Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2023
Sec. Performance Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Highlights in Performance Science: Music Performance Anxiety View all 15 articles

It’s not a virus! Reconceptualizing and de-pathologizing music performance anxiety

  • 1Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Mount Royal Conservatory, Calgary, AB, Canada

Music Performance Anxiety (MPA) is one of the most widespread and debilitating challenges facing musicians, affecting significant numbers of performers in terms of both their personal and professional functioning. Although numerous interventions exist to target MPA, its prevalence remains unchanged since the first large-scale studies of the 1980s, indicating that available interventions are having limited impact. This review synthesizes and critiques existing literature in order to investigate possible reasons for the limited efficacy of current approaches to managing MPA. Key concepts discussed include conceptual and methodological challenges surrounding defining MPA, theoretical perspectives on MPA’s etiology and manifestation, and the coping strategies and interventions used to manage MPA. MPA has predominantly been investigated pathologically and defined as a negative construct manifesting in unwanted symptoms. Based on this conceptualization, interventions largely seek to manage MPA through ameliorating symptoms. This review discusses possible reasons why this approach has broadly not proved successful, including the issue of relaxation being both unrealistic and counterproductive for peak performance, issues associated with intentionally changing one’s state creating resistance thus exacerbating anxiety, and focusing on the presence of, rather than response to, symptoms. Despite 50 years of research, MPA remains an unsolved enigma and continues to adversely impact musicians both on and off the stage. Reconceptualizing MPA as a normal and adaptive response to the pressures of performance may offer a new perspective on it, in terms of its definition, assessment and management, with practical as well as theoretical implications.

1. Introduction

Public perception associates musical performance with entertainment, pleasure and relaxation (Hildebrandt et al., 2012). Indeed, the stage can be a place of creative freedom and artistic excellence, eliciting incredibly positive, even euphoric, states (Kenny, 2011). However, although musicians report higher levels of job satisfaction than other occupational groups (Harper, 2002), significant numbers experience occupation-related physical and psychological health problems that interfere with their personal and/or professional functioning, one of the most prevalent and debilitating of which is music performance anxiety (Fernholz et al., 2019).

Music Performance Anxiety (MPA) occurs in response to perceived threat and manifests physiologically, cognitively, emotionally, and behaviorally, ranging in intensity from normal stress intrinsic to performance to extreme levels of terror (Steptoe, 2001; Burin and Osório, 2017). MPA does not occur in isolation but interacts with stressors associated with a highly demanding, competitive and insecure work environment (Vervainioti and Alexopoulos, 2015) as well as numerous personality traits, including perfectionism, trait anxiety, cognitive style and coping style (Burin et al., 2019).

Experiencing acute anxiety can be psychologically distressing; over time, chronic anxiety can affect all aspects of performers’ lives, including wellbeing, identity, self-worth and relationships (Kenny, 2011; Burin and Osório, 2017; Matei et al., 2018). In extremis, MPA may lead to post-traumatic stress disorders (Moura and Serra, 2021). Research indicates that prolonged neuroendocrine activation and overstimulation of the sympathetic nervous system can codetermine, or even cause, diseases including heart attacks and strokes (Benson and Klipper, 2009; Gomez et al., 2018). Alongside threatening performers’ health and quality of life, MPA can reduce performance enjoyment, cause performance avoidance and impair performance quality (Kenny, 2011). At its most severe, MPA can constitute an occupational disability, terminating studies and careers (Orejudo et al., 2017; Fernholz et al., 2019). Sternbach (1993) describes the toxicity of MPA as making life preceding an important performance an “unremitting purgatory” for both performers, and those around them.

Given MPA’s potentially devastating effects on musicians’ lives and/or careers (Nagel, 2010), it is unsurprisingly one of the most widely researched topics in the fields of performance science and performing arts medicine, and there is now a substantial body of scholarship investigating MPA across a wide range of populations from a diverse range of theoretical and methodological perspectives. You may be wondering why on earth we need YET ANOTHER theoretical review of MPA. Despite decades of research and the development of numerous interventions, MPA’s prevalence remains unchanged since the first large-scale studies in the 1980s (Fishbein et al., 1988; Fernholz et al., 2019), indicating that available interventions are not having a meaningful impact. MPA continues to pose a significant occupational challenge to musicians of all ages, genres, and nationalities, suggesting that gaps remain in our understanding of it (Fernholz et al., 2019). This theoretical review aims to explore possible reasons why MPA remains an unsolved enigma. Sections 2–6 will synthesize and critique existing research across the key concepts and issues in MPA research: Prevalence (2), Definitions (3), Etiology (4), Manifestation (5) and Management (6). Section 7 (Discussion) will then summarize current conceptualizations of MPA, before suggesting a paradigmatic reconceptualization.

2. Prevalence

MPA is described as ubiquitous (Nagel, 2010), an epidemic (McGrath, 2012) and universal (Brandfonbrener, 1999). But just how widespread really is MPA? Studies investigating prevalence among student and professional musicians report figures between 24 and 96%, indicating that MPA affects significant, yet ambiguous, numbers of musicians. While it is clear that MPA poses a serious challenge to students and professionals alike, the variability of findings casts little light on the issue of prevalence, precluding a clear understanding of the scale of the issue. This ambiguity can be attributed to methodological limitations within studies and heterogeneity across studies (Brugués, 2011a), as summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. MPA prevalence among conservatoire students and professional musicians including key methodological characteristics of the studies.

Most problematic are issues surrounding definitions and measurement tools (see Table 1). Unsurprisingly, prevalence varies enormously depending on the definition criteria used (Osborne and Kirsner, 2022). There is no consensus regarding what actually constitutes MPA, both in terms of conceptual clarity within, and heterogenous terminology across, studies. The diversity of terms used, as well as the very different constructs they represent, makes it impossible to know how many musicians experience the same phenomenon. This ambiguity is reflected in the diverse range of measures deployed. Within studies, the lack of a clear operational definition makes it impossible to know what exactly is being measured; across studies, the heterogenous assessment tools used preclude comparison between studies, making it impossible to build a strong evidence base (Kenny et al., 2014; Gembris et al., 2018).

MPA poses a major occupational challenge to significant numbers of musicians at all stages of professional development. However, if prevalence ranges from 24% to ubiquitous, it seems evident we are not all talking about the same phenomenon. Why does this matter? We suggest that the divergence regarding how many musicians experience MPA points to a more fundamental issue – how can we help musicians to manage a thing without knowing what that thing is? A phenomenon which affects a quarter of a particular population has very different implications than a phenomenon which affects all of a population. Arguably, the lack of a universal definition of MPA precludes comprehensive understanding, and thus effective management, of MPA. We now therefore turn to the challenge of defining MPA.

3. Definitions and theoretical perspectives

Performance anxiety, like pornography, is easily recognised but difficult to define.”

This quote by Lederman (1999, p. 117) illustrates a fundamental issue in MPA research – musicians and researchers alike know, intuitively, what MPA is, yet the phenomenon still lacks a common definition (Fernholz et al., 2019). This section will discuss the key challenges surrounding defining MPA: the heterogeneity of terms used to label the phenomenon, and the diverse conceptual constructs these terms represent.

3.1. Terminology

Across literature, different terms are used interchangeably and with conflicting hierarchies of severity (Studer et al., 2011). Pre-2009, authors largely created their own label and definition, applying only to their own study, precluding comparability of data and the formation of a coherent MPA theory. Some authors consider stage fright the most extreme manifestation of performance anxiety (Fishbein et al., 1988; Steptoe, 1989; Brodsky, 1996; Senyshyn, 1999), whereas others place stage fright below performance anxiety on their continuum of severity (Fehm and Schmidt, 2006). Van Kemenade et al. (1995) investigate performance anxiety that affects personal and/or professional functioning and (James, 1998) measures only anxiety which adversely impacts performance, which is both vague and problematic. That stage fright has been considered both more AND less severe than MPA by different researchers adds further confusion to the prevalence statistics reported in Table 1.

Since 2009, researchers have increasingly adopted the definition offered by Kenny (2009) (see Section 3.3). However, discrepancies remain across the literature (Osborne and Kirsner, 2022), inhibiting understanding of the construct being investigated and posing a real challenge to the field in terms of methodological rigor, diagnostic criteria, evaluating prevalence and severity and designing appropriate interventions. In addition to heterogenous terminology, there is divergence regarding the nature of the phenomenon these terms are labeling. We now turn to various conceptualizations of MPA: negative, positive, both and intrinsic to performance. For clarity, this review will only use the term ‘MPA’.

3.2. Conceptualizations of MPA

3.2.1. MPA as negative

MPA is largely researched through a pathological lens (Pecen et al., 2016) and described using medicalized language: ‘condition’ (Barros et al., 2022; Moreno-Gutierréz et al., 2023), ‘syndrome’ (Lederman, 1999), ‘disabling’ (Brantigan et al., 1979; Lederman, 1999), ‘illness’ (Harper, 2002), ‘disease’ (Sousa et al., 2016; Fernholz et al., 2019), and ‘disorder’ (Kenny, 2011; Fernholz et al., 2019; Mumm et al., 2020), requiring ‘treatment’ (Spahn, 2015). The International Classification of Diseases defines performance anxiety as a specific phobia (ICD-10; Dilling and Freyberger, 2015), and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Psychiatric Disorders (DSM 5; Falkai et al., 2015) codes it as a performance-only subtype of social anxiety disorder (SAD) (both cited in Mumm et al., 2020, p. 76–77).

Although some studies indicate correlations between MPA and aspects of SAD (Clark and Agras, 1991; Osborne and Kenny, 2005), recent evidence suggests that MPA and SAD are conceptually and empirically unrelated (Wiedemann et al., 2021). It appears that although MPA and SAD can manifest comorbidly and share similar aspects, they are distinct constructs (Bögels et al., 2010) and SAD is arguably not a useful diagnostic classification (Ruggiero, 2012), even in more extreme manifestations of MPA. In addition to a lack of empirical support for a connection between SAD and MPA, there are conceptual issues with linking the two constructs.

Although fear of evaluation is common to both MPA and the social anxiety disorders MPA tends to be diagnostically clustered with (Hays, 2009), there are arguably qualitative differences between performing and other social situations in terms of the physical and psychological challenges involved in performance (Kenny, 2011; Chang-Arana et al., 2018) as illustrated by the following quote:

“It is one thing to avoid cocktail parties (or funerals) because you feel uncomfortable in the presence of others. It is quite another, however, to diagnose a tenor as having a social phobia as a result of the following: He functions quite well in most social situations but feels at least momentarily daunted anticipating the thousand pairs of eyes and ears watching and listening as he opens his mouth to sing "Comfort Ye, My People," the initial vocal moment in Handel's Messiah” (Hays, 2009 p. 105).

Conflating the two situations risks pathologizing an entirely normal response to the well documented demands of performance (Antonini Philippe et al., 2022) and the very real risks of negative evaluation in musical studies and careers: When dependent on the positive opinion of audition panels, colleagues or audiences to pay one’s mortgage, fear of evaluation arguably seems not so unreasonable!

