/superior temporal regions was related to incongruent versus blank trials (IC < B, Supplementary Figure 1).

Discussion

Here, we aimed at investigating the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD youths during an affective Stroop task. Behaviorally, adolescents with CD made significantly more errors, while RTs did not significantly differ compared to TD. ROI analyses revealed a significant group × task interaction effect reflecting a lack of downregulation of left amygdala activity in response to incongruent task trials for CD compared to TD. This effect was independent of the emotion presented prior to Stroop task performance. Additionally, whole-brain analyses revealed a significant main effect of group representing increased anterior insula activity for CD relative to TD regardless of emotion and task demands.

Contrary to our hypothesis and some previous investigations (Rubia et al., 2009b; Euler et al., 2014; Prateeksha et al., 2014) we did not detect group differences in RTs. However, research has not been conclusive to date and the present finding is in accordance with other studies (Banich et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 2008, 2009a, 2010b). Increased RTs for neutral compared to negative trials and for incongruent compared to congruent trials were detected across all participants. While increased RTs robustly reflect the Stroop effect (Stroop, 1935), shorter RTs for negative compared to neutral stimuli were not expected. However, participants’ responses were more accurate after presentation of neutral relative to negative images. Faster responses at the expense of lower accuracy may be due to heightened stress-related anxiety (Panayiotou and Vrana, 2004; Kosinski, 2008). Moreover, in line with Rubia et al. (2009a), CD youths made more errors than TD, which is contrary to other reports in DBD and TD (Banich et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 2008, 2010b; Euler et al., 2014; Prateeksha et al., 2014).

In line with our second hypothesis, ROI analyses revealed decreased left amygdala activity during Stroop task trials with a high cognitive load (IC < B) in TD. In contrast, CD youths did not show any downregulation of emotion-related brain areas with increasing task difficulty. Unexpectedly, this group difference was independent of the emotionality. We would have expected to detect a difference depending on the emotion presented (i.e., a downregulation after negative images instead of on any image as observed here). Our data suggests that no task-dependent downregulation of left amygdala response takes place in CD as compared to TD, possibly reflecting altered neuronal functioning of left amygdala which may be linked to altered regulatory processes.

In agreement with the findings presented here, an aberrant amygdala activity in DBD has previously been reported during response inhibition (Hwang et al., 2016), facial emotion processing (Marsh et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2009; Holz et al., 2017), emotion processing (Sterzer et al., 2005), stimulus-reinforcement learning, and reward processing (Finger et al., 2011). However, past results are inconsistent regarding the direction of findings. Some studies have detected decreases in amygdala activity during emotion processing in adolescents with CD. These findings have partly been attributed to the presence of CU or psychopathic traits (Marsh et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2009; Finger et al., 2011; Hwang et al., 2016), while others have detected amygdala activity reductions in youths with CD without taking callousness into account (Sterzer et al., 2005; Holz et al., 2017). In contrast, another body of work has reported increased amygdala activity during emotion processing tasks in individuals with CD, especially in those with low CU traits or increased anxiety (Herpertz et al., 2008; Decety et al., 2009; Viding et al., 2012; Sebastian et al., 2014). Additionally, it has been postulated that increased amygdala activity might be related to reactive aggression, which is commonly reported in adolescents with CD and low CU traits (Blair, 2010). These individuals are further often characterized by high anxiety levels linked to a hypersensitive threat system and increased sensitivity to negative stimuli. Altogether, increased anxiety and sensitivity to aversiveness of the adolescents with CD studied here may represent a potential reason for a lacking downregulation of amygdala response as observed here.

In addition to functional MRI evidence, past research has suggested reduced amygdala volumes in adolescents with conduct problems (Sterzer et al., 2007; Huebner et al., 2008; Fairchild et al., 2011; Wallace et al., 2014; Rogers and De Brito, 2016). Together with the findings presented here, evidence thus supports a broader view of the amygdala as a key center of alterations in CD.

