Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 07 August 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging the Gap between Research and Policy in Fostering Social and Emotional Skills View all 8 articles

The Relationship Between Math Anxiety and Math Performance: A Meta-Analytic Investigation

  • 1Faculty of Education, College of Teacher Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 2School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China

Math anxiety (MA) has been suggested to decrease the math performance of students. However, it remains unclear what factors moderate this relationship. The aim of this research was to explore the link between MA and math performance. Studies that explored the math anxiety-performance link, conducted from 2000 to 2019 (84 samples, N = 8680), were identified and statistically integrated with a meta-analysis method. The results indicated a robust negative math anxiety-performance link. Furthermore, regarding the analysis of moderator variables, this negative link was stronger in the studies that involved Asian students, but this link was the weakest in the studies that involved European students. Moreover, this negative link was stronger in the studies within a senior high school group, whereas it was the weakest in the studies within an elementary group. Finally, this negative link was strongest among studies that used a custom test and studies that assessed problem-solving skills. Potential explanations and implications for research and practice are discussed.

Introduction

Math anxiety (MA) has been a matter of concern in education for a long time and refers to the state of fear, tension, and apprehension when individuals engage with math (Ashcraft, 2002; Ashcraft and Ridley, 2005). A range of studies suggested that this phenomenon is a highly prevalent problem among students from elementary schools to universities (Betz, 1978; Ma and Xu, 2004; Rodarte-Luna and Sherry, 2008; Jain and Dowson, 2009; Gunderson et al., 2018). The negative math anxiety-performance link has been found in many empirical studies, which indicates MA would lead to poor performance when individuals deal with math reasoning or solve math problems (Bandalos et al., 1995; Ashcraft and Kirk, 2001; Ashcraft, 2002; Cates and Rhymer, 2003; Ma and Xu, 2004; Miller and Bichsel, 2004). However, the effect sizes of these studies are variable according to different reasons; thus, a systematic analysis is necessary to understand the math anxiety-performance link and the role that moderators play.

There have been nearly 20,200 previous studies related to MA or math performance (Olmez and Ozel, 2012; Necka et al., 2015; Lukowski et al., 2016; Justicia-Galiano et al., 2017), while few systematic studies focus on exploring the math anxiety-performance link during the previous 19 years. Two previous meta-analyses that were conducted 30 or 40 years ago concluded that there was a small but robust negative math anxiety-performance link in students (Hembree, 1990; Ma, 1999). However, for Ma's review, only 26 studies were included for which students originated from elementary and secondary schools. For Hembree's review, the analysis did not focus on the math anxiety-performance link. Therefore, to guide future studies in math education, an up-to-date meta-analysis exploring the math anxiety-performance link in recent years is needed.

Furthermore, based on the studies from 2000 to 2018, a range of potential moderators, including gender, grade level, geographical region, measurement of math anxiety, measurements of math performance, measurement aspects of math performance and publication year, may influence the math anxiety-performance link (Yaratan and Kasapoglu, 2012; Vukovic et al., 2013a; Hill et al., 2016; Gunderson et al., 2018). Moreover, the results of the PISA also indicated that MA could be affected by a range of factors (Foley et al., 2017), such as variations in the methodology and the educational level of participants (e.g., elementary school, junior high school and senior high school) (Krinzinger et al., 2009; Passolunghi et al., 2014). Thus, we sought to examine whether these features moderate the math anxiety-performance link and how these factors moderate this association to provide guidance for future research. Collectively, it would be informative to consider these factors in our present meta-analysis.

Gender

A range of research has shown that gender might modulate the math anxiety-performance link. First, gender has been suggested to modulate math anxiety (Mustafa and KoçAk, 2006); however, the findings were inconsistent. Several studies showed significantly stronger MA in females than in males (Osborne, 2001; Yüksel-Sahin, 2008; Dowker et al., 2012; Gunderson et al., 2018) For example, Maloney et al. (2012) suggested that women might have stronger MA than men when dealing with tasks that involve mathematical skills and numerical skills. Other findings suggested that girls had greater habitual MA than boys; however, they did not experience higher level MA than boys during math content learning or a math content test (Goetz et al., 2013). In light of these inconsistent findings, the effect of gender on the math anxiety-performance link should be explored.

Grade Level

It is suggested that MA might impact performance over time (Ramirez et al., 2013; Vukovic et al., 2013a; Maloney et al., 2015) As the difficulty of math learning increases with age, the math anxiety might also increases (Olmez and Ozel, 2012). Recent studies indicated that a negative math anxiety-performance link existed for young adults (Wu et al., 2017; Gunderson et al., 2018). However, the relationship between MA and math performance remained unclear for the elementary group. Some indicated that MA of primary school students was not related to performance (Thomas and Dowker, 2000; Dowker et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2012; Haase et al., 2012), while other studies have suggested that the math anxiety-performance link existed even at this age (Wu et al., 2012, 2014; Jameson, 2013; Vukovic et al., 2013a). Based on these results, grade level might modulate the math anxiety-performance link.

Geographical Region

Previous studies have implied that geographical region may influence the math anxiety-performance link. For example, the negative math anxiety-performance link was stronger in China than in the USA according to the findings of Ching (2017) and Wu et al. (2017). Ching (2017) found a negative link (r = −0.318) between MA and math performance in young Chinese students. Moreover, Wu et al. (2017) also indicated a negative link (r = −0.23) between MA and math performance in young USA children. In Asia, academic achievement is highly valued, which would lead to the high-level anxiety of Asian students (Ho et al., 2000). In contrast, students seem to be less critical of their academic performance and feel relaxed in European countries. This relationship was also examined by several studies that compared one or two countries (e.g., American, Chinese, and Taiwanese in Ho et al., 2000; Arab and Israeli in Birenbaum et al., 2005). However, no consistent results have been found, which made it difficult to claim a general pattern of this link across cultures. Therefore, additional details regarding how cross-cultural information modulated this negative relationship are needed.

Measurement of Math Performance

Recent studies have suggested that the measurement of different aspects of math performance had variable effects on the correlation between MA and math performance (Ramirez et al., 2016; Lee and Cho, 2018). For example, Beilock and Willingham (2014) found a strongly math anxiety-performance link especially in mathematical reasoning, which assessed advanced problem-solving skills. In contrast, Harari et al. (2013) reported a weak link between MA and digital calculation, which assessed basic computational skills. In terms of the measurement form, both standard tests (e.g., Henschel and Roick, 2017; Wu et al., 2017) and custom assessments (e.g., Necka et al., 2015; Ganley and Mcgraw, 2016) were used to estimate math performance, which have not previously been noted. Overall, the incorporation of the measurement aspects and forms of math performance as two potential moderator variables (measurement aspects of math performance vs. measurement forms of math performance) in the present study was necessary.

