Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 December 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Influence of play on positive psychological development in emerging adulthood: A serial mediation model

  • School of Education and Languages, Hong Kong Metropolitan University, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Introduction: Although the literature reports that play provides substantial psychological benefits for individuals, it is often only emphasized by educators for younger children but less so for emerging adults. This cross-sectional study examined the impact of time spent engaged in play on emerging adults’ emotional intelligence, emotional traits, and resilience.

Methods: Participants (N = 131, 93.9% women) were full-time undergraduate students between the ages of 19 and 25 (M = 21.28, SD = 1.08).

Results: Results showed that play experiences cultivated emotional intelligence and strengthened resilience. Further, the findings supported a serial mediation model in which play was indirectly related to resilience through its effect on emotional intelligence and positive emotion.

Discussion: Play is recognized as a means to relieve stress and protect psychological wellbeing. Emerging adults who experience pleasure, enjoyment, learning, and the acquisition of new skills will benefit from play. Individuals should enhance positive play experiences, particularly sporting activities, to maintain a healthy work-life balance given the positive relationship between play, emotional intelligence, emotional traits, and resilience.

Introduction

Emerging adulthood, a term initially proposed by Arnett (2000), is the distinct period between adolescence and young adulthood, that is, between the ages of 18 and 25. Literature suggest that emerging adulthood is a period of positive development (O'Connor et al., 2016; Lo-oh, 2019). Emerging adults, also known as “Generation Me,” have increasingly been described as narcissistic, self-absorbed, and unhappy (Twenge, 2014). The stereotypical emerging adult has difficulty entering the adult world (Padilla-Walker and Nelson, 2017). Additionally, this role change affects emerging adults, who are often exposed to an array of stressors and increased personal responsibility associated with a more complex social world.

Emerging adulthood is an emotionally complex time during which individuals develop careers and niches in society, enter adult relationships, and encounter potentially high levels of stress due to family, financial, and career obligations (Levinson, 1986; Huang et al., 2021; Fenzel and Richardson, 2022) and often experience emotional turmoil (Feldt et al., 2021). Hence, emerging adults should be helped to express more positive affect and vent negative emotions properly to increase their ability to manage life’s challenges. Emotional development is critical in emerging adults, as it promotes the experience, expression, understanding, and regulation of emotions from birth, as well as the evolution of these capacities at different life stages. Emotional intelligence refers to the individual’s ability to monitor and distinguish their own and others’ feelings and emotions, as well as the capacity to use this information to manage their relationships and guide their thinking and actions (Goleman, 1995; Mayer and Salovey, 1997). According to Salovey and Mayer’s (1990) framework of emotional intelligence, it is crucial for individuals to understand their positive and negative emotions, process emotional information accurately, and have the insight to skillfully use their emotions when solving problems and making plans. Tugade and Fredrickson (2004) supported that positive emotions are significantly related to each of these aspects.

Play is not trivial; it is essential to development. Play includes experiencing pleasure and enjoyment, learning, and acquiring new skills. It fosters psychological well-being, which affects resilience (Denovan and Macaskill, 2017). Numerous studies have confirmed that children perform important tasks during play, and, in turn, experience physical, intellectual, social, and emotional growth (Conner, 2007; Ginsburg et al., 2007). Play provides individuals with a diverse environment to express and explore their feelings, including fears, dreams, and disappointments. Individuals who frequently engage in age-appropriate play have better emotional competence (Bergin and Bergin, 2015). Play contributes to the holistic health and wellbeing of individuals at each stage of their lifespan; however, its role has not yet been fully realized (Bayliss and Etchells, 2016).

A literature review showed that little is known about the impact of play in emerging adulthood, and that the unanticipated consequences of reducing the psychological benefits of play are seldom studied. In particular, studies that relate emotional intelligence, emotional traits, and resilience to individual play experiences are scarce. Thus, the present study explored the relationship between play and its psychological benefits in emerging adults. More specifically, this study explored the impact of play on resilience using emotional intelligence and emotional traits as mediating variables in emerging adulthood.

Psychological benefits of play and resilience during emerging adulthood

Although people know what play is, it can be difficult to define, owing to its complexity in behavior and context (Cooney, 2004). Consequently, the benefits of play are sometimes overlooked. While no consensus on the definition of play exists, some criteria are commonly identified in its depiction. For example, play is intrinsic, spontaneous, repetitive, pleasurable, and voluntary (Payne and Isaacs, 2020). Theoretically, play can be characterized into two major areas: the science of play, which recognizes play as a crucial part of human development that deserves serious study, and the art of play, in which the therapist and the child both participate and where there is joy, pleasure, and freedom (Tanta and Knox, 2015; Zaharia et al., 2022). Schaefer and Drewes (2013) identified 20 core agents of change in play therapy that can improve individuals’ attachment formation, self-expression, emotion regulation, resiliency, and stress management. The healing power of play can stem from its different aspects, such as frequency, type, length, the process of learning, and appropriateness of the content (e.g., Kearney et al., 2017; Yogman et al., 2018). By engaging in play, individuals may discover new ways of utilizing resources, getting support, or using coping strategies, and consequently learn to function effectively even in stressful situations (Mannello et al., 2020).

Resilience refers to a person’s innate ability to successfully cope with change or adversity (Lantieri and Goleman, 2014). Luthar (2006) posited that resilience is a construct that consists of two distinct dimensions: significant adversity and positive adaptation (Luthar et al., 2000). Hu and Gan (2008) defined resilience as a coping process by which individuals experience healthy and constructive adjustment after undergoing some form of severe adversity. Highly resilient people have a greater capacity to learn from setbacks and apply that knowledge to difficult life situations (Salovey et al., 1999; Tugade and Fredrickson, 2004). Thus, it is particularly important to understand resilience during emerging adulthood, as its associated changes in functional capacity have an important impact on life course outcomes (Wood et al., 2018).

