Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 February 2021
Sec. Organizational Psychology

The Impact of Behavioral Biases on Herding Behavior of Investors in Islamic Financial Products

\r\nSajid Mohy Ul Din*Sajid Mohy Ul Din1*Shabra Khalid MehmoodShabra Khalid Mehmood2Arfan ShahzadArfan Shahzad3Israr AhmadIsrar Ahmad4Alla DavidyantsAlla Davidyants5Ayman Abu-RummanAyman Abu-Rumman6
  • 1School of Economics, Finance, and Banking (SEFB), Universiti Utara Malaysia, Changlun, Malaysia
  • 2Lahore Business School, University of Lahore, Lahore, Pakistan
  • 3Othman Yeop Abdullah Graduate School of Business (OYAGSB), Universiti Utara Malaysia, Changlun, Malaysia
  • 4College of Business (COB), Universiti Utara Malaysia, Changlun, Malaysia
  • 5Department of Propaedeutics of Dental Diseases, I. M. Sechenov First Moscow State Medical University (Sechenov University), Zelenograd, Russia
  • 6Business School, Al-Ahliyya Amman University, Al-Salt, Jordan

The study aimed to investigate the impact of behavioral biases on herding for Islamic financial products with the mediation of shariah literacy. An adopted questionnaire from several published studies was used to collect data. The data were collected from 410 respondents and were analyzed with SmartPLS. The results for the direct impact showed that self-attribution, illusion of control, and information availability have a positive and significant impact on herding for Islamic financial products while shariah literacy showed an insignificant impact on herding. The results for mediation showed that previously significant and positive impact turned to insignificant when shariah literacy was introduced as mediating variable between the illusion of control, self-attribution, information availability, and herding. From a theoretical perspective, this study would contribute to the existing body of knowledge of financial decision making from shariah literacy point-out. On the other hand, the findings of this study may be useful for investors to avoid herding in the Islamic financial markets. The authors synthesize the contribution made by behavioral finance studies in extending the knowledge of herding behavior in Islamic financial products with a mediating role of shariah literacy. The key limitation of the study includes data that were collected from three districts of Punjab, Pakistan.

Introduction

Traditionally, the efficient market hypothesis (EMH) theory was considered as one of the key foundations for investment decision making. Under EMH, no investor can earn abnormal returns, over and above from average market returns, based on his knowledge and information-processing capabilities (Alnajjar, 2013; Ross et al., 2016). It assumes that investors are rational and utilize a diverse range of models to shortlist and select the optimal investment opportunities. However, EMH failed to justify market anomalies, which resulted in different financial crisis, i.e., internet bubble burst of the 1990s, dot-com crisis, stock market crash of 2002, and bank leading crises of 1994 (Sharma and Kumar, 2019). The opponents of EMH such as Shefrin and Statman (2011), Pompian (2012), and Barberis (2017) argued that investors not necessarily act rationally as claimed in EMH and that their irrationality may be explained through different behavioral anomalies. EMH also ignores the behavior and different mood swings of investors (Aigbovo and Ilaboya, 2019). Past studies reported that several factors such as herd behavior (Babajide and Adetiloye, 2012), psychological factors (Chaffai and Medhioub, 2018), and emotions and cognitive biases (Shefrin and Statman, 2011) were mainly responsible for irrational investor’s behavior. It is thought that investors have more influence on the word of mouth of their family, friends, and peers. Herd behavior in financial markets led to different financial crises, bubbles, and eventually stock crashes (Precher, 2010). For example, the study of Armansyah (2018) found that herd behavior led to financial crisis and stock market crashes in Argentina (2000–2006) and Asia (1997–1998) and the dot-com crash (2008–2009). Following others is much easier than to investigate a matter and then reach the truth.

Low financial literacy may result in information asymmetry and increased herding (Setyowati et al., 2018). Past studies such as Gerardi et al. (2013), Klapper et al. (2013), Lusardi et al. (2014), and Cueva and Rustichini (2015) reported that low financial literacy was the underlying reason for the 2007–2008 global financial crises. Herding creates instability in the market even if it is rational herding. Sharia literacy may also help investors avoid herding, which may subsequently result in fewer market failures (Razak and Abdullah, 2015). Islam encourages critical and rational thinking self-evaluation and discourages herding. In the Quran, Allah said I give men the power of thinking called aql. By this fact, a rational person must think critically and analyze the situation by his mind not only to follow the acts of others but also to find out the truth from falsehood. Arabic terms ya’qilu (rationalizing) and yufakkiru (thinking) are repeated (more than 20 times) in the holy Quran, describing the importance of thinking activity in all actions (Hashim, 2003). God has forbidden herding in several Quranic verses:

www.frontiersin.org

And when it is said to them, “Follow what Allah has revealed,” they say, “Rather, we will follow that which we found our fathers doing (herding).” Even though their fathers understood nothing, nor were they guided?

www.frontiersin.org

And if you obey most of those upon the earth, they will mislead you from the way of Allah (a crowd may led you to the wrong direction). They follow not accept assumption, and they are not but falsifying.

