Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 May 2021
Sec. Forensic and Legal Psychology

Executive Functions of Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit Applicants and Police Officer Trainees Evaluated With Design Fluency Test

  • 1Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 2Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 3Center for Cognitive and Computational Neuropsychiatry, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Executive functions (EF) represent higher order top-down mechanisms regulating information processing. While suboptimal EF have been studied in various patient groups, their impact on successful behavior is still not well described. Previously, it has been suggested that design fluency (DF)—a test including several simultaneous EF components mainly related to fluency, cognitive flexibility, and creativity—predicts successful behavior in a quickly changing environment where fast and dynamic adaptions are required, such as ball sports. We hypothesized that similar behaviors are of importance in the selection process of elite police force applicants. To test this hypothesis, we compared elite police force applicants (n = 45) with a control group of police officer trainees (n = 30). Although both groups were better than the norm, the elite police force applicants had a significantly better performance in DF total correct when adjusting for sex and age [F(1,71) = 18.98, p < 0.001]. To understand how this capacity was altered by stress and tiredness, we re-tested the elite police force applicants several days during an extreme field assessment lasting 10 days. The results suggested that there was a lower than expected improvement in DF total correct and a decline in the DF3-subtest that includes a larger component of cognitive flexibility than the other subtests (DF1 and DF2). Although there was a positive correlation between the baseline session and the re-test in DF3 [r(40) = 0.49, p = 0.001], the applicants having the highest scores in the baseline test also displayed the largest percentage decline in the re-test [r(40) = −0.46, p = 0.003]. In conclusion, our result suggests that higher order EF (HEF) that include cognitive flexibility and creativity are of importance in the application for becoming an elite police officer but relatively compromised in a stressful situation. Moreover, as the decline is different between the individuals, the results suggest that applicants should be tested during baseline conditions and during stressful conditions to describe their cognitive capacity fully.

Introduction

Executive functions (EF) is an umbrella term to describe the cognitive processes that regulate thought and action, and allow the individual to dynamically adapt to a changing environment (Friedman et al., 2006). EF involve planning, selective and sustained attention, inhibition, multi-tasking, cognitive flexibility, ability to deal with novelty, and problem-solving (Chan et al., 2008). Large-scale brain networks are involved in EF with prefrontal structures especially in focus (Kerns et al., 2004; Tanji and Hoshi, 2008; Mansouri et al., 2009; Stuss, 2011; Cieslik et al., 2015; Koechlin, 2016).

It has been suggested that EF can be sub-divided into core EF (CEF) and higher order EF (HEF) (Luciana et al., 2005; Diamond, 2013). While CEF include simple working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control (Diamond, 2013), HEF are associated with planning, reasoning, and problem-solving (Diamond, 2013). The difference between CEF and HEF is related to the demands of the task, and HEF typically include several CEF simultaneously used for fast and accurate planning and problem-solving (Luciana et al., 2005; Diamond, 2013; Krenn et al., 2018). For example, holding information online to make a simple decision is more related to CEF. Conversely, maintaining and manipulating information in order to strategically organize goal-oriented behavior is more related to HEF. The level of CEF develops to its full capacity earlier in the lifespan than HEF, mostly before early adolescence (Luciana et al., 2005; Best and Miller, 2010).

Previously, research has focused on the relation between deficits of EF and behavior (Luria, 1980), development of EF (Anderson et al.,2001a,b), and their biological correlates in the brain (Stuss, 2011). Deficits in EF have been studied in disorders like ADHD, autism, and traumatic brain injury (Goldstein and Naglieri, 2014). EF are normally distributed among a general population, and disorders like ADHD may be thought of as low EF capacity in this distribution (Das et al., 2012; Crosbie et al., 2013; Petrovic and Castellanos, 2016; Bayard et al., 2018). The impact of the EFs on the other side of the continuum is still not fully explored.

A high EF capacity may be of importance in many different professions. We have, for example, previously postulated that well-developed EF are important in elite team ball-sports since a large amount of information must quickly be processed in a constantly changing environment (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017, 2020). In line with those ideas, it has been suggested that measures of EF, and especially HEF, have an important role in soccer (Verburgh et al., 2014, 2016; Huijgen et al., 2015; Vestberg et al., 2017, 2020; Krenn et al., 2018; Sakamoto et al., 2018; Schumacher et al., 2018; Scharfen and Memmert, 2019) and other ball sports (Alves et al., 2013; Faubert, 2013; Wang et al., 2013, 2016; Lundgren et al., 2016; Alarcón et al., 2017; Liao et al., 2017; Ishihara et al., 2018; Wylie et al., 2018).

Another profession in which EF may be of fundamental importance is law enforcement. A police officer must deal with increasing demands for public safety under pressure from crime and terrorism (Gutshall et al., 2017). Police officers need to be able to handle a wide range of different situations, sometimes also under physical and even death threats. Since a police officer may encounter situations involving a large amount of information flow that is changing quickly (Gutshall et al., 2017), it may be hypothesized that well-functioning EF are a prerequisite for managing the profession, especially when there are time constraints and the stress level is high.

To be accepted to the police officers program, the applicant needs to meet specific requirements regarding physical, medical, and psychological status (Admission Requirements for Police Training, 2020). Medical obligations include absence of chronic somatic and psychiatric disorders. For the physical requirements, individuals must fulfill a specific level of physical work capacity and muscle strength. The psychological demands include a certain level of personal maturity, responsiveness, flexibility, commitment, and responsibility—as well as a well-developed communication skill. These requirements are assessed systematically before the individual is allowed to start in the police academy.