The dynamic process of resilience involves an interplay between risk factors internal and external protective factors which can modify the impact of an adverse life event (Rutter, 2012). Furthermore, psychological resilience and negative emotions seem to be regulated by age, with the association stronger in adults than in children or adolescents (Hu et al., 2015), thus indicating that the resilience of emerging adults needs to be addressed. In designing interventions to promote resilience in emerging adults, a better understanding of their malleable and adaptive resources might be helpful (Luecken and Gress, 2010). Researchers have found a strong link between play and resilience development (Aris and Murgatroyd, 2017). Evidence has shown that individuals may develop through play the physical and emotional resilience necessary to overcome any survival challenge (Schaefer and Drews, 2013; Gray, 2019).

Fredrickson’s (2001) broaden-and-build theory suggests that positive emotions broaden people’s momentary thought-action repertoires and build their enduring personal resources. Thus, individuals who have more positive emotions are better equipped to overcome challenges by developing enduring physical, intellectual, psychological, and social resources (Fredrickson, 2001). However, as indicated previously, the relations of play and emotional development during emerging adulthood have rarely been studied. Therefore, the present study analyzed two emotional constructs (namely, emotional intelligence and emotional traits) as potential mediators in the relationship between play and resilience.

Emotional intelligence and positive emotions as mediators

Emotional intelligence is “the capacity to reason about emotions, and of emotions to enhance thinking” (Mayer et al., 2004, p.197). A classic view of emotional intelligence is the four-branch model, as conceptualized by Mayer et al. (2004): (1) perception of emotions, (2) use of emotions, (3) understanding of emotions, and (4) managing emotions (Mayer et al., 2004; Sarrionandia et al., 2018). Thus, emotional intelligence reflects the ability to manage emotions effectively, productively, and adaptively (Sekhri et al., 2017). Emotional intelligence is an important internal protective factor (Pollard et al., 1999) and serves as an antecedent to resilience (Matthews et al., 2002), rather than encompassing resilience (Magnano et al., 2016). Emotional intelligence may be directly associated with resilience; for instance, it is easier for more emotionally intelligent individuals to adapt to stressful situations (Armstrong et al., 2011).

Emotional intelligence is assumed to buffer the impact of challenging situations through emotional self-awareness, expression, and management (Armstrong et al., 2011). There is considerable evidence showing its positive role in facilitating stress coping, building interpersonal relationships, and managing life’s transitions. Accordingly, emotional intelligence helps individuals to withstand adversity and rebound after encountering difficulties. Research has shown that emotional intelligence and resilience are enhanced through play (e.g., Lester and Russell, 2008; Macintyre, 2012). Play has been recognized as an ideal intervention for almost every conceivable problem (Hellendoorn et al., 1994). Although individuals develop a skill foundation through their experiences (e.g., unstructured play and interacting emotionally with others) in early childhood, these skills continue to be learned and developed later in life (Allen, 2018).

Emotions can be divided into two broad classes: positive and negative. Positive affect reflects a positive mood experience (e.g., happiness), while negative affect represents a negative mood experience (e.g., sadness and fear). Individuals experience different levels of positive and negative emotions while under stress (Folkman, 2008). In addition to eliciting positive emotions, trait emotional intelligence contributes to down-regulating negative emotions (Farnia et al., 2018). A study found that people who are resilient enjoy life and approach it with optimism, zest, and energy, are curious and open to new experiences, and are characterized by high positive emotionality (Block and Kremen, 1996).

The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions (Fredrickson, 1998, 2000, 2001) provides a framework for understanding psychological resilience. Positive emotions build resilience (Cohn et al., 2009) and help buffer stress (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000). However, negative emotions do not affect the benefits of positive emotions. Waugh (2014) stated that “the presence of positive emotions in resilient people does not necessarily coincide with fewer negative emotions” (p. 73). Studies have shown that positive and negative emotions can co-occur during stressful situations (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000; Ong et al., 2006; Scott et al., 2014), and that this co-occurrence may be affected by the ability of resilient people to flexibly adapt their emotional responses to match the demands of environmental circumstances (Waugh et al., 2011). Individuals who experience relatively fewer positive emotions have lower resilience (Tugade and Fredrickson, 2004). In turn, individuals who cope more effectively with hardship, adversity, or trauma develop the ability to efficiently regulate their emotions. Thus, it seems that positive emotionality is an important element of psychological resilience. Further, research has indicated that positive affect can promote resilience through automatic activation of positive emotions when encountering stressful situations (Tugade and Fredrickson, 2008; Sewart et al., 2019). A study showed that highly resilient people proactively cultivated their positive emotionality by strategically eliciting positive emotions through play (Takiguchi et al., 2022). Hence, this study postulated that among the two predicted mediators in the relationship of play and resilience, emotional intelligence would precede positive emotions.

The present study

As noted above, research has indicated that play positively impacts individuals’ psychological development. However, the impact of play on emerging adults has rarely been investigated. In light of this, this study aimed to explore the following hypotheses:

H1: Emerging adults’ play experiences positively predict resilience (H1).

H2: Emerging adults’ play experiences positively predict emotional intelligence (H2a) and positive emotions (H2b).

H3: Emotional intelligence plays an intermediary role in the relationship between play experiences and resilience in emerging adults (H3).

H4: Positive emotions play an intermediary role in the relationship between the play experiences and resilience in emerging adults (H4).

H5: Emotional intelligence and positive emotions operate as serial mediators between emerging adults’ play experiences and resilience (H5).

Materials and method

Design

This cross-sectional study used surveys to identify factors related to the influence of play on the psychological development of emerging adults.

Participants

The participants (N = 131, 93.9% women) were full-time undergraduate students at a Hong Kong university, studying early childhood education. Their ages ranged from 19 to 25 (M = 21.28, SD = 1.08). According to the student records, all participants undertaking this program were aged 25 years and younger. Although seven respondents did not indicate their age, their responses were included in the data set. The demographic characteristics of the study participants are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of participants (N = 131).