Measurement of MA

In light of a range of scales used to measure MA, the variation in optimizing MA might be related to the variation in research findings. Specifically, some studies employed the Mathematics Anxiety Questionnaire (MAQ) to measure MA (Carey et al., 2017), which found no correlation between MA and math performance (Dowker et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2012; Haase et al., 2012). In contrast, some studies employed other questionnaires (e.g., a scale for earlsy MA, The Mathematical Anxiety for Young Children) to measure MA, which showed a negative relationship between MA and math performance (Wu et al., 2012, 2014). Thus, an assessment of whether the measurement of MA could modulate the math anxiety-performance link was also necessary.

Publication Year

Several recent studies found no relationship between MA and math performance (Thomas and Dowker, 2000; Krinzinger et al., 2007, 2009; Harari et al., 2013; Vukovic et al., 2013a; Cargnelutti et al., 2017). For example, Cargnelutti et al. (2017) found no significant relationship (r = 0.04) between math performance and MA in 2nd graders. As educators and researchers have highlighted the importance of relieving the negative effect of MA on math performance, the relationship between MA and math performance might have decreased in recent years. Thus, the publication year should be considered.

Study Purpose

Taken together, the present meta-analysis aimed to quantitatively synthesize these studies to provide an updated and overall view on the math anxiety-performance relationship and investigate the specific variables that may play a role in inconsistencies. First, we calculated the overall effect size of the correlations between MA and math performance. We then examined whether this correlation differed across gender, grade level, geographical regions, measurement of MA, measurement aspects of math performance, measurement forms of math performance and publication year.

Methods

Literature Search

We conducted a literature search for studies on MA and math performance from January 2000 to December 2018 using electronic databases, including Google Scholar, ERIC, EBSCO, Web of Science, ProQuest, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and PsycCRITIQUES. The following key descriptions were used to search for related articles: “mathematics (math) anxiety,” “mathematics(math) performance,” “mathematics(math) outcome,” “mathematics(math) achievement”) “math anxiety” AND “math performance.” We then refined the search following inclusion criteria.

Literature Inclusion Criteria

The articles were further screened based on the following criteria:

(a) MA and math achievement were both measured, and the results were both reported. Each study had to report the relationship between MA and math performance; Studies that only measure MA or only measure math performance were excluded. For example, Olmez and Ozel (2012) only explored MA among sixth and seventh grade Turkish elementary school students and did not investigate further to provide information regarding math performance.

(b) Individual studies should explicitly report the sample size; if studies did not report the sample size (e.g., Lee, 2009), they were all excluded.

(c) Studies that applied an eligible test method to assess MA and math performance were included in the analysis. Review studies were excluded. For example, Dowker et al. (2016) reviewed what research has indicated regarding MA in the last 60 years; Foley et al. (2017) systematically clarified the relationship between MA and math performance based on data from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA). Although they clarified the relationship between MA and math performance, they did not conduct an experiment to adopt data. As a result, they were excluded.

(d) Each study should clearly report the Pearson correlation r or F and t values that could be transformed into r. Studies that only reported the results from regression modeling or multilevel modeling analysis (e.g., Nasser and Birenbaum, 2005; Shores and Shannon, 2007), which did not provide sufficient statistical information that could be transformed into r, were excluded.

(e) Each study should be published in a peer-reviewed journal written in English between 2000 and 2018. Studies that did not meet this criterion were excluded, for example, a study conducted in 1990 (Baya'a, 1990); moreover, non-English studies were not considered in our study.

(f) The grade level of participants in studies should be elementary school to university level; studies that focus on preschool students were excluded. For example, Aslan et al. (2013) explored the link between the MA of teachers and math performance of preschool students. These studies were excluded.

Finally, 49 studies, including 84 independent samples, were included for analysis (Figure 1), as more than one independent sample was included in several studies. Studies that were not consistent with these criteria were excluded from this meta-analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Literature search process.

Coding

To exclude the apparently ineligible studies (e.g., studies in non-English languages), two independent coders screened all studies from title to abstract. Relevant studies were then coded by coders independently at the full-text level to identify variable effect sizes and moderators. Moreover, to identify the potential moderators, a systematic process was employed.

Four categories were coded: (a) study information, which was organized in two categories, including publication year and geographical region (i.e., US, Asia, Europe, and other regions), (b) participant information, including number of total sample size, grade level and gender, and (c) methodology, which was organized in three categories: measurement of MA, measurement forms of math performance, and measurement aspects of math performance. We obtained relevant information regarding the previously described moderator variables through reading the research method sections of each individual studies. And then we got information about the previously described moderator variables. Table 1 showed the final list of study features.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Coded study features as potential moderators.

Two coders were enrolled to code all studies independently, according to the guidelines (Cooper, 2016), following the inclusion criteria previously described. The coders were all trained well and completely understood all coding criteria. To calculate the interrater reliability, coding discrepancies were identified on six original studies, indicating an agreement of 94.91% [i.e., (59–3)/59 = 94.91%] between two coders. Finally, a consensus was reached for disagreements between two coders after discussion at the end of the coding procedure.

Data Analysis

Effect Size Calculation

Pearson's r as an index of the effect size for our meta-analysis was employed (refer to Mcgrath and Meyer, 2006; Fritz et al., 2012, given the benefits of using r as an effect size metric). Some studies did not report the Pearson's r directly; thus, we transferred inference test statistics (t-value, F-value, or chi-square) in those studies to Pearson's r. If only individual studies statistically significant, the conservative effect size was obtained, assuming a p-value of 0.05. According to Cooper et al. (2009), to ensure the stabilization of the sample distribution, we applied Fisher's z transformations. Then these values were transformed back into correlations, with effect sizes and confidence intervals. Thus, each r would go through the Fisher z transformation:

z=12ln(r+1r-1)

In the present study, we applied the random-effects model to calculate effects. The random-effects model assumed that the effect sizes of different samples/studies may come from different populations and different populations had its own sampling distribution (Borenstein et al., 2009; Card, 2011). Taking into account the effects of sample levels, our systematic analysis of potential moderate variables was calculating the random effects at sample levels. The diversity of experimental settings of individual studies was included in our study (e.g., gender, grades, and geographical region), which made a random-effect model an appropriate approach (Cooper, 2016). Moreover, Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA, Version 2.2) was applied to conduct all necessary computations and analyses.

