Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 30 March 2023
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Employee Experience, Occupational Health, and Organizational Supportive Factors: From an Integrated Perspective View all 40 articles

Exploring self-regulation theory as a mechanism of the effects of psychological contract fulfillment: The role of emotional intelligence

Lyonel Lauli
Lyonel Laulié*Gabriel Briceo-JimnezGabriel Briceño-JiménezGisselle Henríquez-GmezGisselle Henríquez-Gómez
  • Departamento de Administración, Facultad de Economía y Negocios, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile

As self-regulation theory has increasingly been used as a theoretical lens to explain the effects of psychological contract evaluations and employee outcomes, we test whether emotional intelligence (an ability for self-regulation) is a potential moderator of these relationships. More concretely, using a multiple times survey design in an education-based organization with 247 participants, we examined whether emotional intelligence moderates the mediation effect of emotional exhaustion on the relationship between psychological contract fulfillment and turnover intentions. Using a structural equations model (SEM) framework, our results support our hypotheses that individuals with low emotional intelligence do not experience the benefits of having fulfilled psychological contracts. Psychological contract fulfillment significantly reduces the likelihood of emotional exhaustion but only for individuals with high emotional intelligence. Consequently, turnover intentions are lower for emotionally intelligent individuals who experience the fulfillment of psychological contracts. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. We conclude our study by suggesting that emotional intelligence should be considered as a relevant individual difference in future psychological contract research.

1. Introduction

Decades of research on psychological contracts (PC) have shown that perceptions of positive employee-employer exchanges produce positive outcomes for organizations and individuals (Zhao et al., 2007; Rousseau, 2011). At the individual level, a central mechanism of these effects is based on self-regulation theory which posits that individuals constantly try to reach individual goals which are evaluated against real experiences. When reality does not conduce individuals to personal goals, a discrepancy is created, and an appropriate response is enacted in order to reduce the experienced discrepancy. Perceptions of discrepancies between the PC and the actual employment experience that are nonconductive to personal goals generate negative emotional responses that in turn activate reparation, renegotiation, or even exit responses (Rousseau et al., 2018). In contrast, positive perceptions of PC fulfillment activate regulatory processes that facilitate positive individual outcomes (i.e., individual behaviors, attitudes, and psychological states). A basic assumption of this theory of self-regulation and psychological contracts is the idea that individuals are indeed able to regulate themselves when high or low fulfillment is experienced. But, does this mechanism operates for all individuals? Does psychological contract fulfillment produce positive effects even on individuals with low regulatory capacities?

In this paper, we test the boundary conditions of self-regulation approaches to the study of psychological contracts by suggesting that some individuals may not be able to exert this regulatory mechanism. In particular, we propose that the model of self-regulation is reasonable for individuals who are able to identify, regulate, and use their emotions (i.e., emotional intelligence) but not necessarily for individuals who have not developed this ability. That is, PC fulfillment should not produce the expected positive effects on individuals with low emotional intelligence. Thus, we test the moderation effect of emotional intelligence on the relationship between PC fulfillment and individual outcomes. More specifically, our model test whether emotional intelligence moderates the mediation effect of PC fulfillment, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intention (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Theoretical model.

With this model, we attempt to make at least two theoretical contributions. First, we test the role of a potentially important (but so far unexplored) individual difference in the PC literature, recognizing the central role of emotion regulation in this phenomenon. Second, we test how emotional intelligence can alter the mediation of PC evaluations, emotional experiences, and behavioral intentions. Thus, we expand the boundary conditions of self-regulation perspectives in the PC literature.

In the following pages, we develop a theory that supports our model, and later we present a study of 247 individuals who responded to electronic surveys using a multiple-times design in an education-based organization. We finally discuss theoretical and managerial implications.

2. Theory development

2.1. PC theory and self-regulation

PC fulfillment is defined as the “employees’ global perceptions that, overall, the organization has fulfilled its terms of the contract in an equitable manner” (Henderson et al., 2008, p. 1,209). Social exchange theory has been the main theoretical base to explain the effects of PC fulfillment at the relational level (Blau, 1964). As individuals experience positive organizational inducements, they are motivated to continue the development of mutually beneficial relationships by reciprocating the organization (i.e., the exchange counterpart) with positive behaviors. At the individual level, however, self-regulation theory has risen recently as a more fine-tuned theory that explains the dynamics of psychological contracts (Rousseau et al., 2018). Self-regulation theory explains human behavior based on the idea of a feedback loop, where individuals monitor the achievement of personal goals, experience emotions caused by their lived experiences, and modify cognitions and behavior toward goal attainment (Lord et al., 2010).