Measures

Perspectives of play experience

Henricks (2015) proposed six perspectives for viewing play: affect, action, interaction, activity, disposition, and context. These perspectives were incorporated into a multiple-choice questionnaire to explore the respondents’ play experiences over the last 12 months. The questions addressed types of play (Table 2), time spent on play/extracurricular activities (Table 3), and playmates (Table 4).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Types of play/extracurricular activities in last 12 months (N = 131).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Time spent on play/extracurricular activities per week in last 12 months (N = 131).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Playmates in last 12 months (N = 131).

The experience of play comprises three dimensions, including enjoyment (e.g., “Did you enjoy the process of play?”), learning (e.g., “Did you learn new knowledge while you were playing?”), and new skills (e.g., “Did you learn new skills while you were playing?”). The items are measured using a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = “totally disagree” to 5 = “totally agree.” This scale measures the experience of play based on prior research (Smith and Vollstedt, 1985; Baboo et al., 2022). Higher scores indicate higher levels of positive play experience. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.77 for the total scale.

Wong and Law emotional intelligence scale

The Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS; Wong and Law, 2002) is a 16-item self-report scale grounded in the ability emotional intelligence theory. The WLEIS measures ability emotional intelligence (Boburka, 2020) based on Mayer et al.’s (2004) four-branch model and has four distinct dimensions: self-emotion appraisal (e.g., I have a good understanding of my own emotions), appraisal of others’ emotion (e.g., I am a good observer of others’ emotions), use of emotion (e.g., I am a self-motivated person), and regulation of emotion (e.g., I have good control of my own emotions). The items are measured using a five-point Likert-type scale that ranges from 1 = “totally disagree” to 5 = “totally agree.” Higher scores indicate higher levels of emotional intelligence. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was.88 for the total scale and ranged from.82 to.94 for the subscales.

Questionnaire of emotional traits

The Questionnaire of Emotional Traits (QET; Chang and Yeh, 2003) is a 32-item self-report questionnaire that measures respondents’ emotional traits across four subscales: happiness (e.g., I am always satisfied with my life), sadness and fear (e.g., I always feel sad), anger and disgust (e.g., I always feel angry), and desire (e.g., I always feel a sense of accomplishment). The items are measured using a four-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 = “definitely do not feel” to 4 = “definitely feel.” The sadness and fear and anger and disgust subscales are reverse scored and summed with the other subscale scores. Higher scores indicate more positive affect. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95 for the total scale and ranged from 0.78 to 0.91 for the subscales.

Resilience scale for Chinese adolescents

The Resilience Scale for Chinese Adolescents (RSCA; Hu and Gan, 2008) comprises 27 items across five different factors: affect control (e.g., I am always discouraged by failure), goal planning (e.g., I have specific goals in my life), help-seeking (e.g., When I’m in trouble and need help, I do not know who to turn to), family support (e.g., My parents always encourage me to do my best), and positive thinking in adversity (e.g., Compared to the result, the process is more beneficial to one’s growth). Items are scored using a five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from “1 = completely disagree” to “5 = completely agree.” Total scores represent the level of resilience, with higher scores representing higher resilience. This measurement was judged to be culturally appropriate for a Chinese sample. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.81 for the total scale and ranged from 0.70 to 0.81 for the subscales.

Procedure

The Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct of the American Psychological Association were adhered to. The study was reviewed and approved by the university’s research ethics committee. The anonymity and confidentiality of the data were ensured. Participants were selected using convenience sampling. Invitations were distributed via email to 500 university students. The email included an invitation letter and a link for students to click on if they agreed to participate in the online survey. Informed consent was required prior to participation. The introductory page assured the participants of anonymity, that their responses would be confidential, and that there was no foreseen physical and psychological harm. Additionally, respondents were informed that their participation was voluntarily that they could withdraw from the study at any stage without consequences.

Data analyses

For descriptive statistical analysis, SPSS 28.0 was used to determine the means and standard deviations of time spent playing, play experiences, play habits, types of play, emotional variables, and participants’ characteristics. A frequency analysis was performed to describe the demographic information and the distribution of age and sex. Inferential statistics were utilized to make judgments on the probability of an observed difference or relationship. Harman’s single-factor test was used to test the majority of the variance, which can be explained by a single factor (McFarlin and Sweeney, 1992). The majority of variance was 22.42%. It can be assumed that no threat of common method bias was present (Podsakoff et al., 2003).

Pearson’s product–moment correlation analysis was to determine the relation between the variables. To test hypotheses 1 to 5, the SPSS PROCESS macro, Model 6 (Version 4.1; Hayes, 2022), and the bootstrapping method (Bollen and Stine, 1990) were used for serial mediation analysis, applying two significant mediators for each analysis. In the analyses, 95% confidence intervals and a 5% significance level were used to determine statistical significance (Cohen et al., 2016). Other than age, there were no missing data.

Results

Correlation analysis of play and the outcome variables

The psychological benefits of play

Correlations between experiences of play and the outcome variables are presented in Table 5. All variables were significantly correlated. Experiences of play had a weak, positive, and statistically significant correlation with emotional intelligence (r = 0.36, p < 0.001), emotional traits (r = 0.19, p < 0.05), and resilience (r = 0.35, p < 0.001). Emotional intelligence had a moderate, positive, and statistically significant correlation with emotional traits (r = 0.48, p < 0.001) and resilience (r = 0.58, p < 0.001). Emotional traits had a strong, positive, statistically significant correlation with resilience (r = 0.69, p <0.001).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among the variables.