www.frontiersin.org

Say, “Indeed, I have been forbidden to worship those you invoke besides Allah.” Say, “I will not follow your desires, for I would then have gone astray, and I would not be of the (rightly) guided.”

www.frontiersin.org

And as for Thamud, We guided them, but they preferred blindness over guidance, so the thunderbolt of humiliating punishment seized them for what they used to earn.

www.frontiersin.org

Indeed, the worst of living creatures in the sight of Allah are the deaf and dumb who do not use reason.

www.frontiersin.org

And we have certainly created for Hell many of the jinn and mankind. They have hearts with which they do not understand, they have eyes with which they do not see, and they have ears with which they do not hear. Those are like livestock; rather, they are more astray. It is they who are the heedless.

www.frontiersin.org

And (for) their saying, “Indeed, we have killed the Messiah, Jesus, the son of Mary, the messenger of Allah.” And they did not kill him, nor did they crucify him; but (another) was made to resemble him to them. And indeed, those who differ over it are in doubt about it. They have no knowledge of it except the following of assumption. And they did not kill him, for certain.

www.frontiersin.org

And do not mix the truth with falsehood or conceal the truth while you know (it).

www.frontiersin.org

(Iblees) said, “By your might, I will surely mislead them all.”

Previous studies such as Raut (2020), Okello et al. (2017); Alaaraj and Bakri (2020), and Khan (2020) examined investor’s decision making with the mediation of financial literacy for non-Islamic financial products while sharia literacy may have the ability to better explain investor’s behavior. According to the best of the authors’ knowledge, little research has been conducted on sharia literacy for Islamic financial products. Hence, the objective of this study is to explain investor decision-making behavior with the mediating role of sharia financial literacy for Islamic financial products.

Literature Review

Prospect theory was developed in 1979 by Kahneman and Tversky (1979) who postulated that investors weight losses more than gains, thereby putting higher preference for certain small profits compared with uncertain large profits. Developed another theory for investment decision making as cited by Pompian (2012), i.e., bounded rationality, which assumes that humans have limited information-processing capability, so therefore, they use different heuristics to make decisions (Shefrin and Statman, 2011). Prospect theory coupled with bounded rationality was used to underpin the theoretical model for this research. As applied to our study, these theories hold that we would expect investors be following herding as a means to choose among different Islamic financial products because they have less sharia financial literacy, fewer information availability and processing capabilities, less illusion of control, and self-attribution.

Herding is a common phenomenon in society, and it is defined by Chiang and Zheng (2010) as the habit of following other investor’s actions. Although the classification of herding is not an easy task, various behavior patterns such as imperfect expectations, frequent changes without much new information, bubbles, trends, and mania and sunspot equilibria of an individual can define it. Herding behavior depends on the degree of uncertainty, insufficient knowledge, and less confidence in information processing (Fernández et al., 2011). Kumar and Goyal (2016) defined that herding arise in the market when an investor does not have enough information and does not have certain knowledge to be relied upon for investment decision. Chen et al. (2020) argued that herding tendency was observed because of lack of information or familiarity with the situation and on actions that have been done previously in the same situation. Due to lack of information of the investors involved in the habit of following other investors, they assume that they are better in processing the best option for investment in decision making. Herding not only affects investor’s returns but may also result in market inefficiencies (Arisanti and Oktavendi, 2020). Herding, in the financial markets, is the tendency of investors to follow the investment behavior of others, probably irrationally (Pompian, 2012). It arises when an investor makes the same decision on the investment made by the majority because of lack of information-processing capabilities and uncertainties (Obeng, 2020). Similarly, Fernández et al. (2011) argued that investors who have inadequate information, scarce knowledge, and less confidence in their beliefs are usually involved in herding behavior. However, herding may lead to irrational investment decisions (Chiang et al., 2013).

Sabir et al. (2019) examined the relationship between illusions of control, self-attribution, herding behavior, and information availability on investor’s decision making. This study followed a survey questionnaire approach to collect data from 300 respondents. The authors concluded that illusion of control and self-attribution favor investor’s herding while greater information availability may lead to more logical, reasoned, and rational behavior, discouraging herding. Another study by Abreu and Mendes (2012) examined the impact of information available on the trading behavior of investors. The data for this study were collected through the questionnaire, and the results showed that investors with more information have a higher tendency to trade more frequently. Similarly, a study by Tauni et al. (2015) also reported a positive relationship between information availability and trading frequency for Chinese investors. Investors spend a significant amount of time in finding, collecting, and analyzing information to make prudent, intelligent, and rational investment decisions. Literature such as Precher (2010), Fernández et al. (2011), Chiang et al. (2013), and Sabir et al. (2018) reported that large investors have less herding tendency than small investors because large investors may utilize diverse sources to collect and analyze available information, thereby refraining to indulge in herding, while small investors may simply follow the large investor’s investment behavior.

