Skip to main content

CONCEPTUAL ANALYSIS article

Front. Psychol., 12 September 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars In: Environmental Psychology View all 10 articles

How schools can aid children’s resilience in disaster settings: The contribution of place attachment, sense of place and social representations theories

  • 1Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, UCL Division of Psychology and Language Sciences & EPICentre, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Tsunami and Disaster Mitigation Research Centre, Department of Family Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Universitas Syiah Kuala, Banda Aceh, Indonesia

Disasters incurred by natural hazards affect young people most. Schools play a vital role in safeguarding the wellbeing of their pupils. Consideration of schools’ psychosocial influence on children may be vital to resilience-building efforts in disaster-vulnerable settings. This paper presents an evidence-based conceptualization of how schools are psychosocially meaningful for children and youth in disaster settings. Drawing on Social Representations and Place Attachment Theories, we explore the nature of group-based meaning-making practices and the meanings that emerge concerning school environments in disaster settings. We contribute a novel understanding of how schools may mitigate psychosocial risk for young people by considering how schools are conceptualised at four levels: (1) as physical environment, (2) as social arena, (3) as a place with individual and (4) group-based significance. In each of these domains schools can foster disaster resilience in young people. This paper highlights the evidence concerning the functions of schools beyond their capacity as educational institutions, critically considering their social and physical functions in their communities. This evidence can inform stakeholders involved in disaster resilience building.

Introduction

Children are uniquely vulnerable to the negative consequences of disasters, in part due to their dependence on adults and their ongoing development (Peek, 2008). Schools are essential sites for safeguarding children and youth in disaster settings (Mutch, 2014; see also, UNISDR, 2014; ACFCSS, 2016; Paci-Green et al., 2020). Though the significance of their role as educational institutions and resource distribution centers in disaster contexts is well-established (e.g., Sakurai et al., 2018; Mirzaei et al., 2019), consideration of schools’ psychosocial influence may be vital to disaster risk management and resilience-building (IASC, 2006; Pacheco et al., 2021). Many works explore the socio-physical dynamics of spaces such as cities or the home (e.g., Bechtel, 2010; Clayton, 2012; Fleury-Bahi et al., 2017; Sawyer et al., 2022), yet to our knowledge, the literature lacks academic conceptualization of the socio-physical elements of schools. This conceptual analysis explores the evidence regarding how children conceptualize schools in disaster settings; it examines how schools may mitigate psychosocial risk for children by considering the function of these conceptualizations from a social psychological perspective. We conclude with a series of recommendations for utilizing schools as hubs for enhancing resilience in disaster contexts.

Before we begin our conceptual analysis, we briefly clarify definitions of key concepts used throughout this paper. Since the United Nations International Strategy for Disaster Reduction adopted the Hyogo Framework for Action (HFA) 2005–2015: ‘Building the Resilience of Nations and Communities’, disaster policy, planning and research has focused on the capacity of the community to bounce back from adversity. Yet despite the rise in popularity of the resilience rhetoric, there has been a lack of interdisciplinary consensus on how the concept is defined (Mayunga, 2007; Bonanno et al., 2015). We view it as the ‘adaptive capacity’ that supports individuals and the community to cope with and recover from adversity (e.g., Berkes, 2007; Paton, 2007; Norris et al., 2008; Masten, 2011; Ungar, 2011). The adversity we refer to is that experienced in settings affected by disasters. While disasters vary in the scale of disruption and loss of life caused, a disaster setting is an area that has experienced widespread human, material, economic and/or environmental loss as a result of the interaction of a hazard (e.g., an earthquake, tsunami, flooding) with social vulnerability (Massazza et al., 2019). Resilience building is facilitated through a system of processes that buffer the impact of such disaster scenarios and improve circumstances in both the short-term response and long-term planning (Pacheco et al., 2021).

Background and framework

This section explores how spaces can protect the psychosocial resilience of individuals and communities; we approach this through lenses of social representations theory (SRT), sense of place and place attachment theory. Subsequent sections apply this knowledge to understand the significance of schools (as physical and social environments) for children and youth in disaster settings. Within this paper, ‘schools’ refers to primary and secondary, but not tertiary, education.

Social representations theory

Social representations are the product of group-based meaning-making practices whereby groups socially construct common knowledge on topics of social relevance (Moscovici, 1961/1976, 1984; Clémence, 2001). Abstract concepts are made concrete by their transformation into elements that are easier for people to engage with and discuss, such as integrating the concept into images or examples with relevance to everyday life (Clémence, 2001). The objectified concepts become fully integrated into contemporary meaning systems when connected to pre-existing meaning systems (Joffe, 2003). Such representations exist not only in belief and discourse but influence, and are inseparable from, social behavior (Sammut and Howarth, 2014; Wagner, 2015). It is therefore useful to draw on SRT to explore the symbolic meaning with which schools are infused in disaster settings.

People can differ in how they represent an entity. When people represent important social issues, their pre-existing cognitive-emotional frameworks are imposed upon the newer ideas; groups within the wider public draw on diverse information sources to understand a phenomenon (Abreu Lopes and Gaskell, 2015). The pre-existing frameworks are shaped by the complex social worlds within which the people exist (e.g., religious, cultural, ethnic, political, socioeconomic and ideological; Clémence, 2001; Staerklé et al., 2011; Wagner, 2012) so that contemporary societies experience a plurality of representations of the same object (Abreu Lopes and Gaskell, 2015). Such frameworks influence belief structures, life experience and knowledge acquisition (Staerklé et al., 2011); they become more salient when a threat is encountered (Joffe, 2003; Jaspal et al., 2022).

Although SRT has been applied to understanding risk (Joffe, 2003; Lemée et al., 2019) and representations of the home (Harries, 2013) in disaster settings, it has yet to be applied to understanding the content or implications of conceptualizations concerning schools in disaster settings.

Sense of place

The literature provides a plethora of definitions and concepts for characterizing the complex processes whereby humans develop connections to places, such as place attachment (Hidalgo and Hernández, 2001; Altman and Low, 2012; Scannell and Gifford, 2016, 2017; Manzo and Devine-Wright, 2020), sense of community (McMillan, 1996; Obst et al., 2002), sense of place (Jorgensen and Stedman, 2001; Pretty et al., 2003; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015), place identity (Twigger-Ross and Uzzell, 1996; Devine-Wright, 2009; Foroudi et al., 2020), rootedness (Relph, 1976; Tuan, 1980), belonging (Inalhan and Finch, 2004; Rogaly and Taylor, 2016; Di Masso et al., 2017), place-making (Trudeau, 2006; Friedmann, 2010; Pierce et al., 2011; Ujang, 2012) and making sense of place (Matthews, 1992; Relph, 2009; Powell, 2010; Convery et al., 2014). Each of these terms appears across disciplines interested in place-related research (e.g., urban studies, psychology, human geography, sociology), and has been operationalized across the research literature within concrete variables (e.g., quantitatively captured in years lived in an area) as well as abstract variables (e.g., qualitatively captured in how one understands one’s experience of or in a place; see Lewicka, 2010; Williams, 2014; Greer et al., 2020). While many authors use these terms interchangeably, a rich literature is dedicated to untangling each of these concepts (e.g., Hashemnezhad et al., 2013; Collins-Kreiner, 2020). Lewicka (2010), who provides an extensive review of several hundred empirical and theoretical works, argues that the literature should turn away from pursuit of defining these terms within rigid parameters and, instead, work toward extending theory and conceptualizations of person-place attachments in under-researched populations and settings. We respond to this challenge: there is little previous thought concerning the meaning ascribed to schools within communities, and none, to our knowledge, concerning this topic in disaster settings.

Schools’ meaning in disaster settings can be informed by understanding how individuals and groups develop psychological ties to places. All environments are social and physical (socio-physical), allowing person-place interactions to be bidirectional on a series of interacting levels: The thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of a person influence the elements of a place, and the elements of a place influence the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of the individual experiencing that place (Sörqvist, 2016). Places contain three components: location (i.e., absolute and relative space), locale (i.e., material features that exist in that space), and sense of place (i.e., affective interactions with elements of that space; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). Location and locale refer to the external elements (built and natural) of a space. Sense of place refers to the affective psychological orientation – memories and experiences - that individuals or groups have in relation to a spatial setting. These contribute to the location and locale becoming a meaningful place (Jorgensen and Stedman, 2001; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). Jorgensen and Stedman (2006) further describe sense of place as a general complex psychosocial structure that organizes beliefs, emotions, and behaviors. Since sense of place acknowledges that the meaning people ascribe to a place is a dynamic, multidimensional product of subjective processes, disaster studies tend to favor sense of place as a lens to explore the psychosocial ways in which communities have been affected by natural hazards (e.g., Chamlee-Wright and Storr, 2009; Smith and Cartlidge, 2011; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015; Bonaiuto et al., 2016). Thus, sense of place research strongly informs and guides this work in conceptualizing schools’ symbolic and affective meaning for communities in disaster settings.