Play and resilience: Serial mediation model

Sequential mediation analysis was used to investigate hypotheses 1 to 5. The hypothesized research model, which states that emotional intelligence (M1) and emotional traits (M2) will function as serial mediators in the relationship between play and resilience, is displayed in Figure 1. The model was implemented using the PROCESS macro for SPSS, and the indirect effects were determined by applying two mediators on the basis of 5,000 bootstrap samples in generating 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals. The results of the PROCESS analyses are tabulated in Table 7. Van Jaarsveld et al. (2010) state that the advantage of this procedure enables isolation of each mediator’s indirect effect: emotional intelligence (Hypothesis 3) and emotional traits (Hypothesis 4). Additionally, this approach allows the investigation of “the indirect effect passing through both of these mediators in a series” (Hypothesis 5) (Van Jaarsveld et al., 2010, p. 1496).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized research model.

The results of the serial mediation analyses are summarized in Figure 2. Confirming Hypothesis 1, the result showed a total effect of emerging adults’ play experiences on resilience, B = 1.87, p < 0.001. When the mediators were included in the analysis, this coefficient was reduced but was still statistically significant (direct effect, c’), B = 0.80, p = 0.02. Play was also found to be a positive predictor of emotional intelligence, B = 1.44, p < 0.001, but not emotional traits (i.e., positive emotion), B = 0.21, p = 0.74. Hypothesis 2 was partially supported. The results are tabulated in Table 6.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The serial mediation model. The serial mediation analysis for the relationship between play and resilience through emotional intelligence and emotional traits. c, total effect of play; c', direct effect of play with mediators controlled. Unstandardized regression coefficients are reported in this figure. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Summary of multiple regression analyses for the serial mediation model.

Table 7 shows the indirect effects and their associated 95% CIs. As shown in the table, the analysis of the indirect mediation by bootstrapping found that the resulting data supported the significance of Path 1 (Play ➔ Emotional Intelligence ➔ Resilience; B = 0.51, SE = 0.19, 95% CI [0.18, 0.93]), confirming Hypothesis 3, and Path 3 (Play ➔ Emotional Intelligence ➔ Emotional Traits ➔ Resilience; B = 0.47, SE = 0.17, 95% CI [0.19, 0.85]), confirming Hypothesis 5. However, Path 2 ([Play ➔ Emotional Traits ➔ Resilience; B = 0.08, SE = 0.28, 95% CI [−0.43, 0.67]) was non-significant. Hypothesis 4 was rejected.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Bootstrapping indirect effects and 95% confidence interval (CI) for the sequential mediational model.

The results revealed a significant indirect effect of play on resilience through emotional intelligence and emotional traits (b = 0.47, t = 2.81), thereby supporting Hypothesis 5. Furthermore, the direct effect of play on resilience in presence of the mediators was also found to be significant (b = 0.80, p < 0.02). Hence, there was partial serial mediation of emotional intelligence and emotional traits on the relationship between play and resilience. The summary of the mediating effects is presented in Table 7. Consistent with previous research, play was related to high levels of resilience; however, as predicted, this relationship was mediated by emotional intelligence. These findings both support and extend prior research on emotional intelligence and resilience. Play appeared to positively affect resilience by enhancing emotional intelligence and positive emotions, which, in turn, predicted resilience.

Discussion

Given the nature of the quantitative data, we conducted a serial mediation analysis and found that emotional intelligence and emotional traits mediated the positive relationship between play and resilience. The results convincingly demonstrated that play is an important predictor of emotional intelligence and resilience. These findings also addressed various concerns about play (i.e., types of play, playtime, and playmates).

Duffy et al. (2019) found an increased rate of anxiety, mood symptoms, and suicidal ideation among college students in the United States from 2007 to 2018. Similar results have been found in studies around the world including Hong Kong and the United Kingdom (e.g., Geulayov et al., 2016; Fong et al., 2022). This suggests that effective interventions for emerging adults are sorely needed at this time. The present study addressed this concern by examining the mediating roles of emotional intelligence and positive emotions in the relationship between play and resilience. In support of Hypothesis 1, this study found that emerging adults’ play experiences positively predicted resilience. This aligned with previous findings showing that leisure activities as a form of play are conducive to resilience (Denovan and Macaskill, 2017). Play serves as an oasis to recharge physically, psychologically, and/or emotionally, which contributes to the resilience of young people (Gilligan, 2000). Next, the current study found that play experiences positively predicted emotional intelligence (supporting Hypothesis 2, H2a), suggesting that emerging adults who reported more positive play experience tended to develop greater emotional intelligence. This finding is supported by Goleman (2006), who posited that play is a way to develop emotional intelligence.

However, this study did not find a significant association between play and positive emotions (not supporting Hypothesis 2, H2b). There are two plausible explanations for this. First, the content of play may involve both positive (e.g., winning, joy, fun, and happiness) and negative themes (e.g., losing, pain, sadness, anger, and frustration). For example, a player may channel their aggression through sports. The act of playing allows people to express and transform unpleasant or horrifying feelings, such as pain, loss, or death (Winnicott, 2017). Play enables people to cognitively resolve conflicting emotions and relieve negative emotional states (Mainemelis and Ronson, 2006). The cognitive element of play may change and generally result in some form of positive effect but only after some time. Second, this study was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic; thus, individuals may have experienced high anxiety and negative emotions, which could have lowered the level of positive emotions. Another study showed that the COVID-19 pandemic created a dynamic interplay of emotions and an increase in negative emotions, particularly anger (Kim et al., 2022). Therefore, further studies are considered.

Our findings showed that emotional intelligence had a mediating effect between play and resilience in emerging adults (supporting Hypothesis 3), showing that play contributed to the development of emotional intelligence, thereby promoting resilience. The instinct to play is ingrained in us throughout our lives (Carroll, 2009) and influences the development of emotional intelligence skills. Individuals with a higher level of emotional intelligence display a more stable sense of resilience throughout life (Shuo et al., 2022). Our findings aligned with previous research and confirmed that emotional intelligence has a significant effect on resilience (Tugade and Fredrickson, 2004).