3.2.2. MPA as positive

An alternative approach posits that MPA is facilitative and enhances performance by generating extra vitality, excitement, alertness, focus and spontaneity (Hamann and Sobaje, 1983; Nagel, 1993; Roland, 1994; Kantor-Martynuska et al., 2018) (See Gannon (2019) for discussion of how MPA can facilitate performance). This quote by virtuoso Andalusian guitarist Pepe Romero illustrates how experiences commonly associated with anxiety can be experienced positively:

“The feeling you get backstage…butterflies in your stomach…your knees shake so much that you can hardly stand, and…you feel like you’re about to throw up?? I LOVE that!! It’s because we’re like a rechargeable battery: those sensations tell us that energy is flowing into our bodies, energy that we need to give to the music and to the audience. So, I don’t fight those feelings, I let them flow right into me, and when I step on stage, I let them flow out to the public so that they become energised too!” (Cited in Helding, 2016, p. 89).

This account only mentions physiological activation, omitting the psychological dimension commonly associated with MPA descriptions (Kenny, 2011). The interaction between performers’ physiological and psychological experiences, as well as performers’ interpretations of physiological activation seem central to our understanding of MPA, and will be discussed in Section 5. As research has largely focused on debilitating MPA, those who experience MPA positively and/or manage it successfully are underrepresented in scientific literature, highlighting an important gap in current understanding.

3.2.3. MPA as both negative and positive

There have been three key attempts to clarify the debate between MPA as wholly negative or positive. Firstly, conceptualizing MPA as a singular but multi-faceted construct encompassing both adaptive and maladaptive components (Wolfe, 1989). Secondly, viewing MPA as two separate constructs (facilitative and debilitative), although there remains disagreement regarding whether the constructs are distinct (Mor et al., 1995) or exist along a linear continuum (Spahn et al., 2016; Brugués, 2019). Thirdly, delineating MPA (negative) from an entirely different, positive, construct (‘performance energy’/‘boost’) describing the performance-enhancing state of anticipation which generates excitement, energy and positive tension (Langendörfer et al., 2006; Nagel, 2010; Simoens et al., 2015). Using one label (‘MPA’) to define two negatively correlated constructs seems bound to confuse. Indeed, it is difficult to think of any other psychological construct where the same word is used to describe two opposite phenomena. Further research is required to untangle these issues.

3.2.4. MPA as intrinsic to performance

Some researchers shift the focus entirely from performer to performance: Sternbach (1993) refers not to MPA, but instead to the performer’s capacity to cope with the anxiety of performing, implying that performing is inherently stressful and the issue is how individuals manage that stress, a view supported by numerous subsequent researchers including Sataloff et al. (1999), Hays (2009) and Spahn (2015) (Stressors associated with musical performance will be discussed in Section 4.2.3). Brodsky (1996) reconceptualized MPA as pertaining to the nature of a career in music in terms of performance and occupational stress, rather than to personality deficiencies or underlying psychopathology. He offered the alternative label of ‘Music-Performers’ Stress Syndrome’ (M-PSS), including the option of various specifiers (e.g., level of intensity) to reflect the individuality of performers’ experiences of MPA. While this seems a valuable contribution to the field, it was never adopted by other researchers. Brodsky additionally proposed a linear continuum of severity moving from ‘career stress’ at one end, to ‘tension in performance’ to ‘performance anxiety’ and finally to ‘stage fright’ at the other end (see Brodsky, 1996, p. 91). While an interesting perspective, Brodsky does not explain why performers would experience performance stress so dramatically differently, or what delineates the four stages. If stress is intrinsic to performance, it remains unclear what constitutes ‘normal’ stress and when stress becomes MPA.

3.2.5. Alternative conceptualizations

There have been a few notable recent attempts to offer a completely different perspective on MPA, including Lawrence (2019), who reconceptualizes MPA as performers’ unconscious desires manifesting in psychological or somatic form, and Skoogh and Frisk (2019) who investigate MPA from an artistic, rather than psychological, perspective. They theorize that MPA is not an individual problem, but rather a structural issue relating to complex interactions between perfectionism and performance values found in western classical music. While offering real insight, neither has yet impacted the predominantly pathologizing approach to defining MPA.

3.3. Kenny’s definition

The definition most widely used today is offered by Kenny (2009):

“The experience of marked and persistent anxious apprehension related to musical performance that has arisen through specific anxiety-conditioning experiences and which is manifested through combinations of affective, cognitive, somatic, and behavioural symptoms. It affects musicians for their entire lives and is at least partially independent of years of training, practice, and level of musical accomplishment. It may or may not impair the quality of the musical performance” (2009, p. 433).

Although this definition is used by most contemporary MPA studies, there are issues worth discussing. Firstly, defining MPA by its ‘symptoms’ perpetuates the pathologizing narrative, conjuring up images of illness and disease. As well as the philosophical issues with medicalizing MPA (see Section 7), if it can be facilitative for some, or simply inherent to performance, then the presence of ‘symptoms’ may not be the key issue to understanding or managing MPA.

Secondly, there is no empirical support for the assertion that MPA “affects musicians for their entire lives.” Are there really no musicians who have managed to overcome it? This argument presumably stems from the issue that MPA studies are pathologically focused, with little account across literature of musicians who successfully manage, or indeed overcome, MPA. Indeed, many studies are fairly cross-sectional, or at least of a limited duration, as opposed to truly long-term, meaning there are minimal (if any) longitudinal data investigating MPA management. Thirdly, MPA is “at least partially independent of years of training, practice, and level of musical accomplishment.” Given that MPA is reported by individuals of all levels of training, experience and expertise (including celebrated artists such as Chopin, Casals, Rubinstein, Horowitz and Rachmaninoff), MPA can presumably be entirely, not partially, independent of expertise (Brugués, 2011a; Kantor-Martynuska et al., 2018).

Lastly, the strand “MPA may or may not impair the quality of the musical performance” tells us very little about the complex relationship between MPA and performance quality, which will be discussed in Section 5.3. It also omits the impact MPA can have on the experience of performing, regardless of whether quality is affected. Across the extensive landscape of MPA literature, a recurring theme is the significant variability with which MPA can manifest, ranging from performance-enhancing, to minimally negative, to debilitating, to career-ending and varying in terms of regularity, performance-setting and manifestation (Nagel et al., 1989; Van Kemenade et al., 1995; Miller and Chesky, 2004; Fehm and Schmidt, 2006; Patson and Loughlan, 2014; Lawrence, 2019). This complexity and multidimensionality is arguably not yet reflected in the prevailing approach to defining MPA.

In sum, the field still lacks a cohesive MPA definition, and we suggest that the main obstacles are (1) terminological and (2) conceptual ambiguities. Regarding terminology – using one term (‘MPA’) to describe a phenomenon which can be positive, mildly challenging, debilitating or a completely normal part of a performer’s life, seems at best unhelpful, at worst deeply problematic. Without a shared language, it becomes impossible to ensure we are all talking about the same thing, which may partially explain the lack of success in supporting musicians to manage MPA – how can we manage a thing effectively without really knowing what that thing is? We urgently need a universal MPA definition to be able to effectively support performers experiencing it.

Regarding conceptual ambiguities, although many argue that the majority (if not all) of performers experience apprehension and nerves, and many experience nerves as positive and performance-enhancing (Powell, 2004), efforts to conceptualize MPA as beneficial or intrinsic to performance have been drowned out by the predominantly pathologizing approach (Lawrence, 2019), with significant implications for its perception, experience and management, as will be discussed in Sections 6.2.2 and 7.

Perhaps the issue is not whether MPA is definitively negative, positive or both, but that musicians clearly experience MPA in vastly different ways. The questions then become, what mediate these differences? How can we conceptualize MPA in a way that adequately captures its individuality, complexity and variability and leads to the development of interventions which can support musicians in real and tangible ways? We now turn to MPA’s etiology and manifestation in the hopes of addressing these fundamental issues.

4. Etiology

4.1. Theoretical perspectives on MPA’s etiology

There is consensus among researchers that MPA is a complex, multidimensional phenomenon produced by the interaction between numerous factors including genetics, biology, environmental stimuli, aversive conditioning experiences, conscious and unconscious anxiety triggers, psychological characteristics and factors relating to musical tasks and performance contexts (Papageorgi et al., 2007; Burin and Osório, 2017; Brugués, 2019). More specifically, there remains debate over which theory best accounts for MPA’s etiology. Theories posited (summarized in Table 2) include cognitive theories, behavioral theories, cognitive behavioral theories, physiological theories, psychoanalytic theory and Barlow’s Anxiety Model (2000), adapted to MPA by Kenny et al. (2004), Spahn (2015), Altenmüller and loannou (2016), Brugués (2019).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of key psychological theories explaining MPA’s etiology.

While these various theoretical perspectives individually offer insight into specific dimensions of MPA’s etiology, none emerges as being comprehensive. Additionally, there is currently no widespread agreement among researchers to support any one theory, all of which lack empirical support (Goren, 2014; Fernholz et al., 2019). While these theories have all contributed significantly to current understanding of MPA (Kenny, 2011), there is a legitimate counterargument that generalizing about MPA’s etiology is futile, as its origins vary significantly from person to person (Brandfonbrener, 1990), arguably suggesting a utilitarian, rather than theoretical, approach.

One such utilitarian approach is Wilson’s (1994) framework, which divides known MPA risk factors into Person, Task, and Situation. Although this tripartite framework offers a pragmatic heuristic to conceptualize the multitudinous factors contributing to MPA’s etiology, two limitations are worth consideration. Firstly, it is clear from the extensive stress literature that it is the individual’s interpretation of a stimulus which causes stress, not the stimulus itself (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). For example, not all musicians reporting similar occupational stressors experience them as equally stressful, or experience similar levels of MPA (Kenny et al., 2004), indicating that stimuli from the Task and Situation domains are subjective and thus filtered through Person. Secondly, the three categories are not distinct, but highly interconnected, with complex interactions both within and between domains. Indeed, one domain’s impact depends on the level of others (Valentine, 2002). Despite these limitations, this is arguably the best fit available, and has been adopted by subsequent researchers including Valentine (2002) and Burin and Osório (2017).

4.2. MPA risk factors

4.2.1. Person

Person refers to intrapersonal factors which are associated with, and/or predict, MPA, and can therefore be considered risk factors. These include demographic variables (gender and sociocultural factors), biographical details (family, teachers and aversive conditioning experiences), personality traits, cognitive style and comorbid challenges. These factors (see Table 3) appear key to understanding how MPA develops and is sustained (Burin and Osório, 2017). Studies consistently report significant associations between MPA and high levels of maladaptive personality traits including maladaptive coping style (van Fenema et al., 2013; Thomas and Nettelbeck, 2014), low self-efficacy (Sinden, 1999; Liston et al., 2003; Kenny and Ackermann, 2015), maladaptive dimensions of perfectionism (Stoeber and Otto, 2006; Stoeber and Eismann, 2007; Kobori et al., 2011; Sarıkaya and Kurtaslan, 2018; Butković et al., 2022; Yang et al., 2022), low self-esteem (Papageorgi et al., 2007; Burin et al., 2019), negative affect (Zinn et al., 2000; Sadler and Miller, 2010), neuroticism (Langendörfer et al., 2006), susceptibility to anxiety (Stephenson and Quarrier, 2005), guilt/shame-proneness (Coşkun-Şentürk and Çırakoğlu, 2018) and trait anxiety (Osborne and Kenny, 2008; Burin et al., 2019).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Intrapersonal factors associated with MPA.