Place attachment

Most scholars consistently uphold that sense of place is a product of place attachment (e.g., Shamsuddin and Ujang, 2008; Kudryavtsev et al., 2012). Even though the literature lacks a clear, single definition of place attachment, it is most consistently described as the subjective bonds people develop with particular places they hold important or meaningful (Hidalgo and Hernández, 2001; Greer et al., 2020). Place attachment has commonality with attachment to a person (Fullilove, 1996), encompassing the emotional and cognitive experience linking people to places (Bonaiuto et al., 2016) and captures the meaning individuals make of their environments and how they interact with those environments (Lewicka, 2005; Greer et al., 2020, p. 307–308).

Scannell and Gifford (2010) synthesize the literature concerning the various elements that contribute to place attachment in a well-cited and evidence-based tripartite model that envisions place attachment as a product of three dynamic elements. The person element concerns the actor who is attached; place concerns the object of attachment, including concrete and abstract elements of a place to which one is attached; psychological process refers to (how the attachment manifests; psychological elements of attachment; see also, Lewicka, 2011; Counted, 2016; Manzo and Devine-Wright, 2020). To contextualize this framework for disaster settings, Jamali and Nejat (2016) provide a broad conceptual map of demographic, socioeconomic, spatial and psychological factors that act as ‘parameters of place attachment’. However, they do not delve into the psychological factors. In a review of the literature on community disaster risk reduction, recovery, and resilience as well as place attachment and young people’s experiences of place in disaster settings, Scannell et al. (2016) argue that place attachment is important for young people’s experiences at each stage of the disaster (e.g., preparedness, experience, recovery and resilience). However, they find that while themes relevant to place attachment are often mentioned in empirical findings in disaster social science research, they are rarely discussed in the context of specific place attachment theory, especially in research on children and youth. A social psychological lens has been notably missing from the place attachment and disaster resilience discourses (Lewicka, 2011; Pacheco et al., 2021). This paper endeavors to address this gap as a social psychological lens can help us to conceptualize how places (i.e., schools) can act as icons of recovery for youth (Cox et al., 2017), such as by fostering emotional regulation and positive affect.

Framework and method

This paper seeks to explore how pupils represent schools in disaster settings and the implications of these representations for resilience building. Existing evidence is assimilated and combined to support arguments using a theory synthesis (Jaakkola, 2020). This is a conceptual integration across multiple literature streams. It offers an enhanced view of a phenomenon by linking previously unconnected pieces in a novel way.

This paper seeks to synthesize the sense of place, place meaning and place attachment literatures insofar as they characterize the person-place bond (see Figure 1). This bond contains psychosocial, affective and bidirectional (see also, Lewicka, 2011) facets. Doing so provides an academic foundation for understanding how schools are ascribed meaning by people in disaster contexts (sense of place) and the socio-physical elements of schools that may influence these representations (place meaning and place attachment). We also consider the nature of individual and group-based (i.e., community) ties to schools in disaster settings and explore identity dynamics that contribute to these environments being represented as personally and symbolically significant.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual framework for understanding the functional resilience building elements of schools. We consider how schools may mitigate psychosocial risk for young people in disaster settings across four elements; these elements are conceptualised based on a unified framework (lens) between place attachment, sense of place and social representations theories.

Schools as meaningful places in disaster settings

Young people’s representations of schools are characterized by cognitive-affective meaning. Beyond their capacity as educational institutions, schools provide their communities with necessary, supportive resources during disaster response and recovery phases. They are often repurposed as shelters or evacuation centers (Mutch, 2015). Beyond these practical functions, schools are also important social environments to which people develop physical, moral, social, emotional, spiritual, aesthetic, and academic attachment (Noddings, 2005; Rich and Schachter, 2012). Such attachments are especially salient for young people and may endure over the life course. For example, young people often represent schools as protective spaces (Sinkkonen, 2012). Research suggests young adults continue to retain these representations; Scannell and Gifford (2017) found that spontaneously visualizing familiar places, including schools, enhanced undergraduates’ sense of belonging, self-esteem, and meaningfulness. These findings suggest that schools have enduring socio-physical qualities and demonstrate that the person-place bond between pupils and schools may provide psychological benefits.

We structure our conceptualization of the place meaning of schools for children and youth in disaster settings by considering the meaning of schools as (1) physical built environments, (2) social arenas, (3) places with personal, and (4) group-based significance. We draw on the Scannell and Gifford (2010) model of place attachment as it provides a systematic pathway through the core elements that interact in fostering a place bond.

Schools as physical places and built-environments

The concept of place, most commonly defined as space endowed with meaning (Relph, 1976; Tuan, 1977; Low and Altman, 1992), is the object of attachment within place-attachment theory (Scannell and Gifford, 2010). It is well-acknowledged that the physical attribute of ‘place’ has been under researched compared to the over-emphasis on the social dimension of place attachment (Stedman, 2003; Droseltis and Vignoles, 2010; Lewicka, 2011; Sebastien, 2020). This absence is particularly notable concerning young people in the disaster literature (Cox et al., 2017). In child samples much of the recent research concerning the meaning of physical places considers the importance of green spaces e.g., (see Little and Derr, 2020 for a review) and the promotion of pro-environmental behaviour through place attachment (Cole et al., 2021). In this body of research, the scale of meaningful built environments in a child’s life vary, from the small-scale (e.g., a bedroom) to the large scale (e.g., a city; Little and Derr, 2020). For children and youth, the physical aspects of such environments may be more salient than for adults. Morgan (2010) explains that adults’ attachment is driven by their feelings of a place and the meaning attributed to those feelings. In contrast, children understand places based on what one can do in the place (e.g., play, self-directed exploration) with little regard for the purpose of the place or the social meanings. As the environment is a passive element in relation to the activity, the bonds fostered in children are initially unconscious but become conscious as children are involved in repeated person-environment interactions and begin to develop feelings about those interactions (Jack, 2010). Significant physical places can benefit young people by satisfying physical and emotional needs, as they have been found to provide a sense of comfort that supports cognitive restoration and emotional regulation (Korpela et al., 2002). Important physical places can also provide a symbolic function. In disaster settings there is emerging evidence that a range of physical places, including the home and school, become symbols of recovery for young people. For example, arts workshops involving youth between the ages of 13–22 across four disaster-affected communities in the United States and Canada highlighted key people, places, and activities that supported their recovery (Cox et al., 2017); these insights were based on local knowledge and lived experience, demonstrating that it is important to collect and document youth perspectives when contextualizing theories of disaster recovery. The finding that physical places symbolize recovery after disaster has been well-evidenced in adult samples (Cox and Perry, 2011; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015).

The development of person-place bonds varies according to developmental need (Morgan, 2010), and the role of schools in children’s lives continues to evolve, as do the nature of the interactions. For example, the journeys to and from school are important person-place interactions for children. They provide valuable opportunities for unstructured interaction with their social and physical worlds, which contribute to the development of their personal and community identities (Jack, 2010; see also O'Brien et al., 2000; Ross, 2007; Scannell and Gifford, 2017). These journeys allow children to actively engage with local space, contributing to secure attachments to the broader school locale and belonging to place (Jack, 2010). Future research could explore whether different modes of transport have unique psychological benefits, for example whether walking may foster attachment to the locale whereas a car journey may foster parent–child attachments. After Hurricane Katrina Forthergill and Peek (2015) conducted interviews and observations of children that revealed playgrounds and ball fields to be important places for recovery. Overall existing research supports the notion that schools exist as important physical places in children’s lives as they provide a context that scaffolds developmental growth and contributes to sustained psychological wellbeing.

Evidence

Schools as built environments

Macro-level assessment of how the overall physical environment directly impacts pupils’ lives

The built environment of schools has been empirically documented to impact pupils directly. For example, environmental psychologists demonstrate that the architectural environment of American primary schools predicts both attendance and academic achievement after controlling for other predictors such as socioeconomic status, ethnicity, school size, and teacher quality (Durán-Narucki, 2008). There is also a positive association between academic achievement and middle-school building conditions, mediated by social climate and student attendance (Maxwell, 2016). The destruction of school settings following natural hazard events prevents children from returning and receiving education (e.g., Mudavanhu, 2014; Adeagbo et al., 2016; Kousky, 2016). Thus, the maintenance of adequate school facilities is essential to protecting children’s right to education and their psychological wellbeing. These studies offer a macro-level assessment of how the overall physical environment directly impacts pupils’ lives, demonstrating that poorer facilities correlate with poor attendance and, therefore, poorer academic outcomes.