It is surprising that, in our study, positive emotions did not play an intermediary role in the relationship between play experiences and resilience in emerging adults (thus, Hypothesis 4 was not supported). In this respect, our findings differed from previous studies. For example, Newman et al. (2014) reported that individuals experience positive emotions through meaningful leisure engagement, which promotes stress-coping abilities and resilience. As discussed in H2b, it is necessary to conduct further research to examine the mediating effect of emotional traits, both positive and negative.

The serial mediation analysis showed that emotional intelligence and positive emotions operate as serial mediators between emerging adults’ play experiences and resilience (supporting Hypothesis 5). This finding is in line with previous studies. Emerging adults develop emotional intelligence through play. Individuals who have higher emotional intelligence cope better with stressful encounters and produce more adaptive responses (Salovey et al., 1999; Lea et al., 2019). Resilient people are often described as being emotionally intelligent (Salovey et al., 1999) and appear to use positive emotions in their favor (Tugade and Fredrickson, 2002). Fredrickson (1998, 2001) claimed that positive emotions have a complementary effect, widening the array of the thoughts and actions that come to mind.

To date, studies have rarely directly examined the mediated relationship between emerging adults’ play experiences and resilience. By examining the mediating roles of emotional intelligence and emotional traits in the relationship between play and resilience, this study extended the literature of play on psychological benefits in emerging adults. According to previous studies (e.g., Saklofske et al., 2007; Shikako-Thomas et al., 2013; Green, 2017), play was positively correlated with individuals’ experiences of pleasure and enjoyment, knowledge acquisition, development of new skills, emotional intelligence, emotional traits, and resilience.

The current study adds to the evidence that emerging adults’ playful experiences have a positive impact on their psychological development. The results indicated that emerging adults had higher emotional intelligence, more positive moods, and greater resilience when they engaged more in positive play experiences. Hence, promoting positive play experience results in improving emotional intelligence and enhancing positive emotions, which allows emerging adults to develop key abilities to rebound from stressful events. Therefore, it is essential that emerging adults engage in play.

Implications for future research, professional practice, and education

Play is considered essential to optimize individual growth and development (Bayliss and Etchells, 2016). Our study found that play is an effective an effective means to enhance emotional intelligence, promote positive emotions, and build resilience. As shown in other studies (Armstrong et al., 2011; Magnano et al., 2016; Sarrionandia et al., 2018), emotional intelligence plays a significant role in resilience.

Research into factors that influence emerging adults’ psychological development is valuable in that it reveals ways in which it can be supported and highlight new directions to consider when developing interventions. First, play is seen as an effective means because of its impact on emotional intelligence and resilience which, in turn, affect emerging adults’ psychological development.

Second, the majority of previous studies focused on children and youth (e.g., Ginsburg et al., 2007; Yogman et al., 2018) or on electronic play (e.g., Müller and Bonnaire, 2021; TaeHyuk Keum and Hearns, 2022). In the present study, most emerging adults spent less than 7 h weekly on play/extracurricular activities (80%) and leisure reading (93%) in the last 12 months. This implies that few people currently engage in regular play/extracurricular activities and leisure reading. Indeed, around 60% of the individuals spent more than 7 h weekly playing video games. Even though exploring the negative impacts of electronic play are necessary, examining its appropriateness and effectiveness for emerging adults is of crucial importance.

Third, universities should make a variety of play activities available to their students. Regardless of age, play evokes enjoyment (Glenn and Knapp, 1987) and promotes intimacy in romantic relationships (Oring, 1984). Play can also serve as a tool for conflict resolution (Betcher, 1981; Glenn and Knapp, 1987; Baxter, 1992). Therefore, play is a particularly significant resource for promoting positive psychological development. In this study, 92.4% of the participants reported that they have playmates and 80.2% always played with friends. However, 7.6% of the participants reported that they have no playmates. Educators should not ignore the risk to this group of students.

Limitations and recommendations for further research

This study had three shortcomings. Frist, everyone can freely choose to play. This study cannot answer which types of play are most effective. Individuals define play according to their subjective perspectives. Further investigation with regard to type, frequency, and duration of play to improve psychological development and mental health outcomes should be considered.

A second limitation of this study is gender bias within the sample as the majority of the participants were women (93.9%). This is reflected by its small sample size and the gender imbalance between the groups. Thus, the results may be unrepresentative of other genders (Ritchie, 2009). Due to sampling convenience and sampling scarcity, the researchers selected individuals if they had some sense about the definition of play (i.e., play knowledge and experience), despite the obvious fact that there were more women than men in the sample. This non-probability sampling was not intended to be representative of a population. The results are subject to auto-selection bias, and the findings might have limited generalizability or external validity. Further studies should investigate larger samples to reduce sampling error and increase external validity.

Third, play is age specific. Individuals at different ages and developmental stages should choose age-appropriate play. Human development encompasses changes in physical, behavioral, cognitive, social, and emotional characteristics over the course of a lifetime. The current study focused only on psychological development. Owing to time limitations and the study’s cross-sectional design, longitudinal data were excluded. In the future, longitudinal studies should evaluate other psychological variables (e.g., self-identity, intimate relationship, parenting styles, childhood play experiences) and investigate the impact of play at various developmental stages.

Conclusion

This study found that emotional intelligence and emotional traits both significantly mediated the relationship between play and resilience. In emerging adults’ play experiences, play was a predictor of resilience, and this relationship was serially mediated by emotional intelligence and positive emotions. Given the value of play, emerging adults should enhance positive play experiences, particularly sporting activities, to maintain a healthy work-life balance and should give consideration to the meaningful role that play has in their lives.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/b2ae5/?view_only=e0352ab1943243dea95b5f2c0b260df4.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Committee, Hong Kong Metropolitan University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allen, V. (2018). Supporting behavior by building resilience and emotional intelligence: A guide for classroom teachers. 2nd edn. UK: Critical Publishing.