Moderator Analyses

The Q test (a heterogeneity test) was employed which refered to the variation between the research results in different studies. A significant Q statistic indicated that individual studies were not come from a common population. This calculation we referred to were all conducted by the CMA.

Evaluation of Publication Bias

Three approaches to explore publication bias were adopted in the study. The first approach was a funnel plot, which clearly presented all effect sizes (Card, 2011). The second method was the fail-safe N (e.g., classic fail-safe N), which estimated the number of studies with non-significant results (unpublished data) needed to cause the mean ES to become statistically non-significant (Rosenthal, 1979). The third method was the Egger's test (Egger, 1997), which was a linear regression method that assessed the publication bias by the funnel plot.

Results

Overview of Primary Studies

After the literature filtering, 49 articles yielding 84 independent samples were included in the present meta-analyses. Table 2 presented study name ES (effect size), N (sample size), gender, grade level and publication year. Table 3 reported measure of MA, measure of math performance, measure aspects of math and geographical region. Table 4 showed the descriptive information of the moderator variables. For the features of the participants: 86% of studies did not analyze the effect of gender differences in the math anxiety-performance link, and only 14% of the reports consider the influence of gender on this link. Second, regarding the grade level of the participants, elementary students account for the largest proportion (53%), while 25% were university students, and few were junior or senior high school students. Furthermore, considering the regions where the primary study was conducted, studies conducted in Europe accounted for the largest proportion (37%). With respect to measurements of MA, the most adopted was the MARS (Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale) (31%), followed by other scales (21%) and the AMAS (Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale) (16%). Finally, considering the measurement forms of math performance, the largest proportion of studies applied standardized tests (61%), followed by a custom test (33%) and other forms (6%). In terms of the measurement aspects of math performance, the largest proportion of studies estimated general skills (47%), followed by the assessment of computation (35%) and problem-solving (8%).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of studies included in the meta-analysis (1).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of studies included in the meta-analysis (2).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistics of included studies.

Overall Analysis

Table 5 presents the major findings that resulted from the meta-analysis. The overall mean ES of the 49 articles was −0.3, with a 95% confidence interval that ranged from −0.35 to −0.28. Cohen (1988, 1992) suggested that ESs of 0.80, 0.50, and 0.20 presented large, medium, and small, respectively. Then, our findings suggested that there was a negative, although somehow weak, relationship between MA and mathematics performance.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance: overall results and moderator analyses.

Q statistics indicated that the effect sizes were heterogeneous (Q = 526.50.10, z = −15.71, p < 0.001), which elucidated the differences of the ESs that were ascribed to sources. Thus, it also noted that the following-up analysis for potential moderator variables may reveal their contribution in this inconsistency.

The “forest plot” (Figure 2) presented the random-effects modeling analysis of the 49 studies and graphically showed the effect size (square dot) and its estimated confidence interval (horizontal lines extending from both sides of the squared dot). First, it was observed that only two effect sizes were positive (i.e., on the right side of the “null” effect line), e.g., Zakaria et al., 2012; Hunt et al., 2017), while the remaining effect sizes were negative. Second, it was clearly shown that the majority of the effect sizes had narrow confidence intervals, while several effect sizes had slightly wide confidence intervals (e.g., Tsui et al., 2007).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot.

Assessment of Publication Bias

To estimate the publication bias, the funnel plot, Rosenthal's fail-safe N method and Egger test were all applied. First, the funnel plot was employed. Circles (referred to effect sizes) were symmetrically around the vertical line in the plot, which indicated no potential bias in our data (Figure 3). Second, Applied Rosenthal (1979) failsafe-N procedure, we obtained a value of 5535 missing studies at the p-value of 0.05. According to instruction of Rosenthal, when value of failsafe-N was larger than 5K + 10 (K represented the number of individual studies), we could safely refuse possible publication bias (Rothstein, 2008). In present research, 5K + 10 were 255 studies, which indicated our research have no publication bias. Egger's test (Egger, 1997) was also applied t = 0.19, p = 0.85, which suggested that the funnel plot was symmetric (Figure 3). Thus, we could safely eliminate the influence of publication bias on the validity of present research.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot.

Moderator Analysis

To test whether the math anxiety-performance link might be moderated by a range of variables (e.g., geographical region, measurement of MA, measurement of math performance, gender, grade level, and publication year), we conducted the heterogeneity test. The Q test (heterogeneity test) have found the differences among effect sizes, thus to explore which potential moderator (i.e., study features) might have played a role in these differences was needed.

Geographical Region

Geographical regions were considered a potential moderate factor. The effect sizes were grouped into four broad groups: US, Europe, Asia and other counties. The statistical heterogeneity among the effect sizes was shown (Q = 29.60, p = 0.01). Specifically, the studies with Asian samples had the largest effect size (r = −0.41), while the studies with European samples showed the smallest effect size (r = −0.21). In addition, the studies with US samples had a larger effect size (r = −0.30) than the corresponding group in Europe, while they had a smaller effect size than the corresponding group in Asia.

Gender

The moderate analyses on gender were conducted through two steps. First, the category on this variable was consistent with Ma's (1999) study, and we compared whether the math anxiety-performance link was significantly different among three categories (male, female, and mixed); the results suggested there were no differences among the three categories (Q = 4.62, p = 0.099). Second, this study tended to extend further to test when to exclude the confound effect from the mixed group and whether the average correlations between male and female were significantly different. Only 7 studies that provided the correlation between MA and math performance or necessary data could be transferred to effect sizes for different genders were included. The results suggested no significant difference between females (r = −0.30) and males (r = −0.18) (Q = 2.73, p = 0.098).

Table 5 Relationship between MA and math performance: overall results and moderator analyses.

Grade Level

Five grade-level groups were formed in this meta-analysis, including an elementary group, junior high group, senior high group, mix group, and university group. The results suggested this variable had significant effects on the math anxiety-performance link (Q = 11.65, p = 0.02). More specifically, the senior high group had the largest math anxiety-performance link (r = −0.44), followed by the junior high group (r = −0.39), university (r = −0.33), elementary (r = −0.27), and mixed group, which had the smallest math anxiety-performance link (r = −0.21).

Publication Years

The current meta-analysis contained the time frame of 19 years (2000–2018). To evaluate whether time had an effect on the math anxiety-performance link, a Pearson correlation was conducted between the publication years and the of all individual studies (r = 0.27, p > 0.05). No correlation was found between effect sizes and the publication years (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Relationship between publication year and effect size.