Micro-elements of spaces

Micro-elements of spaces that carry little meaning for adults have been shown to have great significance for young people (Koller and Farley, 2019). For example, Fleet and Britt (2011) found that children placed significance in a brick wall, which researchers initially saw as meaningless. Drawing on Stedman's (2003) ‘meaning-mediated model’, it is unlikely children were attached to the wall per se, but instead, the meaning represented by the wall, such as warmth and laughter (Koller and Farley, 2019). This is consistent with findings reported by Fleet and Britt (2011), that children often climbed and sat on the wall, creating new narratives of the wall’s significance through play. These person-place interactions also effectively subvert the adult narratives of safety and surveillance, which creates a sense of adventure and freedom that Scannell and Gifford (2016) theorize fosters positive attachments to place elements in children. Other studies have also documented differing affective responses to micro place elements. Koller and McLaren (2014) found that children shared spontaneous and charged emotional responses to hospitals’ physical design and decor that was not shared with adults. This demonstrates the importance of eliciting children’s insight into the meaning with which certain physical aspects of the school environment are endowed, as adults may be unaware of the meanings bestowed on seemingly mundane features of the school environment.

Place loss in disaster settings

The literature concerning place loss in disaster settings provides further insight into the relationship between bonds to physical places and psychological wellbeing. According to the disaster literature, the loss of physical place, most notably the home, is devastating for adults. Feelings of grief and emotional distress often accompany place loss, as cognitive-affective attachments are ruptured (Cox and Perry, 2011). This occurs beyond the initial disaster impact, as ongoing demolition leads to feelings of disorientation throughout the reconstruction period (Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). Such disruption to one’s significant places can lead to “solastalgia,” which refers to the distress produced by environmental change (Albrecht et al., 2007). When “solastalgia” occurs, the environment no longer offers solace, sense of place and place identity, causing feelings of powerlessness that negatively impacts wellbeing (Warsini et al., 2014; Albrecht, 2019; Galway et al., 2019).

The impact of place loss on children is likely to become more salient for children post-disaster since they become aware of the attachment they had to the destroyed place and experience exacerbated feelings of distress due to the suddenness and unexpected nature of the impact (Relph, 1976; Cheng and Chou, 2015). To understand the symbolic significance of the loss of physical place, we turn to research that examines material loss through the lens of Moscovici's (2001) social representations theory. The home is often depicted as a place of safety, security and relaxation. Harries (2013) found that residents at risk of flooding in the UK are motivated to protect elements of the home that facilitate feelings of safety; the elements which function in this way are largely determined by social representations. However, traumatic or repeated damage to a home can threaten residents’ ontological security (Hawkins and Maurer, 2011; Harries, 2013), which occurs when a sense of trust in the stability of the home environment is undermined. Just as the home is associated with notions of continuity and safety for adults (see Mallett, 2004 for a review), many young people often see the school as a place of inclusion and safety (Butler et al., 2017). As children spend a large amount of time at school, a similar process may occur: the group-based sense of the school as a safe space (pre-disaster) may be challenged by a school becoming a place of danger, especially where there has been a threat to life (e.g., building collapse during an earthquake). This will have an emotional impact on the children. If both a sense of safety and of danger are held simultaneously, this may lead to what Moscovici (1984) terms ‘cognitive polyphasia’: representations may be plural and even contradictory, activated depending on the social context. For example, post-disaster children may simultaneously represent the school as a place of safety when among supportive peers and adults, and danger when witnessing infrastructural damage. Although this provides insight into the unique subjectivity of person-place bonds for children (versus adults) and peripherally informs our understanding of meanings made of schools, this area remains under-researched.

The process of rebuilding schools should be emphasized in community response and recovery plans. Scholars argue that involving children and youth in the design efforts is likely to benefit their wellbeing and cultivate positive place-attachment bonds to the place they have agency in creating. For example, Koller and Farley (2019) advocate for the right of children to be involved in the design of the spaces they inhabit. Research from disaster-affected areas has also shown that young people eager to be active in community recovery post-disaster (Peek, 2008; Taylor and Peace, 2015). Further, Pivik (2010) argues that children’s insights into place differ from adults’ and documents instances where children have identified barriers to the inclusion of disabled children that relevant adult stakeholders were unable to identify. The unique student perspective should be harnessed when physically rebuilding the school post-disaster to ensure the built environment meets young people’s needs, to truly ‘build back fairer’ (Sendai Framework for Disaster Risk Reduction 2015–2030) from a child-centered perspective.

Schools as social arenas

This paper has thus far explored place meanings in terms of the physical, built environment. However, place is often considered a dual concept that incorporates a social element within the physical environment (Riger and Lavrakas, 1981; Scannell and Gifford, 2010; Koller and Farley, 2019). The relationship between both dimensions is symbiotic: bonds to physical places facilitate meaningful networks of relationships, just as meaningful relationships shape the meanings attributed to a physical place (Tuan, 1977; Hay, 1992). Schools are key sites for developing and maintaining social relationships, especially for children (Ellis, 2005). Yet, little research has explored or conceptualized the role of schools as social arenas and how they may come to exist as meaningful places for communities in disaster settings. This section outlines the social aspects of schools that serve significant functions in supporting children and youth in resilience building and psychosocial recovery.

Social support networks

For children in disaster settings, social support networks are vital resources. They introduce a plethora of protective psychosocial factors, such as sense of belonging and connection, into their lived experiences; these buttress psychological wellbeing. The notion that social support can ‘buffer’ the negative effects of stress on mental health for children and adults is well supported by contemporary research (Olstad et al., 2001; Cohen, 2004; Sharp et al., 2018; McGoron et al., 2020). As young people are happiest in places that facilitate access to peers and supportive adults, schools are especially significant social arenas for children in disaster settings because these environments provide access to multiple social actors (Chawla, 1992, cited by Ellis, 2005).

Schools provide opportunities for unique social connections that would not normally exist outside the school environment but are vital for safeguarding children’s wellbeing. For example, positive teacher-student relationships are protective against a series of risk factors for children, including depression, neglect, and bereavement (Wang et al., 2013; Sharp et al., 2018). These adult-child relationships are unique to the school settings (i.e., non-familial) and are often central in safeguarding children (Bhadra, 2016). After Hurricane Katrina, for young people required to change school, the positive support received from school staff was instrumental in supporting their wellbeing (Barrett et al., 2008). Teachers have been shown to go beyond their traditional roles to aid children in processing their disaster experiences, which involves regulating their own emotional responses to model effective coping (Mooney et al., 2021).

Schools also provide the greatest opportunity for friendships amongst children; these social relationships have been found to promote positive coping with psychological distress following a disaster. For example, a seminal study found children’s friendships to be the most salient providers of emotional support and coping assistance following a hurricane – more so than parents and teachers (Prinstein et al., 1996). Emerging insights from disaster settings also show friendships as drivers of returning to school. Empatika (2018) reports that young people in Palu, Indonesia, ranked highly their desire to return to school and reconnect with friends following the 2018 earthquake and tsunami. Play and sport are critical school-based activities that scaffold such peer-to-peer social connection; they foster psychological wellbeing and post-traumatic growth for children in disaster settings (Henley, 2005; Ray and Bratton, 2010; Goodyear-Brown, 2019). These activities are also drivers of childhood attachment to schools as play and sport are mechanisms through which children engage in valued person-place interactions and build a salient, positive place-bond with their schools (Scannell and Gifford, 2016).

After a disaster, the restoration and rebuilding of schools can symbolize the community’s resilience. School recovery allows children to return to normalcy and replace their emotional crises with the joy of being surrounded by other children and having a space to learn and play simultaneously (Fernandez et al., 2015; e.g., Telford and Cosgrave, 2006). Young people are more likely to engage in their usual activities (relative to local context) when they believe their teachers and friends support them, even when faced with considerable difficulties (Wickrama and Kaspar, 2007). It is especially important for children to re-engage in play, as it can alleviate traumatic stress (Fernandez et al., 2015). School recovery also benefits communities since it allows parents and guardians to focus on returning to their regular work, to sustain their families, while their children are at school. Returning to work activities also aids in the recovery of communities by contributing to economic recovery. Communities with high social capital and a history of community activities can take an active role in the process of economic recovery and contribute to its success and speed (Nakagawa and Shaw, 2004), which is essential in safeguarding children’s wellbeing.