Arisanti and Oktavendi (2020) investigated the relationship between information type (good governance business shariah disclosure) and herding. The data for this study were collected from the listed companies at Indonesia Stock Exchange, and the results revealed a significant impact of information disclosure on herding. Another study by Tiniç et al. (2020) also examined the relationship between informed trading and herding for Borsa, Turkey, investors. This study found a significant relationship between informed trading and herding, and the effect of herding would be stronger in the case of short-selling restriction. The findings of this study confirmed the presence of an information cascade framework, which holds that investors outweigh other investors’ information compared with their private information and, thus, follow others.

Availability of information reduces the risk in the market. Cognitive attributes of investors are affected by information availability when investors have sufficient knowledge and they decide to invest. Many firms do not give information about their firms, so investors invest by following others who have more experience. Shefrin and Statman (2011) claimed that investors may ignore material information and start following others because of the fear of failure. Investors show herd behavior to preserve their reputation. If the failure occurs on the decision that was made by an individual on his attribution, he will suffer from a high amount of regret; but if the failure occurs on the decision made by many other investors, the amount of regret is low. In an efficient market, investors are rational, and prices of securities will reflect all the available information (Ross et al., 2016). In reality, the situation is different when investors’ decisions depend upon their behaviors and emotions. According to Easley and Kleinberg (2010), investors follow the decisions of others, although they know that others’ decisions may also be wrong instead; there is no solid reason to mitigate the actions of others, but still, they follow others, believing that others might have more relevant information. Similarity, Chaffai and Medhioub (2018) stated that it is hard for investors to logically analyze the new information that is available, and due to this reason, investors avoid the available information and take decision by following other investors in the market.

The illusion of control is a situation in which investors overstated their skills to control events, and they consider that the outcomes of unpleasant events are in their hands (Metilda, 2015). The individual investor who is affected by the illusion of control thinks that his skills are very worthy and productive. He considers himself very skillful and can take decisions in any situation. It is demonstrated that investors having an illusion of control attribution are consider as a more risk-taker in investing (Boyd and Vozikis, 1994). The illusion of control is a bias in which individual investors overestimated their capability to control any events, and they think that the outcomes of unpleasant events are in their control (Metilda, 2015). Fellner (2009) examined the illusion of control bias in the portfolio allocation of individual investment. Her study used z-Tree computerized experiment, which was developed by Fischbacher in 2007. The experiment was conducted in the Max Planck Research Laboratory. The age of participants was from 18 to 32, and their average income is 12.5 $. In line with excessive extrapolation, the higher the number of observed positive prior outcomes, the more likely is a positive prediction and in turn a higher investment. Another study of Ajmal et al. (2011) examined the illusion of control in perceived market efficiency. This study used a questionnaire, and regression model was applied. The results showed that the illusion of control has a significant impact on an investor’s decision making. Handoyo et al. (2020) examined the impact of different behavioral biases (anchoring, loss aversion, illusion of control, and overconfidence) on herding. This study found that behavioral biases have a significant impact on investment decisions, especially for naïve investors.

Self-attribution is described as situation in which an individual attributes success to his skill and failures due to unpleasant events (Hoffmann and Post, 2014). Pompian (2012) explained that self-attribution is a motivational and cognitive component. Generally, an individual gives accomplishment credit to his intrinsic aspects, e.g., ability and knowledge, and blames other external conditions for his failure. The study of Mushinada (2020) concluded that self-attribution and overconfidence could significantly impact investment decisions.

Yusuff and Mansor (2016) indicated that decision making of investors is affected by Islamic financial literacy, individual religiosity level, and product knowledge. These are the few factors that influence the individual decision-making process. Islamic financial literacy plays a major role in impelling Muslims to make the right choice in their decision of investment. The Muslims must have information on finance management because it can direct them to make a decent choice that is understandable in the Islamic point of view. A decent choice will assist them be successful in this world and hereafter. Only good understanding of Islamic principles enforces them to educate themselves about Islamic financial literacy and invest in it. Prior studies show that people with a high level of Islamic financial literacy tend to put their assets in Islamic investment. The government is trying to increase the level of Islamic financial literacy and provide additional Quran and Hadith education to put public belief in Islamic principles. It is found that there is a significant relationship between Islamic financial literacy and financial management planning. A person is better in managing his finances if his level of financial literacy is high; and if a person has high Islamic financial literacy, then he prefers Islamic investment (Boon et al., 2011; Arrondel et al., 2013; Agarwal et al., 2015). Widityani et al. (2020) found that the tendency for Islamic products would be higher among investors with high Islamic financial literacy. Similarly, past studies such as Glaser and Weber (2007), Hung et al. (2009), Boon et al. (2011), Arrondel et al. (2013), and Agarwal et al. (2015) argued that information availability along with shariah literacy could have a significant impact on financial decision making and choice of financial products, i.e., conventional or Islamic. For instance, a person with more information availability and higher Islamic financial literacy is more likely to invest in Islamic financial products. Claimed that Islamic financial literacy may produce better financial returns for investors and rationally investment behavior. Based on literature, Figure 1 shows the research framework and hypotheses that may be proposed.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research framework. H1, self-attribution has a significant impact on herding for Islamic financial products; H2, illusion of control has a significant impact on herding for Islamic financial products; H3, information availability has a significant impact on herding for Islamic financial products; H4, self-attribution has a significant impact on shariah literacy; H5, illusion of control has a significant impact on shariah literacy; H6, information availability has a significant impact on shariah literacy; H7: shariah literacy has a significant impact on herding for Islamic financial products; H8: shariah literacy mediates the relationship between self-attribution, illusion of control, information availability, and herding for Islamic financial products.