Significantly higher demands are required for joining an elite police force, such as the Swedish counterterror intervention unit [Nationella Insatsstyrkan (NI)] (Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit, 2020). Their primary assignment is to deal with difficult situations related to terrorism and support other police units in cases like hostage-taking and robbery. The basic requirements for becoming a member of NI are considerably higher than for the regular police force (Halsius, 2007; Work at Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit, 2020). Apart from high physical performance, such positions require individuals who can think independently, have a realistic sense of perception, and function well in groups—even under heavy stress (Halsius, 2007; Work at Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit, 2020).

The admission process for NI is divided into different steps and takes several months to complete. The assessments are time-consuming and resource-demanding. They also require extensive physical and mental abilities (Werkelius, 1997). In the final step, the applicants will be closely observed during a 10-day extreme assessment test. The individual ability to use tactics, as well as being creative, work in teams, and perseverance, will be assessed under heavy stress (Werkelius, 1997; Halsius, 2007). Only a fraction of the applicants reaches the final level of testing, as most do not pass the earlier tests.

Situations of danger, that, for example, involve armed offenders, are associated with a high degree of stress, and may change and escalate quickly. It may be argued that individuals being recruited as operators for NI therefore should be able to cope optimally and find the most creative solutions in order to make the best decisions in such extreme circumstances. They should have a high capacity for cognitive flexibility to adjust dynamically in a quickly changing environment. They should also be resistant to stress and time constraints and inhibit some automatic behaviors that may be dangerous in specific contexts. The fast decisions made by an elite police officer may also result in unnecessary injuries and deaths of both police officers and offenders as well as in collateral casualties. Thus, both choices of most effective goal-directed behaviors and avoiding mistakes in such situations involve optimal EF apart from other cognitive abilities, emotional reactivity, and personality traits. Several of these requirements are similar to team sports, e.g., fast online problem-solving and high capacity to dynamically adapt in a quickly changing environment associated with an extensive information load. The military academy, a branch close to the police, also highlights flexibility paired with creativity as critical features for the military defense of the future (Finkel, 2011). The same reasoning may be applied for future successful elite police officers.

Previous research on police officers has studied how working memory relates to successful shooting behavior (Kleider et al., 2010) and indicates that EF is vitally important in extreme situations. Research on military special forces has focused on how stress influences working memory and learning (Taverniers et al., 2010; Morgan et al., 2011; Gutshall et al., 2017) and suggests that stress decreases the capacity of CEF. Studies on general population also suggest that lack of sleep (Yoo et al., 2007; Killgore, 2010; Lim and Dinges, 2010; Ma et al., 2015; Krause et al., 2017; Floros et al., 2020) and cognitive tiredness (Blain et al., 2016) have a negative impact on cognition, including EF. However, it is not clear how HEF capacity (including cognitive flexibility and creativity) changes when elite officers are under heavy physical and psychological pressure and sleep-deprived. Moreover, EF and HEF capacity of elite police officers has not been studied in relation to ordinary officers.

In the present study, we suggest that the ability to dynamically adapt in a rapidly changing environment involving a large amount threat is a core aspect for successful behavior both in the stress test that elite police applicants undergo and in situations where elite police officers may encounter a potential danger such as armed offenders or hostage taking. Arguably, such behavior should be associated with high EF capacity, especially HEF, as these cognitive abilities are fundamental for quickly solving complex problems. Performance in these contexts must also be malleable to mentally and physically stressful and exhausting situations, possibly affecting individuals with low EF capacity most. Based on these ideas, we more specifically hypothesized that: (1) selected applicants to the final field test have better HEF than regular police officer trainees and the normal population, (2) cognitive capacity of HEF would decline during a physical and psychologically stressful period, and (3) NI applicants with best baseline results would also have the best results in a stressful situation. We were especially interested in design fluency (DF) capacity as it has previously been shown to be a suitable test for successful behavior in a quickly changing environment where creative solutions are needed (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017, 2020).

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the local ethical committee (Regionala etikprövningsnämnden i Stockholm; Dnr 2015/528-31/4) and was performed in full compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects were given verbal and written information on the study and gave their verbal and written informed consent to participate.

Participants

Forty-five subjects were recruited to the test-group (NI applicants, NIA) and 30 subjects were recruited to the control-group (police officer trainee, POT).

Applicants to NI are required to have completed police and police aspirant training. Additionally, the applicant should have good eyesight, be well-trained, and have an aptitude for tactics and shooting. The NIA-group (n = 45; 44 men and one woman; Age range 27–41 years; Mean age = 31.7 years, SD = 3.33) consisted of applicants who passed psychological and physical screening tests, to be admitted to the counterterror intervention unit (NI). All applicants and their test results from the baseline testing were used. No subjects were excluded or failed in the baseline test situation. Several of the applicants failed the extreme field assessment test before the different re-tests of EF were performed and, thus, fewer subjects were re-tested (Re-test 1: n = 40, Re-test 2: n = 39, and Re-test 3: n = 36).

The POT-group (n = 30; 24 men and six women; Age range 22–39 years; Mean age = 27.7 years, SD = 4.70) consisted of participants who came from the basic training of police officers in the police academy of Stockholm. The school management asked two police classes (N ∼ 70), whether they would like to participate in the study. Out of these individuals, 30 police students chose to participate in the study. The POT-group was slightly but significantly younger in average than the test-group [t(73) = 4.365, p < 0.001].

Materials

In the present study, we used tests from two test batteries: The Delis–Kaplan EF System (D-KEFS) test battery (Delis et al.,2001a,b; Shunk et al., 2006) and The CogStateSports (CS) computerized concussion testing (re-brand as Axon Sport) (Collie et al., 2003; Straume-Naesheim et al., 2005). All tests included in the present study have been standardized to a normal population of different age spans and sex.