Sense of connectedness between community members

Beyond what they symbolize for children, schools are important social arenas that facilitate and embed a vital sense of connectedness between community members. When a community harbors a sense of social connectedness (e.g., between family, friends, and neighbors) before a disaster, it benefits from a greater sense of community and camaraderie post-event, which then promotes its adaptive potential and resilient capacity (Thornley et al., 2014; Mutch, 2015). This effect is consistent with the ‘social cure’ in the context of public health, where group-based processes of social support and social integration are found to contribute to positive health outcomes (Haslam et al., 2018). However, communities require appropriate local infrastructure and a community hub for community connectedness to benefit preparedness, adaptation, and recovery in disaster settings (Thornley et al., 2014). Schools are well-documented as ideal community hubs for local disaster risk management planning: from a disaster risk perspective, schools are often built better and built safer; from a psychosocial perspective, they are familiar, stable, and often locally accessible environments in times of emergency (Leadbeater, 2013; Mutch, 2015; Oktari et al., 2018; Amini Hosseini and Izadkhah, 2020).

Overall, social networks play an important role in promoting wellbeing and resilience for children and communities. Cohesion in the community reduces the mental health burden on a community post-disaster. As social connections are documented protective factors, which buffer against (dis)stress, the importance of promoting and protecting social connection within communities in disaster settings is emphasized. Schools are meaningful places that act as repositories of social relationships with protective functions vital to safeguarding the wellbeing of children and their communities post-disaster.

Schools as places of personal significance

The paper has thus far explored the meaningfulness of schools as built-environments and as social arenas, supporting the wellbeing and resilience of children in disaster settings. It moves on to the personal elements contributing to the person-place bond for these children. Specifically, we consider the nature of individual factors, such as lived experience, and group-based ties, such as religion and history, in disaster settings. We demonstrate how such psychosocial ties are often the cause and consequence of complex identity dynamics, which influence how schools are represented.

Place identity

The process of person-place bonding is marked by direct and indirect interactions between the person and the place (Scannell and Gifford, 2010). These interactions include what one does in the place (e.g., activities, social interactions) and how one feels about it (e.g., safe, comfortable, welcome). The paper has previously discussed the significance of what one does in the place, and now considers the significance of how one feels about a place.

When the meaningfulness of a place deepens over time, place attachments can evolve further into place identity: a process through which individuals come to incorporate cognitions about the physical environment into their self-definitions (Clayton, 2003; Gifford, 2014). Prolonged and repeated exposure imbue environments with meaning at the individual level, especially where these exposures provide opportunities for interaction with the environment and people within it (Lewicka, 2011; Anton and Lawrence, 2014). Repeated experiences of places in childhood contribute greatly to lifelong person-place bonds (Jack, 2010), which function in a similar way psychologically to an interpersonal attachment (e.g., Fried, 2000; Kelly and Hosking, 2008; Morgan, 2010; Donovan et al., 2012; Scannell and Gifford, 2014, 2017).

Child-school bonds can comprise strong affective, social, and cognitive elements that often endure throughout the life-course. The mechanisms that shape such feelings about a place and foster such enduring person-place bonds also shape sense of self and community, and influence psychological wellbeing (Ellis, 2005). There is a well-established link between identity consistency and psychological wellbeing in the academic literature (Rogers, 1951; Phinney et al., 2001; Greenaway et al., 2016, as cited in Suh, 2002); this includes place identity as it is a substructure of social identity, similar to gender or social class (Shamsuddin and Ujang, 2008; Qazimi, 2014). Though the complex relationship between identity and wellbeing is not clearly understood at present, the literature reflects that the gaining of identity and consistency can protect and enhance wellbeing (e.g., Ysseldyk et al., 2012; Praharso et al., 2017) while disruptions to identity can be severely problematic and are linked with deleterious outcomes.

When place identities are threatened, this can also lead to place-protective action, including local opposition to proposed developments to the built environment, such as wind turbines (e.g., Stedman, 2002; Devine-Wright, 2009). Following disasters, schools may be relocated, merged or closed if they are in a dangerous zone or no longer have enough pupils to remain viable. After the Canterbury earthquake, this was found to exacerbate the social and emotional stress of a trauma-affected community (Mutch, 2018). Although unexplored in the existing literature concerning schools, place identity may impact the community response to such closures, in the form of protests and legal action. Thus, identification with places that are stable, enduring environments can act as a protective mechanism, but can also become problematic and undermine resilience when left unmitigated. For example, Bihari et al. (2012) found that place attachment was associated with greater knowledge of wildfires and effective preparedness across six communities in the United States. However, Donovan et al. (2012) found place attachment to territory and landmarks interacted with culture to minimize evacuation behaviors for an Indonesian community under threat of volcanic activity. Beliefs in protective ceremonies and spiritual forces minimized evacuation behaviours.

Meaning-making and appraising traumatic events

Schools may foster positive meaning-making outcomes because they exist as trusted places to which pupils and communities harbor positive cognitive and affective ties. As schools provide many resources to support their communities, accessing these places post-disaster can support adaptive coping practices. For example, being in a state of disrupted identity can be traumatic, especially for children who may not have the cognitive-affective tools to independently cope with or appraise events. Such adverse responses to traumatic experiences are due to the loss of ‘meaningful’ resources, including psychological and cultural resources (Hobfoll, 1989). Meaning-making is a key process through which people rectify disturbances in their sense of identity and maintain homeostasis (see Linley and Joseph, 2005; Park, 2010; King and Hicks, 2021). Finding meaning in a traumatic experience is an example of this process; which can be a stressful-inducing process, but also has the potential to lead to outcomes that enhance psychological wellbeing (King and Hicks, 2021). The meaning-making process is central to supporting recovery after disaster (Park and Blake, 2020). Schools are a vital resource drawn upon to influence how stressful a disaster is for its pupils. For example, after Hurricane Katrina, pupils who felt more connected to the school they had been relocated to reported fewer negative symptoms and more protective factors (Barrett et al., 2008). Schools may potentially act as places that can activate positive representations post-disaster as they are familiar spaces.

Social representations and attachments to schools

To understand the psychosocial significance of schools in disaster settings, it is necessary to consider how they are valued by the communities within which they exist. The significance that individuals ascribe to a place is shaped by their lived experience in the context of the group-based meaning system of their community (Bruner, 1990; Van Patten and Williams, 2008); when a person’s social world has already identified a particular place as meaningful, the place symbolizes the group-based cognitions that formed the social representation(s) of that place, according to how or why it is valued by the group (Low and Altman, 1992; Joffe, 2003; Scannell and Gifford, 2010). In Social Representational terms, this section explores how the socio-cultural group representations of the school become internalized in the individual, that is, “how the ‘we’ becomes contained in the responses of the ‘I’” (Joffe, 2003, p.60).

Representations of schools influence how these places are encountered and utilized by their communities. Like religious institutions or museums, schools are associated with their specific function and are valued according to how these functions serve their communities. Social representations that circulate in a given culture refer to shared understandings of phenomena among a specific group (Joffe, 2003). While there is plurality of representations of school depending on socio-cultural, historical context, and group-specific ideologies, schools tend to be regarded as trustworthy places dedicated to the betterment of the character and knowledge of their pupils and cultivating their growth and resilience (Bryan, 2005; Luetz, 2019). In addition, in disaster settings, schools are also regarded as disaster risk reduction centers that support their communities at all stages of disaster: preparedness, response, and recovery (e.g., Sakurai et al., 2018). Schools facilitate the resilient recovery of post-disaster communities through educational disaster preparedness programs, staff safeguarding of children’s wellbeing peri- and post-event, and converting the building into a resource distribution center (Mutch, 2015). By bringing people together in a shared and familiar space, schools also promote a ‘culture of caring’ in communities post-disaster (Mutch, 2015). Each of these functions reinforces the community’s representation of their local school as a trustworthy place and further informs their representations of the significance of schools in disaster settings. As schools continue to support their communities in this way, shared beliefs that they are valuable becomes increasingly reinforced.

The significance of schools as symbols can also be understood by considering the psychosocial functions of memorials. Bonder (2009) explains that memorials help individuals and communities reappraise past traumatic events, while also existing to remind them about conduct and future events. It is well established that a sense of shared identity emerges amongst survivors in post-disaster settings, as the communal experience of the disaster prompts a sense of ‘we-ness’ (Rodríguez et al., 2006; Drury et al., 2016). While post-disaster gains in social capital are often short-term (Kaniasty and Norris, 1993), memorials can function to maintain these ties and evolve into a source of enduring community resilience (Ntontis et al., 2020). Memorials are also inextricably linked to space, thus if schools are used as a site of memorial the physical space of the school has the potential to become an ‘anchor of shared identity’ (Ntontis et al., 2020, p. 7) rooted in deeply person-centric elements of individual and group attachment. Therefore, schools can benefit from harnessing this sense of ‘we-ness’ experienced by shared fate after an earthquake in order to build resilient communities in highly seismic/disaster-prone regions.