Measurement of MA

We also explored whether the scales used to test math anxiety had an effect on the math anxiety-performance link. The results suggested that the measurement of MA had no effect on the math anxiety-performance link (Q = 6.54, p = 0.48). Specifically, individual studies that used other scales (e.g., the Mathematics Anxiety Scale for Elementary School Students [MASESS; Bindak, 2005] had the largest math anxiety-performance link (r = −0.41), followed by studies that used the MAS (r = −0.40), MARS (r = −0.35), MAQ (r = −0.28), MASYC (r = −0.26), CMAQ (r = −0.24), SEMA (r = −0.24), and AMAS (r = −0.24), and individual studies that used the MASC had the smallest math anxiety-performance link (r = −0.15).

Measurement of Math Performance

The classification criteria used to code the “math performance” variable are different. In terms of the testing aspects of the math performance test, two skills were highlighted: calculation and problem solving. In this study, the measurement aspects of the test had significant effects on the math anxiety-performance link (Q = 17.001, p < 0.01). Studies that evaluated mixed math performance (calculation and problem solving) reported the largest effects (r = −0.37). In addition, it is worth noting that studies in which the test assessed problem-solving skills in math performance reported larger effects (r = −0.33), while studies that assessed the calculation ability in math performance reported smaller effects (r = −0.21). In terms of the form of the math performance test, three types of math performance tests were identified: custom test, standardized test and others. There was a significant difference among these conditions (Q = 13.02, p = 0.001). The results suggested that studies that adopted a standardized test custom test reported larger effects (r = −0.33) than studies that adopted a standardized test (r = −0.28). Moreover, the studies that adopted other math tests reported the largest effects (r = −0.51).

Discussion

In summary, this meta-analysis illustrated a significantly negative effect size in the math anxiety-performance association. Furthermore, six moderator variables that were assumed have effect on the association between MA and math performance were tested in the present study. The size of the links differed across geographical regions, grade level, measurement of MA, measurement forms of math performance and measurement aspects of math performance.

The Significant Correlation Between MA and Math Performance

With a significantly negative effect size (r = −0.32), the result was consistent with previous individual studies (Miller and Bichsel, 2004; Rodarte-Luna and Sherry, 2008) and was also consistent with meta studies in general (Hembree, 1990; Ma, 1999). To further interpret the results of the meta-analysis, we compared our results with mainly two previous meta-analytic reviews (Hembree, 1990; Ma, 1999) and other studies, which examined the reciprocal relationship between MA and math achievement.

First, as the meta-analysis by Ma (1999) was limited to individual studies conducted from 1978 to 1992 across elementary to senior high schools, it can be argued that our investigation has expanded and extended Ma's (1999) study, in terms of the time period (2000–2018 vs. 1978–1992) and grade level (elementary- senior high school vs. elementary-university). In addition, Ma (1999) found a smaller effect size (r = −0.27) than the effect size in our meta-analysis study (r = −0.32). Similarly, in another meta-analysis that explored the construct of MA, the meta-analysis by Hembree (1990) did not focus on the math anxiety-performance association, although some analyses were conducted on this topic. Thus, our study updated the findings in terms of the time period (2000–2018 vs. before 1990). Furthermore, Hembree (1990) reported a greater effect size (r = −0.31) than Ma's (1999) studies, although it was smaller than the effect size that we found in this study. Our results are also consistent with other individual studies (Reali et al., 2016; Justicia-Galiano et al., 2017; Lauer et al., 2018). For example, Reali et al. (2016) explored the relationship between MA and math performance among Colombian students and obtained a negative correlation (r = −0.27); this result was also found among children aged 8 to 12 years (Justicia-Galiano et al., 2017). Thus, the effect size in our study implied a robust negative math anxiety-performance link.

To explain this negative link, two theories were posed. The Deficit Theory claims that poor performance in a math test would lead to higher anxiety and uncomfortable experiences in the future. That is, bad math performance would deficit the willingness to study math and lead to MA (Berch and Mazzocco, 2007; Carey et al., 2016). Moreover, the debilitating anxiety model suggested that MA would influence math performance by cognitive interference. For example, individuals who have higher levels of MA frequently avoid involving math learning. Thus, they have less chance to practice their math learning skills (Ashcraft, 2002; Carey et al., 2016). Overall, these two theories explained why negative correlation between MA and math performance existed.

Moderation Effects

Regarding the specific factors that modulate the math anxiety-performance association, we identified 6 modulate variables. The results differed across geographical regions, grade level, measurement of MA, measurement forms of math performance and measurement aspects of math performance.

Moderating Role of Geographical Regions

The results indicated that the relationship significantly differed among geographical regions. Specifically, the math anxiety-performance relationship was suggested to be the strongest in the studies that involved Asian students and was the second strongest in the studies that involved US students, whereas it was the weakest in the studies that involved European students, which was partly consistent with previous studies (Ching, 2017; Wu et al., 2017). This finding also extended the previous meta-analyses by Ma (1999) and Hembree (1990). Ma (1999) did not provide details regarding whether geographical regions (US vs. Europe vs. Asia vs. other regions) modulated the math anxiety-performance association. Similarly, Hembree (1990) also did not include this factor in his analysis. Furthermore, for Asian students, the more significantly robust negative math anxiety-performance association indicated that the more anxiety they experience in their math learning, the worse learning performance they would achieve.

To our knowledge, in Asian, teachers and parents all highly valued academic achievement of their students (children). For some parents, they even according to grades to predict whether their children would be success in the future. Moreover, there were stiff competitions in school. For example, students would experience entrance examinations as the first step of their formal schooling (Ho et al., 2000). Thus, understandably, Asian students always compared their own rankings with rankings of other students and overvalued academic achievements. furthermore, to meet their goals, Asian students always pushed them hard. Once they cannot achieve their goals, they would experience anxiety and disappointed in themselves (Whang and Hancock, 1994). As a result, the negative math anxiety-performance association of Asian students might attribute to these factors.

Furthermore, in the Western European countries where not overemphasized the relationship between academic achievement and success, students did not overvalue their academic performance. And they often experience relaxed math learning environment. For students who originated from the United States, although they highlighted the significance of learning math, students seem to experience less anxiety.