Google Scholar

Aris, S., and Murgatroyd, S. (2017). Beyond resilience from mastery to mystery: A workbook for personal mastery and transformational change. Canada: FutureTHINK.

Google Scholar

Armstrong, A. R., Galligan, R. F., and Critchley, C. R. (2011). Emotional intelligence and psychological resilience to negative life events. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 331–336. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am. Psychol. 55, 469–480. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baboo, S., Kanna, Y., and Bennett, C. N. (2022). “A systematic review on the neuro-cognitive correlations of game-based learning in higher educational learning environments,” in Handbook of Research on Acquiring 21st Century Literacy Skills through Game. ed. C. A. Lane (Hershey, PA: IGI Global), 58–77.

Google Scholar

Baxter, L. A. (1992). Forms and functions of intimate play in personal relationships. Hum. Commun. Res. 18, 336–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1992.tb00556.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayliss, R., and Etchells, F. (2016). “The concept of health and wellbeing across the lifespan” in Play in Healthcare for Adults: Using Play to Promote Health and Wellbeing Across the Adult Lifespan. eds. A. Tonkin and J. Whitaker (Oxford: Routledge), 59–69.

Google Scholar

Bergin, C. C., and Bergin, D. A. (2015). Child and Adolescent Development in Your Classroom 2nd Edn. Boston: Cengage.

Google Scholar

Betcher, R. W. (1981). Intimate play and marital adaptation. Psychiatry 44, 13–33. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1981.11024088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J., and Kremen, A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 349–361. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.2.349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boburka, R. R. (2020). “Emotional intelligence, assessment of,” in The Wiley Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences: Model and Theories. eds. B. J. Carducci and C. S. Nave (New York: Wiley), 513–518.

Google Scholar

Bollen, K. A., and Stine, R. (1990). Direct and indirect effects: classical and bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociol. Methodol. 20, 115–140. doi: 10.2307/271084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, K. (2009). The Red Rubber Ball at Work: Elevate Your Game through the Hidden Power of Play. New York McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Chang, P. J., and Yeh, Y. C. (2003). Emotional trait and state scale. Available at: http://www3.nccu.edu.tw/~ycyeh/instrument-english/2002%20emotional%20trait%20and%20state.pdf

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. F., Korevaar, D. A., Altman, D. G., Bruns, D. E., Gatsonis, C. A., Hooft, L., et al. (2016). STARD 2015 guidelines for reporting diagnostic accuracy studies: explanation and elaboration. BMJ Open 6:e012799. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., and Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion 9, 361–368. doi: 10.1037/a0015952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, B. (2007). Unplugged Play: No Batteries. No Plugs. Pure Fun. New York: Workman.

Google Scholar

Cooney, M. H. (2004). Is play important? Guatemalan kindergartners' classroom experiences and their parents' and teachers' perception of learning through play. J. Res. Child. Educ. 18, 261–277. doi: 10.1080/02568540409595040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denovan, A., and Macaskill, A. (2017). Building resilience to stress through leisure activities: a qualitative analysis. Ann. Leisure Res. 20, 446–466. doi: 10.1080/11745398.2016.1211943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffy, M. E., Twenge, J. M., and Joiner, T. E. (2019). Trends in mood and anxiety symptoms and suicide-related outcomes among U.S. undergraduates, 2007–2018: evidence from two national surveys. J. Adolesc. Health 65, 590–598. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.04.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farnia, F., Nafukho, F. M., and Petrides, K. V. (2018). Predicting career decision-making difficulties: the role of trait emotional intelligence, positive and negative emotions. Front. Psychol. 9:1107. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldt, R., Bejar, M., Lee, J., and Louison, R. (2021). Vocational identity resources in emerging adulthood: associations with facets of dispositional mindfulness. Career Dev. Q. 69, 2–18. doi: 10.1002/cdq.12245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenzel, L. M., and Richardson, K. D. (2022). The stress process among emerging adults: spirituality, mindfulness, resilience, and self-compassion as predictors of life satisfaction and depressive symptoms. J. Adult Dev. 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s10804-021-09384-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S. (2008). The case for positive emotions in the stress process. Anxiety Stress Cop. 21, 3–14. doi: 10.1080/10615800701740457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. Am. Psychol. 55, 647–654. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.6.647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, T. C. T., Cheng, Q., and Yip, P. S. F. (2022). Change in suicidal ideation and associated factors among young adults in Hong Kong from 2018 to 2019: a latent transition analysis. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 57, 1027–1038. doi: 10.1007/s00127-021-02203-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Rev. General Psychol. 2, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2000). Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-being. Prev. Treat. 3:1. doi: 10.1037/1522-3736.3.1.31a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geulayov, G., Kapur, N., Turnbull, P., Clements, C., Waters, K., Ness, J., et al. (2016). Epidemiology and trends in non-fatal self-harm in three centres in England, 2000–2012: findings from the multicentre study of self-harm in England. BMJ Open 6:e010538. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilligan, R. (2000). Adversity, resilience and young people: the protective value of positive school and spare time experiences. Child. Soc. 14, 37–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1099-0860.2000.tb00149.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsburg, K. R., American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Communications, and American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent–child bonds. Pediatrics 119, 182–191. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenn, P. J., and Knapp, M. L. (1987). The interactive framing of play in adult conversations. Commun. Q. 35, 48–66. doi: 10.1080/01463378709369670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence. New York Bantam Books, Inc.

Google Scholar

Goleman, D. (2006). Social Intelligence: The New Science of Human Relationships. New York, NY Bantam Books.

Google Scholar

Gray, P. (2019). “Evolutionary functions of play: practice, resilience, innovation, and cooperation,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Play: Developmental and Disciplinary Perspectives. eds. P. K. Smith and J. L. Roopnarine (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 84–102.