Methodology

The objective of this study is to examine the impact of self-attribution, illusion of control, information availability, and herding for Islamic financial products with the mediating role of sharia literacy. This study utilized a quantitative research approach to address the study objective. The data for this study were collected through an adoptive questionnaire on a five-point Likert scale. The items for information availability, self-attribution, illusion of control, and herding were adopted from Sabir et al. (2018), while Islamic sharia literacy items were adopted from Albaity and Rahman (2018). The population of the study was composed of investors investing in Islamic financial products and 11 banks that were targeted for data collection, four pure Islamic, and seven conventional banks with Islamic banks.

The principle outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986) was used to justify the mediating role of Islamic sharia literacy. As per Baron and Kenny (1986), a prior relationship between variables, i.e., independent, dependent, and mediating, must exist. Partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) is used to test the hypothesis. The PLS-SEM technique is superior to other statistical methods in many ways. For example, it has no sample size restriction, is effective for statistical model building along with forecasting, is precise and accurate in estimation, has soft modeling assumptions, does not require normality of data, and is suitable especially in case of mediation (Iacobucci et al., 2007; Osborne, 2011; Hair et al., 2014, 2017; Ramli and Nartea, 2016). Moreover, SEM is a combination of two powerful statistical approaches, exploratory factor analysis and structural path analysis, which enable the simultaneous assessment of the measurement model and the structural model (Hair et al., 2017).

Data Analysis

A total of 500 questionnaires were distributed among respondents, 422 respondents returned the filled questionnaires, and 410 were used for the analysis. A researcher like Cain et al. (2017) and Hair et al. (2017) suggested that data need to be checked for multivariate skewness and kurtosis using the software available at1. The results showed that the present study data were not multivariate normal; thus, we continued to use the SmartPLS, which is a non-parametric analysis software due to multivariate normality issues. An analysis like descriptive analysis, reliability, convergent validity, discriminant validity, loadings, and path analysis was performed to analyze the collected data.

Table 1 reports descriptive statistics, calculated through SPSS, of the respondent’s profile. The results revealed that 63.2% of respondents were male, and 36.8% of females responded to the questionnaire. Descriptive statistics further reported that the majority of respondents (46.3%) were from 26–35 age bracket followed by 29.8% belonging to 18–25 age bracket. Descriptive statistics also reported that majority of the respondents hold either a graduate or post-graduate degree, thereby enabling them to intelligently respond to the questions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Respondent’s descriptive profile.

The validity of the constructs was observed by a measurement model with convergent validity and discriminant validity. To observe the convergent validity of the constructs, factor loadings, composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) were used. According to Hair et al. (2014), if the factor loading of items is ≥0.50, CR ≥ 0.80, and AVE ≥ 0.50, then the constructs’ validity is convergent or otherwise. The values for factor loadings were greater than 0.5 as reported in Table 2; hence, the model has good fitness. Moreover, the value of CR for all variables satisfied the criteria except for SA. Similarly, the values of average variance were greater than 0.5, indicating the presence of convergent validity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Convergent validity.

The value of the hetrotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio is explained in Table 2. HTMT ratio is the geometric means of heterotrait–heteromethod correlations divided by the means of the monotrait–heteromethod correlations. The HTMT ratio is an effective way to observe the discriminant validity. According to a model is well fitted if the discriminant validity of the HTMT ratio should be less than 0.85. In Table 3, values of all HTMT ratios are less than 0.85, which present the discriminant validity of the model.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. HTMT discriminant validity.

In the next phase, after the evaluation of the measurement model, we move to the evaluation of the structural model in SmartPLS. For gaining consistent result, we evaluate structure model through SmartPLS bootstrapping. The structural model gives us results through path coefficient, standard error, t-value, p-value, R2, Q2, and the final decision of a hypothesis (Hair et al., 2014). To get direct and mediating results of variables, the bootstrapping method was applied (Hair et al., 2014; Soto-Acosta et al., 2016).

Table 4 reports factor loading and variance inflation factor (VIF). VIF analysis was performed to check for potential multicollinearity issues. The value of VIF may be used as a general criterion to figure out potential multicollinearity; for instance, VIF equal to or less than one indicates no correlation; a value between one and five shows moderate collection. In contrast, a value higher than five indicates a strong correlation among variables and the possibility of multicollinearity. As all the values reported above for VIF are close to one, it may be presumed that there is no multicollinearity issue.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Direct results.