When employment promises are fulfilled, more propitious conditions are perceived by employees, helping them reach valued personal goals, and experience positive feelings about the employment relationship and the convenience of continuing social exchanges in the future (Castanias and Helfat, 1991; Conway and Briner, 2002). The positive affect caused by PC fulfillment and perceptions of goal conduciveness have implications for the perception of exchange relationships as well (Forgas and George, 2001; Isen, 2001). Similarly, emotions play a significant role in the perceptions of fulfillment. PC fulfillment does not only create positive emotions, but it is more likely to be perceived where discrepancies do not generate negative emotions (Kiefer et al., 2022), suggesting that PC fulfillment involves both perceptions and emotions (Conway and Briner, 2002; Rousseau, 2011). Because of the important mediating role of affect in PC theory, a logical extension of these conclusions is that experiences of PC fulfillment should prevent individuals from experiencing negative emotions and exit cognitions, a result that has received meta-analytic support (e.g., Zhao et al., 2007). As Figure 1 suggests, we expect PC fulfillment to be related to turnover intention through the effects of emotional exhaustion, and we developed those relationships in the following paragraphs.

In longer periods of time, PC fulfillment should prevent individuals from having negative work experiences. For instance, previous research has shown that PC fulfillment was negatively related to emotional exhaustion (Gakovic and Tetrick, 2003; Abdalla et al., 2021), defined as the “feelings of being emotionally overextended and depleted of one’s emotional resources” (Maslach and Goldberg, 1998, p. 64). Emotional exhaustion is a phenomenon that occurs after the depletion, over time, of personal psychological resources. We expect to observe that PC fulfillment deters the occurrence of emotional exhaustion because (i) PC fulfillment injects socio-emotional resources into individuals through the effects of goal attainment and (ii) PC fulfillment reduces the need for individuals to focus on creating strategies to reduce discrepancies and obtain valued goals. To support this point, previous PC research has shown that low past PC fulfillment experiences alter present PC fulfillment (Robinson et al., 1994; Coyle-Shapiro and Kessler, 2002), buffering the effects on important outcomes such as burnout (Zhong et al., 2021).

2.2. The moderating role of emotional intelligence

In this article, we argue that the expected effects of self-regulation in PC theory are less likely to occur for individuals who are less capable of identifying and using the additional resources that PC fulfillment brings about. In particular, we study the moderating effect of emotional intelligence on the relationship between PC fulfillment and emotional exhaustion. Although PC research has already acknowledged the role of individual differences before (e.g., Kickul and Lester, 2001; Coyle-Shapiro and Neuman, 2004; Raja et al., 2004; Hermida and Luchman, 2013), emotional intelligence has been rarely studied as a theoretically relevant moderator.

We understand emotional intelligence (EI) as “the ability to carry out accurate reasoning about emotions and the ability to use emotions and emotional knowledge to enhance thought (Mayer et al., 2008).1 EI is a between-person individual difference that has been conceptualized as a multi-dimensional ability including the ability to evaluate own emotions, evaluate the emotions of others, regulate own emotions, and use emotions for goal achievement (Mayer et al., 2016). Because of these abilities, an individual’s EI has been positively associated with work performance (Joseph and Newman, 2010; O'Boyle et al., 2011) and other work criteria such as acute stress, leadership emergence, or negotiation value created (e.g., Elfenbein et al., 2007; Côté et al., 2010; Kotsou et al., 2019; Lea et al., 2019).

Moreover, EI has been a significant mechanism of self-regulation theories. It is well-established that emotional intelligence has a positive effect on emotion regulation (e.g., Laborde et al., 2014; Thomas and Zolkoski, 2020), with documented direct effects on emotional exhaustion as high IE individuals are more likely to exert higher efforts on goal striving processes, injecting positive feelings through goal achievement (e.g., Moon and Hur, 2011; Tziner et al., 2020). In addition to the direct effects of EI, individuals are also likely to interact differently with job-specific situations and contextual factors. This distinct theoretical mechanism of the effects of emotional intelligence has been dubbed the “moderation model” by EI scholars (Côté, 2014; Pradhan and Jena, 2018).

We propose that individuals with high rates of EI have a higher capacity for self-regulation that allows them to interact more positively in exchanges with the organization. First, because of their capacity to regulate their own emotions, a high EI makes individuals less vulnerable to negative disruptions, recovering faster from PC breaches. PC research suggests individuals with low self-control are more likely to be affected by PC breaches (Bal et al., 2011; Restubog et al., 2015; Balogun et al., 2018). Considering that breach is more the norm than the exception in many organizations (Robinson and Rousseau, 1994), high EI individuals are less likely to be affected by small breaches of the PC. That is, high EI may use less time in the repair phase of the PC dynamics (Rousseau et al., 2018). On the other side, the level of reactivity of negative events in individuals with low EI is high (Bechtoldt and Schneider, 2016). Lanciano et al. (2012) found that individuals that scored low EI ruminated less about previous stressful experiences.