Moderating Role of Grade Level

In terms of grade level, we found significant differences in the math anxiety-performance link. Specifically, students from senior high school showed the highest negative math anxiety-performance correlation, and students from elementary school showed the lowest negative math anxiety-performance correlation. This result was somewhat different from the findings in Ma's (1999) study, which suggested no grade differences in the math anxiety-performance relationship (for the Grades 4 through 6 vs. Grades 10 through 12 comparison; for the Grades 7 through 9 vs. Grades 10 through 12 comparison). In contrast, this result was in line with recent studies that suggested the negative math-performance relationship surfaced in secondary education (Ashcraft and Krause, 2007; Maloney and Beilock, 2012; Hill et al., 2016). It is understandable that this link develops stronger in the high school educational period. During the higher educational period, students are exposed to more difficult math curriculum, while more cognitive engagement is acquired. Furthermore, as the onset of adolescence, this stage was important for students to develop social and emotional regulation skills. These changes might undoubtedly influence how students engage in math, which, in turn, was likely to affect their emotional reaction toward the subject.

Moderating Role of the Measurement of MA

We found the measurement of MA modulates the math anxiety-performance link, which could explain, in part, why research found variable correlations between MA and math performance (Dowker et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2012; Haase et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2012, 2014). Specifically, the individual studies that applied other questionnaires (e.g., the Mathematics Anxiety Scale for Elementary School Students (MASESS; Bindak, 2005) to test the MA reported the highest negative correlation. First, although all measurements exhibited good reliability, some individual studies extracted the items from different MA scales and combined them to assess the MA. Second, it is suggested that the aspects of MA that were estimated by these questionnaires were somewhat different from each other. For example, we found that some studies measured whether MA related to the inability to successfully solve math questions (Krinzinger et al., 2007, 2009; Dowker et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2012; Haase et al., 2012). Interestingly, no math anxiety-performance link was detected in these studies. Other studies explored anxiety in math-related situations (Wu et al., 2012, 2014; Ramirez et al., 2013). In these studies, the participants did not experience the failure of math question-solving and were only instructed to evaluate the level of anxious they experienced when they calculate mathematical questions or when they were in mathematical situations.

Moderating Role of the Measurement of Math Performance

First, consistent with prior studies (Ramirez et al., 2016; Lee and Cho, 2018), we found the measurement aspects of math performance modulated the math anxiety-performance link, with a stronger negative correlation in studies that evaluated problem solving skills than in studies that evaluated basic calculation skills. Compared with a complex problem-solving test, the basic calculation was easier and occupied less cognitive resources. Individuals would solve these problems with a more relaxed mood and experience less frustration; thus, they would have better math performance. Second, the measurement forms of math performance were also shown to modulate the negative math anxiety-performance link, with a stronger negative correlation in studies that adopted custom tests than in studies that adopted standard tests. The custom tests were used to assess specific aspects of math and the test items were extracted from more than one test file (Ashkenazi and Danan, 2017), while standard tests were used to assess the general ability of math with standard evaluation criteria. The difficulty level of custom tests might be difficult to control; however, the difficulty level of standard tests had been assessed very well.

Moderating Role of Gender

This study indicated there were no significant gender differences in the math anxiety- achievement link, although prior research found a significantly greater level of MA in females than in males (Ho et al., 2000; Osborne, 2001; Yüksel-Sahin, 2008). In our studies, only 7 studies provided the raw data (correlation value and sample size) for a meta-analysis of the math anxiety- achievement link in different genders. Thus, this result should be interpreted with caution. First, this finding is consistent with Ma's (1999) finding, which suggests no differences in the math anxiety-performance link for boys and girls. Furthermore, the results that suggested gender less affected the math performance is in line with studies that showed no gender differences or less gender difference in gender equal countries (Spelke, 2005; Guiso et al., 2008; Elsequest et al., 2010; Goetz et al., 2013). Finally, previous studies suggested there were gender differences in MA. Specifically, girls reported more MA than boys (Devine et al., 2012). However, there is no direct evidence that indicates gender modulated the math anxiety-performance link. Taken together, it seems no gender differences in the math anxiety-performance relationship existed.

Moderating Role of Publication Year

This study also reported there is no significant effect of publication year. The studies that found no negative math anxiety-performance may be attributed to other factors. It is conceivable that a non-significant difference was identified in publication years, which suggested that the negative math anxiety-performance relationship is robust among recent 19 years. Although educators have proposed alleviating the negative emotion toward the MA, it is still a long way to go.

Limitations and Implications

Despite the novel findings and implications of this study, several limitations should be clarified. First, only 7 of the 49 studies included measured the gender difference in the math anxiety-performance link, while the other studies all reported the math anxiety-performance link based on a mixed gender sample. Thus, as mentioned above, the interpretation of the lack of a moderate effect should proceed with caution.

Second, although the present study obtained studies conducted in multiple countries (i.e., China, Turkey and England), these studies were all reported by English. If we considered the studies written in other languages, our findings would be enrich in this field.

Furthermore, to develop a better understanding regarding the math anxiety-achievement link, additional details (e.g., moderator analysis) could be given for a range of math performance tests (e.g., computation, logical reasoning, or concept understanding). Unfortunately, limited by the levels of a moderator variable and restricted by the number of studies included in this meta-analysis, we did not perform a more detailed analysis separately for test types of math performance.

Therefore, future studies could extend further to explore the following questions. Moreover, some implications based on our results can be applied in an educational context. First, to develop a better understanding of the effect of gender on the math anxiety-performance link, more individual studies should be included in future studies. Although a range of previous studies explored the relationship between gender and MA and tended to support that girls suffered more anxiety in the math study context, the evidence suggested that gender did not modulate the relationship between MA and math performance (Hembree, 1990; Ma, 1999; Devine et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2012). Thus, to explore strong evidence for a correlation between gender and the math anxiety-performance link, additional researches should be conducted; these explorations would help researchers and educators to adopt eligible methods to relieve the MA and promote the math performance of students. For female students, girls are often told that boys would have a better performance than girls, which would pose a threat to their attitude towards math learning (Beasley and Fischer, 2012). Thus, one way to relieve their MA is to eliminate stereotype threats toward them regarding their math learning and encourage them in the math learning context. For example, encouraging female students when they solve a problem successfully rather than consoling them by saying, “It's OK girls cannot solve this problem.”, which may pose a stereotype threat for them. Second, according to our results, the strongest math anxiety-performance link was found among senior high school students. Thus, the mitigation of the MA of students in senior high school is essential. For educators in senior high school, increasing the difficulty of learning materials step by step to prevent students from feeling anxious regarding mathematics learning is necessary. Thus, students would have a solid mathematical knowledge base. This rule in line with previous evidence, which suggested that a solid foundation in mathematical knowledge is beneficial for relieving MA and improving math performance (Beilock and Willingham, 2014). Finally, our results suggest different effects of the measurement of math performance on the math anxiety-performance link. Thus, for educators, they should consider the characteristics of each type of mathematical knowledge and design corresponding lessons. Using this approach, students would adapt effortlessly to the progress of the math course.