The results for the direct relationship (as reported in Table 4 and Figure 2) reported that all variables, i.e., information availability (β = 0.244, p < 0.00), illusion of control (β = 0.192, p < 0.00), and self-attribution (β = 0.161, p < 0.00), showed a positively significant impact on herding. The results for the direct relationship reported that all variables, i.e., information availability (β = 0.451, p < 0.01), illusion of control (β = 0.108, p < 0.01), and self-attribution (β = 0.169, p < 0.00), showed a positively significant impact on shariah literacy.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Structural model.

Discussion

This result for herding is in in-line with the findings of Daniel et al. (1998), Fernández et al. (2001, 2011), Nofsinger (2005), Epstein and Schneider (2008), Abreu and Mendes (2012), Nguyen and Schüßler (2012), Stefan and David (2013), Metilda (2015), Mishra and Metilda (2015), Pavlovic (2018), and Narasimha and Mushinada (2018).

According to authors like Keynes (1937), Bikhchandani and Sharma (2000), Chang et al. (2000), Shiller (2003), Chiang and Zheng (2010), Hassairi (2011), and Bashir et al. (2014) market inefficiencies, low self-attribution, and less illusion of control and information cascade framework may lead to herding. In an inefficient market, like Pakistan, investors may believe that either they do not have complete information or others may have better information (information cascade framework), and they may become uncertain and doubtful, which will lead them to follow the herding. Where perfect information does not exist, the feeling of uncertainty leads individuals to think that others are better informed. The greater the investors’ feeling of uncertainty, the more likely they doubt their information. Consequently, investors try to obtain information by observing the financial decision making of others participating in the market.

The results for shariah literacy (β = 0.161, p > 0.005) and herding were found to be insignificant, suggesting that shariah literacy may promote rational thinking and independent decision-making behavior among investors rather than inducing them to follow the crowd. This result is in line with the findings of Atkinson and Messy (2012), Setyowati et al. (2018), and Biplob and Abdullah (2019), who also found that higher literacy will encourage people to think and act rationally.

The results presented in Table 5 reported an indirect relationship between information availability, illusion of control, self-attribution, and herding with the mediating impact of shariah literacy through bootstrapping. The previously significant relationships between information availability, illusion of control, self-attribution, and herding turned out to be insignificant with the mediation of shariah literacy. This suggests that shariah literacy could have a significant impact on controlling cognitive biases and encouraging rational investment behavior.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Indirect results.

Table 6 shows that Q2 values of the items on HB are greater. Eight out of nine items in the endogenous latent variable, i.e., SME performance, are greater than 0. The result deduced that predictive relevance is present in the model. As a conclusion, when a PLS-SEM model exhibits predictive relevance, it will predict well the data points of indicators.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. PLS Predictive.

Conclusion

Due to cognitive biases and low shariah literacy, individual investors follow herd behavior in Islamic Financial Products. Our study acknowledged/documented the influence of cognitive biases information availability, illusion of control, and self-attribution on individual herd behavior in Islamic financial products. Islamic financial literacy is not developed enough, so our study shows the effect of cognitive biases in individual investor decision making. Usually, investors are not financially literate; they are unable to evaluate the good investment products. Most of the investors do not have shariah literacy, or they are not intelligent enough to convert information into knowledge. Thus, in an uncertain situation where information is not fully available, investors indulge in herding behavior because they thought others are better in accessing the good investment decision. Results indicate that cognitive biases information availability, illusion of control, and self-attribution encourage herding behavior in Islamic financial products, but shariah financial literacy mitigates the result of herding behavior in Islamic financial products. The outcome of this study helps us to tell why investors indulge in herding behavior in taking an investment decision. This will also be helpful for investors to predict the different biases in their behavior. This study focuses on the only individuals located in Sialkot, Gujrat, and Gujranwala; and due to time limitation, the sample size is also small. Further research can be done on a large sample size in many different cities to get more general conclusions.

Practical Implications

The findings of this study have several practical implications for financial managers. As mentioned earlier, herding either positively or negatively would result in financial instability, thereby causing market crashes and investors losing billions of dollars. Shariah literacy would help investors to act rationally, thereby improving market efficiency.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Zeeshan Ahmed (Assistant Professor University of Lahore). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SD and SM wrote the article. IA collected the data. AS performed the data analysis and discussion. AD and AA-R significantly contributed to address reviewers comments to improve overall write-up of article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://webpower.psychstat.org/models/kurtosis

References

Abreu, M., and Mendes, V. (2012). Information, overconfidence and trading: do the sources of information matter? J. Econ. Psychol. 33, 868–881.

Google Scholar

Agarwal, S., Amromin, G., Ben-david, I., Chomsisengphet, S., and Evanoff, D. D. (2015). Financial literacy and financial planning: evidence from india sumit. J. Hous. Econ. 27, 1–52.

Google Scholar

Aigbovo, O., and Ilaboya, O. J. (2019). Does Behavioural Biases Influences Individual Investment Decisions, Vol. 10. Benin City: University of Benin, 68–89.