Conclusion

This paper has presented an evidence-based conceptualization of how and why schools exist as meaningful environments for children and their communities in disaster settings. It has considered the physical and social environments of schools and their significance at the individual and group levels (e.g., community). We have explicated a series of specific functions within each of these domains that make schools distinctly meaningful to their communities and highlighted the capacity of schools to foster community resilience and safeguard the wellbeing of children. We have also considered the nature of individual and shared ties to schools in disaster settings and demonstrated how such psychosocial ties are often the cause and consequence of complex identity dynamics that contribute to these environments being represented as symbolically significant. The mechanisms uncovered concerning how schools can provide these functions have important implications for the role of schools in mitigating the adverse impacts of disasters.

Recommendations for future research

Based on the evidence concerning place bonds, we have attempted to synthesize existing frameworks to contribute a holistic conceptualization of how schools can bolster resilience in disaster settings. We intend for this knowledge to allow academics and practitioners in disaster preparedness and response to better understand and harness the school environments’ latent capacity to improve and protect community members before, during, and after disasters. For example, by exploring schools through a broad social psychological lens of place-bonds and attachment, we highlight that rebuilding the school’s physical infrastructure should be a priority in community disaster response and recovery efforts. We use a social representations theory approach to highlight that schools are community resources that can be used to foster community integration and cohesion, provide children a sense of stability and continuity, and provide pragmatic support to community members. Each of these functions safeguards wellbeing and fosters resilience across a series of psychosocial domains. Thus, while the loss of the physical place is traumatic, rebuilding a school after a disaster may symbolize community and communal continuity. Contemporary scholarly works have only begun to capture this notion; Dimension.ai, an opensource database that offers analytics of linked data, including grants, publications, datasets, patents, and policy documents, reports that interest in schools in disaster settings has been steadily increasing. This paper provides a foundation for dialogue to consider the socio-physical function of schools in communities and the lives of the individuals who spend time in these spaces (e.g., pupils, staff, parents/families). Future research should also aim to establish insight into how place attachment functions in seismic areas pre-disaster.

Limitations

This paper expands on the contributions and syntheses provided by well-cited, prominent works concerning the person-place bond. However, it does not adhere to a single model or rigid conceptual framework. We acknowledge that this approach may have left some aspects of the place bond to schools unexplored and recommend that future research expand on our preliminary conceptual insights by empirically exploring the role and meaning of schools in disaster settings. We consider existing knowledge in social psychology, such as the person-place-process (Scannell and Gifford, 2010) and the self-other-environment (Gustafson, 2001) models of place attachment, but future research would benefit from approaching this topic through other lenses in order to deepen understanding. While this paper has regarded schools and the school environment as a place with positive valence, we also acknowledge that schools may not provide a positive experience for all people or in all places. For example, some schools have a more positive climate than others, which can impact mental health (Patalay et al., 2020). Just as social representations of the ‘home’ as a sanctuary do not reflect the lived experience of everyone (Mallett, 2004), we acknowledge that this may also be the case for representations of the school. Furthermore, some young people may have negative experiences of bullying, lack of belonging, and loneliness. This concern is especially important to note as children with special educational needs may be more vulnerable to feeling excluded (e.g., Cullinane, 2021). Future research should explicitly explore the critical role played by schools for children with special physical and educational needs, as schools may have different value and significance than captured in this paper. Finally, it must be recognized that the type of attachments pupils have may vary according to their age; future research should empirically explore the psychosocial role of schools for children and adolescents separately.

Author contributions

E-MP, EP, and HJ: conceptualisation. E-MP and EP: writing – original draft preparation. E-MP, EP, RO, and HJ: writing – review and editing. HJ: supervision and funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to a published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the UK Research and Innovation (UKRI)/Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) Global Challenges Research Fund (GCRF) for Equitable Resilience [grant number ES/T002956/1].

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Andrea Bernardino for his help and support with the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abreu Lopes, C., and Gaskell, G. (2015). “Social representations and societal psychology,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations. eds. E. Andreouli, G. Gaskell, G. Sammut, and J. Valsiner (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 29–42. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781107323650.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ACFCSS (2016). ASEAN Common Framework for Comprehensive School Safety. Available at: https://www.preventionweb.net/files/submissions/51261_publicationaseancommonframework.pdf (Accessed March 17, 2022).

Google Scholar

Adeagbo, A., Daramola, A., Carim-Sanni, A., Akujobi, C., and Ukpong, C. (2016). Effects of natural disasters on social and economic well being: a study in Nigeria. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 17, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2016.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albrecht, G. (2019). Earth Emotions: New Words for a New World. New York: Cornell University Press.

Google Scholar

Albrecht, G., Sartore, G.-M., Connor, L., Higginbotham, N., Freeman, S., Kelly, B., et al. (2007). Solastalgia: the distress caused by environmental change. Australas. Psychiatry 15, S95–S98. doi: 10.1080/10398560701701288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, I., and Low, S.M. (2012). Place Attachment. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Amini Hosseini, K., and Izadkhah, Y. O. (2020). From “earthquake and safety,” school drills to “safe school-resilient communities”: a continuous attempt for promoting community-based disaster risk management in Iran. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 45, 101512–101112. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anton, C. E., and Lawrence, C. (2014). Home is where the heart is: the effect of place of residence on place attachment and community participation. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 451–461. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, E., Ausbrooks, C., and Martinez-Cosio, M. (2008). The school as a source of support for Katrina-evacuated youth. Child. Youth Envir. 18, 202–236.

Google Scholar

Bechtel, R. B. (2010). “Environmental psychology,” in The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. eds. I. B. Weiner and W. E. Craighead (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 578–581. doi: 10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkes, F. (2007). Understanding uncertainty and reducing vulnerability: lessons from resilience thinking. Nat. Hazards 41, 283–295. doi: 10.1007/s11069-006-9036-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhadra, S. (2016). “Psycho-social support for protection of children in disasters,” in Child Safety, Welfare and Well-Being. ed. D. Sibnath (New Delhi: Springer), 259–278.

Google Scholar

Bihari, M., Hamin, E. M., and Ryan, R. L. (2012). Understanding the role of planners in wildfire preparedness and mitigation. ISRN Forestry 2012, 1–12. doi: 10.5402/2012/253028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonaiuto, M., Alves, S., De Dominicis, S., and Petruccelli, I. (2016). Place attachment and natural hazard risk: research review and agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 48, 33–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonanno, G. A., Romero, S. A., and Klein, S. I. (2015). The temporal elements of psychological resilience: an integrative framework for the study of individuals, families, and communities. Psychol. Inq. 26, 139–169. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.992677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonder, J. (2009). On memory, trauma, public space, monuments, and memorials. Places 21, 62–69.

Google Scholar

Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Bryan, J. (2005). Fostering educational resilience and achievement in urban schools through school-family-community partnerships. Prof. Sch. Couns. 8, 219–227.

Google Scholar

Butler, J., Kane, R., and Morshead, C. (2017). “It’s my safe space”: student voice, teacher education, and the relational space of an urban high school. Urban Educ. 52, 889–916. doi: 10.1177/0042085915574530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamlee-Wright, E., and Storr, V. H. (2009). Club goods and post-disaster community return. Ration. Soc. 21, 429–458. doi: 10.1177/1043463109337097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawla, L. (1992). “Childhood place attachments,” in Place attachment. eds. I. Altman and S. M. Low (Boston, MA: Springer US), 63–86. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-8753-4_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C.-K., and Chou, S.-F. (2015). The influence of place change on place bonding: a longitudinal panel study of renovated park users. Leis. Sci. 37, 391–414. doi: 10.1080/01490400.2015.1021883

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, S. (2003). “Environmental identity: a conceptual and an operational definition,” in Identity and the Natural Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature. eds. S. Clayton and S. Opotow (Cambridge, MA, US: MIT Press), 45–65.

Google Scholar

Clayton, S.D. (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Clémence, A. (2001). “Social positioning and social representations,” in Representations of the Social: Bridging Theoretical Traditions. eds. K. Deaux and G. Philogène (Malden: Blackwell Publishing), 83–95.

Google Scholar

Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. Am. Psychol. 59, 676–684. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, L. B., Coleman, S., and Scannell, L. (2021). Place attachment in green buildings: making the connections. J. Environ. Psychol. 74:101558. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins-Kreiner, N. (2020). Hiking, sense of place, and place attachment in the age of globalization and digitization: the Israeli case. Sustainability 12, 45–48. doi: 10.3390/su12114548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Convery, I., Corsane, G., and Davis, P. (2014). Making Sense of Place Multidisciplinary Perspectives. Woodbridge: The Boydell Press.