Second, it is expected that high EI individuals can use the socio-cognitive resources injected by high PC fulfillment in helping them to better regulate their unpleasant feelings in other domains of the working experience and the consequent effect outside the workplace. In addition, high EI individuals are more aware of unpleasant feelings caused by low PC fulfillment, valuing the fulfillment of employment promises, and creating relief in emotional demands when fulfillment occurs. Previous research has shown that individuals with high EI exert increased attention to positive emotions (Szczygieł and Mikolajczak, 2017; Lea et al., 2019). In contrast, individuals with low EI have more difficulties creating strategies for self-regulation and are less likely to perceive PC fulfillment as a relevant factor that determines goal conduciveness. The burden of emphasizing negative feelings in life events and the need to constantly ruminate about strategies to reach personal goals should affect their levels of emotional exhaustion regardless of the perceptions of the fulfillment of the PC.

These arguments are consistent with a moderation effect of emotional intelligence in the negative relationship between PC fulfillment and emotional exhaustion. The PC fulfillment-emotional exhaustion relationship is expected to be observed for individuals with high EI, but not for individuals with low EI (no significant relationship; a neutralization effect). Thus, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 1: Emotional intelligence moderates the negative relationship between psychological contract fulfillment and emotional exhaustion, such that the effect is stronger for individuals with high emotional intelligence.

Previous research has provided evidence for a significant relationship between emotional exhaustion and turnover cognitions and behaviors (e.g., Boles et al., 1997; Wright and Cropanzano, 1998; Karatepe and Uludag, 2007; Giao et al., 2020). Emotional exhaustion is a personal experience of energy depletion which in many cases is solved by exiting the situation that provokes that state. At the same time, the PC literature has long sustained the mediating effect of emotional experiences in the relationship between PC evaluations (breach/fulfillment) and turnover intentions. For example, in an SEM meta-analysis, Zhao et al. (2007) showed that a model with emotions as a mediator had the best fit with the data when explaining how PC evaluations impact employee outcomes, such as turnover intentions. The dynamic model of PC processes also conceptualized negative affect as an important mediator of PC evaluations and exit behaviors [See Figure 5 in Rousseau et al. (2018)]. Basically, individuals who experience negative PC evaluations are more likely to search for different employment or occupational options in order to achieve self-set goals, taking emotion as key information to create appropriate behavioral responses (Kiefer and Antoni, 2019). These relationships suggest a mediation effect where PC fulfillment predicts turnover intentions through the effects of emotional exhaustion.

This mediation effect, however, is likely to be affected by the emotional abilities of individuals. As suggested in hypothesis 1, we expect to observe high levels of emotional exhaustion for individuals with low EI in general, regardless of their perceptions of PC fulfillment. Thus, the mediation effect of PC fulfillment, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intentions is expected to occur for individuals high in EI, but not necessarily for individuals low in EI.

Hypothesis 2: The mediating role of emotional exhaustion in the relationship between psychological contract fulfillment and turnover intention is moderated by employee emotional intelligence, with the effect being more substantial for individuals with high emotional intelligence.

3. Materials and methods

3.1. Data collection

To test our hypotheses, we collected quantitative data as part of a larger study about psychological contracts in teams. Participants were recruited from 8 educational institutions and one administrative department of a well-known municipality in Chile. We received support to conduct this study from the municipality as part of a series of organizational initiatives to improve HR management practices. The data was obtained using two-wave self-administered web-based surveys, which were separated by a lag time of 2 months between each survey. This was intended to reduce the potential of common method variance (Podsakoff et al., 2003). In the first survey, we collected PC fulfillment and emotional intelligence, whereas, in the second survey, we collected data about emotional exhaustion and turnover intentions. The data was matched by the research team using employee IDs, following recommendations of our Institutional Ethics Committee to protect participants’ confidentiality.

The HR department provided the updated personnel records of the organization, including emails from 247 employees. Both surveys were distributed to their email addresses through Qualtrics’ mailing tool. The sample size was 181 responses for the first survey and 176 responses for the follow-up survey. In sum, the sample consisted of 150 matched responses, including employees who answered both surveys. The final sample only included subjects who successfully completed the two surveys (137 individuals) and who successfully answered an attention check item (we eliminated 23 responses due to lack of attention). This decision improved the quality of the collected data, providing a final sample of 114 responses. Our final sample size was lower than we expected, although high enough to make a rigorous test of our main hypotheses. All employees were between 25 and 64 years old (Mean = 39.16 years, S.D. = 9.89). Most participants were female (82%) and the tenure ranged from 0.16 to 35 years (Mean = 7.55 years, S.D. = 8.00).