Google Scholar

Ajmal, S., Muti, M., and Shah, Z. A. (2011). Impact of illusion of control on perceived efficiency in pakistani financial markets. Abasyn J. Soc. Sci. 5, 100–110.

Google Scholar

Alaaraj, H., and Bakri, A. (2020). The effect of financial literacy on investment decision making in southern lebanon. Int. Bus. Account. Res. J. 4, 37–43. doi: 10.15294/ibarj.v4i1.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albaity, M., and Rahman, M. (2018). The intention to use Islamic banking?: an exploratory study to measure Islamic financial literacy. Int. J. Emerg. Mark. 15, 988–1012. doi: 10.1108/ijoem-05-2018-0218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alnajjar, M. I. M. (2013). Behavioural inferences of stock market participants. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. 39, 384–390.

Google Scholar

Arisanti, I., and Oktavendi, T. W. (2020). Herding behaviour in sharia stock?: the moderation effect of good governance business sharia disclosure. J. Account. Invest. 21, 45–60.

Google Scholar

Armansyah, R. F. (2018). Herd behavior and indonesian financial crisis. J. Adv. Manag. Sci. 6, 86–89. doi: 10.18178/joams.6.2.86-89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrondel, L., Debbich, M., and Savignac, F. (2013). Financial literacy and financial planning in france financial literacy and financial planning in france. Numeracy 6, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Atkinson, A., and Messy, F. A. (2012). Measuring Financial Literacy: Results of the OECD/International Network on Financial Education (INFE) Pilot Study. Paris: OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

Babajide, A. A., and Adetiloye, K. A. (2012). Investors’ behavioural biases and the security market?: an empirical study of the nigerian security market. Account. Finance Res. 1, 219–229.

Google Scholar

Barberis, N. (2017). Behavioural Finance: Asset Prices and Investor Behavior. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social the moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bashir, T., Fatima, T., Ilyas, H., and Chudhary, A. (2014). Impact of cognitive profile and information uncertainty on herding behavior of investor. IOSR J. Bus. Manag. 16, 68–74. doi: 10.9790/487x-16156874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bikhchandani, S., and Sharma, S. (2000). Herd behavior in financial markets. IMF Staff Pap. 47, 279–310.

Google Scholar

Biplob, H., and Abdullah, M. F. (2019). The importance of islamic financial literacy for muslims: a general review. Islamic And Civilisational Renewal 10, 106–117.

Google Scholar

Boon, T. H., Yee, H. S., and Ting, H. W. (2011). Financial literacy and personal financial planning in klang Valley, Malaysia. Int. J. Econ. Manag. 5, 149–168.

Google Scholar

Boyd, N. G., and Vozikis, G. S. (1994). The influence of self-efficacy on the development of entrepreneurial intentions and actions. Entrepreneurship Theory Pract. 18, 63–77. doi: 10.1177/104225879401800404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain, M. K., Zhang, Z., and Yuan, K.-H. (2017). Univariate and multivariate skewness and kurtosis for measuring nonnormality?: prevalence, influence and estimation. Behav. Res. Methods 49, 1716–1735. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0814-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaffai, M., and Medhioub, I. (2018). Herding behaviour in Islamic GCC stock market?: a daily analysis. Int. J. Islamic Middle Eastern Finance Manag. 11, 182–193. doi: 10.1108/imefm-08-2017-0220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, E. C., Cheng, J. W., and Khorana, A. (2000). An examination of herd behaviour in equity markets: an international perspective. J. Bank. Finance 24, 1651–1679. doi: 10.1016/s0378-4266(99)00096-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, A., He, J., Liang, M., and Su, G. (2020). Crowd response considering herd effect and exit familiarity under emergent occasions: a case study of an evacuation drill experiment. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 556:124654. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2020.124654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, T. C., Li, J., Tan, L., and Nelling, E. (2013). Dynamic herding behavior in pacific-basin markets?: evidence and implications. Mult. Finance J. 17, 165–200. doi: 10.17578/17-3/4-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, T. C., and Zheng, D. (2010). An empirical analysis of herd behaviour in global stock markets. J. Bank. Finance 34, 1911–1921. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2009.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cueva, C., and Rustichini, A. (2015). Is financial instability male-driven? Gender and cognitive skills in experimental asset markets?. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 119, 330–344. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2015.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., Subrahmanyam, A., Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., and Subrahmanyam, A. (1998). Investor psychology and security market under- and overreactions. J. Finance 53, 1839–1885. doi: 10.1111/0022-1082.00077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easley, D., and Kleinberg, J. (2010). Networks, Crowds, and Markets?: Reasoning about a Highly Connected World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 43–44.