Conclusion

The present study, which included a meta-analysis of 49 studies, indicated a robust negative math anxiety-performance correlation, and this relationship was moderated by geographical region, grade level, measurement of MA, measurement aspects of math performance and measurement forms of math performance. The math anxiety-performance link was stronger among Asian students than among European/American students. Moreover, this negative link was strongest among senior high school students. Finally, this negative link was strongest among studies using custom tests and studies that assessed problem-solving skills.

Author Contributions

JZ provided the idea, designed this study, contributed to data collection, the manuscript writing, and paper revise. NZ designed this study, contributed to data collection, the manuscript writing, and paper revise. QK provided the idea, contributed to study design, data analysis and paper revise. All authors approval of the version to be published and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

The major project of humanities and social science key research base from the Ministry of education, China, Research on the Assessment of Chinese students' Mathematics competency (16JJD880023).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

* Abu-Hilal, M, M. (2000). A structural model for predicting mathematics achievement: its relation with anxiety and self-concept in mathematics. Psychol. Rep. 86, 835–847. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3.835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 11, 181–185. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Ashcraft, M. H., and Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. J. Exp. Psychol. General 130, 224–237. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math anxiety. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 243–248. doi: 10.3758/BF03194059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Ridley, K. S. (2005). “Math anxiety and its cognitive consequences: a tutorial review,” in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, eds J. I. D. Campbell. (New York, NY, NY: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

* Ashkenazi, S., and Danan, Y. (2017). The role of mathematical anxiety and working memory on the performance of different types of arithmetic tasks. Trends Neurosci. Edu. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2017.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslan, D., Gürgah Oğul, I., and Taş, I. (2013). The impacts of preschool teachers' mathematics anxiety and beliefs on children's mathematics achievement. Int. J. Human. Soc. Sci. Invent. 2, 45–49.

Google Scholar

Bandalos, D. L., Yates, K., and Thorndikechrist, T. (1995). Effects of math self-concept, perceived self-efficacy, and attributions for failure and success on test anxiety. J. Edu. Psychol. 87, 611–623. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.4.611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baya'a, N. F. (1990). Mathematics anxiety, mathematics achievement, gender, and socio-economic status among Arab secondary students in Israel. Int. J. Mathemat. Edu. Sci. Technol. 21, 319–324. doi: 10.1080/0020739900210221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beasley, M. A., and Fischer, M. J. (2012). Why they leave: the impact of stereotype threat on the attrition of women and minorities from science, math and engineering majors. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 15, 427–448. doi: 10.1007/s11218-012-9185-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beilock, S. L., and Willingham, D. T. (2014). Ask the cognitive scientist-math anxiety: can teachers help students reduce it. Am. Educ. 38, 28–43.

Google Scholar

Berch, D. B. E., and Mazzocco, M. M. M. E. (2007). Why is math so hard for some children? the nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatrics 29, 241–241. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31817aefe8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betz, N. E. (1978). Prevalence, distribution, and correlates of math anxiety in college students. J. Counsel. Psychol. 25, 441–448. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.25.5.441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bindak, R. (2005). Mathematics anxiety scale for primary school students. Sci. Eng. J. Firat Univ. 17, 442–448.

Google Scholar

Birenbaum, M., Nasser, F., and Tatsuoka, C. (2005). Large-scale diagnostic assessment: mathematics performance in two educational systems. Edu. Res. Eval. 11, 487–507. doi: 10.1080/13803610500146137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Birgin, O., Baloglu, M., Çatlioglu, H., and Ramazan, G. (2010). An investigation of mathematics anxiety among sixth through eighth grade students in Turkey. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 20, 654–658. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Fixed-effect versus random-effects models. Introd. Meta-Anal. 77:85. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386.ch13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Giles, G. E., Rapp, D. N., Taylor, H. A., Kanarek, R. B., et al. (2013). Learning to relax: evaluating four brief interventions for overcoming the negative emotions accompanying math anxiety. Learn. Indivi. Diff. 27, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Buelow, M. T., and Frakey, L. L. (2013). Math anxiety differentially affects WAIS-IV arithmetic performance in undergraduates. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 28, 356–362. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Card, N. A. (2011). Applied Meta-Analysis for Social Science Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., and Szücs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Front. Psychol. 6:1987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., and Szücs, D. (2017). The modified abbreviated math anxiety scale: a valid and reliable instrument for use with children. Front. Psychol. 8:11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Cargnelutti, E., Tomasetto, C., and Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? a longitudinal study of grade 2 to grade 3 children. Cogn. Emot. 31, 755–764. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1147421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cates, G. L., and Rhymer, K. N. (2003). Examining the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance: an instructional hierarchy perspective. J. Behav. Educ. 12, 23–34. doi: 10.1023/A:1022318321416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Ching, B. H. H. (2017). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in chinese children. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 51, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, H. (2016). Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis: A Step-by-Step Approach, Vol. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., and Valentine, J. C. (2009). The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Russell Sage Foundation.

Google Scholar

* Daneshamooz, S., Alamolhodaei, H., and Darvishian, S. (2012). Experimental research about effect of mathematics anxiety, working memory capacity on students' mathematical performance with three different types of learning methods. ARPN. J. Sci. Technol. 2, 313–321.

Google Scholar

* Devine, A., Fawcett, K., Dénes, S, and Dowker, A. (2012). Gender differences in mathematics anxiety and the relation to mathematics performance while controlling for test anxiety. Behav. Brain Funct. 8:33. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Dowker, A., Bennett, K., and Smith, L. (2012). Research article attitudes to mathematics in primary school children. Child Dev. Res. 1–10. doi: 10.1155/2012/124939

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., and Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: what have we learned in 60 years? Front. Psychol. 7:508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsequest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., and Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 103–127. doi: 10.1037/a0018053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Erturan, S., and Jansen, B. (2015). An investigation of boys' and girls' emotional experience of math, their math performance, and the relation between these variables. Eur. J. Psychol. Edu. 30, 421–435. doi: 10.1007/s10212-015-0248-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foley, A. E., Herts, J. B., Borgonovi, F., Guerriero, S., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2017). The math anxiety-performance link: a global phenomenon. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 26, 52–58. doi: 10.1177/0963721416672463