Whole-brain results provided further insights into insula activity during the affective Stroop task while distinguishing between insular subdivisions. CD exhibited increased activity in anterior insula implicated in affective and cognitive processing (Mutschler et al., 2009; Chang et al., 2013). Additionally, CD showed decreased activity in posterior insula, an area involved in sensorimotor processing (Mutschler et al., 2009; Chang et al., 2013). Our observations are in line with a broader view of the insula in integrating emotion and cognition in healthy adolescents (Chang et al., 2013; Pavuluri and May, 2015), whereas alterations thereof could be hypothesized to reflect an increased allocation of neuronal resources for emotion and cognitive processing, potentially related to a maturational delay in youths with CD. However, the observed differences emerged from a main effect of group and therefore need to be interpreted carefully. Future studies shall determine whether right amygdala and insula show significant co-activations (Kober et al., 2008) during task trials following negative stimuli, reflecting on the role of the insula in transferring sensory information to the amygdala (Shelley and Trimble, 2004). In this regard, previous evidence has demonstrated decreased functional connectivity between amygdala and insula in adolescents with disruptive behavior both with (Finger et al., 2012) and without (Hwang et al., 2016) increased CU traits. Overall, alterations in connectivity between amygdala and insula have been observed in a variety of psychiatric disorders linked to emotion processing and regulation deficits such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Hulvershorn et al., 2014), high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (Ebisch et al., 2011), depression (Bebko et al., 2015), post-traumatic stress syndrome (Rabinak et al., 2011), or generalized anxiety disorder (Roy et al., 2013).

Limitations

For the present study design we used child-appropriate negative and neutral pictures (DAPS; Cordon et al., 2013). However, the short presentation (150 ms) and moderate image valence might have resulted in a reduced impact for CD youths. Also, the use of static images might not represent an ideal ecologically valid method to investigate adolescents’ response to emotional situations, despite of their common use in affective neuroscience studies. Future studies might consider the use of emotional video clips [e.g., “The Champ,” commonly used for emotion induction (Gross and Levenson, 1995; Hewig et al., 2005)] to overcome this limitation. Moreover, we cannot exclude that confounding factors or comorbidities could have influenced the results. Additionally, the results presented here characterize a group of CD youths on the lower spectrum of CU traits. Interpretation should therefore be drawn with caution. Finally, behavioral and neuronal analyses revealed no three-way interactions (group × emotion × task). This is likely due to the intricate nature of the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition, which has proven to be very challenging to capture. This is also reflected in earlier studies. For example, a previous study with similar aims neuronally reported a group × emotion × task interaction within the superior frontal gyrus and caudate, however, no such effect was detected in the behavioral analyses (Hwang et al., 2016). Nevertheless, our results are in favor of previous findings of an altered interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD.

Conclusion

We here provide evidence for the neuronal characteristics of the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition interaction in CD. More specifically, we observed a significant lack of downregulation of left amygdala activity in response to incongruent task trials and increased anterior insula activity for CD relative to TD during affective Stroop task performance. Behaviorally, CD scored significantly lower than TD youths, while RTs did not differ. While no three-way interactions (group × emotion × task) were detected, our results still support previous findings of an altered interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD. Overall, the present findings extend knowledge on the neurocognitive mechanisms in CD youths and support emotion dysregulation as a core deficit in CD. Future studies shall focus on investigating the interaction of emotion processing and response inhibition in CD subgroups (e.g., variations in CU traits, impulsivity, or anxiety). A deeper characterization of adolescents with CD is particularly relevant as a diagnosis of CD in adolescence represents a risk factor for psychiatric disorders such as antisocial personality disorder or substance use disorders in later adolescence and adulthood (Fergusson et al., 2005; Biederman et al., 2008). Early prevention and targeted interventions may reduce the individual and societal burden associated with a diagnosis of CD.

Author Contributions

NR, PS, and CS: conception and design of the experiments. LF, WM, MP, EF, LW, and FE: data collection. LF, NR, PS, and CS: data analysis and interpretation. LF, NR, PS, and CS: drafting the paper. LF, NR, WM, MP, EF, LW, FE, MS, PS, and CS: revision and final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

This study was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) (Grant No. 105314M_150282). LF received funding from the Freiwillige Akademische Gesellschaft Basel and the antelope@university career program (University of Basel). NR received a 2017–2019 Jacobs Foundation early career research grant.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the families that have participated in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01961/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq
  2. ^ www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Andershed, H., Hodgins, S., and Tengstrom, A. (2007). Convergent validity of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI): association with the Psychopathy Checklist: youth Version (PCL:YV). Assessment 14, 144–154.