3.2. Measures

All measures of this study were applied using 5-point Likert scales (1: Completely disagree – 5: Completely agree). The surveys were applied in their Spanish versions, which can all be found in escalas.unegocios.cl. Psychological contract fulfillment was captured using Robinson and Morrison’s (2000) classic scale of global PC fulfillment. To better align our theory to our measure, we only used the fulfillment items, eliminating two negatively worded items, addressing previous concerns that Robinson and Morrison’s measure capture both fulfillment and breach (Conway and Pekcan, 2019). This procedure was already utilized in Laulié (2017) in a similar population, showing excellent psychometric properties. A sample item is “Almost all of the promises made to me by my employer have been kept so far.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93. To capture emotional intelligence, we used an ability-based EI instrument developed by Law et al. (2004) and validated in Spanish by Madrid (2020). The instrument contemplates four dimensions, including recognition of own emotions, recognition of emotions in others, regulation of own emotions, and use of emotions. An item example is “I have good control of my emotions.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87. Emotional exhaustion was captured using an 8-item subscale of the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory and validated by Halbesleben and Demerouti (2005). A previous Spanish version of this scale was used by Laulié et al. (2022). An item example is “During my work, I often feel emotionally drained.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86. Finally, turnover intention was captured by participants using a 3-item scale developed by Cammann et al. (1983). A sample item is “I often seriously think about quitting.” Alpha was 0.85. All descriptive statistics are included in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations.

3.3. Analyses

First, we used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to evaluate our measurement model. To test model fit, we used typical indexes and some of the commonly used standards in the literature (CFI > 0.90, RMSEA<0.08, SRMR<0.08). Our theoretical model was then compared against alternative measurement models. Second, to test our main hypotheses, we used structural equation modeling (SEM). This approach can reduce bias in the calculation of estimates in a mediation or moderation analysis by managing the effects of measurement error (see Ledgerwood and Shrout, 2011). For hypothesis 1, we used a full indicator method for latent interaction constructs (Marsh et al., 2013; Collier, 2020). For this method, we created interaction terms for each pair of mean-centred indicators of the independent and moderator variables (PC fulfillment and emotional intelligence, respectively), which then serve as indicators of a latent interaction unobserved construct. Third, to test hypothesis 2, we calculated an index of moderated mediation (Hayes, 2015; Hayes et al., 2017) and observed the indirect effects of PC fulfillment on turnover intentions through the effects of emotional intelligence depending on the level of the moderator variable (emotional intelligence). All analyses were carried out using SPSS, AMOS, and the PROCESS macro.

4. Results

4.1. Confirmatory factor analysis

We first examined the measurement model that form our key constructs (PC fulfillment, emotional intelligence, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intention). The hypothesized model (see Table 2) indicates an overall excellent fit of a four-factor model (χ2 = 111.402, df = 107, CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.019; SRMR = 0.076). Then, we tested several alternative models, merging some latent constructs (models 2 to 6 in Table 2). All the alternatives resulted in a worse fit to the data. These results suggest that individuals differentiate the four constructs, providing evidence for discriminant validity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analysis.

4.2. Hypothesis testing

We also created a structural equation model to evaluate our hypothesized relationships. These results are summarized in Table 3. The initial model containing only the predictor, mediator, and outcome shows an excellent fit to the data (χ2 = 50.282, df = 56, CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.00; SRMR = 0.055) and provides evidence to confirm some of the expected relationships. PC fulfillment (β = −0.20, p < 0.10) was significantly related to emotional exhaustion, with higher fulfillment reducing the levels of emotional exhaustion. At the same time, emotional exhaustion was significantly related to turnover intentions (β = 0.28, p < 0.01). These results are aligned with PC theory and previous findings in the literature.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. SEM hypothesis testing.

Hypothesis 1 predicted a moderation effect of emotional intelligence on the relationship between PC fulfillment and emotional exhaustion. Our results show that, after including emotional intelligence as a predictor of emotional exhaustion (β = −0.88, p < 0.01), the latent interaction construct was significantly related to emotional exhaustion (β = −0.81, p < 0.05), providing evidence to support our hypothesis. Figure 2 plots the shape of the moderation effect. As hypothesized, when emotional intelligence is high, there is a negative relationship between PC fulfillment and emotional exhaustion. In contrast, when emotional intelligence is low, the relationship is neutralized (not significant). Individuals with low emotional intelligence presented on average high levels of emotional exhaustion. This evidence supports hypothesis 1.23

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Moderation effect of emotional intelligence on PC fulfillment and emotional exhaustion.

Hypothesis 2 predicted a moderated mediation effect of emotional intelligence on the mediation of PC fulfillment, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intention. Table 4 shows the main results of this test. When emotional intelligence is low or average, the indirect effects were not significantly different from zero. In contrast, when emotional intelligence was high, the indirect effect was significant. The index of moderated mediation was also significantly different from zero, providing evidence to support hypothesis 2.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Test of moderated mediation.