Google Scholar

Counted, V. (2016). Making sense of place attachment: towards a holistic understanding of people-place relationships and experiences. Environ. Space Place 8, 7–32. doi: 10.5840/esplace2016811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. S., and Perry, K. M. E. (2011). Like a fish out of water: reconsidering disaster recovery and the role of place and social capital in community disaster resilience. Am. J. Community Psychol. 48, 395–411. doi: 10.1007/s10464-011-9427-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. S., Scannell, L., Heykoop, C., Tobin-Gurley, J., and Peek, L. (2017). Understanding youth disaster recovery: the vital role of people, places, and activities. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 22, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2017.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullinane, M. (2021). An exploration of the sense of belonging of students with special educational needs. REACH 33, 2–12.

Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P. (2009). Rethinking NIMBYism: the role of place attachment and place identity in explaining place-protective action. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 19, 426–441. doi: 10.1002/casp.1004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Masso, A., Dixon, J., and Hernández, B. (2017). “Place attachment, sense of belonging and the micro-politics of place satisfaction,” in Handbook of Environmental Psychology and Quality of Life Research. eds. G. Fleury-Bahi, E. Pol, and O. Navarro (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 85–104. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-31416-7_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, K., Suryanto, A., and Utami, P. (2012). Mapping cultural vulnerability in volcanic regions: the practical application of social volcanology at Mt Merapi, Indonesia. Environ. Hazards 11, 303–323. doi: 10.1080/17477891.2012.689252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Droseltis, O., and Vignoles, V. L. (2010). Towards an integrative model of place identification: dimensionality and predictors of intrapersonal-level place preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, J., Brown, R., González, R., and Miranda, D. (2016). Emergent social identity and observing social support predict social support provided by survivors in a disaster: solidarity in the 2010 Chile earthquake. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 209–223. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durán-Narucki, V. (2008). School building condition, school attendance, and academic achievement in New York City public schools: a mediation model. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 278–286. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, J. L. (2005). Place and identity for children in classrooms and schools. J. Canad. Assoc. Curriculum Stud. 3, 55–73.

Google Scholar

Empatika (2018). “Let’s Get Back to our Routine”: Listening to children who were affected by Central Sulawesi earthquake and tsunami. Available at: https://resourcecentre.savethechildren.net/document/lets-get-back-our-routine-listening-children-who-were-affected-central-sulawesi-earthquake/?embed=1 (Accessed March 17, 2022).

Google Scholar

Fernandez, G., Shaw, R., and Abe, M. (2015). “Lessons from the recovery of the education sector after the Indian ocean tsunami,” in Recovery from the Indian Ocean Tsunami: A Ten-Year Journey R. Shaw (Tokyo: Springer Japan), 43–58.

Google Scholar

Fleet, A., and Britt, C. (2011). “Seeing spaces, inhabiting places: hearing school beginners,” in Researching young children's perspectives: Debating the ethics and dilemmas of educational research with children. ed. D. Harcourt (Hoboken: Taylor & Francis), 143–162.

Google Scholar

Fleury-Bahi, G., Pol, E., and Navarro, O. (2017). Handbook of Environmental Psychology and Quality of Life Research. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Foroudi, M. M., Balmer, J. M. T., Chen, W., Foroudi, P., and Patsala, P. (2020). Explicating place identity attitudes, place architecture attitudes, and identification triad theory. J. Bus. Res. 109, 321–336. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.12.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forthergill, A., and Peek, L. (2015). Children of Katrina. Texas: University of Texas Press.

Google Scholar

Fried, M. (2000). Continuities and discontinuities of place. J. Environ. Psychol. 20, 193–205. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1999.0154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedmann, J. (2010). Place and place-making in cities: a global perspective. Plan. Theory Pract. 11, 149–165. doi: 10.1080/14649351003759573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fullilove, M. T. (1996). Psychiatric implications of displacement: contributions from the psychology of place. Am. J. Psychiatr. 153, 1516–1523. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.12.1516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galway, L. P., Beery, T., Jones-Casey, K., and Tasala, K. (2019). Mapping the solastalgia literature: a scoping review study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:2662. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16152662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, R. (2014). Environmental psychology matters. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 541–579. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodyear-Brown, P. (2019). Trauma and Play Therapy: Helping Children Heal. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Greenaway, K. H., Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., and Jetten, J. (2016). Social identities promote well-being because they satisfy global psychological needs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 294–307. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greer, A., Binder, S. B., Thiel, A., Jamali, M., and Nejat, A. (2020). Place attachment in disaster studies: measurement and the case of the 2013 Moore tornado. Popul. Environ. 41, 306–329. doi: 10.1007/s11111-019-00332-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafson, P. E. R. (2001). Meanings of place: everyday experience and theoretical conceptualizations. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 5–16. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2000.0185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harries, T. (2013). “Responding to flood risk in the UK,” in Cities at Risk. eds. H. Joffe, T. Rossetto, and J. Adams (Dordrecht: Springer), 45–72.

Google Scholar

Hashemnezhad, H., Heidari, A. A., and Mohammad Hoseini, P. (2013). Sense of place and place attachment. Int. J. Architect. Urban Develop. 3, 5–12.

Google Scholar

Haslam, S. A., McMahon, C., Cruwys, T., Haslam, C., Jetten, J., and Steffens, N. K. (2018). Social cure, what social cure? The propensity to underestimate the importance of social factors for health. Soc. Sci. Med. 198, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkins, R. L., and Maurer, K. (2011). ‘You fix my community, you have fixed my life’: the disruption and rebuilding of ontological security in New Orleans. Disasters 35, 143–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7717.2010.01197.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hay, R. B. (1992). Being politically correct or enquiring: a reply to bell (what we talk about when we talk about love: a comment on Hay). Area 24, 411–412.

Google Scholar

Henley, R. (2005). Helping Children Overcome Disaster Trauma Through Post-Emergency Psychosocial Sports Programs. Biel/Bienne: Swiss Academy for Development.

Google Scholar

Hidalgo, M. C., and Hernández, B. (2001). Place attachment: Conceptual and empirical questions. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 273–281. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources. A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 44, 513–524. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.44.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IASC (2006). IASC Guidance Note on Using the Cluster Approach to Strengthen Humanitarian Response. New York, United States of America and Istanbul, Türkiye: Inter-Agency Standing Committee.

Google Scholar

Inalhan, G., and Finch, E. (2004). Place attachment and sense of belonging. Facilities 22, 120–128. doi: 10.1108/02632770410540333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaakkola, E. (2020). Designing conceptual articles: four approaches. AMS Rev. 10, 18–26. doi: 10.1007/s13162-020-00161-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, G. (2010). Place matters: the significance of place attachments for children's well-being. Br. J. Soc. Work 40, 755–771. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bcn142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamali, M., and Nejat, A. (2016). Place attachment and disasters: knowns and unknowns. J. Emerg. Manag. 14, 349–364. doi: 10.5055/jem.2016.0299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaspal, R., Fino, E., and Breakwell, G. M. (2022). The COVID-19 own risk appraisal scale (CORAS): development and validation in two samples from the United Kingdom. J. Health Psychol. 27, 790–804. doi: 10.1177/1359105320967429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, H. (2003). Risk: from perception to social representation. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 55–73. doi: 10.1348/014466603763276126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, B. S., and Stedman, R. C. (2001). Sense of place as an attitude: lakeshore owners attitudes toward their properties. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 233–248. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, B. S., and Stedman, R. C. (2006). A comparative analysis of predictors of sense of place dimensions: attachment to, dependence on, and identification with lakeshore properties. J. Environ. Manag. 79, 316–327. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2005.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaniasty, K., and Norris, F. H. (1993). A test of the social support deterioration model in the context of natural disaster. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 395–408. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, G., and Hosking, K. (2008). Nonpermanent residents, place attachment, and “sea change” communities. Environ. Behav. 40, 575–594. doi: 10.1177/0013916507302246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, L. A., and Hicks, J. A. (2021). The science of meaning in life. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 561–584. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-122921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koller, D., and Farley, M. (2019). Examining elements of children's place attachment. Child. Geogr. 17, 491–500. doi: 10.1080/14733285.2019.1574336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koller, D., and McLaren, C. (2014). Children's emotional responses to a paediatric hospital atrium. Child. Soc. 28, 451–464. doi: 10.1111/chso.12002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korpela, K., Kyttae, M., and Hartig, T. (2002). Restorative experience, self-regulation, and children's place preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 22, 387–398. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2002.0277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kousky, C. (2016). Impacts of natural disasters on children. Future Child. 26, 73–92. doi: 10.1353/foc.2016.0004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudryavtsev, A., Krasny, M. E., and Stedman, R. C. (2012). The impact of environmental education on sense of place among urban youth. Ecosphere 3, art29–art15. doi: 10.1890/ES11-00318.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leadbeater, A. (2013). Community leadership in disaster recovery: a case study. Aust. J. Emerg. Manag. 28, 41–47. doi: 10.3316/ielapa.512222935902794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemée, C., Fleury-Bahi, G., and Navarro, O. (2019). Impact of place identity, self-efficacy and anxiety state on the relationship between coastal flooding risk perception and the willingness to cope. Front. Psychol. 10:499. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2005). Ways to make people active: the role of place attachment, cultural capital, and neighborhood ties. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 381–395. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2010). What makes neighborhood different from home and city? Effects of place scale on place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 35–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2011). Place attachment: how far have we come in the last 40 years? J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 207–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linley, P. A., and Joseph, S. (2005). The human capacity for growth through adversity. Am. Psychol. 60, 262–264. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.3.262b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, S., and Derr, V. (2020). “The influence of nature on a child’s development: connecting the outcomes of human attachment and place attachment,” in Research Handbook on Childhoodnature: Assemblages of Childhood and Nature Research. eds. A. Cutter-Mackenzie-Knowles, K. Malone, and E. B. Hacking (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 151–178.