D-KEFS

Delis–Kaplan EF system measures different aspects of EF, and the subtests used in this study were DF, Color-Word Interference Test, and Trail Making Test mirroring our previous studies on EF in ball sports (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017, 2020). D-KEFS is used in clinical assessments of patients, and there are well-described norms for the general population (Delis et al.,2001a,b; Shunk et al., 2006).

Design Fluency measures online multi-processing, including creativity, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (Homack et al., 2005; Swanson, 2005). DF is a non-verbal psychomotor test where dots are combined in a square with four lines using a pen. In Condition 1, the subject has to find as many different combinations as possible of binding together filled dots under time pressure (60 s). The subject is not allowed to use the same solution twice. The subject has to remember previous responses in a working memory and update it with new rules (i.e., not repeat previous combinations). He/she must use response inhibition in order not to repeat responses. The participant also needs to use scanning to find new possible solutions. In Condition 2, unfilled dots have been added into the square, and the subject should combine them with lines as in Condition 1. The filled dots are still present, but the participant should not use them in the task. In Condition 3, both filled and unfilled dots are present in the square. The task is to connect lines as above but also to switch between the filled and unfilled dots. The test is demanding due to its requirement to use creativity and cognitive flexibility (Delis et al., 2001a). A combination score (DF Total Correct) of the three subtests of DF was used as previously (Vestberg et al., 2012), to represent HEF (Vestberg et al., 2017, 2020) and capture both “simple creativity and fluency” and “advanced creativity” (with a higher demand on both inhibition and cognitive flexibility) mirroring the variability of problem solutions needed in action (Delis et al., 2001a). We also explored the results from the different subtests (DF1, 2, and 3) as they differ in general demands on EF and cognitive flexibility. Especially, latent structure analysis has suggested that specifically DF3 involves a switching component suggesting a substantial cognitive flexibility aspect (Karr et al., 2018).

As previously (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2020), we used additional EF tests including Trail Making Test (TMT) (NIA and POT) and Color Word Interference (CWI) (results only for NIA) (Czerniak et al., 2015) as additional exploratory tests of general EFs, since they do not include the creativity and problem-solving components present in DF that was the main focus of this study.

CogStateSports

CogStateSports (CS) is a non-verbal psychomotor test battery measuring basic attention, cognitive process speed, decision-making, speed and accuracy of short-term memory, and encoding of working memory (Collie et al., 2003; Straume-Naesheim et al., 2005). The subjects are shown different play cards on a computer screen and have to react as fast and correct as possible using different key responses. In the first test (“Processing speed”), the subject has to respond to any card that is displayed measuring simple response time. In the second test (“Attention”), the subject has to respond whether the card is red or black. This test measures simple attention. In a third test (“Learning”), the subject has to respond if he or she has seen the displayed card any time earlier in the test sequences. The test measures of more demanding working memory and learning. In the fourth test (“Working memory”), the subject has to decide if the previous card is the same as the card before, a measure of simple working memory (i.e., one-back memory-test).

Procedure

The baseline testing (including all tests described above) of the NIA-group was performed in a standardized procedure in a quiet and secluded environment from May till June 2015. The POT-group was similarly assessed with the same tests from January till October 2016. A re-test of DF, TMT, and CWI was performed on the NIA-group in the extreme field assessment approximately 14 days later at different days for the different tests (Day 1: DF; Day 2: TMT; and Day 3: CWI). In the extreme field assessment, the applicants were pushed to their limits physically and mentally; meanwhile, the individuals were tested in their ability to handle the pressure, function in a group and to make adequate decisions. Importantly, all applicants went through the same stress tests at the same timepoints. The NIA-group and the POT-group had different test assessors.

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0.0. Shapiro–Wilk test was used to test distributions for normality. Levene’s test was used to test the homogeneity of variances between the groups.

Hypothesis Testing Focusing on DF

An ANCOVA was used to compare the results of the two groups (The NIA-group vs. the POT-group) after adjusting for age and sex. We further used a paired sample T-test to assess if subjects in the NIA-group performed differently at the re-test (during field assessment) as compared to baseline testing. We used Pearson’s correlation to test for the relation between baseline scores and re-test scores. For any test (DF Total correct, DF1, 2, or 3) that showed a difference between baseline testing and the re-test, we also tested for the relation between baseline scores and percent drop in test results during the re-test, adjusting for the cognitive support functions as Processing speed, Working memory accuracy, and Resting heart rate (as a proxy for physical fitness; Sandvik et al., 1993; Jensen et al., 2013). One-sample T-tests were used to compare the main cognitive test results (DF Total Correct as well as its sub-components DF1, DF2, and DF3) of the NIA-group and the POT-group with the D-KEFS norm.

Additional Exploratory Analyses Focusing on Other EF-Tests

Independent T-tests were used to compare the NIA-group with the POT-group concerning the additional exploratory tests presented above. We also performed an exploratory-paired sample T-test to examine whether the other D-KEFS tests were different at baseline vs. the re-test sessions.

Results

Our specific hypotheses pertained to the results from the DF total Correct and its subtests for NIA compared to the control group in the baseline assessment, as well as the results of NIA in baseline assessment compared to the field assessment. Levene’s test indicated equal variances assumed for the dependent variable across the groups. The model assumptions were met using Shapiro–Wilk test for normality of the residuals.