Google Scholar

Baker, R. H., Clanton, R. L., Rogers, J. C., and De Brito, S. A. (2015). Neuroimaging findings in disruptive behavior disorders. CNS Spectr. 20, 369–381. doi: 10.1017/S1092852914000789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banich, M. T., Crowley, T. J., Thompson, L. L., Jacobson, B. L., Liu, X., Raymond, K. M., et al. (2007). Brain activation during the Stroop task in adolescents with severe substance and conduct problems: a pilot study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 90, 175–182. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bebko, G., Bertocci, M., Chase, H., Dwojak, A., Bonar, L., Almeida, J., et al. (2015). Decreased amygdala–insula resting state connectivity in behaviorally and emotionally dysregulated youth. Psychiatry Res. 231, 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Dolan, C., Hughes, S., Mick, E., Monuteaux, M. C., et al. (2008). The long-term longitudinal course of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder in ADHD boys: findings from a controlled 10-year prospective longitudinal follow-up study. Psychol. Med. 38, 1027–1036. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. (2010). Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 12, 76–82. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0086-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J., Veroude, K., and Buitelaar, J. K. (2016). Neuro-cognitive system dysfunction and symptom sets: a review of fMRI studies in youth with conduct problems. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 91, 69–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). “Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox,” in Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, eds B. Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. McNeilage, and J. Morton (Dordrecht: Springer).

Google Scholar

Campbell, S. B., Shaw, D. S., and Gilliom, M. (2000). Early externalizing behavior problems: toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Dev. Psychopathol. 12, 467–488.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chan, S. C., Raine, A., and Lee, T. M. (2010). Attentional bias towards negative affect stimuli and reactive aggression in male batterers. Psychiatry Res. 176, 246–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L. J., Yarkoni, T., Khaw, M. W., and Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference. Cereb. Cortex 23, 739–749. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordon, I. M., Melinder, A. M., Goodman, G. S., and Edelstein, R. S. (2013). Children’s and adults’ memory for emotional pictures: examining age-related patterns using the developmental affective photo system. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 339–356. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., and Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation–a possible prelude to violence. Science 289, 591–594.

Google Scholar

Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., Akitsuki, Y., and Lahey, B. B. (2009). Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Biol. Psychol. 80, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebisch, S. J., Gallese, V., Willems, R. M., Mantini, D., Groen, W. B., Romani, G. L., et al. (2011). Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula regions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1013–1028. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erskine, H. E., Norman, R. E., Ferrari, A. J., Chan, G. C., Copeland, W. E., Whiteford, H. A., et al. (2016). Long-Term outcomes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 841–850. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Euler, F., Sterzer, P., and Stadler, C. (2014). Cognitive control under distressing emotional stimulation in adolescents with conduct disorder. Aggress. Behav. 40, 109–119. doi: 10.1002/ab.21508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairchild, G., Passamonti, L., Hurford, G., Hagan, C. C., von, dem Hagen, E. A., et al. (2011). Brain structure abnormalities in early-onset and adolescent-onset conduct disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 624–633. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10081184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Calder, A. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2013). Research review: evaluating and reformulating the developmental taxonomic theory of antisocial behaviour. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 924–940. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fergusson, D. M., John Horwood, L., and Ridder, E. M. (2005). Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 837–849.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Finger, E. C., Marsh, A., Blair, K. S., Majestic, C., Evangelou, I., Gupta, K., et al. (2012). Impaired functional but preserved structural connectivity in limbic white matter tracts in youth with conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder plus psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Res. 202, 239–244. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Blair, K. S., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Ng, P., et al. (2011). Disrupted reinforcement signaling in the orbitofrontal cortex and caudate in youths with conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder and a high level of psychopathic traits. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 152–162. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10010129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitag, C. M., Konrad, K., Stadler, C., De Brito, S. A., Popma, A., Herpertz, S. C., et al. (2018). Conduct disorder in adolescent females: current state of research and study design of the FemNAT-CD consortium. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27, 1077–1093. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1172-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgolewski, K. J., Varoquaux, G., Rivera, G., Schwarz, Y., Ghosh, S. S., Maumet, C., et al. (2015). NeuroVault.org: a web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain. Front. Neuroinformatics 9:8. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cogn. Emot. 9, 87–108. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2006.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., and Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during Stroop task performance. Neuroimage 49, 2662–2670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herpertz, S. C., Huebner, T., Marx, I., Vloet, T. D., Fink, G. R., Stoecker, T., et al. (2008). Emotional processing in male adolescents with childhood-onset conduct disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 781–791.

Google Scholar

Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., and Bartussek, D. (2005). Brief report. Cogn. Emot. 19, 1095–1109.