Gender (being female) is unequivocally associated with MPA (González et al., 2018; Dobos et al., 2019; Butković et al., 2022), consistent with findings from the general population (de Figueiredo Rocha, 2020). There is also wide agreement that dysfunctional cognitions and cognitive style (rumination, worry, disruptive thoughts, dysfunctional attentional focus, judgmental attitude, catastrophizing and fear of negative evaluation) play a crucial role in MPA’s development and sustenance (Kenny et al., 2014; Lupiáñez et al., 2022). A recent study by Sokoli et al. (2022) investigated the extent to which components of MPA (affective, cognitive and somatic) vary according to factors including practice, musical experience, instrument group, gender and age. Their findings of significant associations between age, gender and instrument group and different MPA components, as well as between specific MPA components (anxious feelings and breathing-related complaints) and decrease in self-reported performance quality have important implications for MPA’s assessment and management (For further investigations of intrapersonal factors associated with MPA’s etiology, see Sadler and Miller, 2010; Burin et al., 2019; Papageorgi, 2020; Wiedemann et al., 2020; Osborne and Kirsner, 2022; Sokoli et al., 2022; Aubry and Küssner, 2023; Kirsner et al., 2023).

As negative personality constructs are associated with MPA, so too are positive ones (Chattin, 2019). Studies have identified several constructs which correlate negatively with MPA including resilience and adaptive coping (Nordin-Bates, 2012; Pecen et al., 2016), openness, conscientiousness and perceived control (Chattin, 2019), positive/realistic cognitive self-statements and performance appraisal (Clark, 1989) and high self-efficacy (González et al., 2018; Sarıkaya and Kurtaslan, 2018; Dempsey and Comeau, 2019). Research is increasingly recognizing the key role of self-compassion in supporting performers across domains – for further discussion, see Baltzell (2016); Kelley and Farley (2019); Lyon and Plisco (2020); Walton et al. (2022). Consistent with the pathological focus of most MPA research, studies largely focus on traits which exacerbate MPA and there is a gap in understanding regarding constructs which protect against MPA. Greater clarity is needed regarding traits and processes which support performers to manage MPA effectively. We turn now to the extrinsic factors associated with MPA’s etiology: Task and Situational factors.

4.2.2. Task

Task refers to aspects relating to performance tasks: technical insecurities that cause uncertainty and/or physical tension at the instrument, insufficient preparation, difficulty of repertoire/attempting repertoire beyond one’s ability interpretation of the score, and memorization worries (Kenny et al., 2014; Burin and Osório, 2017; Burin et al., 2019; Ginsborg, 2019). All of these factors are regularly cited by performers as key causes of MPA (Burin et al., 2019). However, they are all highly subjective and thus arguably mediated through Person.

4.2.3. Situation

Situation can be divided into factors relating to performance mode and to occupational stressors. Within performance mode, solo performance is more associated with MPA than ensemble performance (Brugués, 2019) and public performance and auditions are more associated with MPA than rehearsal or practice (Yoshie et al., 2009; Brugués, 2019). Occupational stressors include the following:

• Social and environmental conditions (Antonini Philippe et al., 2019)

• Financial insecurity (Dobson, 2011; Gross and Musgrave, 2016; Brugués, 2019)

• Job uncertainty (uncertainty about regular employment, highly competitive industry) (Sousa et al., 2016; Brugués, 2019; Burin et al., 2019)

• Demanding work schedule (irregular, unpredictable and anti-social hours, long hours of rehearsal, grueling tour schedules, travel, jetlag) (Burin and Osório, 2017; Brugués, 2019)

• Separation from family (Vervainioti and Alexopoulos, 2015; Sousa et al., 2016; Brugués, 2019)

• Demands of ‘professional sociability’ (especially for freelancers), associated with ‘alcohol as career facilitator’ (Dobson, 2011; Brugués, 2019)

• Environmental challenges (venue): unfamiliar halls, generally unsatisfactory/challenging performance conditions, suboptimal acoustics, temperature, poor seating, lighting and unsafe or cramped backstage, audience characteristics (Parasuraman and Purohit, 2000; Papageorgi et al., 2007)

• Interpersonal challenges: comparison/competition with peers, lack of artistic control/integrity, social tensions (problems with stand partner, back-stabbing between colleagues, lack of confidence in colleagues, issues with management, perceived lack of support from significant others), critical (abusive) verbal style of conductors and frequent changes in leadership (conductor) requiring constant adaptation to different personalities/musical styles (Parasuraman and Purohit, 2000; Burin et al., 2019)

• Performance pressures: scrutiny from audiences, critics & colleagues [managing unrealistic standards of perfection (Kenny, 2004; Dobson, 2011; Burin and Osório, 2017)]

Numerous studies have shown associations between occupational stress and MPA, indicating that they are not independent domains (Voltmer et al., 2012; van Fenema et al., 2013). Based on their qualitative systematic review, Vervainioti and Alexopoulos (2015) concluded that the occupational stressors facing musicians are highly interconnected, forming a complex web between MPA and factors intrinsic and extrinsic to the performer. Across the literature, almost all professional musicians report occupational stress, and many cite it as a key contributor to MPA (Kenny et al., 2014).

While the nature of the association between MPA and situational factors is not entirely clear, there are several plausible links. Given the significance of fear of negative evaluation in predicting MPA, it is unsurprising that performance modes with greater exposure (solo performance and public performance/auditions) are more associated with MPA than those with lower exposure (ensemble performance and rehearsal/practice). Regarding occupational stressors, these are largely beyond performers’ control (Clark et al., 2014), which may contribute to a sense of anxiety. As discussed, all stimuli are filtered through the individual, which could explain why highly anxious musicians are more likely to experience occupational stressors as stressful (Steptoe, 1989). While occupational stressors are inherent to the profession and therefore cannot be removed, protecting musicians from their negative impact will arguably be easier with greater understanding of the mechanisms underpinning the relationship between them and MPA. Seemingly, coping effectively with MPA could reduce the stress of occupational stressors and vice versa. Please see Gross (2015) for discussion of the interaction between emotion regulation, coping and emotional experience.

In sum, MPA is broadly viewed within a multi-factor model, where the degree of anxiety experienced depends on the interaction between a constellation of factors intrinsic and extrinsic to the performer, including biological and demographic variables, personality constructs, and task-based/situational factors associated with musical performance (Antonini Philippe et al., 2022). We turn now how and why MPA manifests, and how it can affect performance and/or performer.

5. MPA manifestation and impact

5.1. Why MPA manifests

MPA comprises a constellation of partially independent yet interactive responses to perceived threat (Kenny, 2006). In very basic terms, the amygdala constantly scans our environment for threat; when it interprets sensory information as threatening, it triggers the release of stress hormones adrenaline (from the sympathetic nervous system, SNS) and cortisol (via the hypothalamic–pituitary adrenal (HPA) pathway), activating a range of physiological responses which maximize the body’s capacity to deal with the threat (McCarty, 2016). This phenomenon, known as the fight or flight response (Cannon, 1927) can be triggered consciously or unconsciously (Kenny, 2006). Performers vary in their response to anxiety-provoking stimuli for a wide range of reasons including aversive performance experiences (particularly during formative years) (Osborne and Kirsner, 2022) and the intrapersonal factors discussed in Section 4.2.1 which may exacerbate individuals’ perception of a performance as threatening, thus triggering, as well as sustaining, MPA.

The fight or flight response seemingly evolved to protect humans from physical threat endangering their survival. However, the amygdala has not yet evolved to distinguish between physical and psychological threats and triggers the same alarm system in response to both (Kenny, 2011), which is why performing a Mozart concerto can elicit the same physiological response as encountering a tiger. Barlow (2002) differentiates between ‘true alarms’ (automatic physiological activation in response to real danger) and ‘false alarms’ (learned fear responses triggered in the absence of real danger). In the case of true alarms, such as fleeing a burning building, the fight/flight response is vital to survival. However, in the case of false alarms (psychological threats), excessive SNS activity can be problematic, interfering with the fine motor skills necessary for musical performance and potentially creating emotional distress (Kenny, 2009).

Numerous theories have sought to explain what causes so-called ‘false alarms’, including classic operant conditioning (anxiety-conditioning experiences), vicarious (observational) learning, heightened neurobiological hyperreactivity, and psychoanalytic theories, where the audience represents parents or other significant caregivers (Kenny, 2009). While no account is comprehensive, and causes are most likely multi-determined (Kenny, 2009), it seems clear that a prerequisite for MPA to manifest is the performer’s initial perception of a stimulus (performance) as threatening (Osborne and McPherson, 2019).

5.2. How MPA manifests

5.2.1. Physiology

Physiological responses to perceived threat include increased cardiovascular activity (tachycardia, palpitations and increased blood pressure), respiratory changes (hyperventilation, difficulty controlling breathing, and shortness of breath), dry mouth, throat constriction (difficulty swallowing), blurred vision, perspiration and clamminess, hot or cold flushes, dizziness, urinary urgency, gastrointestinal activity (butterflies, nausea, vomiting, upset stomach, diarrhea), excessive muscle tension and fatigue, reduced motor control, coordination and agility, muscle spasms/tremor, transient limb paralysis, uncontrollable shaking, numbness or tingling in extremities and hormonal changes (Spahn, 2015; Altenmüller and Ioannou, 2016; Guyon et al., 2020; Turan et al., 2022). Excessive muscular tension is particularly problematic as it can disrupt the fine motor control required for performance and increase risk of PMRDs (Yoshie et al., 2009; Pell, 2020; Vivas et al., 2021). Given the unpleasantness of these experiences, it is unsurprising that controlling them is often the most urgent goal for performers (Zhukov, 2019).

Although physiological data are often used to assess MPA level, evidence to support their use as a proxy for MPA is equivocal (Yoshie et al., 2009). Indeed, research shows that performers with significantly elevated heart rate and blood pressure can report low MPA while performers showing normal physiological parameters can report intense MPA and that physiological arousal often correlates positively with optimal performance (Craske and Craig, 1984; Spahn et al., 2010; Endo et al., 2014; Studer et al., 2014). These findings suggest that physiological activation seemingly becomes problematic only when met with/experienced alongside psychological distress – the importance of negative cognitive appraisal in generating and exacerbating MPA arguably cannot be understated (Steptoe, 2001; Osborne and Kenny, 2008). We therefore turn now to the psychological components of MPA.