Comparison Between NIA and POT

DF Total Correct

We used a general linear model with the result of DF Total Correct as dependent variable and group, age, and sex as independent variables. We found that there was a large effect (Cohen’s d = 1.03) between groups, F(1,71) = 18.98, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.21, suggesting that NIA-group performs better than POT-group. Age and sex did not have a significant effect on the score of DF Total Correct [Age, F(1,71) = 3.17, p = 0.079, ηp2 = 0.043; Sex, F(1,71) = 0.37, p = 0.55, ηp2 = 0.005]. The mean values of the NIA-group and the POT-group are presented in Figure 1. Since few female subjects were included in the two groups (one in the NIA-group and six in the POT-group), we also performed the analyses with only male subjects. As expected, the results were similar (see Supplementary Material).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of all the DF subtests (DF1, DF2, DF3) and the DF total score (DF Total C) in NIA-group and the POT-group.

DF Subtests

We found that there was a significant effect of group for DF1 [F(1,71) = 11.06, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14; Cohen’s d = 0.79], DF2 [F(1,71) = 6.97, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.089; Cohen’s d = 0.63], and DF3 [F(1,71) = 22.66, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.24; Cohen’s d = 1.12], suggesting that NIA-group performs better than POT-group. Age and sex did not have a significant effect on the scores, except for DF3 where there also was a significant effect of Age [F(1,71) = 4.76, p < 0.032, ηp2 = 0.063].

Baseline Assessment Compared to the “Field” Assessment in NIA

DF Total Correct

A paired sample t-test indicated that DF Total Correct scores were not significantly different between baseline assessment (M = 14.98, SD = 2.44) and the “field” assessment (M = 15.30, SD = 2.27), t(39) = −0.86, p = 0.39. There was a significant correlation between the results of the baseline assessment and the results obtained during the “field” assessment, r(40) = 0.49, p = 0.001.

DF Subtests

DF1

A paired sample t-test indicated that DF1 scores were significantly different between baseline assessment (M = 13.95, SD = 2.84) and the re-test [M = 15.25, SD = 2.84; t(39) = −0.2.53, p = 0.016] suggesting an improvement of test results by approximately 9.3%. There was a significant correlation between the results of the baseline assessment and the re-test [r(40) = 0.35, p = 0.029].

DF2

A paired sample t-test indicated that DF2 scores were not significantly different between baseline assessment (M = 13.68, SD = 2.67) and the re-test [M = 13.98, SD = 1.78; t(39) = −0.8, p = 0.43]. There was a significant correlation between the results of the baseline assessment and the re-test [r(40) = 0.5, p = 0.001].

DF3

A paired samples t-test indicated that the scores from DF3 were significant different between baseline assessment (M = 14.2, SD = 2.38) compared with re-test [M = 12.83, SD = 2.23; t(39) = 3.79, p = 0.001] suggesting a worsening of test results by approximately 9.6%. There was also a significant correlation between the results of the baseline assessment and re-test [r(40) = 0.49, p = 0.001].

Relation Between Baseline and Re-test of DF3

In order to better understand the results above suggesting a worsening between DF3 results at baseline and at the re-test, we explored whether the subjects that had the best DF3 result at baseline also had the smallest reduction in performance during the field assessment (re-test). We therefore first correlate the baseline DF3 results for each individual with their baseline vs. re-test difference in percentage. We observed a significant negative correlation r(40) = −0.46, p = 0.003, i.e., higher DF3 results at baseline indicate a higher drop in the re-test result. This effect remained when we adjusted for Process speed, Working memory, and Resting heart rate as independent variables in an Ancova [F(1,35) = 9.16, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.21; Cohen’s d = 1.03]. The cognitive capacity including Process speed and Working memory as well as Resting heart rate (as a proxy for physical fitness; Sandvik et al., 1993; Jensen et al., 2013) did not have any significant effect on the result. Thus, the subjects with the highest scores on DF3 had the most dramatic fall of the results in the re-test performed during the extreme condition. However, subjects with higher results in baseline often still displayed high scores in the re-test assessment (Figures 2A,B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) The DF3 results from the baseline measurements and from the stress test for all NIA participants. (B) The difference in DF3 results between baseline and stress test in percent change as compared to baseline scores for all NIA participants.

Exploratory Tests

Results of the comparison with the norm, as well as the results for the other performed tests, are of exploratory nature in order to generate hypotheses for further studies (see Supplementary Material). In general, those results suggest that both groups were better than the norm in the performed tests (except for TMT4 and CS Processing speed for the POT-group). In all tests where the two tested groups were compared, the NIA-group outperformed the POT-group significantly. Finally, while NIA-group became significantly worse in CWI3 during re-test, there was a non-significant trend that indicated that they became better in CWI4 and TMT4 during the re-test assessment. Moreover, there was a positive correlation between baseline test results and re-test results from the extreme field assessment for TMT4, CWI3, and CWI4 in the NIA-group.

Discussion

The present study on elite police applicants forwards previous results on cognition of elite players in ball sports (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017, 2020; Verburgh et al., 2014, 2016; Huijgen et al., 2015; Lundgren et al., 2016; Alarcón et al., 2017; Ishihara et al., 2018; Sakamoto et al., 2018) and suggests that EFs, and especially HEFs, also are decisive in other professions involving rapid flexibility and creative decision-making. In line with our initial hypothesis, the police officers that have passed all the tests until the last stage of the draft for the Swedish counterterror intervention unit, i.e., the NIA-group, were significantly better in the DF test than control group (POT-group). The results suggest that HEF may be of vital importance for becoming an elite police officer. Our additional exploratory analysis (Supplementary Material) also indicated that both police groups were somewhat better than the norm in test mirroring the CEF capacity but with a smaller effect size compared with DF and the HEF results. Although both groups were better than the norm in general attention tests from CogSport as well as TMT from D-KEFS, the NIA-group was significantly better than the POT-group in those tests (except for Learning). Altogether, the present results were similar to our previous study of adult elite and semi-elite soccer players (Vestberg et al., 2012) as well as when we studied two levels of play in elite players (Vestberg et al., 2020) in that the higher level players were better than the lower level players on DF, but both groups showed better results than the norm. The overall picture suggests that HEF is related to police officers able to past the steps in the selection to NI. One interpretation of this is that demanding EF and fast alternating and multifaceted creativity plays a vital role in the recruitment of the Swedish counterterror intervention unit (NI).