5. Discussion

In this article, we proposed that emotional intelligence is a relevant individual difference that can explain the mediation of PC fulfillment, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intention. Our results support that proposition, posing interesting implications for future research.

Considering emotional intelligence as an ability that has been related to self-regulation capacities, our study provides a reasonable way to empirically test the explanatory power that self-regulation theory has on explaining how psychological contracts operate in individuals. Our results suggest that PC fulfillment is directly related to outcomes such as emotional exhaustion and turnover intentions, but only for individuals with high regulatory capacities. That is, the socio-emotional resources that PC fulfillment injects may not be sufficient for individuals with low emotional intelligence, who display higher levels of emotional exhaustion regardless of their perceptions of PC fulfillment. Thus, our results provide interesting boundary conditions of Rousseau et al. (2018) theory of PC and self-regulation.

From the point of view of social exchange theory, our results also suggest that social exchanges may be more productive for individuals with high emotional intelligence. Although decades of research have shown that creating trust in social exchanges is a ubiquitous phenomenon in social psychology (e.g., Colquitt et al., 2007), our results show that there may be individuals who are more apt to establish more beneficial social exchanges with other parties than others. These individuals are more likely to perceive organizational inducements as positive resources that can benefit their welfare. In contrast, individuals with high emotional intelligence are also more likely to exit poor exchanges. Our test of moderated mediation suggests that only individuals with high emotional intelligence are more likely to have higher turnover intentions when experiencing low PC fulfillment. That is, these individuals are more likely to exit relationships that are not fulfilling or beneficial for them. In addition, future research should explore whether other variables that could be markers of self-regulation abilities (different than emotional intelligence) may create similar boundary conditions of the effects of PC fulfillment on relevant employee outcomes.

A puzzling possibility that our findings suggest, is that as emotionally intelligent individuals are more likely to identify beneficial or detrimental relationships with the organization, they may also affect the perception of other individuals in the same group of workers (e.g., Lopes et al., 2004; Jamshed and Majeed, 2019). Based on theories of social influence, PC theory has suggested in the past that PC evaluations can be shared by specific individuals (Ho, 2005; Ho and Levesque, 2005) or teams as a whole (Laulié and Tekleab, 2016; Tekleab et al., 2020). Future research should explore whether emotionally intelligent individuals are more likely to influence other individuals’ perceptions of the PC (Griep et al., 2019).

Our findings have additional implications for the PC literature. For instance, a plausible explanation of some of the weak effects of PC fulfillment on outcomes may be the result of having samples with individuals with lower emotional intelligence. For example, Zhao et al. (2007) found a small relationship between PC breach and individual performance. We speculate that that relationship should be stronger for highly emotionally intelligent individuals. We recommend future research to use emotional intelligence as a relevant control for future PC research.

Our results also show interesting and unexpected results. Although our main goal was to contrast individuals with high vs. low emotional intelligence, the results show that the mediation of PC fulfillment, emotional exhaustion, and turnover intention was not significant for average emotional intelligence. Future research should investigate whether this result was due to a lack of power or some idiosyncratic characteristic of our sample, or whether it is a more generalizable property of the effects of PC evaluations. Similarly, future research should replicate these results in samples from other countries or industries in order to study its external validity.

We would like to finish this article by acknowledging some strengths and limitations. First, our final sample size was lower than we expected, although high enough to make a rigorous test of our main hypotheses. Second, we were not able to control for job characteristics or other individual differences that may interact with our key constructs. Future research may expand this possibility. Third, our sample and methodology have several strengths worth mentioning, such as the use of attention checks to improve the quality of our data, and the use of multiple surveys to reduce the potential problem of common method bias. We believe this study makes several contributions to the PC literature.

6. Conclusion

Representing the employment “deal in the mind,” PCs have been theorized to have powerful effects on employee outcomes through self-regulation processes that are activated when high or low fulfillment is experienced. In this paper, we explored whether this theoretical mechanism operates for all individuals. Our results show that the answer is “no.” Individuals with low emotional intelligence seem to be unaffected by positive or negative evaluations of the PC. We hope our results expand the PC literature and encourage future research to better understand how individual differences change the experience of the employment relationship.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/csdk8/?view_only=f690e5bc4a454573a1672ef30f857e01.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité de Ética de la Facultad de Economía y Negocios de la Universidad de Chile. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

Using CRedIT Taxonomy, the authors declare the following contributions: LL and GH-G: conceptualization. GB-J: data curation and software. GB-J, LL, and GH-G: formal analysis and investigation. LL: methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, and original draft. LL and GB-J: validation and visualization. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

LL would like to thank the support of the Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo de Chile, ANID FONDECYT/INI 11190044.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^We take this definition in contrast to other trait-based models (Petrides and Furnham, 2003; Petrides, 2011).

2. ^As a robustness check, we tested this model with and without other controls (age, tenure, gender), but the results were not altered by the inclusion of controls (these results are also available upon request to the corresponding author).