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., and Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. General. 141, 2–18. doi: 10.1037/a0024338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Galla, B. M., and Wood, J. J. (2012). Emotional self-efficacy moderates anxiety-related impairments in math performance in elementary school-age youth. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 52, 118–122. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Ganley, C. M., and Mcgraw, A. L. (2016). The development and validation of a revised version of the math anxiety scale for young children. Front. Psychol. 7:1181. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goetz, T., Bieg, M., Ludtke, O., Pekrun, R., and Hall, N. C. (2013). Do girls really experience more anxiety in mathematics? Psychol. Sci. 24, 2079–2087. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., and Zingales, L. (2008). Diversity: culture, gender, and math. Science 320, 1164–1165. doi: 10.1126/science.1154094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Gunderson, E. A., Park, D., Maloney, E. A., Beilock, S. L., and Levine, S. C. (2018). Reciprocal relations among motivational frameworks, math anxiety, and math achievement in early elementary school. J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 21–46. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1421538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haase, V. G., Júlio-Costa, A., Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Oliveira, L. D. F. S., Micheli, L. R., and Wood, G. (2012). Math self-assessment, but not negative feelings, predicts mathematics performance of elementary school children. Child Dev. Res. 2012:982672. doi: 10.1155/2012/982672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Harari, R. R., Vukovic, R. K., and Bailey, S. P. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young children: an exploratory study. J. Exp. Edu. 81, 538–555. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2012.727888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. J. Res. Math. Edu. 21, 33–46. doi: 10.2307/749455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Henschel, S., and Roick, T. (2017). Relationships of mathematics performance, control and value beliefs with cognitive and affective math anxiety. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 55, 97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Hill, F., Mammarella, I. C., Devine, A., Caviola, S., Passolunghi, M. C., and Szücs, D. (2016). Maths anxiety in primary and secondary school students: Gender differences, developmental changes and anxiety specificity. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 48, 45–43. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Ho, H. Z., Senturk, D., Lam, A. G., Zimmer, J. M., Hong, S., Okamoto, Y., et al. (2000). The affective and cognitive dimensions of math anxiety: a cross-national study. J. Res. Math. Edu. 31, 362–379. doi: 10.2307/749811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Hoffman, B. (2010). “I think I can, but I'm afraid to try”: the role of self-efficacy beliefs and mathematics anxiety in mathematics problem-solving efficiency. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 20, 276–283. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Hunt, T. E., Bhardwa, J., and Sheffield, D. (2017). Mental arithmetic performance, physiological reactivity and mathematics anxiety amongst U.K. primary school children. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 57, 129–132. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jain, S., and Dowson, M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety as a function of multidimensional self-regulation and self-efficacy. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 34, 240–249. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jameson, M. M. (2013). The development and validation of the children's anxiety in math scale. J. Psychoedu. Assess. 31, 391–395. doi: 10.1177/0734282912470131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Justicia-Galiano, M. J., Martín-Puga, M. E., Linares, R, and Pelegrina, S. (2017). Math anxiety and math performance in children: the mediating roles of working memory and math self-concept. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 1–26. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Karasel, N., Ayda, O., and Tezer, M. (2010). The relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematical problem solving skills among primary school students. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2, 5804–5807. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Karimi, A., and Venkatesan, S. (2009). Mathematics anxiety, mathematics performance and academic hardiness in high school students. Int. J. Edu. Sci. 1, 33–37. doi: 10.1080/09751122.2009.11889973

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krinzinger, H., Kaufmann, L., Dowker, A., Thomas, G., Graf, M., Nuerk, H. C., et al. (2007). German version of the math anxiety questionnaire (fra) for 6- to 9-year-old children. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 35:341. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917.35.5.341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krinzinger, H., Kaufmann, L., and Willmes, K. (2009). Math anxiety and math ability in early primary school years. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 27, 206–225. doi: 10.1177/0734282908330583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Kyttälä, M., and Björn, P. M. (2014). The role of literacy skills in adolescents' mathematics word problem performance: controlling for visuo-spatial ability and mathematics anxiety. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 29, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Lauer, J. E., Esposito, A. G., and Bauer, P. J. (2018). Domain-specific anxiety relates to children's math and spatial performance. Dev. Psychol. 9, 1–27. doi: 10.1037/dev0000605

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. (2009). Universals and specifics of math self-concept, math self-efficacy, and math anxiety across 41 PISA 2003 participating countries. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 19, 355–365. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K., and Cho, S. (2018). Magnitude processing and complex calculation is negatively impacted by mathematics anxiety while retrieval-based simple calculation is not. Int. J. Psychol. 53, 321–329. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Lukowski, S. L., Ditrapani, J., Jeon, M., Wang, Z. J., Schenker, V., Doran, M. M., et al. (2016). Multidimensionality in the measurement of math-specific anxiety and its relationship with mathematical performance. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 70, 229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Luo, X., Wang, F., and Luo, Z. (2009). Investigations and analysis on mathematics anxiety of middle school students. J. Math. Edu. 2, 12–19.