Comparing baseline DF results for the NIA-group with the results on the same test in a re-test “field” assessment when the subjects were under substantial physical and psychological stress suggests a decline in cognitive function. The technical manual of D-KEFS (Delis et al., 2001b) indicates that an expected increase of the results between the baseline and the re-test should be approximately 15% due to loss of the novelty factor. Here, the results for DF Total Correct only showed an average increase of 2%. When specifically analyzing the subtests, a corresponding increase was observed in a DF1, but the results for DF3 showed an average 9.6% decrease of the result. This suggests that physical and psychological pressures most severely affect cognitive flexibility, which is the component that is most evident in DF3 as compared to the other subtests of DF (Karr et al., 2018). Alternatively, the observed results may be due to the fact that DF3 requires higher general focus than the other subtests.

Interestingly, the results show, contrary to our hypothesis, that the subjects with the highest baseline performance lost more of the capacity, in percent, than the subjects with low or moderate baseline performances. However, our result also suggests a positive correlation between baseline and the re-test assessment, and several of the individuals with the highest performance in the baseline test still had the some of the best results in the field test. Our interpretation of this result is that high baseline capacity of EF may also predict high capacity when a subject is under stress, although some high-performers have more difficulty reaching their maximum capacity when they are under heavy pressure. This is in line with the idea of an inverted U-cure of prefrontal top-down cognitive capacity (Cools and D’Esposito, 2011). Lower cognitive functions like process speed and working memory, as well as resting heart rate (as a proxy for physical fitness), did not have any effect on this result. The finding suggests that EF should be tested both during baseline conditions and during stressful conditions in order to fully describe an individuals’ cognitive capacity.

The relation between the present results and previous research on elite players of ball sport is striking (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017, 2020; Verburgh et al., 2014, 2016; Huijgen et al., 2015; Lundgren et al., 2016; Alarcón et al., 2017; Ishihara et al., 2018; Sakamoto et al., 2018). Successful ballplayers must quickly adapt, change strategy, and inhibit responses—even after extreme tiredness and stress. Many of these abilities are referred to as “game intelligence” in sports (Stratton et al., 2004). We have argued that such abilities would essentially equal EF in a neuropsychological framework (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017). The fast and creative problem-solving together with dynamic adaption due to quick changes on the soccer field suggests that HEF are especially important. Altogether, it seems that some of the behaviors required reaching an elite level in ball sports overlap with the requirements of elite police officers.

The importance of EF in police tasks has previously been suggested. For example, it has been shown that the capacity of the working memory correlates with successful shooting behavior among polices officers (Kleider et al., 2010). In a computerized test situation, officers with low working memory capacity shot more unarmed targets and fewer armed targets than officers with high capacity of the working memory. Controlling for working memory speed did not influence the result. This result indicates that working memory high capacity in form of accuracy is essential for successful behavior in armed police shooting interventions. Interestingly, the Swedish Police force seldom uses tests that objectively measure CEF or HEF, including creative and flexible problem-solving in action. Still, individuals with stronger HEF seem to do better in the required procedure or, alternatively, are more interested in applying for the position.

Working memory and other EF may be easily affected by stress and negative emotions (Taverniers et al., 2010), and a consequence of a declined working memory capacity seems to increase errors (Kleider et al., 2010; Taverniers et al., 2010; Gutshall et al., 2017). Likewise, it has been suggested that lack of sleep (Yoo et al., 2007; Killgore, 2010; Lim and Dinges, 2010; Ma et al., 2015; Krause et al., 2017; Floros et al., 2020) and mental tiredness (Blain et al., 2016) may also have a negative effect on EF. All of these potential negative effects on EF may be relevant for elite police force officers. Possibly, a higher baseline EF capacity may form a buffer that could prevent a critical functional deficit in an extreme situation (Floros et al., 2020).

In the present study, we did not measure IQ as we were specifically interested in the capacity to dynamical adapt in a quickly changing environment, a behavior not captured by general intelligence measurements. It has been suggested that HEF and fluid intelligence are synonymous (Diamond, 2013). However, while there is some correlation between EF-tests and tests of fluid intelligence (Conway et al., 2002; Egger et al., 2011; van Aken et al., 2016) especially when it comes to updating and working memory (Friedman et al., 2006; Friedman and Miyake, 2017; Krumm et al., 2018), the relationship is not complete (Conway et al., 2002; Egger et al., 2011; van Aken et al., 2016). The relation between EF and crystalized intelligence is substantially lower (Conway et al., 2002; Egger et al., 2011; van Aken et al., 2016). In line with this, we have previously shown that while EF-test correlated with measurement of successful behaviors, Raven’s matrices (Roca et al., 2010), a test that often is used as a proxy for fluid intelligence, did not (Vestberg et al., 2017). IQ and/or general mental ability (g) are like the concept of fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence only to some aspects related to EF (Ardila et al., 2000; Friedman et al., 2006; Friedman et al., 2008; Wingo et al., 2013; Ardila, 2018). Nonetheless, although there is a separation between EF and IQ, IQ probably represents a unique component in successful behavior of elite police officers that need to be investigated.