Google Scholar

Low, S. M., and Altman, I. (1992). “Place attachment,” in Place Attachment. eds. I. Altman and S. M. Low (Boston, MA: Springer US), 1–12.

Google Scholar

Luetz, J. M. (2019). “Disaster-resistant schools for disaster-resilient education,” in Quality Education. eds. W. Leal Filho, A. M. Azul, L. Brandli, P. G. Özuyar, and T. Wall (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 1–17.

Google Scholar

Mallett, S. (2004). Understanding home: a critical review of the literature. Sociol. Rev. 52, 62–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-954X.2004.00442.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzo, L., and Devine-Wright, P. (2020). Place Attachment: Advances in Theory, Methods and Applications. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Massazza, A., Brewin, C. R., and Joffe, H. (2019). The nature of “natural disasters”: survivors’ explanations of earthquake damage. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 10, 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s13753-019-0223-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S. (2011). Resilience in children threatened by extreme adversity: frameworks for research, practice, and translational synergy. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 493–506. doi: 10.1017/s0954579411000198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, M.H. (1992). Making Sense of Place: Children’s Understanding of Large-Scale Environments. Savage, US: Barnes & Noble Books.

Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. E. (2016). School building condition, social climate, student attendance and academic achievement: a mediation model. J. Environ. Psychol. 46, 206–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayunga, J. S. (2007). Understanding and applying the concept of community disaster resilience: a capital-based approach. Summer Acad. Soc. Vulnerabil. Resil. Build. 1, 1–16.

Google Scholar

McGoron, L., Riley, M. R., and Scaramella, L. V. (2020). Cumulative socio-contextual risk and child abuse potential in parents of young children: can social support buffer the impact? Child Fam. Soc. Work 25, 865–874. doi: 10.1111/cfs.12771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMillan, D. W. (1996). Sense of community. J. Community Psychol. 24, 315–325. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6629(199610)24:4<315::AID-JCOP2>3.0.CO;2-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirzaei, S., Mohammadinia, L., Nasiriani, K., Dehghani Tafti, A. A., Rahaei, Z., Falahzade, H., et al. (2019). School resilience components in disasters and emergencies: a systematic review. Trauma Mon. 24, 1–13. doi: 10.5812/traumamon.89481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mooney, M., Tarrant, R., Paton, D., Johnston, D., and Johal, S. (2021). The school community contributes to how children cope effectively with a disaster. Pastor. Care Educ. 39, 24–47. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2020.1774632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, P. (2010). Towards a developmental theory of place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovici, S. (1961/1976). La psychanalyse: Son image et son public. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Google Scholar

Moscovici, S. (1984). “The phenomenon of social representations,” in Social Representations. eds. R. M. Farr and S. Moscovici (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 3–69.

Google Scholar

Moscovici, S. (2001). “Why a theory of social representation?” in Representations of the social: Bridging theoretical traditions. eds. K. Deaux and G. Philogène (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing), 8–35.

Google Scholar

Mudavanhu, C. (2014). The impact of flood disasters on child education in Muzarabani District, Zimbabwe. Jàmbá 6:8. doi: 10.4102/jamba.v6i1.138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutch, C. (2014). The role of schools in disaster preparedness, response and recovery: what can we learn from the literature? Pastor. Care Educ. 32, 5–22. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2014.880123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutch, C. (2015). Leadership in times of crisis: dispositional, relational and contextual factors influencing school principals’ actions. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 14, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2015.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutch, C. (2018). “It was like having the roots pulled out from underneath your feet”: Currere and post-disaster school closures in New Zealand. Currere Exch. J. 2, 40–52.

Google Scholar

Nakagawa, Y., and Shaw, R. (2004). Social capital: a missing link to disaster recovery. Int. J. Mass Emerg. Disasters 22, 5–34.

Google Scholar

Noddings, N. (2005). Identifying and responding to needs in education. Camb. J. Educ. 35, 147–159. doi: 10.1080/03057640500146757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfefferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F., and Pfefferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for disaster readiness. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 41, 127–150. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9156-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntontis, E., Drury, J., Amlôt, R., Rubin, G. J., and Williams, R. (2020). Endurance or decline of emergent groups following a flood disaster: implications for community resilience. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 45:101493. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Brien, M., Jones, D., Sloan, D., and Rustin, M. (2000). Children's independent spatial mobility in the urban public realm. Childhood 7, 257–277. doi: 10.1177/0907568200007003002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obst, P., Smith, S. G., and Zinkiewicz, L. (2002). An exploration of sense of community, part 3: dimensions and predictors of psychological sense of community in geographical communities. J. Community Psychol. 30, 119–133. doi: 10.1002/jcop.1054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oktari, R. S., Shiwaku, K., Munadi, K., Syamsidik,, and Shaw, R. (2018). Enhancing community resilience towards disaster: the contributing factors of school-community collaborative network in the tsunami affected area in Aceh. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 29, 3–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2017.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olstad, R., Sexton, H., and Søgaard, A. J. (2001). The Finnmark study. A prospective population study of the social support buffer hypothesis, specific stressors and mental distress. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 36, 582–589. doi: 10.1007/s127-001-8197-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, E.-M., Bisaga, I., Oktari, R. S., Parikh, P., and Joffe, H. (2021). Integrating psychosocial and WASH school interventions to build disaster resilience. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 65:102520. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paci-Green, R., Varchetta, A., McFarlane, K., Iyer, P., and Goyeneche, M. (2020). Comprehensive school safety policy: a global baseline survey. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 44:101399. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2019.101399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol. Bull. 136, 257–301. doi: 10.1037/a0018301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., and Blake, E. C. (2020). “Resilience and recovery following disasters: the meaning making model,” in Positive Psychological Approaches To Disaster: Meaning, Resilience, and Posttraumatic Growth. ed. S. E. Schulenberg (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 9–25.

Google Scholar

Patalay, P., O'Neill, E., Deighton, J., and Fink, E. (2020). School characteristics and children's mental health: a linked survey-administrative data study. Prev. Med. 141:106292. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.106292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paton, D. (2007). Measuring and monitoring resilience in Auckland. Lower Hutt: GNS Science.

Google Scholar

Peek, L. (2008). Children and disasters: understanding vulnerability, developing capacities, and promoting resilience-an introduction. Child. Youth Environ. 18, 1–29.

Google Scholar

Phinney, J. S., Romero, I., Nava, M., and Huang, D. (2001). The role of language, parents, and peers in ethnic identity among adolescents in immigrant families. J. Youth Adolesc. 30, 135–153. doi: 10.1023/A:1010389607319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, J., Martin, D. G., and Murphy, J. T. (2011). Relational place-making: the networked politics of place. Trans. Inst. Br. Geogr. 36, 54–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-5661.2010.00411.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pivik, J. R. (2010). The perspective of children and youth: how different stakeholders identify architectural barriers for inclusion in schools. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 510–517. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, K. (2010). Making sense of place: mapping as a multisensory research method. Qual. Inq. 16, 539–555. doi: 10.1177/1077800410372600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Praharso, N. F., Tear, M. J., and Cruwys, T. (2017). Stressful life transitions and wellbeing: a comparison of the stress buffering hypothesis and the social identity model of identity change. Psychiatry Res. 247, 265–275. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pretty, G. H., Chipuer, H. M., and Bramston, P. (2003). Sense of place amongst adolescents and adults in two rural Australian towns: the discriminating features of place attachment, sense of community and place dependence in relation to place identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 273–287. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00079-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinstein, M. J., La Greca, A. M., Vernberg, E. M., and Silverman, W. K. (1996). Children's coping assistance: how parents, teachers, and friends help children cope after a natural disaster. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 25, 463–475. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2504_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qazimi, S. (2014). Sense of place and place identity. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. Res. 1, 306–310. doi: 10.26417/ejser.v1i1.p306-310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ray, D. C., and Bratton, S. C. (2010). “What the research shows about play therapy: twenty-first century update,” in Child-Centered Play Therapy Research: The Evidence Base for Effective Practice. eds. J. N. Baggerly, D. C. Ray, and S. C. Bratton (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 3–33.