Google Scholar

Holz, N. E., Boecker-Schlier, R., Buchmann, A. F., Blomeyer, D., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Baumeister, S., et al. (2017). Ventral striatum and amygdala activity as convergence sites for early adversity and conduct disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 261–272. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, T., Vloet, T. D., Marx, I., Konrad, K., Fink, G. R., Herpertz, S. C., et al. (2008). Morphometric brain abnormalities in boys with conduct disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 540–547. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181676545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulvershorn, L. A., Mennes, M., Castellanos, F. X., Di Martino, A., Milham, M. P., Hummer, T. A., et al. (2014). Abnormal amygdala functional connectivity associated with emotional lability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 351.e1–361.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, S., Nolan, Z. T., White, S. F., Williams, W. C., Sinclair, S., and Blair, R. J. (2016). Dual neurocircuitry dysfunctions in disruptive behavior disorders: emotional responding and response inhibition. Psychol. Med. 46, 1485–1496. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, A. P., Laurens, K. R., Herba, C. M., Barker, G. J., and Viding, E. (2009). Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 95–102. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., et al. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kober, H., Barrett, L. F., Joseph, J., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lindquist, K., and Wager, T. D. (2008). Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 42, 998–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosinski, R. J. (2008). A Literature Review on Reaction Time. Clemson, SA: Clemson University, 10.

Google Scholar

Lahey, B. B., and Waldman, I. D. (2012). Annual research review: phenotypic and causal structure of conduct disorder in the broader context of prevalent forms of psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 536–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02509.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., Lahey, B. B., Winters, A., and Zera, M. (2000). Oppositional defiant and conduct disorder: a review of the past 10 years, part I. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 1468–1484. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 712–720. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathias, C. W., Stanford, M. S., Marsh, D. M., Frick, P. J., Moeller, F. G., Swann, A. C., et al. (2007). Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. 151, 231–242.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mutschler, I., Wieckhorst, B., Kowalevski, S., Derix, J., Wentlandt, J., Schulze-Bonhage, A., et al. (2009). Functional organization of the human anterior insular cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 457, 66–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nock, M. K., Kazdin, A. E., Hiripi, E., and Kessler, R. C. (2006). Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychol. Med. 36, 699–710. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panayiotou, G., and Vrana, S. R. (2004). The role of self-focus, task difficulty, task self-relevance, and evaluation anxiety in reaction time performance. Motiv. Emot. 28, 171–196.

Google Scholar

Pavuluri, M., and May, A. (2015). I feel, therefore, I am: the insula and its role in human emotion, cognition and the sensory-motor system. AIMS Neurosci. 2, 18–27.

Google Scholar

Petermann, F. E. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edn. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.

Google Scholar

Petermann, F. P. (2011). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Chennai: Pearson Assessment.

Google Scholar

Prateeksha, S., Roopesh, B. N., and Vijayasagar, J. K. (2014). Response inhibition in children with conduct disorder: a preliminary report. Ind. J. Health Wellbeing 5, 1325–1330.

Google Scholar

Rabinak, C. A., Angstadt, M., Welsh, R. C., Kenndy, A. E., Lyubkin, M., Martis, B., et al. (2011). Altered amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2:62. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raschle, N., Zuk, J., Ortiz-Mantilla, S., Sliva, D. D., Franceschi, A., Grant, P. E., et al. (2012). Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raschle, N. M., Fehlbaum, L. V., Menks, W. M., Euler, F., Sterzer, P., and Stadler, C. (2017). Investigating the neural correlates of emotion–cognition interaction using an affective stroop task. Front. Psychol. 8:1489. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raschle, N. M., Lee, M., Buechler, R., Christodoulou, J. A., Chang, M., Vakil, M., et al. (2009). Making MR imaging child’s play - pediatric neuroimaging protocol, guidelines and procedure. J. Vis. Exp. 29:1309. doi: 10.3791/1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raschle, N. M., Menks, W. M., Fehlbaum, L. V., Tshomba, E., and Stadler, C. (2015). Structural and functional alterations in right dorsomedial prefrontal and left insular cortex co-localize in adolescents with aggressive behaviour: an ALE meta-analysis. PLoS One 10:e0136553. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, J. C., and De Brito, S. A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 64–72. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, A. K., Fudge, J. L., Kelly, C., Perry, J. S., Daniele, T., Carlisi, C., et al. (2013). Intrinsic functional connectivity of amygdala-based networks in adolescent generalized anxiety disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 290.e2–299.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K. (2011). “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: a review. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e69–e87. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Cubillo, A., Smith, A. B., Woolley, J., Heyman, I., and Brammer, M. J. (2010a). Disorder-specific dysfunction in right inferior prefrontal cortex during two inhibition tasks in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 287–299. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Halari, R., Cubillo, A., Mohammad, A. M., Scott, S., and Brammer, M. (2010b). Disorder-specific inferior prefrontal hypofunction in boys with pure attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with pure conduct disorder during cognitive flexibility. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1823–1833. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Halari, R., Smith, A. B., Mohammad, M., Scott, S., and Brammer, M. J. (2009a). Shared and disorder-specific prefrontal abnormalities in boys with pure attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with pure CD during interference inhibition and attention allocation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 669–678. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02022.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Halari, R., Smith, A. B., Mohammed, M., Scott, S., Giampietro, V., et al. (2008). Dissociated functional brain abnormalities of inhibition in boys with pure conduct disorder and in boys with pure attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 889–897. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Halari, R., Matsukura, F., Mohammad, M., Taylor, E., et al. (2009b). Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure ADHD during sustained attention. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 83–94. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08020212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, C. L., McCrory, E. J., Dadds, M. R., Cecil, C. A., Lockwood, P. L., Hyde, Z. H., et al. (2014). Neural responses to fearful eyes in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Psychol. Med. 44, 99–109. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shelley, B. P., and Trimble, M. R. (2004). The insular lobe of Reil–its anatamico-functional, behavioural and neuropsychiatric attributes in humans–a review. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 5, 176–200.