Apart from EF and IQ, other cognitive functions such as emotional regulation could be important for elite police forces acting in quickly changing and potentially harmful environments. Although emotional regulation may be partially separated from EF, it represents similar top-down regulatory mechanisms, and the capacities of EF and emotional regulation are highly correlated (Petrovic and Castellanos, 2016). Therefore, measuring EF capacity may serve as a proxy for emotional regulation capacity of an individual. Moreover, social cognition abilities, spatial cognition, perceptual capacities, and grit may also be of importance for a successful function of elite police officers. Similarly, the predictive validity of personality traits should be investigated. Tedeholm et al. (2021) showed that individuals from Swedish counterterror intervention unit had lower results on the neuroticism scale and higher results on the conscientiousness and the extraversion scale than the norm in Swedish population (Tedeholm et al., 2021). However, in contrast to measurements of cognitive capacity, personality tests are subjective and their relation to behavior is still not fully evaluated.

A better understanding of the role of EF and how they shape successful behaviors in a constantly changing environment may strengthen future recruitment of elite police officers to become more effective in finding the right individuals suitable for the position. The role of the EF as a buffer to prevent critical functional deficits in cognitively stressful and changing environments needs to be studied further.

Limitations

The subjects in the NIA-group were in average older than the subjects in the POT-group, possibly suggesting that life experience and more experience as a police officer could contribute to the results. However, the ANCOVA showed that age did not have any major effect on the results. Moreover, few female subjects were part of the tested groups and the groups also differed in number of female subjects (only one female subject was part of the tested NIA-group and six were part of the POT-group). This may have an effect on the results. However, we both controlled for sex in our main model when we compared groups and performed a supplementary analysis with male subjects only that did not change the main conclusions (presented in Supplementary Material). The comparison results between the NIA-group and the POT-group are not adjusted for the participants’ physical advantage. So we cannot exclude that it could affect the results. However, in the exploratory part of the study, we adjust for resting heart rate as a proxy for physical fitness. This adjustment did not have any significant impact on the EF results. We have not controlled for IQ, a measurement that, in general, is used in recruitment for police officers. Due to this, we cannot rule out that IQ may impact on the results. However, because of the weak relation between IQ and EF (Ardila et al., 2000; Conway et al., 2002; Friedman et al., 2006, 2008; Roca et al., 2010; Egger et al., 2011; Wingo et al., 2013; van Aken et al., 2016; Friedman and Miyake, 2017; Vestberg et al., 2017; Ardila, 2018; Krumm et al., 2018), this would be unlikely. The impact of other cognitive factors and personality on successful elite police behavior was not measured in the present study and should be assessed in the future. In the study, we used two assessors, one for each group. This could potentially confound the results comparing the NIV-group with the POT-group. Even if the assessors are trained to give the same instructions and use the tests in the same way, there may be differences between them. Notably, the NIA-group had better mean results than the POT-group in all performed tests, including the CogSport tests that are computer-based and need no verbal instructions, guidance, or assessment by a supervisor suggesting small assessor confound.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because some data contain classified information. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to PP, predrag.petrovic@ki.se.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Regionala etikprövningsnämnden i Stockholm; Dnr 2015/528-31/4. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TV and PT tested subjects. TV, PT, and PP analyzed the data. All authors wrote the manuscript and were involved in designing and planning the study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The present study was funded by Karolinska Institutet och PRIMA.

Conflict of Interest

TV and PP have been involved as consultants in cognitive testing outside research. PP has stocks in company that tests cognition.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580463/full#supplementary-material

References

Admission requirements for police training (2020). Webpage of the Swedish Police (in Swedish). Available online at: https://polisen.se/om-polisen/bli-polis/ansoka-till-polisutbildningen/antagningskrav-till-polisutbildningen/ (accessed March 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Alarcón, F., Ureña, N., Castillo Díaz, A., Martín, D., and Vélez, D. (2017). Executive functions predict expertise in basketball players. J. Sport Psychol. 5, 71–74.