5.2.2. Cognitions

Research consistently indicates the prominence of dysfunctional cognitions in highly anxious performers (Kenny and Osborne, 2006; Sokoli et al., 2022). While cognitions are generally discussed as one dimension, it may be helpful to divide them into two discrete (albeit somewhat overlapping) categories: cognitive content (specific worries) and cognitive style (type of thinking). Cognitive content (worries), centered on the perceived threat of risk or danger (Salmon, 1990) include worrying about memory lapses, errors, technical or musical incompetence, negative evaluation, failure, embarrassment or humiliation, and inability to control the effects of physiological arousal (McGrath, 2012; Fernholz et al., 2019). Although cognitions are often discussed as a response to physiological activation, they can also be part of the initial anxiety response – anxiety distorts cognitive style and constricts thinking, manifesting in a maladaptive cognitive style characterized by irrational and self-defeating failure-focused cognitive patterns (Osborne and Franklin, 2002; Altenmüller and Ioannou, 2016).

A fundamental aspect of MPA seems to be the disruption of task-oriented cognitions – Steptoe (2001) identifies three key types of disruption: (1) catastrophizing (exaggerating the probability and impact of negative/disastrous events in performance) which is strongly positively correlated with MPA (Valentine, 2002), (2) preoccupation with possible negative evaluation, and (3) heightened perception of physiological changes and interpreting them as indicating loss of control and inevitable collapse. Other maladaptive cognitive styles include negative self-talk (excessive focus on perceived inadequacies), irrational beliefs (such as conflating one’s performance with one’s self-worth), excessive focus on task-irrelevant thoughts provoked by internal or external distractions and rumination (self-doubts, negative and judgmental thoughts about oneself and one’s performance, projected criticism from the audience and excessive focus on technical inadequacies) (Perdomo-Guevara, 2014; Spahn, 2015; Zhukov, 2019). Anxious cognitions can impair concentration and focus, drain self-confidence and interfere in the creative process (McGrath, 2012; Patson and Loughlan, 2014). In extremis, negative post-performance rumination can resemble post-traumatic stress disorder (Sternbach, 1993).

5.2.3. Emotions

The fundamental experience of anxiety is arguably anxious apprehension– a future-based state characterized by feelings of helplessness based on the sense that one can neither predict nor control possible threats (Kenny, 2009). Other commonly reported emotions associated with MPA include dread, fear, panic, terror, stress, insecurity, irritability, anger, moodiness, embarrassment, denial, depression, distress, shame, frustration and guilt (McGrath, 2012; Spahn, 2015; Kantor-Martynuska et al., 2018; Fernholz et al., 2019).

5.2.4. Behaviors

Behavioral responses encompass a range of conscious and unconscious behaviors such as overt physical expressions of anxiety (alterations in body language such as postural distortions, tensed or hunched shoulders and distressed or deadpan facial expressions) (Patson and Loughlan, 2014), isolation from others (Roland, 1994), avoidance behaviors (Lederman, 1999; Fernholz et al., 2019) such as avoiding practicing difficult technical passages (Lehrer and Woolfolk, 1982), or performance avoidance (Salmon, 1990), and general unrest (agitation, fidgetiness, escapist tendencies) (Steptoe, 2001; Spahn, 2015). Performers may also engage in ‘safety behaviours’ such as alcohol or drug consumption, distraction techniques (Fernholz et al., 2019) and compulsive, ritualized, behaviors such as repetitive practice, repeatedly checking instrument or moistening lips (Patson and Loughlan, 2014; Spahn, 2015; Altenmüller and Ioannou, 2016). It is unclear whether these behaviors are manifestations of MPA, coping strategies to manage MPA, or both. We turn now to MPA’s impact on performance quality, an issue of paramount importance to musicians (Kenny, 2011).

5.3. How does MPA impact performance quality?

Numerous theories have sought to explain the complex relationship between anxiety (or arousal) and performance quality across a range of domains. The first, and most widely used, is the Yerkes and Dodson (1908) ‘Inverted U’, which posits that performance quality is highest when arousal is moderate; below or above this optimal functioning zone, performance quality decreases (Williamon, 2004). Although this relationship seems well-established, it only considers physiological arousal, rendering it overly simplistic. Fazey and Hardy’s (1988) Catastrophe Theory adds a third dimension – cognition – arguing that the Yerkes-Dodson Law holds when cognitive anxiety is low, but when cognitive anxiety is high, the quality of performance can deteriorate catastrophically (Hardy and Parfitt, 1991). For detailed discussion of the Yerkes-Dodson and Catastrophe Models across performance domains, see Ruggiero (2012).

The last two decades have seen the development of models which focus specifically on the relationship between MPA (as opposed to arousal/performance anxiety in other domains) and performance quality (Zinn et al., 2000; Kirchner, 2003; Papageorgi et al., 2007; Chow and Mercado, 2020; Osborne and Kirsner, 2022). Collectively, these five models, summarized in Table 4, illuminate different aspects of the complex and multidimensional relationship between MPA and performance quality and contribute significantly to current understanding. We will now suggest four areas for possible further exploration.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Models exploring how MPA disrupts performance.

5.3.1. Performers who experience MPA yet do not demonstrate performance problems

The Yerkes-Dodson ‘inverted U’ has underpinned academic discussion of anxiety’s impact on performance quality across domains for over a century, leading to the widely held perception that performance quality deteriorates beyond a certain level of anxiety. However, evidence to support this is not clear-cut (Osborne et al., 2020). Despite evident cognitive differences between low and high anxious performers, highly anxious performers do not necessarily display impaired performance (Kenny, 2004; Kantor-Martynuska et al., 2018). In their study of professional orchestral musicians, Kenny et al. (2014) found that even the most highly anxious performers rarely experienced performance catastrophes, or indeed any significant impairment of performance quality. Indeed, numerous renowned artists (including Pablo Casals, Steven Osborne, Vladimir Horowitz, Maria Callas and Andrea Bocelli) have publicly disclosed their personal struggles with MPA, while maintaining highly successful musical careers. From their continued critical acclaim, one can reasonably infer that they managed to perform to a consistently high level, despite self-reported debilitating anxiety, seemingly refuting the premise that excessive arousal causes performance deterioration.

It seems that musicians can perform to high levels of excellence while experiencing significant levels of MPA, but that their wellbeing and enjoyment of performance can still suffer (Kenny, 2011). This suggests that (1) existing models account for MPA’s impact on performance, but not performer, and (2) that further research is needed to understand which variables or processes moderate the relationship between MPA and performance quality.

5.3.2. The interaction between MPA responses

Only one of the models (Kirchner, 2003) unpacks the interaction of MPA responses. As cognitions underlie behaviors and interact reciprocally with emotions and physiology (Valentine, 2002; Perdomo-Guevara, 2014), negative cognitions can trigger interaction between all four domains, creating a vicious circle whereby responses ‘feed off’ each other and can become overwhelming and difficult to manage (De Felice, 2004; Oyan, 2006; Kenny, 2011). Wolfe (1989) offers the example of a flutist experiencing dry mouth, whose worries about dry mouth exacerbate dryness and increase anxiety, in turn exacerbating dry mouth and so on. Seemingly, the interaction between responses may offer a more useful way to understand their impact than their presence. While interaction between MPA responses seems key to understanding its impact, the relational aspect of responses is under researched (Stanson, 2019) and prevailing theories are not addressing adequately how interaction works, or how it can be effectively managed to mitigate its impact on performance.

5.3.3. Performers’ interpretations of MPA

Research in both music and sport indicates that anxiety is multidimensional, with intensity and direction of symptoms forming distinct constructs (Jones and Swain, 1992; Mor et al., 1995; Miller and Chesky, 2004). Growing evidence suggests that performers’ interpretation (cognitive appraisal) of anxiety impacts performance significantly more than its presence (Osborne and Kenny, 2008; Clark et al., 2014; Osborne and McPherson, 2019; Osborne and Kirsner, 2022), with facilitative interpretations enhancing performance and debilitative interpretations leading to decreased enjoyment of performing and deleterious effects on performance quality (Yoshie et al., 2009). Performers’ interpretation of MPA as positive or negative is seemingly mediated by a range of factors including performing experiences, task mastery, self-efficacy and perceived control over anxiety symptoms (Osborne and Kirsner, 2022).

Research investigating performers’ interpretations of MPA has largely focused on the positive/negative binary. However, acceptance (versus resistance) may offer an alternative framework to understand performers’ interpretations of MPA, and its impact on performance. Anxiety-related psychopathology is characterized by experiential avoidance of fear (Schanche et al., 2020), whereas psychological acceptance correlates positively with improved coping strategies (Baer, 2003) and negatively with emotional dysregulation and other indices of poor mental health (Hitchcock et al., 2016). The consistent findings that performers’ response to MPA has greater impact than its presence has important implications for understanding how MPA can impact performance and the ‘response’ stage should arguably be a critical component in MPA models.

5.3.4. MPA and attentional focus

“My experience is what I attend to” [William James, cited in Tremayne and Morgan (2016), p. 389].

Across domains, optimal performance demands intense concentration and focus (Talbot-Honeck, 1994). Indeed, within musical performance the centrality of attention regulation has long been recognized (Rife et al., 2000; Kenny, 2011). One of MPA’s most common and challenging effects is attentional changes – both the narrowing of focus (Valentine, 2002) and the shift in focus from task-relevant to task-irrelevant, manifesting in negative self-evaluation, worries, rumination and preoccupation with physiological changes (Liston et al., 2003; Kenny, 2011). Essentially, anxiety hijacks attention from the music (Salmon and Meyer, 1992; Oyan, 2006). Humans have finite attentional capacity (Kantor-Martynuska et al., 2018) – when attention is both constricted by anxiety and depleted attentional resources are hijacked by negative cognitions, there is insufficient bandwidth to focus on task-relevant processes, which can impair performance, in turn exacerbating physiological, cognitive and emotional responses (Kenny, 2009).

Attentional Control Theory (ACT, Eysenck et al., 2007) offers a compelling account of the complex relationship between attentional focus and performance quality. According to this extramusical theory, anxiety decreases attentional control and processing efficiency by overloading working memory with anxious cognitions and increased focus on threat-based stimuli, thus impairing executive function. Eysenck and Calvo (1992) and Eysenck et al. (2007) differentiate between effectiveness and efficiency in performance: effectiveness refers to performance quality, and seems independent of anxiety, while efficiency (the interaction between effectiveness and deployment of personal resources) is impaired by anxiety as negative cognitions disturb task-relevant processing. When anxiety induces performers to deploy compensatory strategies/resources, performance effectiveness (quality) overall will not be impaired.

This finding is significant as it arguably explains how experience and expertise can mediate MPA’s impact on performance quality – the elite performers discussed above may have sufficient resources to enhance their efficiency, thus maintaining high levels of performance effectiveness. This may also explain why performers can perform to very high levels, but at a high personal cost in terms of their wellbeing and reduced enjoyment of performance. Studies have been conducted using ACT to predict anxiety-related performance outcomes across domains cognitive performance (Eysenck et al., 2007), athletics (Causer et al., 2011) and music performance (Ruggiero, 2012). A study by Oudejans et al. (2017) found that music students experienced a significant increase in self-reported negative cognitions immediately before choking (crumbling under pressure), suggesting that anxiety-induced attentional changes may cause performance deterioration in less expert/experienced performers. In their study comparing external versus internal focus conditions, Mornell and Wulf (2019) found increased musical expression and technical accuracy in the external focus group. Collectively, these studies suggest that planning and training particular attentional foci could optimize learning and performance outcomes. Further research applying ACT to musical performance could offer valuable insight into the different constructs of effectiveness and efficiency, and help performers navigate the challenges associated with reduced bandwidth under pressure.