3. ^An alternative explanation would be that individuals with low emotional intelligence tend to see personal situations as more negative, and thus this effect may create a negative bias in perceptions of PC fulfillment. This possibility seems less plausible as the correlation of PC fulfillment and emotional intelligence is low and not significant (Table 1).

References

Abdalla, M. J., Said, H., Ali, L., Ali, F., and Chen, X. (2021). COVID-19 and unpaid leave: impacts of psychological contract breach on organizational distrust and turnover intention: mediating role of emotional exhaustion. Tour. Manag. 39, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2021.100854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bal, P. M., Chiaburu, D. S., and Diaz, I. (2011). Does psychological contract breach decrease proactive behaviors? The moderating effect of emotion regulation. Group Organ. Manag. 36, 722–758. doi: 10.1177/10596011114235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balogun, A. G., Oluyemi, T. S., and Afolabi, O. A. (2018). Psychological contract breach and workplace deviance: does emotional intelligence matter? J. Psychol A. 28, 8–14. doi: 10.1080/14330237.2018.1426808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechtoldt, M. N., and Schneider, V. K. (2016). Predicting stress from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings: emotional intelligence and testosterone jointly predict cortisol reactivity. Emotion 16, 815–825. doi: 10.1037/emo0000134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Google Scholar

Boles, J. S., Johnston, M. W., and Hair, J. F. Jr. (1997). Role stress, work-family conflict and emotional exhaustion: inter-relationships and effects on some work-related consequences. J. of Pers. Sell. Sales Manag. 17, 17–28. doi: 10.1080/08853134.1997.10754079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cammann, C., Fichman, M., Jenkins, D., and Klesh, J. (1983). “Overall job satisfaction,” in Taking the measures of work: A guide to validate scales for organizational research and diagnosis. ed. D. L. Fields (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications).

Google Scholar

Castanias, R. P., and Helfat, C. E. (1991). Managerial resources and rents. J. Manag. 17, 155–171. doi: 10.1177/014920639101700110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, J. E. (2020). Applied structural equation modeling using AMOS: Basic to advanced techniques. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., and LePine, J. A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: a meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk-taking and job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 909–927. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, N., and Briner, R. B. (2002). Full-time versus part-time employees: understanding the links between work status, the psychological contract, and attitudes. J. Voc. Behav. 61, 279–301. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.2001.1857

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, N., and Pekcan, C. (2019). “Psychological contract research: older, but is it wiser?” in Handbook of research on the psychological contract at work. eds. Y. Griep and C. Cooper (Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing Inc.), 10–34.

Google Scholar

Côté, S. (2014). Emotional intelligence in organizations. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 1, 459–488. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Côté, S., Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., and Miners, C. T. H. (2010). Emotional intelligence and leadership emergence in small groups. Leadership. Q. 21, 496–508. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.03.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyle-Shapiro, J. A. M., and Kessler, I. (2002). Exploring reciprocity through the lens of the psychological contract: employee and employer perspectives. Eur. J. of Work and Organ. Psychol. 11, 69–86. doi: 10.1080/13594320143000852

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyle-Shapiro, J. A., and Neuman, J. H. (2004). The psychological contract and individual differences: the role of exchange and creditor ideologies. J. Voc. Behav. 64, 150–164. doi: 10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00031-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elfenbein, H. A., Foo, M. D., White, J. B., Tan, H. H., and Aik, V. C. (2007). Reading your counterpart: the benefit of emotion recognition ability for effectiveness in negotiation. J. Nonverbal Behav. 31, 205–223. doi: 10.1007/s10919-007-0033-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P., and George, J. M. (2001). Affective influences on judgments and behavior in organizations: an information processing perspective. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 86, 3–34. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2001.2971

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gakovic, A., and Tetrick, L. E. (2003). Psychological contract breach as a source of strain for employees. J. Bus. Psychol. 18, 235–246. doi: 10.1023/A:1027301232116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giao, H. N. K., Vuong, B. N., Huan, D. D., Tushar, H., and Quan, T. N. (2020). The effect of emotional intelligence on turnover intention and the moderating role of perceived organizational support: evidence from the banking industry of Vietnam. Sustainability 12:1857. doi: 10.3390/su12051857

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griep, Y., Cooper, C., Robinson, S., Robinson, D. M., Hansen, S. D., Tomprou, M., et al. (2019). “Psychological contracts: Back to the future,” in Handbook of research on the psychological contract at work. eds. Y. Griep and C. Cooper (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar).