Google Scholar

Epstein, L. G., and Schneider, M. (2008). Ambiguity, information quality, and asset pricing. J. Finance 63, 197–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6261.2008.01314.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fellner, G. (2009). Illusion of control as a source of poor diversification?: experimental evidence. J. Behav. Finance 10, 55–57. doi: 10.1080/15427560902740006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, B., Garcia-Merino, T., Mayoral, R., Santos, V., and Vallelado, E. (2011). Herding, information uncertainty and investors’ cognitive profile. Q. Res. Financial Mark. 3, 7–33. doi: 10.1108/17554171111124595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, B., Garcia-Merino, T., Mayoral, R., Santos, V., and Vallelado, E. (2001). The role of the interaction between information and behavioral bias in explaining herding. Ments Financial Econ. Account. Manag. Valladolid Spain 1–34. doi: 10.21095/ajmr/2015/v8/i1/88220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerardi, K., Goette, L., and Meier, S. (2013). Numerical ability predicts mortgage default. Proc. Nal. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 11267–11271. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220568110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaser, M., and Weber, ÆM. (2007). Overconfidence and trading volume. Geneva Risk Insurance Rev. 32, 1–36. doi: 10.1007/s10713-007-0003-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F. J., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C., and Sarstedt, M. (2014). “A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM),” in Long Range Planning, 1st edn, Vol. 46, Los Angeles: SAGE Publications Ltd.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Matthews, L. M., Matthews, R. L., and Sarstedt, M. (2017). PLS-SEM or CB-SEM?: updated guidelines on which method to use. Int. J. Multivar. Data Anal. 1, 107–123. doi: 10.1504/IJMDA.2017.087624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handoyo, S. D., Rispantyo, and Widarno, B. (2020). Pengaruh overconfidence, illusion of control, anchoring, loss aversion pada pengambilian keputusan investasi oleh mahasiswa unisri sebagai investor pemula. J. Akuntansi Sist. Teknol. Inf. 15, 411–421.

Google Scholar

Hashim, R. (2003). Rosnani Hashim. Intell. Discou. 11, 27–50.

Google Scholar

Hassairi, S. A. (2011). Herd behavior and market stress?: the case of four european countries. Int. Bus. Res. 4, 53–67.

Google Scholar

Hoffmann, A. O. I., and Post, T. (2014). Self-attribution bias in consumer financial decision-making?: how investment returns affect individuals’. Belief in Skill. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. Forthcoming 31, 23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2014.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, A. A., Parker, A. M., and Yoong, J. K. (2009). Defining and measuring financial literacy. SSRN Electron. J. 708, 1–29. doi: 10.1057/9781403983053_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacobucci, D., Saldanha, N., and Deng, X. (2007). A meditation on mediation: evidence that structural equations models perform better than regressions. J. Consu. Psychol. 17, 139–153. doi: 10.1016/s1057-7408(07)70020-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–292. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keynes, J. M. (1937). General theory of employment. Q. J. Econ. 51, 209–223.

Google Scholar

Khan, D. (2020). Cognitive Driven Biases, Investment Decision Making?: The Moderating Role of Financial Literacy. 1–25. Avaliable at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3514086 (accessed January 5, 2020).

Google Scholar

Klapper, L., Lusardi, A., and Panos, G. A. (2013). Financial literacy and its consequences?: evidence from Russia during the financial crisis. J. Bank. Finance 37, 3904–3923. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2013.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, S., and Goyal, N. (2016). Evidence on rationality and behavioural biases in investment decision making abstract. Q. Res. Financial Mark. 8, 270–287. doi: 10.1108/qrfm-05-2016-0016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lusardi, A., Mitchell, O. S., Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2014). the economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. J. Econo. Lit. 52, 5–44. doi: 10.1257/jel.52.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metilda, M. (2015). Role of illusion of control bias in herding behaviour. Adarsh J. Manag. Res. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.21095/ajmr/2015/v8/i1/88220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishra, K. C., and Metilda, M. J. (2015). A study on the impact of investment experience, gender, and level of education on overconfidence and self-attribution bias. IIMB Manag. Rev. 27, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/j.iimb.2015.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mushinada, V. N. C. (2020). Journal of behavioral and experimental finance are individual investors irrational or adaptive to market dynamics? J. Behav. Exp. Finance 25:100243. doi: 10.1016/j.jbef.2019.100243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narasimha, V., and Mushinada, C. (2018). Self-attribution, overconfidence and dynamic market volatility in indian stock market. Global Bus. Rev. 21, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Nguyen, T., and Schüßler, A. (2012). Investment decisions and socio-demographic characteristics – empirical evidence from germany. Int. J. Econ. Finance 4, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Nofsinger, J. R. (2005). Social mood and financial economics social mood and financial economics. J. Behav. Finance 6, 144–160. doi: 10.1207/s15427579jpfm0603_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeng, G. (2020). Behavioural finance; a concept or catalyst explaining distortions in investment decision. Eur. J. Bus. Manag. Res. 5, 1–13. doi: 10.24018/ejbmr.2020.5.1.59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okello, G. C. B., Ntayi, J. M., Munene, J. C., and Malinga, C. A. (2017). Review of International Business and Strategy. Bingley: Emerald Publishing Limited, 1–22.

Google Scholar

Osborne, J. W. (2011). Best practices in using large, complex samples: the importance of using appropriate weights and design effect compensation. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 16. doi: 10.7275/2kyg-m659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlovic, N. (2018). Factors affecting Herd Behaviour in Buying Decisions influenced by Online Communities. AMCIS 1–10.