In sum, we suggest that developing a comprehensive MPA model requires further insight into (1) understanding performers who experience MPA yet do not demonstrate performance problems, (2) the nature of the interaction between MPA responses, (3) how performers’ interpretation of their experience impacts performance and (4) the relationship between attentional changes and performance quality/experience. We turn now to the ways in which musicians seek to manage MPA.

6. Managing MPA

Given the myriad ways MPA can threaten performance, it is unsurprising that musicians utilize a range of strategies to try and mitigate its impact. This section will discuss the two routes musicians take to manage MPA: Coping strategies and interventions. Coping here encompasses the conscious/unconscious application of behaviors/strategies musicians employ to manage MPA; interventions here are more formalized programs based on the explicit training of specific strategies to manage MPA, delivered by a trained practitioner. As these two topics are qualitatively different, they will be presented separately.

6.1. Coping with MPA

Coping is defined as the “constantly changing cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage specific external and/or internal demands that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources of the person” (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984, p. 141). There is broad consensus that it is not the type or quantity of stress that determines its impact, but how the individual copes with stress (Lazarus, 2006). While there is agreement regarding what coping is, the models used to categorize coping strategies vary across the literature. Distinctions include adaptive versus maladaptive, effective versus ineffective (Wolfe, 1990), problem-orientated versus emotion-orientated (Studer et al., 2011), appraisal versus avoidance (Langendörfer et al., 2006), cognitive versus behavioral (Burin and Osório, 2017), and trait versus state (Langendörfer et al., 2006). The aim of this discussion is not to classify musicians’ coping strategies using theoretical models, but rather to offer an overview of the coping strategies musicians use, and their relationship with MPA.

Over the past 35 years, numerous researchers have investigated the coping strategies musicians use to manage MPA. Studies range in methodology (both qualitative and quantitative), population (conservatoire and university students, amateur and professional, classical and jazz) and location, including: the UK (Steptoe and Fidler, 1987; Steptoe, 1989), North America (Wolfe, 1990; MacAfee and Comeau, 2022), Australia (Roland, 1994; Kenny et al., 2014), Italy (Biasutti and Concina, 2014), Spain (Hernández et al., 2018; Lupiáñez et al., 2022), Switzerland (Studer et al., 2011), Estonia (Kiik-Salupere and Ross, 2020), Turkey (Cornett and Urhan, 2021; Yadigaroğlu, 2021), Malaysia (Zakaria et al., 2013), Taiwan (Huang and Song, 2021) and Brazil (Burin et al., 2019). Across these studies, the strategies most commonly used to cope with MPA include positive self-talk, meditation, distraction and social support, relaxation exercises (muscle relaxation and deep breathing), increased practice, mock performance practice, ritualized behaviors and substances including beta blockers, alcohol and illicit drugs. Use of coping strategies varied between students and professionals, by gender, and by nationality (prayer was included in the Malaysian and Turkish studies, indicating the importance of cultural context).

There is a complex and seemingly paradoxical relationship between coping and MPA in that the two constructs are positively correlated – greater use of coping is associated with increased MPA (Steptoe, 2001). There are a number of possible explanations for this. Firstly, more anxious musicians are more likely to engage in deliberate coping behaviors which may prove effective, but MPA levels remain higher than their nonanxious colleagues (Steptoe and Fidler, 1987; Lehrer et al., 1990). Secondly, strategies deployed to alleviate MPA may be counterproductive (Langendörfer et al., 2006): Studies investigating specific coping strategies show positive correlations between substance use and MPA (Park, 2010) and dysfunctional coping (social support and avoidance strategies) and MPA (Biasutti and Concina, 2014). Thirdly, coping is a multidimensional construct with both adaptive and maladaptive components. Effective (adaptive) coping is associated with beneficial physiological states, reduced negative affect, perceiving anxiety as facilitative and adaptive MPA (Wolfe, 1990; Langendörfer et al., 2006; Papageorgi et al., 2007; Park, 2010). Conversely, inadequate (maladaptive) coping is significantly positively correlated with, and significantly predicts, MPA (Sinden, 1999; Langendörfer et al., 2006; van Fenema et al., 2013; Thomas and Nettelbeck, 2014). Lastly, coping is highly individual – the same strategies can be effective or ineffective depending on the individual, and indeed, strategies used by an individual may vary in efficacy across different performance contexts (Burin and Osório, 2017). For further discussion on the relationship between coping and MPA, see McNeil et al. (2022).

Researchers agree that effective coping is vital to managing MPA (Spahn et al., 2016; Osborne and McPherson, 2019). However, there remain gaps in current understanding of the relationship between coping and MPA: what mediates the efficacy of different coping strategies? Are some ‘better’ than others, or does their efficacy depend wholly on the individual? Are strategies more effective when combined? One interesting omission across these studies concerns what, exactly, musicians are trying to achieve through their coping efforts. If their aim is to eliminate MPA, this may be unrealistic, even undesirable, given the necessity of arousal for optimal performance, thus rendering the coping strategies ineffective. Arguably, being explicit about, and possibly reframing, what exactly the coping strategies are being used for may offer a different perspective on their efficacy.

Problematically, there remains little consensus regarding what actually constitutes effective coping. There are two main possible reasons for this ambiguity. Firstly, as discussed, coping is highly individual. Secondly, musical performance is largely researched through a pathological lens, focusing on musicians who experience debilitating MPA and therefore arguably cope inadequately. The few studies which have investigated the psychological characteristics of performers through a non-pathological lens found flexibility of coping and the deployment of a range of effective coping strategies (including positive anxiety appraisal and adaptive lifestyle habits) to be key (MacNamara et al., 2008; Pecen et al., 2018). Despite the significance of coping in predicting and sustaining, or protecting against, MPA, the complex interaction between the two constructs has not yet been studied sufficiently (Hernández et al., 2018).

Of particular concern is the prevalent use of substances to manage MPA (Kenny et al., 2014; Hernández et al., 2018; Burin et al., 2019). Alcohol impairs fine-motor skills, reaction time, memory and coordination, all vital for performance, and can lead to addiction and serious health issues (Zhukov, 2019). In Burin et al.’s (2019) study of 240 Brazilian musicians, 25% of participants who used alcohol to manage MPA showed signs of alcohol abuse. Beta-blockers can also have problematic side-effects and become psychologically addictive (Studer et al., 2011). Based on their study of Australian orchestral musicians, Kenny et al. (2014) reported that 30% of participants rely on beta-blockers to manage their anxiety. Indeed, the British Association of Performing Arts Medicine suggest beta blockers and tranquilizers as MPA management options on their website [British Association for Performing Arts Medicnie (BAPAM), n.d.], which suggests (1) that we are urgently in need of healthy and effective solutions to manage MPA and (2) the extent to which the music profession has normalized the use of medical substances to manage anxiety. To be clear, this is not in any way a moral crusade, but with so many musicians at both student and professional level, relying on substances to perform, it seems clear that musicians are experiencing real stress in large numbers and lack adequate coping strategies to manage it.

In sum, while musicians clearly utilize a range of coping strategies, these vary significantly in terms of healthfulness – ranging from healthy to destructive (Helding, 2016) – and efficacy, with significant numbers of musicians employing largely ineffective strategies (Kenny, 2005; Burin et al., 2019). Studies frequently report a disparity between the percentage of musicians using a particular strategy, and the percentage rating that strategy as being effective, indicating a lack of knowledge regarding which strategies actually help. Research suggests that in the absence of guidance regarding effective coping, musicians are likely to engage in coping mechanisms which may inadvertently exacerbate MPA (including seeking social support and avoidance) (Biasutti and Concina, 2014), whereas those who are taught effective coping strategies learn to manage the effects of MPA on performance (Juncos et al., 2017; Osborne and McPherson, 2019). Given its crucial role, it is concerning that so many musicians report limited coping skills. We turn now to available MPA interventions.

6.2. MPA interventions

In the 50 years since the first documented MPA intervention (Wardle, 1969), a plethora of interventions have emerged from a diverse range of theoretical perspectives. These include pharmacotherapy, psychodynamic and psychoanalytic therapies, cognitive, behavioral and cognitive-behavioral therapies, mindfulness-and-acceptance-based approaches, performance coaching, multimodal interventions, virtual reality and exposure therapies, among others. As a comprehensive review of available MPA interventions is beyond the scope of this review, Table 5 defines the major types of intervention, discusses their theoretical underpinnings and signposts the reader to empirical studies investigating their efficacy. There is a curious tension between individual intervention studies and prevalence rates: Studies broadly report statistically significant change at the granular level, yet this change is not seen at the population level. MPA’s unchanged prevalence indicates that current approaches are not having a meaningful impact overall. This section will review the current state of play of intervention research, from methodological and conceptual perspectives.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Definitions and theoretical underpinnings of MPA interventions with signposting to relevant empirical studies.

6.2.1. Methodological critique

From the studies cited in Table 5, one could reasonably infer that the state of play regarding MPA interventions is advanced, with a range of effective interventions to choose from. However, systematic reviews and meta-analyses tell a somewhat different story, reporting some positive evidence but arguing that evidence is largely weakened by methodological limitations. Although numerous MPA interventions have been developed and evaluated, it has not yet been possible to build a robust evidence base for any single intervention due to a range of methodological weaknesses within studies, methodological heterogeneity across studies and a lack of replication (McGinnis and Milling, 2005; Goren, 2014; Matei and Ginsborg, 2017; Fernholz et al., 2019; Zhukov, 2019).

One key methodological limitation is the lack of consistency in MPA definitions across studies and a lack of clarity regarding the theoretical and conceptual issues underpinning MPA within studies (Kenny, 2005). The terms MPA, performance anxiety, and stage fright continue to be used interchangeably across intervention studies, often without clarifying their exact meanings (Fernholz et al., 2019). The lack of a clear and consistent definition makes it difficult to know exactly what changes are occurring both within, and across, studies. Conceptual issues regarding MPA definitions will be discussed below. The lack of clarity regarding what constitutes MPA and the heterogeneity of MPA definitions used is, of course, reflected in the disparate outcome measures used, including some non-validated instruments (Kenny, 2005; Burin and de Lima Osório, 2016). In their systematic review, Fernholz et al. (2019) concluded it was often unclear exactly what was being measured, precluding clear interpretation of results within studies and comparison across studies and recommend using validated and standardized measurement tools.

As problematic as what is being measured, is what is not measured. A good evaluation is a crucial precursor to any intervention (Nagel, 2010), yet intervention studies often omit pre-test scores (Kenny, 2005; McGinnis and Milling, 2005; Zhukov, 2019). There is also a lack of process evaluations including adherence and acceptability, and of interrogation into specific components of interventions in terms of mediating and moderating variables (McGinnis and Milling, 2005), which is especially problematic in complex or combined interventions. A lack of follow-up data (longitudinal studies) makes it impossible to know long-term efficacy (Kenny, 2005; McGinnis and Milling, 2005).