Google Scholar

Halbesleben, J. R., and Demerouti, E. (2005). The construct validity of an alternative measure of burnout: investigating the english translation of the Oldenburg burnout inventory. Work Stress. 19, 208–220. doi: 10.1080/02678370500340728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behav. Res. 50, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., Montoya, A. K., and Rockwood, N. J. (2017). The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modeling. Aus. Marketing. J. 25, 76–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ausmj.2017.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, D. J., Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., Bommer, W. H., and Tetrick, L. E. (2008). Leader-member exchange, differentiation, and psychological contract fulfillment: a multilevel examination. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 1208–1219. doi: 10.1037/a0012678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermida, R., and Luchman, J. N. (2013). The moderating role of locus of causality in the relationship between source of information and psychological contract breach perceptions. J. Bus. Psychol. 28, 221–232. doi: 10.1007/s10869-012-9274-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, V. T. (2005). Social influence on evaluations of psychological contract fulfillment. Acad. Manag. Rev. 30, 113–128. doi: 10.5465/amr.2005.15281438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, V. T., and Levesque, L. L. (2005). With a little help from my friends (and substitutes): social referents and influence in psychological contract fulfillment. Organ. Science 16, 275–289. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1050.0121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isen, A. M. (2001). An influence of positive affect on decision making in complex situations: theoretical issues with practical implications. J. Cons. Psychol. 11, 75–85. doi: 10.1207/S15327663JCP1102_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamshed, S., and Majeed, N. (2019). Relationship between team culture and team performance through lens of knowledge sharing and team emotional intelligence. J. of Know. Manag. 23, 90–109. doi: 10.1108/JKM-04-2018-0265

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, D. L., and Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 54–78. doi: 10.1037/a0017286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karatepe, O. M., and Uludag, O. (2007). Conflict, exhaustion, and motivation: a study of frontline employees in northern Cyprus hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 26, 645–665. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2006.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kickul, J., and Lester, S. W. (2001). Broken promises: equity sensitivity as a moderator between psychological contract breach and employee attitudes and behavior. J. Bus. Psychol. 16, 191–217. doi: 10.1023/A:1011105132252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, T., and Antoni, A. (2019). “Putting emotion first—an emotion-centred approach to understanding the psychological contract,” in Handbook of research on the psychological contract at work. eds. Y. Griep and C. Cooper (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing).

Google Scholar

Kiefer, T., Barclay, L. J., Conway, N., and Briner, R. B. (2022). An event-based approach to psychological contracts: the importance of examining everyday broken and fulfilled promises as discrete events. J. Organ. Behav. 43, 1377–1395. doi: 10.1002/job.2656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotsou, I., Mikolajczak, M., Heeren, A., Grégoire, J., and Leys, C. (2019). Improving emotional intelligence: a systematic review of existing work and future challenges. Emotion Rev. 11, 151–165. doi: 10.1177/1754073917735902

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Lautenbach, F., Allen, M. S., Herbert, C., and Achtzehn, S. (2014). The role of trait emotional intelligence in emotion regulation and performance under pressure. Pers. Individ. Diff. 57, 43–47. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanciano, T., Curci, A., Kafetsios, K., Elia, L., and Zammuner, V. L. (2012). Attachment and dysfunctional rumination: the mediating role of emotional intelligence abilities. Pers. Individ. Diff. 53, 753–758. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laulié, L. (2017). Toward a better understanding of psychological contract fulfillment (pcf) at the team level. Detroit: Wayne State University.

Google Scholar

Laulié, L., Briceño, G., and Pavez, I. (2022). Does “good acting requires a good cast?” a replication study of how team deep acting dispersion influence individual outcomes. Estudios de Administración 29, 66–80. doi: 10.5354/0719-0816.2022.66826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laulié, L., and Tekleab, A. G. (2016). A multi-level theory of psychological contract fulfillment in teams. Group Organ. Manag. 41, 658–698. doi: 10.1177/1059601116668972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., and Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 483–496. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lea, R. G., Davis, S. K., Mahoney, B., and Qualter, P. (2019). Does emotional intelligence buffer the effects of acute stress? A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledgerwood, A., and Shrout, P. E. (2011). The trade-off between accuracy and precision in latent variable models of mediation processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 1174–1188. doi: 10.1037/a0024776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J. B., Schütz, A., Sellin, I., and Salovey, P. (2004). Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Pers. Soc. Psych. Bull. 30, 1018–1034. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, R. G., Diefendorff, J. M., Schmidt, A. M., and Hall, R. J. (2010). Self-regulation at work. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 543–568. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madrid, H. P. (2020). Escala de inteligencia emocional WLEIS en Español. available at: https://www.hectormadrid.cl/materiales-de-investigacion (accessed November 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., Hau, K. T., and Nagengast, B. (2013). “Structural equation models of latent interaction and quadratic effects” in Structural equation modeling: A second course, eds. G. R. Hancock and, R. O. Mueller Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C., and Goldberg, J. (1998). Prevention of burnout: new perspectives. Appl. Prev. Psychol. 7, 63–74. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(98)80022-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Salovey, P. (2016). The ability model of emotional intelligence: principles and updates. Emot. Rev. 8, 290–300. doi: 10.1177/1754073916639667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R., and Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abilities: emotional intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 507–536. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moon, T. W., and Hur, W. M. (2011). Emotional intelligence, emotional exhaustion, and job performance. Soc. Behav. Pers. 39, 1087–1096. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.8.1087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Boyle, E. H., Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., Hawver, T. H., and Story, P. A. (2011). The relation between emotional intelligence and job performance: a meta-analysis. J. Organ. Behav. 32, 788–818. doi: 10.1002/job.714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V. (2011). “Ability and trait emotional intelligence,” in The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences, eds. T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. Stummvon, and A. Furnham (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell Publishing), 656–678.

Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., and Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: behavioural validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. Eur. J. Personal. 17, 39–57. doi: 10.1002/per.466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., Mac Kenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pradhan, S., and Jena, L. K. (2018). Emotional intelligence as a moderator in abusive supervision-intention to quit relationship among Indian healthcare professionals. Asia-Pac. J. Bus. Adm. 10, 35–49. doi: 10.1108/APJBA-09-2017-0089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raja, S., Johns, G., and Ntalianis, F. (2004). The impact of personality on psychological contracts. Acad. Manag. J. 47, 350–367. doi: 10.5465/20159586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restubog, S. L. D., Zagenczyk, T. J., Bordia, P., Bordia, S., and Chapman, G. J. (2015). If you wrong us, shall we not revenge? Moderating roles of self-control and perceived aggressive work culture in predicting responses to psychological contract breach. J. Manag. 41, 1132–1154. doi: 10.1177/0149206312443557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. L., Kraatz, M. S., and Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Changing obligations and the psychological contract: a longitudinal study. Acad. Manag. J. 37, 137–152. doi: 10.5465/256773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. L., and Morrison, W. E. (2000). The development of psychological contract breach and violation: a longitudinal study. J. Organ. Behav. 21, 525–546. doi: 10.1002/1099-1379(200008)21:5%3C525::AID-JOB40%3E3.0.CO;2-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. L., and Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Violating the psychological contract: not the exception but the norm. J. Organ. Behav. 15, 245–259. doi: 10.1002/job.4030150306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, D. M. (2011). “The individual–organization relationship: the psychological contract,” in APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. ed. S. Zedeck (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association).

Google Scholar

Rousseau, D. M., Hansen, S. D., and Tomprou, M. (2018). A dynamic phase model of psychological contract processes. J. Organ. Behav. 39, 1081–1098. doi: 10.1002/job.2284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szczygieł, D., and Mikolajczak, M. (2017). Why are people high in emotional intelligence happier? They make the most of their positive emotions. Pers. Individ. Diff. 117, 177–181. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tekleab, A. G., Laulié, L., De Vos, A., De Jong, J. P., and Coyle-Shapiro, J. A. (2020). Contextualizing psychological contracts research: a multi-sample study of shared individual psychological contract fulfilment. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 29, 279–293. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2019.1608294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C., and Zolkoski, S. (2020). Preventing stress among undergraduate learners: the importance of emotional intelligence, resilience, and emotion regulation. Front. Educ. 5, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2020.00094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tziner, A., Fein, E. C., Kim, S. K., Vasiliu, C., and Shkoler, O. (2020). Combining associations between emotional intelligence, work motivation, and organizational justice with counterproductive work behavior: a profile analysis via multidimensional scaling (PAMS) approach. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, T. A., and Cropanzano, R. (1998). Emotional exhaustion as a predictor of job performance and voluntary turnover. J. Appl. Psychol. 83, 486–493. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H. A. O., Wayne, S. J., Glibkowski, B. C., and Bravo, J. (2007). The impact of psychological contract breach on work-related outcomes: a meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 60, 647–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00087.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, M., Wayne, S. J., and Michel, E. J. (2021). When the past and the present collide: contrast effect of sequential psychological contract breaches on employee outcomes. J. Manag. 49, 913–943. doi: 10.1177/01492063211061255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychological contract fulfillment, emotional intelligence, self-regulation, emotional exhaustion, turnover intention, structural equation modeling

Citation: Laulié L, Briceño-Jiménez G and Henríquez-Gómez G (2023) Exploring self-regulation theory as a mechanism of the effects of psychological contract fulfillment: The role of emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 14:1090094. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1090094

Received: 04 November 2022; Accepted: 13 March 2023;
Published: 30 March 2023.

Edited by:

Junhua Dang, Uppsala University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Sulphey M. M., Prince Sattam Bin Abdulaziz University, Saudi Arabia
Denghao Zhang, Renmin University of China, China

Copyright © 2023 Laulié, Briceño-Jiménez and Henriquez-Gómez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lyonel Laulié, llaulie@fen.uchile.cl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.