Google Scholar

Relph, E.C. (1976). Place and Placelessness. London: Pion.

Google Scholar

Relph, E. C. (2009). A pragmatic sense of place. Environ. Architect. Phenomenol. 20, 24–31.

Google Scholar

Rich, Y., and Schachter, E. P. (2012). High school identity climate and student identity development. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 37, 218–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riger, S., and Lavrakas, P. J. (1981). Community ties: patterns of attachment and social interaction in urban neighborhoods. Am. J. Community Psychol. 9, 55–66. doi: 10.1007/BF00896360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, H., Trainor, J., and Quarantelli, E. L. (2006). Rising to the challenges of a catastrophe: the emergent and prosocial behavior following hurricane Katrina. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 604, 82–101. doi: 10.1177/0002716205284677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogaly, B., and Taylor, B. (2016). Moving Histories of Class and Community: Identity, Place and Belonging in Contemporary England. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK.

Google Scholar

Rogers, C.R. (1951). Client-Centered Therapy: Its Current Practice, Implications, and Theory. Oxford, England: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Ross, N. J. (2007). ‘My journey to school …’: foregrounding the meaning of school journeys and Children's engagements and interactions in their everyday localities. Child. Geograph. 5, 373–391. doi: 10.1080/14733280701631833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, A., Bisri, M. B. F., Oda, T., Oktari, R. S., Murayama, Y., Nizammudin,, et al. (2018). Exploring minimum essentials for sustainable school disaster preparedness: a case of elementary schools in Banda Aceh City, Indonesia. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 29, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2017.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sammut, G., and Howarth, C. (2014). “Social representations,” in Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology. ed. T. Teo (New York, NY, Springer New York), 1799–1802.

Google Scholar

Sawyer, I., Fardghassemi, S., and Joffe, H. (2022). How the home features in young adults’ representations of loneliness: the impact of COVID-19. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1–25. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., Cox, R. S., Fletcher, S., and Heykoop, C. (2016). “That was the last time I saw my house”: the importance of place attachment among children and youth in disaster contexts. Am. J. Community Psychol. 58, 158–173. doi: 10.1002/ajcp.12069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2010). Defining place attachment: a tripartite organizing framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2014). “Comparing the theories of interpersonal and place attachment,” in Place Attachment: Advances in Theory, Methods and Research. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (New York: Routledge), 23–36.

Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2016). Place attachment enhances psychological need satisfaction. Environ. Behav. 49, 359–389. doi: 10.1177/0013916516637648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2017). The experienced psychological benefits of place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 51, 256–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastien, L. (2020). The power of place in understanding place attachments and meanings. Geoforum 108, 204–216. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2019.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamsuddin, S., and Ujang, N. (2008). Making places: the role of attachment in creating the sense of place for traditional streets in Malaysia. Habitat Int. 32, 399–409. doi: 10.1016/j.habitatint.2008.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, J. G., Sharp, J. C., and Young, E. (2018). Academic boredom, engagement and the achievement of undergraduate students at university: a review and synthesis of relevant literature. Res. Pap. Educ. 35, 144–184. doi: 10.1080/02671522.2018.1536891

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silver, A., and Grek-Martin, J. (2015). “Now we understand what community really means”: reconceptualizing the role of sense of place in the disaster recovery process. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinkkonen, M. (2012). Attachment of young people to their home district. Youth Soc. 45, 523–544. doi: 10.1177/0044118X11423014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. S., and Cartlidge, M. R. (2011). Place attachment among retirees in Greensburg, Kansas. Geogr. Rev. 101, 536–555. doi: 10.1111/j.1931-0846.2011.00116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörqvist, P. (2016). Grand challenges in environmental psychology. Front. Psychol. 7:583. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staerklé, C., Clémence, A., and Spini, D. (2011). Social representations: a normative and dynamic intergroup approach. Polit. Psychol. 32, 759–768. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00839.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stedman, R. C. (2002). Toward a social psychology of place: predicting behavior from place-based cognitions, attitude, and identity. Environ. Behav. 34, 561–581. doi: 10.1177/0013916502034005001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stedman, R. C. (2003). Is it really just a social construction? The contribution of the physical environment to sense of place. Soc. Nat. Resour. 16, 671–685. doi: 10.1080/08941920309189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1378–1391. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, H., and Peace, R. (2015). Children and cultural influences in a natural disaster: flood response in Surakarta, Indonesia. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 13, 76–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2015.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Telford, J., and Cosgrave, J. (2006). Joint Evaluation of the International Response to the Indian Ocean Tsunami: Synthesis Report. Stockholm, Sweden: Tsunami Evaluation Coalition (TEC).

Google Scholar

Thornley, L., Ball, J., Signal, L., Lawson-Te Aho, K., and Rawson, E. (2014). Building community resilience: learning from the Canterbury earthquakes. Kōtuitui 10, 23–35. doi: 10.1080/1177083X.2014.934846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trudeau, D. (2006). Politics of belonging in the construction of landscapes: place-making, boundary-drawing and exclusion. Cult. Geogr. 13, 421–443. doi: 10.1191/1474474006eu366oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuan, Y.-F. (1977). Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Tuan, Y.-F. (1980). Rootedness versus sense of place. Landscape 24, 3–8.

Google Scholar

Twigger-Ross, C. L., and Uzzell, D. L. (1996). Place and identity processes. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 205–220. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ujang, N. (2012). Place attachment and continuity of urban place identity. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 49, 156–167. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ungar, M. (2011). The Social Ecology of Resilience: A Handbook of Theory and Practice. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

UNISDR (2014). Worldwide Initiative for Safe Schools. Available at: https://www.preventionweb.net/files/45656_worldwideinitiativeforsafeschools.pdf (Accessed March 17, 2022).

Google Scholar

Van Patten, S. R., and Williams, D. R. (2008). Problems in place: using discursive social psychology to investigate the meanings of seasonal homes. Leis. Sci. 30, 448–464. doi: 10.1080/01490400802353190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, W. (2012). “Social representation theory,” in The Encyclopedia of Peace Psychology (New York: Blackwell)

Google Scholar

Wagner, W. (2015). “Representation in action,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations. eds. G. Sammut, E. Andreouli, G. Gaskell, and J. Valsiner (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press), 12–28. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781107323650.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., Brinkworth, M., and Eccles, J. (2013). Moderating effects of teacher–student relationship in adolescent trajectories of emotional and behavioral adjustment. Dev. Psychol. 49, 690–705. doi: 10.1037/a0027916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warsini, S., Mills, J., and Usher, K. (2014). Solastalgia: living with the environmental damage caused by natural disasters. Prehosp. Disaster Med. 29, 87–90. doi: 10.1017/S1049023X13009266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickrama, K. A. S., and Kaspar, V. (2007). Family context of mental health risk in tsunami-exposed adolescents: findings from a pilot study in Sri Lanka. Soc. Sci. Med. 64, 713–723. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. R. (2014). “Beyond the commodity metaphor, revisited: some methodological reflections on place attachment research,” in Place Attachment: Advances in Theory, Methods, and Research. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (New York: Routledge), 89–99.

Google Scholar

Ysseldyk, R., Haslam, S. A., Matheson, K., and Anisman, H. (2012). Love thine enemy? Evidence that (ir) religious identification can promote outgroup tolerance under threat. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 15, 105–117. doi: 10.1177/1368430211410996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resilience, children, schools, disaster, place attachment, social representations, sense of place, risk management

Citation: Pacheco E-M, Parrott E, Oktari RS and Joffe H (2022) How schools can aid children’s resilience in disaster settings: The contribution of place attachment, sense of place and social representations theories. Front. Psychol. 13:1004022. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1004022

Received: 26 July 2022; Accepted: 23 August 2022;
Published: 12 September 2022.

Edited by:

César O. Tapia-Fonllem, University of Sonora, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Glenda Garza-Terán, University of Sonora, Mexico
Maria Fernanda Durón-Ramos, Instituto Tecnológico de Sonora, Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Pacheco, Parrott, Oktari and Joffe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elinor Parrott, Elinor.parrott.20@ucl.ac.uk

These authors have equally contributed to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.