Most studies use very small sample sizes, meaning that the statistical power is insufficient to justify definitive conclusions regarding interventions’ efficacy (Kenny, 2011; Goren, 2014; Zhukov, 2019). Overall, studies with larger and more homogenous subjects are needed (Brugués, 2011b). Studies often do not use a control group (Kenny, 2005; Matei and Ginsborg, 2017; Zhukov, 2019). This is especially problematic when studies use performance tasks to assess MPA, as differential results could be attributed to time spent practising, task familiarity or repeated exposure to the task. Few studies explicate the diagnostic inclusion/exclusion criteria used, or the level of MPA experienced by participants: heterogeneity in participants’ levels of MPA may obscure significant treatment effects (Kenny, 2011; Fernholz et al., 2019). In their systematic review, Burin and de Lima Osório (2016) reported that the majority of studies reviewed included no information regarding participants’ levels of MPA, whether normal or ‘pathological’ and argue that this information is crucial to draw conclusions regarding interventions’ efficacy in managing MPA. While it seems self-evident, McGinnis and Milling (2005) argue that research evaluating the efficacy of interventions designed to ameliorate MPA must be conducted with performers who actually experience MPA, and that findings based on participants with subclinical MPA levels cannot be used to support conclusions regarding interventions’ efficacy for MPA. Lastly, one reason it has not yet been possible to build a robust evidence base for any one intervention is the lack of standardized techniques utilized across different interventions within categories. The heterogeneity of treatment modalities, intervention duration and dosage (intensity) preclude comparison across studies of similar interventions (Kenny, 2005; Burin and de Lima Osório, 2016). Within individual studies, limitations include the absence of a manual detailing treatment protocol and the use of multiple therapists, or interventions being delivered by the principal investigator, risking researcher bias (McGinnis and Milling, 2005).

Based on their systematic reviews, Kenny (2005) and McGinnis and Milling (2005) concluded that there is an urgent need for more methodologically robust studies investigating MPA interventions, and highlighted key methodological weaknesses including conceptual ambiguities, inadequate assessment measures and small and heterogenous samples. Fifteen years later, the state of play remains much the same with recent systematic reviews recommending cautious interpretation of positive results based on a body of knowledge which remains inconclusive, inconsistent, and methodologically weak (Burin and de Lima Osório, 2016; Fernholz et al., 2019; Zhukov, 2019). In sum, key methodological limitations across intervention studies include disparate definitions and outcome measures, missing data, small and heterogenous samples, lack of control group, unclear inclusion/exclusion criteria and heterogenous intervention components within categories and lack of engagement with mechanisms of change.

If these methodological limitations of individual studies reporting positive change were the only issue, then replication would be the obvious next step. However, while these methodological limitations certainly weaken the evidence base for any one intervention, they do not explain why effects being reported at the study level are not seen at the population level. Poor-quality evidence does not mean interventions are not working, which poses an important question left unanswered by systematic reviews and meta-analyses: Why are interventions not having a meaningful impact on MPA? While there is no single answer, there are several possibilities worth exploring, including accessibility issues, MPA conceptualizations, and excessive focus on symptoms.

6.2.2. Conceptual critique

Based on individual studies, it is entirely plausible that effective interventions do exist, but that their impact is limited due to accessibility barriers including prohibitive cost or ineffective dissemination. For example, Spahn et al.’s (2016) multimodal intervention seems highly effective, but its individually tailored design precludes widespread availability, and it has not yet entered the mainstream of interventions – it is unclear where or how one would access it. Overall, empirically-validated cost-effective and accessible interventions are lacking.

In addition to logistical accessibility issues, musicians may not seek help due to the stigma (both real and perceived) still associated with MPA (McGrath, 2012; van Fenema et al., 2013). While there is now seemingly greater openness around physical injuries (Horvath, 2001; Brandfonbrener, 2004), research suggests that this openness has not yet reached psychological issues, with musicians reportedly associating MPA with personal weakness and/or shame (Juncos et al., 2017). As stigma (associated with feelings of shame, disgrace or secrecy) plays a significant role in determining health-seeking behaviors outside of music (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Bharadwaj et al., 2017), this could logically apply to musicians as well.

As discussed in Section 3.2.1, MPA is predominantly conceptualized pathologically, which implies that it is an affliction to be prevented, avoided or eliminated to perform well and happily (Senyshyn and O’Neill, 2001). Based on this conceptualization, interventions largely seek to manage MPA through ameliorating symptoms. Although logical, this approach is problematic in a number of ways. Firstly, there is an implicit assumption that fewer or no ‘symptoms’ is the desired outcome, yet research consistently indicates that some anxiety (or arousal) is intrinsic, even beneficial, for performance (Kenny, 2005; Matei and Ginsborg, 2017) and that reducing anxiety may not be a useful, or even optimal, goal (Kirchner, 2003; Nagel, 2010; Lawrence, 2019). Indeed, relaxation is both unrealistic and counterproductive for peak performance in high-pressure contexts (Pecen et al., 2016), as illustrated by these quotes:

“Relax at the Tchaikovsky competition? Relax when you’ve blown $900 to fly across the country for your two-minute audition for the only symphony flute job to come open this year? Relax when your symphony job security depends on near-perfect technique in each and every concert until you’re tenured? I don’t think so” (Greene, 2012, p. 1).

“Trying to calm down before a stressful performance may not only be futile – but counterproductive to boot!” (Kageyama, 2022).

Secondly, research suggests that ‘symptoms’ themselves may in fact not be the issue. In their evaluation of a mental skills training program for conservatoire students, Clark and Williamon (2011) found no significant differences in state or trait anxiety post intervention, yet participants reported more facilitative views toward MPA, indicating altering one’s interpretation may alleviate MPA’s impact more effectively than reducing it. While MPA research has largely focused on whether performers interpret MPA as either positive or negative, research outside of the musical domain suggests that anxiety-related psychopathology is characterized by experiential avoidance of fear, not the experiencing of it. In this framework of understanding anxiety, the reaction to, not the presence of, anxiety is where problems arise, which has important implications for how we define MPA – perhaps not as the anxiety itself, but in the reaction to the anxiety?

Thirdly, focusing on the presence of ‘symptoms’ does not address the underlying mechanisms by which MPA can disturb performance, as discussed in Section 5.3. Focusing on these mechanisms (e.g., interaction, interpretation and attention regulation) may be more effective.

Fourthly, excessive focus on symptoms overlooks the constellation of other factors associated with MPA, including personality traits and occupational stress, as discussed in Section 4. Indeed, several interventions focus only on one domain (cognitions or behaviors), thus not sufficiently addressing MPA’s multidimensionality (Brandfonbrener, 1999). Interventions which neglect underlying psychological factors are unlikely to succeed (Pecen et al., 2016). Conceptualizing MPA as a multidimensional construct involving complex interaction between the individual and their environment suggests a more holistic approach to its management: MPA interventions should ideally address the full range of MPA processes alongside the numerous vulnerability factors associated with it (Osborne and Franklin, 2002).

Lastly, efforts to reduce MPA largely rely on intentionally changing one’s state (e.g., from anxious to relaxed). There is growing awareness that efforts to reduce unwanted experience creates resistance, inadvertently exacerbating anxiety, as illustrated by this quote:

“Any attempt to not feel the fear splits the performer psychically into two persons, the feeler and the repressor. It is the splitting, not the fear, that limits capability…it is the attempt not to feel rather than the feeling that impairs the performance” (Conable and Conable, 1995, p. 115).

Despite the wide range of interventions on offer, there remains no established way of dealing with MPA. Section 7 will briefly summarize the current state of play, before suggesting a reconceptualization of MPA.

7. Discussion: summary and reconceptualization

MPA affects numerous musicians across all ages, nationalities and musical genres. It occurs in response to threat and manifests in physiological, cognitive, emotional and behavioral ‘symptoms’ which can negatively affect performer and/or performance. It co-varies with a wide range of maladaptive personality traits as well as situational stressors associated with the performance context and is clustered diagnostically within the anxiety disorders landscape. MPA is widely considered to be a significant occupational hazard, and musicians use a range of coping strategies to manage it, many of which are ineffective and/or unhealthy. Despite the existence of numerous interventions, unchanged prevalence figures indicate these are not having a meaningful impact. After decades of research, we still do not really know how best to offer appropriate support to musicians experiencing MPA. We suggest that prevailing views on MPA are conceptually flawed and crucially, not serving musicians.

Zooming out briefly, the broader conversation around mental health has seen a shift over the last 40 years, from the biomedical model traditionally used in psychiatry, to the biopsychosocial model (Engel, 1977). This model emphasizes the importance of psychological and social factors alongside biological factors in understanding illness (Alonso, 2004). Although still awaiting empirical support, Manchester (2011) and Mumm et al. (2020) have convincingly proposed applying the biopsychosocial model to understanding MPA’s etiology. While the biopsychosocial model offers a useful heuristic to understand the many factors associated with MPA’s development, we suggest moving away from both the biomedical and biopsychosocial models, as both are used to describe illness. We propose removing any medical connotation from MPA and reconceptualizing it as an adaptive response to a unique constellation of demands and stressors, which will now be discussed.

7.1. The soil MPA grows in

As discussed in Section 4.2, MPA is associated with/predicted by a number of intrinsic factors including trait anxiety, neuroticism, low self-esteem, maladaptive dimensions of perfectionism, guilt- and shame-proneness, and maladaptive cognitive style characterized by rumination, fear of negative evaluation and judgmental attitude. Across relevant literature, these traits are (implicitly) viewed as individual deficiencies, with minimal discussion of the soil they grow in.

Research clearly shows the challenges facing musicians on the path to a professional career, including fear experienced due to excessive demands from parents or teachers (Mumm et al., 2020), managing the consequences of early specialization, grueling practice schedules, social isolation, psychological pressure (Pecen et al., 2016), neuromuscular and musculoskeletal overuse and irregular sleep and work schedules (Pecen et al., 2018). In her book ‘Music from the Inside Out’, Tomlinson (2018) writes

“In our culture, judgement underpins everything. Learning to play an instrument to professional standards is unbelievably demanding. You need skill, commitment and discipline; you need to master your instrument, faithfully interpret the music you are playing, manage your nerves and above all, be free enough to express yourself. And if you are doing this in a culture that has judgement at its core, there are consequences: judgement is dangerous” (p. 3).

If we look at MPA as a societal rather than an individual phenomenon, many, if not all, of the traits associated with MPA start to seem less like coincidental, intrinsic deficiencies, and more like natural, even predictable, adaptations to years of intensive musical training in competitive and often un-nurturing environments that have judgment at their core, as Tomlinson suggests. Arguably, to be a classical musician and NOT a perfectionist, or neurotic, or fear negative evaluation, would be a radical act of exceptionalism given the educational and professional cultures within which musicians often spend their formative years. We turn now to the extrinsic factors associated with MPA’s etiology.