Google Scholar

Green, J. (2017). I’m OK! Building Resilience through Physical Play St Paul, MN, Redleaf Press.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach 3rd ed New York, NY The Guilford.

Google Scholar

Hellendoorn, J., van der Kooij, R., and Sutton-Smith, B. (Eds.) (1994). Play and Intervention. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Google Scholar

Henricks, T. C. (2015). Play as experience. Am. J. Play 8, 18–49.

Google Scholar

Hu, Y. Q., and Gan, Y. Q. (2008). Development and psychometric validity of the resilience scale for Chinese adolescents. Acta Psychol. Sin. 40, 902–912. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.00902

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, T., Zhang, D., and Wang, J. (2015). A meta-analysis of the trait resilience and mental health. Personal. Individ. Differ. 76, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Heflin, C. M., and Validova, A. (2021). Material hardship, perceived stress, and health in early adulthood. Ann. Epidemiol. 53, 69–75.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2020.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kearney, B., Ritzenthaler, H., Gray, G., and Yoder, W. (2017). Using Joyful Activity to Build Resiliency in Children in Response to Toxic Stress. Ohio Guidestone.

Google Scholar

Kim, D., Park, C., Kim, E., Han, J., and Song, H. (2022). Social sharing of emotion during the COVID-19 pandemic. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 25, 369–376. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2021.0270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lantieri, L., and Goleman, D. (2014). Building Emotional Intelligence: Techniques to Cultivate Inner Strength in Children. Boulder, CO: Sounds True.

Google Scholar

Lea, R. G., Davis, S. K., Mahoney, B., and Qualter, P. (2019). Does emotional intelligence buffer the effects of acute stress? A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 10:810. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lester, S., and Russell, W. (2008). Play for a Change – Play, Policy and Practice: A Review of Contemporary Perspectives. London: Play England.

Google Scholar

Levinson, D. J. (1986). A conception of adult development. Am. Psychol. 41, 3–13. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.41.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo-oh, L. J. (2019). From positive psychology to positive development: overcoming adversity and flourishing in emerging adulthood. J. Cult. Soc. Develop. 46, 39–50. doi: 10.7176/JCSD

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luecken, L. J., and Gress, J. L. (2010). “Early adversity and resilience in emerging adulthood,” in Handbook of Adult Resilience. eds. J. W. Reich, A. J. Zautra, and J. S. Hall (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 238–257.

Google Scholar

Luthar, S. S. (2006). “Resilience in development: a synthesis of research across five decades,” in Developmental Psychopathology: Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation. eds. D. Cicchetti and D. J. Cohen, 740–795. doi: 10.1002/9780470939406.ch20.Wiley

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., and Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 71, 543–562. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macintyre, C. (2012). Enhancing Learning through Play: A Developmental Perspectives for Early Years Settings. 2nd Edn. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Magnano, P., Craparo, G., and Paolillo, A. (2016). Resilience and emotional intelligence: which role in achievement motivation. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 9, 9–20. doi: 10.21500/20112084.2096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mainemelis, C., and Ronson, S. (2006). “Ideas are born in fields of play: towards a theory of play and creativity in organization settings,” in Research in Organizational Behavior: An Annual Series of Analytical Essays. ed. B. M. Staw, Vol. 27 (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 81–132.

Google Scholar

Mannello, M., Casey, T., and Atkinson, C. (2020). “Article 31: play, leisure, and recreation,” in International Handbook on Child Rights and School Psychology. eds. B. Nastasi, S. Hart, and S. Naser (New York: Springer), 337–348.

Google Scholar

Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., and Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional Intelligence: Science and Myth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., and Salovey, P. (1997). “What is emotional intelligence?” in Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications. eds. P. Salovey and D. J. Sluyter (New York, NY: Basic Books), 3–34.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2004). TARGET ARTICLES: emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications. Psychol. Inq. 15, 197–215. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McFarlin, D. B., and Sweeney, P. D. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. Acad. Manag. J. 35, 626–637. doi: 10.2307/256489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, T., and Bonnaire, C. (2021). Intrapersonal and interpersonal emotion regulation and identity: a preliminary study of avatar identification and gaming in adolescents and young adults. Psychiatry Res. 295:113627. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, D. B., Tay, L., and Diener, E. (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: a model of psychological mechanisms as mediating factors. J. Happiness Stud. 15, 555–578. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9435-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, M., Sanson, A. V., Toumbourou, J. W., Hawkins, M. T., Letcher, P., Williams, P., et al. (2016). “Positive development and resilience in emerging adulthood” in The Oxford Handbook of Emerging Adulthood. ed. J. J. Arnett (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 601–614.

Google Scholar

Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., and Wallace, K. A. (2006). Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 730–749. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oring, E. (1984). Dyadic traditions. J. Folk. Res. 21, 19–28.

Google Scholar

Padilla-Walker, L. M., and Nelson, L. J. (2017). “Flouring in emerging adulthood: an understudied approach to the third decade of life,” in Flourishing in Emerging Adulthood: Positive Development during the Third Decade of Life. eds. L. M. Padilla-Walker and L. J. Nelson (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 431–444.

Google Scholar

Payne, V. G., and Isaacs, L. D. (2020). Human Motor Development: A Lifespan Approach. 10th edn. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollard, J. A., Hawkins, J. D., and Arthur, M. W. (1999). Risk and protection: are both necessary to understand diverse behavioral outcomes in adolescence? Soc. Work. Res. 23, 145–158. doi: 10.1093/swr/23.3.145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritchie, T. D. (2009). “Research, gender bias in,” in Encyclopedia of Gender and Society. ed. J. O’Brien, Vol. 1 (San Diego, CA: SAGE), 714–715.

Google Scholar

Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 335–344. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Rohr, B. A., and Andrews, J. J. W. (2007). Personality, emotional intelligence and exercise. J. Health Psychol. 12, 937–948. doi: 10.1177/1359105307082458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., Bedell, B., Detweiler, J. B., and Mayer, J. D. (1999). “Coping intelligently: emotional intelligence and the coping process,” in Coping: The Psychology of What Works. ed. C. R. Snyder (New York: Oxford University Press), 141–164.