Google Scholar

Ma, X. (1999). Assessing the relationship between attitude toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics: a meta-analysis. J. Res. Math. Edu. 30, 520–540. doi: 10.2307/749772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., and Xu, J. (2004). Determining the causal ordering between attitude toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Am. J. Edu. 110, 256–280. doi: 10.1086/383074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., and Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 404–406. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2015). Intergenerational effects of parents' math anxiety on children“s math achievement and anxiety. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1480–1488. doi: 10.1177/0956797615592630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., Waechter, S., Risko, E. F., and Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Reducing the sex difference in math anxiety: the role of spatial processing ability. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 22, 380–384. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcgrath, R. E., and Meyer, G. J. (2006). When effect sizes disagree: the case of r and d. Psychol. Methods 11, 386–401. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.4.386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Miller, H., and Bichsel, J. (2004). Anxiety, working memory, gender, and math performance. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 37, 591–606. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mustafa, B., and KoçAk, R. (2006). A multivariate investigation of the differences in mathematics anxiety. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 40, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasser, F., and Birenbaum, M. (2005). Modeling mathematics achievement of Jewish and Arab eighth graders in Israel: the effects of learner-related variables. Edu. Res. Eval. 11, 277–302. doi: 10.1080/13803610500101108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Necka, E. A., Moriah, S. H., and Lyons, I. M. (2015). The role of self-math overlap in understanding math anxiety and the relation between math anxiety and performance. Front. Psychol. 6:1543. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Núñez-Peña, M. I., Suárez-Pellicioni, M., and Bono, R. (2013). Effects of math anxiety on student success in higher education. Int. J. Edu. Res. 58, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Olmez, I. B., and Ozel, S. (2012). Mathematics anxiety among sixth and seventh grade turkish elementary school students. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 46, 4933–4937. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, J. W. (2001). Testing stereotype threat: does anxiety explain race and sex differences in achievement?. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 26, 291–310. doi: 10.1006/ceps.2000.1052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passolunghi, M. C., Ferreira, T. I. R., and Tomasetto, C. (2014). Math-gender stereotypes and math-related beliefs in childhood and early adolescence. Learn. Individ. Diff. 34, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Passolunghi, M. C., Sara, C., Ruggero, D. A., Chiara, P., and Mammarella, I. C. (2016). Mathematics anxiety, working memory, and mathematics performance in secondary-school children. Front. Psychol. 7:42. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school: the role of problem solving strategies. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 83–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. J. Cogn. Dev. 14, 187–202. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.664593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Reali, F., Jiménez-Leal, W., Maldonado-Carreño, C., Devine, A., and Szücs, D. (2016). Examining the link between math anxiety and math performance in colombian students. Rev. Colom. de Psicol. 25, 369–379. doi: 10.15446/rcp.v25n2.54532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodarte-Luna, B., and Sherry, A. (2008). Sex differences in the relation between statistics anxiety and cognitive/learning strategies. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 33, 327–344. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothstein, H. R. (2008). Publication bias as a threat to the validity of meta-analytic results. J. Exp. Criminol. 4, 61–81. doi: 10.1007/s11292-007-9046-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Schillinger, F. L., Vogel, S. E., Diedrich, J., and Grabner, R. H. (2018). Math anxiety, intelligence, and performance in mathematics: insights from the German adaptation of the abbreviated math anxiety scale (AMAS-G). Learn. Indiv. Diff. 61,109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Sherman, B. F., and Wither, D. P. (2003). Mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement. Math. Edu. Res. J. 15, 138–150. doi: 10.1007/BF03217375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shores, M. L., and Shannon, D. M. (2007). The effects of self-regulation, motivation, anxiety, and attributions on mathematics achievement for fifth and sixth grade students. School Sci. Math. 107, 225–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1949-8594.2007.tb18284.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Skaalvik, E. M. (2018). Mathematics anxiety and coping strategies among middle school students: relations with students' achievement goal orientations and level of performance. Soc. Psychol. Edu. 21, 709–723. doi: 10.1007/s11218-018-9433-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Sorvo, R., Koponen, T., Viholainen, H., Aro, T., Räikkönen, E., Peura, P., et al. (2017). Math anxiety and its relationship with basic arithmetic skills among primary school children. Br. J. Educat. Psychol. 87, 309–327. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? a critical review. Am. Psychol. 60, 950–958. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, G., and Dowker, A. (2000). “Mathematics anxiety and related factors in young children.” in British Psychological Society Developmental Section Conference. Bristol

Google Scholar

* Tsui, J. M., Michèle, M. M., and Mazzocco. (2007). Effects of math anxiety and perfectionism on timed versus untimed math testing in mathematically gifted sixth graders. Roeper Rev. 29, 132–139. doi: 10.1080/02783190709554397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vukovic, R. K., Kieffer, M. J., Bailey, S. P., and Harari, R. R. (2013a). Mathematics anxiety in young children: concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Vukovic, R. K., Roberts, S. O., and Green Wright, L. (2013b). From parental involvement to children's mathematical performance: the role of mathematics anxiety. Early Edu. Dev. 24, 446–467. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2012.693430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whang, P. A., and Hancock, G. R. (1994). Motivation and mathematics achievement: comparisons between Asian-American and non-Asian students. Contempor. Edu. Psychol. 19, 302–322. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1994.1023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, G., Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Júlio-Costa, A., Micheli, L. R., Krinzinger, H., Kaufmann, L., et al. (2012). Math anxiety questionnaire: similar latent structure in Brazilian and German school children. Child Dev. Res. 2012:610192. doi: 10.1155/2012/610192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Woodard, T. (2004). The effects of math anxiety on post-secondary developmental students as related to achievement, gender, and age. Inquiry 9:5. doi: 10.16953/deusbed.74839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Wu, S. S., Amin, H., Barth, M., Malcarne, V., and Menon, V. (2012). Math anxiety in second and third graders and its relation to mathematics achievement. Front. Psychol. 3:162. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Wu, S. S., Chen, L., Battista, C., Smith Watts, A. K., Willcutt, E. G., and Menon, V. (2017). Distinct influences of affective and cognitive factors on children's non-verbal and verbal mathematical abilities. Cognition 166, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Wu, S. S., Willcutt, E. G., Escovar, E., and Menon, V. (2014). Mathematics achievement and anxiety and their relation to internalizing and externalizing behaviors. J. Learn. Disabil. 47, 503–514. doi: 10.1177/0022219412473154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaratan, H., and Kasapoglu, L. (2012). Eighth grade students' attitude, anxiety, and achievement pertaining to mathematics lessons. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 46, 162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.05.087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Yüksel-Sahin, F. (2008). Mathematics anxiety among 4th and 5th grade Turkish elementary school students. Int. Electron. J. Math. Educ. 3, 179–192.

Google Scholar

* Zakaria, E., and Nordin, N. (2008). The effects of mathematics anxiety on matriculation students as related to motivation and achievement. Eur. J. Math. Sci. Technol. Edu. 4, 27–30. doi: 10.12973/ejmste/75303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

* Zakaria, E., Zain, N. M., Ahmad, N. A., and Erlina, A. (2012). Mathematics anxiety and achievement among secondary school students. Am. J. Appl. Sci. 9, 1828–1832. doi: 10.3844/ajassp.2012.1828.1832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*^Indicates all studies(n = 49) entered in our meta-analysis.

Keywords: math anxiety, math performance, meta-analysis, students, moderator analysis

Citation: Zhang J, Zhao N and Kong QP (2019) The Relationship Between Math Anxiety and Math Performance: A Meta-Analytic Investigation. Front. Psychol. 10:1613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01613

Received: 04 March 2019; Accepted: 26 June 2019;
Published: 07 August 2019.

Edited by:

Rubén Fernández-Alonso, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain

Reviewed by:

Robert Reeve, The University of Melbourne, Australia
Álvaro Postigo, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Zhang, Zhao and Kong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jing Zhang, zhangjing8309@foxmail.com; Nan Zhao, zn92324@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work as first authors

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.