7.2. Performance demands

Research unequivocally highlights the demands of musical performance, including the relentless pressure to perform to near-perfect levels (McNeil et al., 2022), executing complex motor, cognitive, psychological and emotional skills/processes under close scrutiny and in highly anxiogenic situations (Hays, 2009; Antonini Philippe et al., 2019; de Figueiredo Rocha, 2020). The success and longevity of a musical career depends on musicians’ ability to delivery consistently high-quality performances under pressure (Kenny, 2011; Williamon et al., 2013). While musicians are often compared to athletes in terms of the psychophysical demands of elite performance, musicians receive none of the specialist training or support routinely offered to athletes, including physiotherapy, nutrition, and psychological skills training.

Compounding the pressures of performance are the realities of working within an insecure and highly competitive profession – well-documented occupational challenges include long and anti-social working hours, adverse working conditions, the challenges of touring and financial unpredictability (Voltmer et al., 2012; van Fenema et al., 2013; Sousa et al., 2016). Based on their 2016 study investigating musicians’ mental health, Gross & Musgrave write “Music making is therapeutic but making a career out of music is destructive…whereas artists find solace in the production of music, the working conditions of forging a musical career are traumatic.” (2016, p. 12).

Unsurprisingly, sustaining the demands of high-level performance against the backdrop of a challenging profession often generates stress and anxiety for performers (Antonini Philippe et al., 2022). Indeed, research suggests that managing this anxiety is a “constant battle even for the most accomplished musicians and performers” (Tang and Ryan, 2020, p. 1). Seemingly, MPA poses a challenge that many, if not all, performers must meet (McGinnis and Milling, 2005; Spahn et al., 2010), suggesting that it is just part of the fabric of life as a performing musician. As Hays (2009) asks, “when a condition is ubiquitous, is it diagnosable?” (p. 105), succinctly problematizing diagnosing a ‘normative aspect of performance’.

7.3. Conclusion

In 2020, biological anthropologists Syme and Hagen (2020) published a ground-breaking paper in which they suggest reclassifying ‘diseases of the mind’ in an entirely new framework consisting of: (1) highly heritable (rare) disorders including schizophrenia and OCD, (2) disorders associated with age-related deterioration such as Alzheimer’s disease, (3) a mismatch between ideal and modern environments including ADHD and (4) functional responses to adversity, including PTSD and anxiety disorders. We propose a similarly radical paradigm shift – removing any medical connotation from MPA and reconceptualizing it as a functional response to adversity, where adversity represents the intersection between managing the demands of high-pressure performing situations, a competitive and insecure professional environment, and personality traits associated with elite musical training. Acknowledging this wider context seems key in understanding how MPA develops and sustains, not as an individual deficiency, but as a functional response to a highly challenging education and performance environment.

The World Health Organization (WHO, 2004) defines anxiety as a “normal and healthy reaction to perceived danger that triggers a variety of physical, mental, and behavioral changes in order to facilitate a speedy response” (cited in Osborne and Kirsner, 2022, p. 206). While performing a Mozart concerto may not be the same as encountering a hungry tiger, we suggest there are many rational reasons why performers might, on some level, perceive performing as threatening. The vulnerability and exposure inherent in performance, as well as the reliance on the positive opinion of others (colleagues, critics, agents, fixers, concert promoters or audiences) to pay one’s rent, arguably pose a very real and practical threat, making anxiety a rational response. The recent headline news regarding Arts Council England and the BBC funding cuts offer a flavor of the fragility of the musical ecosystem and the very real threat this poses to musicians’ livelihoods and wellbeing.

Anxiety is a biologically adaptive response, which has ensured human survival for millennia. Although MPA is almost universally described using medical language (‘symptoms’, ‘disorder’, ‘treatment’ etc.), we suggest that the body’s ability to react to what it perceives as threatening is not a sign of illness, but rather signifies a functioning autonomic nervous system. As an analogy: smoke alarms exist to detect and alert us to fire. If our smoke alarm activates in response to burnt toast, we would not disable it, but rather understand that an overly-cautious alarm is preferable to the opposite. We might open a window or wave a tea towel at the agitated alarm, to reassure it that there is no fire. Imagine if we could similarly learn to reassure our nervous system – to understand that it is doing exactly what it evolved to do, and to thank it for its tireless service, to see it as a protector, not a disease.

We question the use of medicalized language and propose using more neutral terms ‘response’, ‘manifestation’, or ‘experience’. This may seem like semantics, but consider the felt impact on a performer when discussing their ‘symptoms’ – it immediately conjures up images of infection or disease – MPA is arguably neither, and we suggest that using the same language to describe it is deeply problematic. Additionally, diagnostic labeling has ‘pejorative implications for normative behaviour’ (Hays, 2009) and arguably perpetuates the stigma, shame and silence still surrounding MPA (Patson and Loughlan, 2014). Re-imagining ‘symptoms’ as ‘the body seeking to protect itself from threat, as organisms evolved to do’ could open up new ways of thinking about how we meet challenging experiences. In sum, MPA is not a virus! Reconceptualizing MPA as a normal and adaptive response to the well-documented stressors associated with performance and a competitive and insecure profession, could profoundly change the conversation around how performers can be supported throughout their musical lives.

8. Potential developments: implications for theory and practice

Our proposed reconceptualization of MPA, from disorder to adaptive response, has implications for both theory and practice, which will now be discussed.

8.1. Implications for theory

Based on this review, we suggest the following as areas for potential future research:

• Terminological clarity: Using the same term (‘MPA’) to describe a phenomenon which can be positive, mildly challenging, a psychological disorder or a completely normal part of a performer’s life seems at best, unhelpful, at worst deeply problematic. We suggest that developing a coherent MPA theory requires greater terminological clarity.

• Conceptual clarity: A new MPA definition which moves away from the medical model and encompasses the complexity and individuality with which MPA manifests, while acknowledging the adaptive role MPA plays in terms of preparing the body to deal with perceived threat.

• A new standardized assessment tool is required to reflect a broader conceptualization of MPA, and to enable comparability across intervention studies from different theoretical perspectives.

• As research has largely focused on debilitating MPA, those who experience MPA positively and/or manage it successfully are underrepresented in scientific literature, highlighting an important gap in current understanding.

• Impact of MPA on performer and performance: the factors which mediate the intensity and interpretation of MPA (and thus its impact on performer and/or performance) are currently scattered across the literature. Further research is needed to amalgamate these factors into one coherent theory which accounts for MPA’s impact on performance and/or performer.

• MPA Etiology – Longitudinal research investigating the development of traits which predict or protect against MPA within musical training environments such as specialist music schools and conservatoires, as well as qualitative research to better understand the relationship between MPA and its comorbidities.

8.2. Implications for practice: the role of music education

Given the importance of coping with MPA in establishing and maintaining a successful performing career, you might think that it would be core business for music schools and conservatoires. Indeed, a few institutions have integrated empirically evaluated interventions into their curricula – see Candia et al. (2023) and Spahn et al. (2016) for notable examples. However, this is far from standard practice – research shows that teaching effective coping is not yet embedded in musical training (Pecen et al., 2016; Araújo et al., 2017) [As a comprehensive review of the field of music education is beyond the scope of this paper, please see the following sources for further discussion: Hildebrandt (2009), Hildebrandt and Nübling (2004), and Mazzarolo et al. (2023)].

We suggest that music education plays a critical role in the formation of perceptions and beliefs around performance, and is therefore a formative place for the development of a problematic or constructive approach to MPA. Where else than in music education – from the earliest music lessons to the training of professionals and future teachers – can a reconceptualization of MPA and the integration of effective coping strategies succeed? We suggest that musical training should, as standard practice, (1) include psychoeducation regarding the physical and psychological demands of performance (including understanding the body’s inbuilt stress response), and (2) the training of effective and empirically-validated practice and performance strategies to manage these demands [see Hildebrandt (2009) for an example of a learnable ‘stage disposition’ which includes the training of performance-appropriate distribution of muscle tension and attentional focus for peak performance]. Equipping students with a solid understanding of MPA as well as the skills to manage it seems vital in preparing the next generation of musicians to perform healthily and happily.

Author contributions

RH conducted the literature search for this review and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. RH and TC developed the ideas for both content and structure. Both authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the London Arts and Humanities Partnership.

Acknowledgments

Huge thanks to the RH’s directing supervisor Rosie Perkins and external supervisor TC, for their invaluable support and mentorship throughout RH’s doctoral studies. Enormous gratitude also to Richard Waters, for hearing these ideas in every iteration, and for sharing the inevitable ups and the downs of the research process.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alonso, Y. (2004). The biopsychosocial model in medical research: the evolution of the health concept over the last two decades. Patient Educ. Couns. 53, 239–244. doi: 10.1016/S0738-3991(03)00146-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altenmüller, E., and Ioannou, C. I. (2016). “Chapter 7 – music performance: expectations, failures, and prevention” in Performance psychology. eds. M. Raab, B. Lobinger, S. Hoffmann, A. Pizzera, and S. Laborde (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 103–119.

Google Scholar

Antonini Philippe, R., Cruder, C., Biasutti, M., and Crettaz von Roten, F. (2022). The Kenny music performance anxiety inventory-revised (K-MPAI-R): validation of the Italian version. Psychol. Music 51, 565–578. doi: 10.1177/03057356221101430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonini Philippe, R. A., Kosirnik, C., Ortuñu, E., and Biasutti, M. (2019). Between flow and anxiety: A study of the factors affecting musicians’ performances. Available at: https://serval.unil.ch/resource/serval:BIB_850B8A3468F7.P001/REF.pdf

Google Scholar

Appel, S. S. (1976). Modifying solo performance anxiety in adult pianists. J. Music. Ther. 13, 2–16. doi: 10.1093/jmt/13.1.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araújo, L. S., Wasley, D., Perkins, R., Atkins, L., Redding, E., Ginsborg, J., et al. (2017). Fit to perform: an investigation of higher education music students’ perceptions, attitudes, and Behaviors toward health. Front. Psychol. 8:1558. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aubry, L., and Küssner, M. B. (2023). Music performance anxiety and its relation to parenting style and sensory processing sensitivity. Jahrbuch Musikpsychologie 31:e155. doi: 10.5964/jbdgm.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltzell, A. L. (2016). “Self-compassion, distress tolerance, and mindfulness in performance” in Mindfulness and performance. ed. A. L. Baltzell (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 53–77.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1991). “Self-efficacy conception of anxiety” in Anxiety and self-focused attention. eds. R. Schwarzer and R. A. Wicklund (Reading: Harwood Academic Publishers), 89–110.

Google Scholar

Barbar, A. E. M., de Souza Crippa, J. A., and de Lima Osório, F. (2014). Performance anxiety in Brazilian musicians: prevalence and association with psychopathology indicators. J. Affect. Disord. 152, 381–386. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. Am. Psychol. 55, 1247–1263. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. 2nd Edn. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Barros, S., Marinho, H., Borges, N., and Pereira, A. (2022). Characteristics of music performance anxiety among undergraduate music students: a systematic review. Psychol. Music 50, 2021–2043. doi: 10.1177/03057356211066967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartel, L. R., and Thompson, E. G. (1994). Coping with performance stress: A study of professional orchestral musicians in Canada. Q. J. Music Teach. Learn. 5, 70–78.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., and Clark, D. A. (1988). Anxiety and depression: an information processing perspective. Anxiety Res. 1, 23–36. doi: 10.1080/10615808808248218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellinger, D., Wehrmann, K., Rohde, A., Schuppert, M., Störk, S., Flohr-Jost, M., et al. (2023). The application of virtual reality exposure versus relaxation training in music performance anxiety: A randomized controlled study. Review. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-2967418/v1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, H., and Klipper, M. (2009). The relaxation response. New York, NY: Harper Collins.