Google Scholar

Alves, H., Voss, M., Boot, W., Deslandes, A., Cossich, V., Inacio Salles, J., et al. (2013). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in elite volleyball players. Front. Psychol. 4:36. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, V. A., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., and Catroppa, C. (2001a). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 20, 385–406. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, V. A., Northam, E., and Wrennall, J. (2001b). Developmental Neuropsychology: A Clinical Approach. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Ardila, A. (2018). Is intelligence equivalent to executive functions?(texto en ingles). Psicothema 30:159. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2017.329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A., Pineda, D., and Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00159-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayard, F., Nymberg Thunell, C., Abé, C., Almeida, R., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G., et al. (2018). Distinct brain structure and behavior related to ADHD and conduct disorder traits. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 3020–3033. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0202-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blain, B., Hollard, G., and Pessiglione, M. (2016). Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of daylong cognitive work on economic decisions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 6967–6972. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1520527113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, R. C. K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., and Chen, E. Y. H. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieslik, E. C., Mueller, V. I., Eickhoff, C. R., Langner, R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 48, 22–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collie, A., Maruff, P., Makdissi, M., McCrory, P., McStephen, M., and Darby, D. (2003). CogSport: reliability and correlation with conventional cognitive tests used in postconcussion medical evaluations. Clin. J. Sport Med. 13, 28–32. doi: 10.1097/00042752-200301000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., and Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30, 163–183. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cools, R., and D’Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U–shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crosbie, J., Arnold, P., Paterson, A., Swanson, J., Dupuis, A., Li, X., et al. (2013). Response inhibition and ADHD traits: correlates and heritability in a community sample. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 497–507. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9693-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czerniak, S. M., Sikoglu, E. M., Navarro, A. A. L., McCafferty, J., Eisenstock, J., Stevenson, J. H., et al. (2015). A resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging study of concussion in collegiate athletes. Brain Imaging Behav. 9, 323–332. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9312-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., and Easteal, S. A. (2012). Population-based study of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and associated impairment in middle-aged adults (ADHD symptoms in middle-aged adults). PLoS One 7:e31500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001a). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Examiner’s Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001b). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1–132. doi: 10.1037/t15082-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, J. I. M., Van Aken, L., Kessels, R. P. C., Wingbermühle, E., Van Der Veld, W., and Verhoeven, W. M. A. (2011). P03-46 - Fluid intelligence and executive functioning: partial overlap in patients with psychiatric disorders. Eur. Psychiatry 26, 1215–1256. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(11)72920-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faubert, J. (2013). Professional athletes have extraordinary skills for rapidly learning complex and neutral dynamic visual scenes. Sci. Rep. 3:1154. doi: 10.1038/srep01154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, M. (2011). On Flexibility, Recovery from Technological and Doctrinal Surprise on the Battlefield. Standford, CA: Stanford Security Studies. doi: 10.1515/9780804777155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floros, O., Axelsson, J., Almeida, R., Tigerström, L., Lekander, M., Sundelin, T., et al. (2020). Domain specific alterations in cognitive conflicts and adjustments after sleep deprivation related to subclinical ADHD-symptoms. Biol. Psychoiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimag. 29:S141. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.11.255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 86, 186–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., and Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 17, 172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., and Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 137, 201–225. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, S., and Naglieri, J. A. (2014). Handbook of Executive Functioning. Cham: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutshall, C. L., Hampton, D. P., Sebetan, I. M., Stein, P. C., and Broxtermann, T. J. (2017). The effects of occupational stress on cognitive performance in police officers. Police Pract. Res. 18, 463–477. doi: 10.1080/15614263.2017.1288120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halsius, P. (2007). Nationella Insatsstyrkan, Nutid och Framtid. Umeå: Polisutbildningen vid Umeå Universitet.