Google Scholar

Stadler, C., Sterzer, P., Schmeck, K., Krebs, A., Kleinschmidt, A., and Poustka, F. (2007). Reduced anterior cingulate activation in aggressive children and adolescents during affective stimulation: association with temperament traits. J. Psychiatr. Res. 41, 410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stadlin, C., Pérez, T., Schmeck, K., Gallo, A. D., and Schmid, M. (2016). Konstruktvalidität und Faktorenstruktur des deutschsprachigen Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) in einer repräsentativen Schulstichprobe. Diagnostica 62, 85–96. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Steiner, H., Daniels, W., Stadler, C., and Kelly, M. (2017). Disruptive Behavior: Development, Psychopathology, Crime, and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Sterzer, P., and Stadler, C. (2009). Neuroimaging of aggressive and violent behaviour in children and adolescents. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3:35. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.035.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterzer, P., Stadler, C., Krebs, A., Kleinschmidt, A., and Poustka, F. (2005). Abnormal neural responses to emotional visual stimuli in adolescents with conduct disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterzer, P., Stadler, C., Poustka, F., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2007). A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy. Neuroimage 37, 335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, M. C. J., Bland, R. C., and Newman, S. C. (1994). Antisocial personality disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 89, 63–70.

Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Dadds, M. R., Lockwood, P. L., Cecil, C. A., De Brito, S. A., et al. (2012). Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 1109–1116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, G. L., White, S. F., Robustelli, B., Sinclair, S., Hwang, S., Martin, A., et al. (2014). Cortical and subcortical abnormalities in youths with conduct disorder and elevated callous-unemotional traits. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 456.e1–465.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, F. L., Chassin, L., Lee, M., Haller, M., and King, K. (2017). Roles of response inhibition and gene–environment interplay in pathways to adolescents’. externalizing problems. J. Res. Adolesc. 27, 258–277. doi: 10.1111/jora.12270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2009). Artifact Detection Tools (ART) [Online]. Available: http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/ [accessed 16, 2018].

Young, S. E., Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Willcutt, E. G., Corley, R. P., Haberstick, B. C., et al. (2009). Behavioral disinhibition: liability for externalizing spectrum disorders and its genetic and environmental relation to response inhibition across adolescence. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 117–130. doi: 10.1037/a0014657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: conduct disorder, emotion processing, response inhibition, amygdala, insula

Citation: Fehlbaum LV, Raschle NM, Menks WM, Prätzlich M, Flemming E, Wyss L, Euler F, Sheridan M, Sterzer P and Stadler C (2018) Altered Neuronal Responses During an Affective Stroop Task in Adolescents With Conduct Disorder. Front. Psychol. 9:1961. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01961

Received: 30 May 2018; Accepted: 24 September 2018;
Published: 18 October 2018.

Edited by:

Roberto Viviani, Universität Innsbruck, Austria

Reviewed by:

Derek G. V. Mitchell, University of Western Ontario, Canada
Maia Pujara, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States

Copyright © 2018 Fehlbaum, Raschle, Menks, Prätzlich, Flemming, Wyss, Euler, Sheridan, Sterzer and Stadler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lynn V. Fehlbaum, Lynn.Fehlbaum@upk.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.