Google Scholar

Salovey, P., and Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 9, 185–211. doi: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarrionandia, A., Ramos-Díaz, E., and Fernández-Lasarte, O. (2018). Resilience as a mediator of emotional intelligence and perceived stress: a cross-country study. Front. Psychol. 9:2653. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, C. E., and Drewes, A. A. (2013). The Therapeutic Powers of Play: 20 Core Agents of Change. Hoboken, NY Wiley.

Google Scholar

Scott, S. B., Sliwinski, M. J., Mogle, J. A., and Almeida, D. M. (2014). Age, stress, and emotional complexity: results from two studies of daily experiences. Psychol. Aging 29, 577–587. doi: 10.1037/a0037282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekhri, P., Sandhu, M., and Sachdev, V. (2017). Emerging understanding of emotional intelligence of teenagers. Int. J. Clin. Pediatr. Dentist. 10, 289–292. doi: 10.5005/jp-journals-10005-1452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sewart, A. R., Zbozinek, T. D., Hammen, C., Zinbarg, R. E., Mineka, S., and Craske, M. G. (2019). Positive affect as a buffer between chronic stress and symptom severity of emotional disorders. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7, 914–927. doi: 10.1177/2167702619834576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shikako-Thomas, K., Shevell, M., Lach, L., Law, M., Schmitz, N., Poulin, C., et al. (2013). Picture me playing—a portrait of participation and enjoyment of leisure activities in adolescents with cerebral palsy. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 1001–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuo, Z., Xuyang, D., Xin, Z., Xuebin, C., and Jie, H. (2022). The relationship between postgraduates’ emotional intelligence and well-being: the chain mediating effect of social support and psychological resilience. Front. Psychol. 13:865025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.865025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. K., and Vollstedt, R. (1985). On defining play: an empirical study of the relationship between play and various play criteria. Child Dev. 56, 1042–1050. doi: 10.2307/1130114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

TaeHyuk Keum, B., and Hearns, M. (2022). Online gaming and racism: impact on psychological distress among black, Asian, and Latinx emerging adults. Games Cult. 17, 445–460. doi: 10.1177/15554120211039082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takiguchi, Y., Matsui, M., Kikutani, M., and Ebina, K. (2022). The relationship between leisure activities and mental health: the impact of resilience and COVID-19. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being, 1–19. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12394LEISURE

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanta, K. J., and Knox, S. H. (2015). “Play” in Occupational Therapy for Children and Adolescents. eds. J. Case-Smith and J. C. O'Brien. 7th Edn. (St. Louis: Elsevier), 483–497.

Google Scholar

Tugade, M. M., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2002). “Positive emotions and emotional intelligence,” in The Wisdom of Feelings: Psychology processes in emotional intelligence. eds. L. F. Barrett and P. Salovey (New York, NY: Guilford), 319–340.

Google Scholar

Tugade, M. M., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 320–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tugade, M. M., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2008). “Positive emotions and emotional intelligence” in Emotional Intelligence: Perspectives on Educational and Positive Psychology. eds. J. C. Cassady and M. A. Eissa (New York: Peter Lang), 146–167.

Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M. (2014). Generation Me: Why today’s Young Americans are More Confident, Assertive, Entitled—And More Miserable than Ever Before. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Van Jaarsveld, D. D., Walker, D. D., and Skarlicki, D. P. (2010). The role of job demands and emotional exhaustion in the relationship between customer and employee incivility. J. Manag. 36, 1486–1504. doi: 10.1177/0149206310368998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waugh, C. E. (2014). “The regulatory power of positive emotions in stress: a temporal-functional approach” in The Resilience Handbook: Approaches to Stress and Trauma. eds. M. Kent, M. C. Davis, and J. W. Reich (New York, NY: Routledge), 73–85.

Google Scholar

Waugh, C. E., Thompson, R. J., and Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Flexible emotional responsiveness in trait resilience. Emotion 11, 1059–1067. doi: 10.1037/a0021786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winnicott, D. W. (2017). Playing and Reality, 2nd Edn. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Wong, C., and Law, K. S. (2002). Wong and Law emotional intelligence scale. Psyc TESTS Dataset. doi: 10.1037/t07398-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, D., Crapnell, T., Lau, L., Bennett, A., Lotstein, D., Ferris, M., et al. (2018). Emerging adulthood as a critical stage in the life course. In N. Halfon, C. B. Forrest, R. M. Lerner, and E. M. Faustman (Eds.), Handbook of life Course Health Development (pp. 123–144). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International.

Google Scholar

Yogman, M., Garner, A., Hutchinson, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., and Golinkoff, R. M., Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, & Council on Communications and Media (2018). The power of play: a pediatric role in enhancing development in young children. Pediatrics 142:e20182058. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-2058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaharia, A., Dell’Angela, L., Sander, D., and Samson, A. C. (2022). “Play and games: means to support emotional development” in The Oxford Handbook of Emotional Development. eds. D. Duke, A. Samson, and E. Walle (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 354–370.

Google Scholar

Keywords: play, emerging adulthood, emotional intelligence, resilience, positive emotions, serial mediation, broaden-and-build theory

Citation: Ho WY (2022) Influence of play on positive psychological development in emerging adulthood: A serial mediation model. Front. Psychol. 13:1057557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1057557

Received: 29 September 2022; Accepted: 17 November 2022;
Published: 06 December 2022.

Edited by:

Anna Parola, University of Naples Federico II, Italy

Reviewed by:

María del Mar Simón Márquez, University of Almeria, Spain
Jie Luo, Guizhou Normal University, China

Copyright © 2022 Ho. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wing W. Y. Ho, wyho@hkmu.edu.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.