Google Scholar

Homack, S., Lee, D., and Riccio, C. A. (2005). Test review: delis-kaplan executive function system. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 27, 599–609. doi: 10.1080/13803390490918444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huijgen, B. C. H., Leemhuis, S., Kok, N. M., Verburgh, L., Oosterlaan, J., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., et al. (2015). Cognitive functions in elite and sub-elite youth soccer players aged 13 to 17 Years. PLoS One 10:e0144580. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishihara, T., Kuroda, Y., and Mizuno, M. (2018). Competitive achievement may be predicted by executive functions in junior tennis players: an 18-month follow-up study. J. Sports Sci. 37, 755–761. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2018.1524738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, M. T., Suadicani, P., Hein, H. O., and Gyntelberg, F. (2013). Elevated resting heart rate, physical fitness and all-cause mortality: a 16-year follow-up in the Copenhagen Male Study.(Report). Heart 99:882. doi: 10.1136/heartjnl-2012-303375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karr, J. E., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G. L., and Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). Examining the latent structure of the delis–kaplan executive function system. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 381–394. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acy043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D. S. (2010). “Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition,” in Progress in Brain Research, eds G. A. Kerkhof and H. P. A. V. Dongen (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 105–129. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleider, H. M., Parrott, D. J., and King, T. Z. (2010). Shooting behaviour: how working memory and negative emotionality influence police officer shoot decisions. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 707–717. doi: 10.1002/acp.1580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koechlin, E. (2016). Prefrontal executive function and adaptive behavior in complex environments. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 37, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, A. J., Simon, E. B., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., et al. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18:404. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krenn, B., Finkenzeller, T., Würth, S., and Amesberger, G. (2018). Sport type determines differences in executive functions in elite athletes. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 38, 72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumm, G., Arán Filippetti, V., and Gutierrez, M. (2018). The contribution of executive functions to creativity in children: what is the role of crystallized and fluid intelligence? Think. Skills Creat. 29, 185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2018.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, K.-F., Meng, F.-W., and Chen, Y.-L. (2017). The relationship between action inhibition and athletic performance in elite badminton players and non-athletes. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 12, 574–581. doi: 10.14198/jhse.2017.123.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. A. (2010). Meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol. Bull. 136, 375–389. doi: 10.1037/a0018883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., and Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Dev. 76, 697–712. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00872.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundgren, T., Högman, L., Näslund, M., and Parling, T. (2016). Preliminary investigation of executive functions in elite ice hockey players. J. Clin. Sport Psychol. 10, 324–335. doi: 10.1123/jcsp.2015-0030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher Cortical Functions in Man. New York. NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-8579-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, N., Dinges, D. F., Basner, M., and Rao, H. (2015). How acute total sleep loss affects the attending brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sleep 38, 233–240. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansouri, F. A., Tanaka, K., and Buckley, M. J. (2009). Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10:141. doi: 10.1038/nrn2538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, C. A., Russell, B., McNeil, J., Maxwell, J., Snyder, P. J., Southwick, S. M., et al. (2011). Baseline burnout symptoms predict visuospatial executive function during survival school training in special operations military personnel. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 494–501. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrovic, P., and Castellanos, F. X. (2016). Top-down dysregulation—from ADHD to emotional instability. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:70. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roca, M., Parr, A., Thompson, R., Woolgar, A., Torralva, T., Antoun, N., et al. (2010). Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain 133, 234–247. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, S., Takeuchi, H., Ihara, N., Ligao, B., and Suzukawa, K. (2018). Possible requirement of executive functions for high performance in soccer. PLoS One 13:e0201871. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandvik, L., Erikssen, J., Thaulow, E., Erikssen, G., Mundal, R., and Rodahl, K. (1993). Physical fitness as a predictor of mortality among healthy, middle-aged norwegian men. New Engl. J. Med. 328, 533–537. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199302253280803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfen, H. E., and Memmert, D. (2019). Measurement of cognitive functions in experts and elite athletes: a meta-analytic review. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 843–860. doi: 10.1002/acp.3526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacher, N., Schmidt, M., Wellmann, K., and Braumann, K.-M. (2018). General perceptual-cognitive abilities: age and position in soccer. PLoS One 13:e0202627. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shunk, A. W., Davis, A. S., and Dean, R. S. (2006). TEST review: Dean C. Delis, Edith Kaplan & Joel H. Kramer, Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, 2001. $415.00 (complete kit). Appl. Neuropsychol. 13, 275–227. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1304_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stratton, G., Reilly, T., Richardson, D., and Williams, A. M. (2004). Youth Soccer: From Science to Performance. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/978020364413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straume-Naesheim, T. M., Andersen, T. E., and Bahr, R. (2005). Reproducibility of computer based neuropsychological testing among Norwegian elite football players. Br. J. Sports Med. 39(Suppl. 1), i64–i69. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2005.019620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T. (2011). Traumatic brain injury: relation to executive dysfunction and the frontal lobes. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 24, 584–589. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834c7eb9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, J. (2005). The delis-kaplan executive function system:a review. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 20, 117–128. doi: 10.1177/0829573506295469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit (2020). Webpage of the Swedish Police (in Swedish). Available online at: https://polisen.se/om-polisen/polisens-arbete/nationella-insatsstyrkan/ (accessed March 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Tanji, J., and Hoshi, E. (2008). Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. Physiol. Rev. 88, 37–57. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taverniers, J., Van Ruysseveldt, J., Smeets, T., and von Grumbkow, J. (2010). High-intensity stress elicits robust cortisol increases, and impairs working memory and visuo-spatial declarative memory in Special Forces candidates: a field experiment. Stress 13, 324–334. doi: 10.3109/10253891003642394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedeholm, P. G., Sjöberg, A., and Larsson, A. C. (2021). Personality traits among Swedish counterterrorism intervention unit police officers: a comparison with the general population. Pers. individ. Diff. 168:e110411. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Aken, L., Kessels, R. P. C., Wingbermühle, E., van Der Veld, W. M., and Egger, J. I. M. (2016). Fluid intelligence and executive functioning more alike than different? Acta Neuropsychiatr. 28:31. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verburgh, L., Scherder, E. J. A., van Lange, P. A. M., and Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS One 9:e91254. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verburgh, L., Scherder, E. J. A., Van Lange, P. A. M., and Oosterlaan, J. (2016). Do elite and amateur soccer players outperform non-athletes on neurocognitive functioning? A study among 8-12 year Old children. PLoS One 11:e0165741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T., Gustafson, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2012). Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS One 7:e34731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T., Reinebo, G., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2017). Core executive functions are associated with success in young elite soccer players. PLoS One 12:e0170845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T. J. R., Almeida, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2020). Level of play and coach-rated game intelligence are related to performance on design fluency in elite soccer players. Sci. Rep. 10:9852. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66180-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, B., Guo, W., and Zhou, C. (2016). Selective enhancement of attentional networks in college table tennis athletes: a preliminary investigation. PeerJ 4:e2762. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C.-H., Chang, C.-C., Liang, Y.-M., Shih, C.-M., Chiu, W.-S., Tseng, P., et al. (2013). Open vs. closed skill sports and the modulation of inhibitory control. PLoS One 8:e55773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werkelius, C. (1997). Nationella Insatsstyrkan, en Svensk Antiterroriststyrka. Stockholm: Stockholms universitet.

Google Scholar

Wingo, J., Kalkut, E., Tuminello, E., Asconape, J., and Han, S. D. (2013). Executive functions, depressive symptoms, and college adjustment in women. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 20, 136–144. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2012.670154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Work at Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit (2020). Available at: https://polisen.se/om-polisen/jobba-hos-polisen/var-verksamhet/jobb-insatsstyrkan/ (accessed March 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Wylie, S. A., Bashore, T. R., Van Wouwe, N. C., Mason, E. J., John, K. D., Neimat, J. S., et al. (2018). Exposing an “Intangible” cognitive skill among collegiate football players: enhanced interference control. Front. Psychol. 9:49. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S.-S., Gujar, N., Hu, P., Jolesz, F. A., and Walker, M. P. (2007). The human emotional brain without sleep &#x2014; a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr. Biol. 17, R877–R878. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive functions, police, counterterror intervention unit applicants, cognitive flexibility, Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System, Design Fluency Test, special forces

Citation: Vestberg T, Tedeholm PG, Ingvar M, Larsson AC and Petrovic P (2021) Executive Functions of Swedish Counterterror Intervention Unit Applicants and Police Officer Trainees Evaluated With Design Fluency Test. Front. Psychol. 12:580463. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580463

Received: 07 July 2020; Accepted: 19 February 2021;
Published: 11 May 2021.

Edited by:

Colleen M. Berryessa, Rutgers University, Newark, United States

Reviewed by:

Anna Maria Dåderman, University West, Sweden
Samuel Adjorlolo, University of Ghana, Ghana

Copyright © 2021 Vestberg, Tedeholm, Ingvar, Larsson and Petrovic. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Predrag Petrovic, predrag.petrovic@ki.se

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.