Google Scholar

Pompian, M. M. (2012). Behavioural Finance and Investor Types: Managing Behavior to Make Better Investment Decisions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Precher, R. R. Jr. (2010). Unconscious herding behavior as the psychological basis of financial market trends and patterns. J. Psychol. Financial Mark. 2, 120–125. doi: 10.1207/s15327760jpfm0203_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramli, N. A., and Nartea, G. (2016). Mediation effects of firm leverage in malaysia?: partial least squares - structural equation modeling. Int. J. Econ. Financial Issues 6, 301–307.

Google Scholar

Raut, R. K. (2020). Past behaviour, financial literacy and investment decision-making process of individual investors. Int. J. Emerg. Mark. 15, 1243–1263. doi: 10.1108/IJOEM-07-2018-0379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Razak, L. A., and Abdullah, R. (2015). Exploratory research into islamic financial literacy in brunei darussalam. Int. Centre Educ. Islamic Finance 62–87.

Google Scholar

Ross, S. A., Westerfield, R. W., Jaffe, J. F., and Jordan, B. D. (2016). Corporate Finance, 11th Edn. Toronto, ONT: Mcgraw Hill.

Google Scholar

Sabir, S. A., Mohammad, H. B., and Shahar, H. B. K. (2018). The effect of illusion of control and self attribution on herding behaviour with a moderating role of information availability: a case of retail investors of pakistan stock exchange. Opción 34, 2675–2689.

Google Scholar

Sabir, S. A., Mohammad, H. B., and Shahar, H. B. K. (2019). The role of overconfidence and past investment experience in herding behaviour with a moderating effect of financial literacy: evidence from Pakistan stock exchange. Asian Econ. Financial Rev. 9, 480–490. doi: 10.18488/journal.aefr.2019.94.480.490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Setyowati, A., Harmadi, H., and Sunarjanto, S. (2018). Islamic financial literacy and personal financial planning?: a socio-demographic study. Int. J. Econ. Manag. 5, 149–168.

Google Scholar

Sharma, A., and Kumar, A. (2019). A review paper on behavioural finance: study of emerging trends. Q. Res. Financial Mark. 12, 137–157. doi: 10.1108/qrfm-06-2017-0050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shefrin, H., and Statman, M. (2011). “Behavioural finance in the financial crisis: market efficiency, minsky, and keynes,” in Rethinking the Financial Crisis, Working Paper, Santa Clara, CA: Santa Clara University.

Google Scholar

Shiller, R. J. (2003). Behavioural Finance. J. Econ. Perspect. 17, 83–104.

Google Scholar

Soto-Acosta, P., Popa, S., and Palacios-Marqués, D. (2016). E-business, organizational innovation and firm performance in manufacturing SMEs: an empirical study in Spain. Technol. Econ. Dev. Econ. 22, 885–904. doi: 10.3846/20294913.2015.1074126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefan, S., and David, D. (2013). Recent developments in the experimental investigation of the illusion of control. A meta-analytic review. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43:37.

Google Scholar

Tauni, M. Z., Fang, H. X., Rao, Z., and Yousaf, S. (2015). The influence of Investor personality traits on information acquisition and trading behaviour?: evidence from Chinese futures exchange. Personal. Individ. Differ. 87, 248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiniç, M., Iqbal, M. S., and Mahmud, S. F. (2020). Information cascades, short-selling constraints, and herding in equity markets. Borsa Istanbul Rev. 20, 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.bir.2020.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widityani, S. F., Faturohman, T., and Rahadi, R. A. (2020). Do socio-demographic characteristics and financial literacy matter for selecting islamic financial products. J. Islamic Monetary Econ. Finance 6, 51–76.

Google Scholar

Yusuff, N., and Mansor, F. (2016). Proposed model for the factors influencing muslim investors in choosing islamic unit. J. Glob. Bus. Soc. Entrepreneurship 1, 113–120.

Google Scholar

Keywords: illusion of control, self-attribution bias, Islamic financial products, behavioral biases, herding, Islamic (shari’ah) financial products

Citation: Din SMU, Mehmood SK, Shahzad A, Ahmad I, Davidyants A and Abu-Rumman A (2021) The Impact of Behavioral Biases on Herding Behavior of Investors in Islamic Financial Products. Front. Psychol. 11:600570. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.600570

Received: 03 September 2020; Accepted: 14 December 2020;
Published: 04 February 2021.

Edited by:

Nicola Mucci, University of Florence, Italy

Reviewed by:

Essia Ahmed, University of Nizwa, Oman
Azam Abdelhakeem Khalid Ahmed, Sultan Idris University of Education, Malaysia

Copyright © 2021 Din, Mehmood, Shahzad, Ahmad, Davidyants and Abu-Rumman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sajid Mohy Ul Din, sajidmohyuldingsk@yahoo.com; Sajidmohyulding@yahoo.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.