Google Scholar

Bharadwaj, P., Pai, M. M., and Suziedelyte, A. (2017). Mental health stigma. Econ. Lett. 159, 57–60. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2017.06.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biasutti, M., and Concina, E. (2014). The role of coping strategy and experience in predicting music performance anxiety. Music. Sci. 18, 189–202. doi: 10.1177/1029864914523282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bien Aime, J. K. (2011). Managing performance anxiety in music students: Using a solution focused approach [Doctoral Dissertation]. Fort Lauderdale, FL: Nova Southeastern University.

Google Scholar

Bissonnette, J., Dubé, F., Provencher, M. D., and Moreno Sala, M. T. (2015). Virtual reality exposure training for musicians: its effect on performance anxiety and quality. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 30, 169–177. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2015.3032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bögels, S. M., Alden, L., Beidel, D. C., Clark, L. A., Pine, D. S., Stein, M. B., et al. (2010). Social anxiety disorder: questions and answers for the DSM-V. Depress. Anxiety 27, 168–189. doi: 10.1002/da.20670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braden, A. M., Osborne, M. S., and Wilson, S. J. (2015). Psychological intervention reduces self-reported performance anxiety in high school music students. Front. Psychol. 6:195. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M. (2004). Collectivistic versus individualistic cultures: A comparison of American, Australian and Chinese music education students’ self-esteem. Music. Educ. Res. 6, 57–66. doi: 10.1080/1461380032000182830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandfonbrener, A. G. (1986). An overview of the medical problems of musicians. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 34, 165–169. doi: 10.1080/07448481.1986.9939631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandfonbrener, A. G. (1990). Beta blockers in the treatment of performance anxiety. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5, 23–26.

Google Scholar

Brandfonbrener, A. G. (1999). Performance anxiety." different strokes for different folks". Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 14, 101–102.

Google Scholar

Brandfonbrener, A. G. (2004). Healthier music students: can medicine and music prescribe in concert. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 19, 1–2. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2004.1001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brantigan, C. O., Brantigan, T. A., and Joseph, N. (1979). The effect of beta blockade on stage fright. A controlled study. Rocky Mt. Med. J. 76, 227–233.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

British Association for Performing Arts Medicnie (BAPAM). (n.d.) Managing performance anxiety. BAPAM. Available at: https://www.bapam.org.uk/managing-performance-anxiety/

Google Scholar

Brodsky, W. (1996). music performance anxiety reconceptualized: A critique of current research practices and findings. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 11, 88–98.

Google Scholar

Brodsky, W., and Sloboda, J. A. (1997). Clinical trial of a music generated vibrotactile therapeutic environment for musicians: Main effects and outcome differences between therapy subgroups. J. Music. Ther. 34, 2–32. doi: 10.1093/jmt/34.1.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooker, E. (2018). Music performance anxiety: A clinical outcome study into the effects of cognitive hypnotherapy and eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing in advanced pianists. Psychol. Music 46, 107–124. doi: 10.1177/0305735617703473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugués, A. O. (2011a). Music performance anxiety—part 1. A review of its epidemiology. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 26, 102–105. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2011.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugués, A. O. (2011b). Music performance anxiety—part 2: A review of treatment options. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 26, 164–171. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2011.3026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugués, A. O. (2019). Music performance anxiety: A comprehensive update of the literature. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Google Scholar

Burin, A. B., Barbar, A. E. M., Nirenberg, I. S., Osório, F. D. L., Burin, A. B., Barbar, A. E. M., et al. (2019). Music performance anxiety: perceived causes, coping strategies and clinical profiles of Brazilian musicians. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 41, 348–357. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2018-0104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burin, A. B., and de Lima Osório, F. (2016). Interventions for music performance anxiety: results from a systematic. Arch. Clin. Psychiatry 43, 116–131. doi: 10.1590/0101-60830000000097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burin, A. B., and Osório, F. L. (2017). Music performance anxiety: A critical review of etiological aspects, perceived causes, coping strategies and treatment. Arch. Clin. Psychiatry 44, 127–133. doi: 10.1590/0101-60830000000136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butković, A., Vukojević, N., and Carević, S. (2022). Music performance anxiety and perfectionism in Croatian musicians. Psychol. Music 50, 100–110. doi: 10.1177/0305735620978692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Candia, V., Kusserow, M., Margulies, O., and Hildebrandt, H. (2023). Repeated stage exposure reduces music performance anxiety. Front. Psychol. 14:1146405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. Am. J. Psychol. 39, 106–124. doi: 10.2307/1415404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., Smith, N. C., and Williams, A. M. (2011). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion 11, 595–602. doi: 10.1037/a0023225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M.-Y. (2011). The relationship between music performance anxiety, age, self-esteem, and performance outcomes in Hong Kong music students [PhD thesis]. Durham: Durham University.

Google Scholar

Chang, J. C., Midlarsky, E., and Lin, P. (2003). Effects of meditation on music performance anxiety. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 18, 126–130. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2003.3022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang-Arana, Á. M., Kenny, D. T., and Burga-León, A. A. (2018). Validation of the Kenny Music Performance Anxiety inventory (K-MPAI): A cross-cultural confirmation of its factorial structure. Psychol. Music 46, 551–567. doi: 10.1177/0305735617717618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chattin, L. B. (2019). The big five personality types and music performance anxiety in collegiate piano students. [PhD Thesis]. Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina.

Google Scholar

Cheng, Y. (2020). Psychological intervention of music therapy on performance anxiety of vocal music students. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica 29:1333. doi: 10.24205/03276716.2020.375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, K., and Mercado, E. (2020). Performance anxiety and the plasticity of emotional responses. Cognit. Emot. 34, 1309–1325. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2020.1749568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. B. (1989). Performance-related medical and psychological disorders in instrumental musicians. Ann. Behav. Med. 11, 28–34. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm1101_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. B., and Agras, W. S. (1991). The assessment and treatment of performance anxiety in musicians. Am. J. Psychiatry 148, 598–605. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.5.598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, T., Lisboa, T., and Williamon, A. (2014). An investigation into musicians’ thoughts and perceptions during performance. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 36, 19–37. doi: 10.1177/1321103X14523531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, T., and Williamon, A. (2011). Evaluation of a mental skills training program for musicians. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 23, 342–359. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.574676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, L. K., Osborne, M. S., and Baranoff, J. A. (2020). Examining a group acceptance and commitment therapy intervention for music performance anxiety in student vocalists. Front. Psychol. 11:1127. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Bodner, E. (2019). Music performance skills: A two-pronged approach–facilitating optimal music performance and reducing music performance anxiety. Psychol. Music 47, 521–538. doi: 10.1177/0305735618765349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conable, B., and Conable, W. (1995). How to learn the Alexander technique: A manual for students. Harrisburg, PA: Andover Press.

Google Scholar

Cornett, V., and Urhan, G. (2021). Performance anxiety experiences and coping techniques of Turkish music students and their teachers. Int. J. Music. Educ. 39, 504–519. doi: 10.1177/02557614211005907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coşkun-Şentürk, G., and Çırakoğlu, O. C. (2018). How guilt/shame proneness and coping styles are related to music performance anxiety and stress symptoms by gender. Psychol. Music 46, 682–698. doi: 10.1177/0305735617721338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, W. J., and Kenardy, J. (1993). Performance anxiety, social phobia, and setting effects in instrumental music students. J. Anxiety Disord. 7, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(93)90020-L

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craske, M. G., and Craig, K. D. (1984). Musical performance anxiety: the three-systems model and self-efficacy theory. Behav. Res. Ther. 22, 267–280. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(84)90007-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czajkowski, A.-M. L., Greasley, A. E., and Allis, M. (2020). Mindfulness for musicians: A mixed methods study investigating the effects of 8-week mindfulness courses on music students at a leading conservatoire. Music. Sci. 26, 259–279. doi: 10.1177/1029864920941570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Felice, M. G. (2004). Mindfulness meditation: A new tool for understanding and regulating musical performance anxiety. An affective neuroscientific perspective [DMA Dissertation]. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.

Google Scholar

de Figueiredo Rocha, S. (2020). Music performance anxiety: pilot study with gender endocrine biomarkers. EC Psychol. Psychiatry 9, 01–05.

Google Scholar

Deen, D. R. (1999). Awareness of breathing; keys to the moderation of MPA [Doctoral Dissertation]. Lexington, KY: University of Kentucky.

Google Scholar

Dempsey, E., and Comeau, G. (2019). Music performance anxiety and self-efficacy in young musicians: effects of gender and age. Music Perform. Res. 9, 60–79.

Google Scholar

Diaz, F. M. (2018). Relationships among meditation, perfectionism, mindfulness, and performance anxiety among collegiate music students. J. Res. Music. Educ. 66, 150–167. doi: 10.1177/0022429418765447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dilling, H., and Freyberger, H. J. (2015). Taschenführer zur ICD10-Klassifikation psychischer Störungen. 8th Edn. Göttingen: Hogrefe. (secondary source, cited in Osborne & Kirsner, 2022)

Google Scholar

Dobos, B., Piko, B. F., and Kenny, D. T. (2019). Music performance anxiety and its relationship with social phobia and dimensions of perfectionism. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 41, 310–326. doi: 10.1177/1321103X18804295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobson, M. C. (2011). Insecurity, professional sociability, and alcohol: young freelance musicians’ perspectives on work and life in the music profession. Psychol. Music 39, 240–260. doi: 10.1177/0305735610373562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, D., Downs, M. F., Golberstein, E., and Zivin, K. (2009). Stigma and help seeking for mental health among college students. Med. Care Res. Rev. 66, 522–541. doi: 10.1177/1077558709335173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Endo, S., Juhlberg, K., Bradbury, A., and Wing, A. M. (2014). Interaction between physiological and subjective states predicts the effect of a judging panel on the postures of cellists in performance. Front. Psychol. 5:773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science 196, 129–136. doi: 10.1126/science.847460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Errico, A. S. (2012). The effects of a researcher-designed intervention on elementary students’ music performance anxiety levels [Doctoral Dissertation]. Boston, MA: Boston University.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cognit. Emot. 6, 409–434. doi: 10.1080/02699939208409696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkai, P., Wittchen, H. U., and Döpfner, M. (2015). Diagnostisches und statistisches manual psychischer Störungen DSM-5®. Göttingen: Hogrefe. (secondary source, cited in Osborne & Kirsner, 2022)

Google Scholar

Farnsworth-Grodd, V. (2012). Mindfulness and the self-regulation of music performance anxiety [Doctoral Dissertation]. Auckland: Auckland University.

Google Scholar

Faur, A. L., Pintea, S., Vaida, S., and Opre, A. N. (2022). The efficacy of cognitive and behavioral interventions upon music performance anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Music 51, 357–372. doi: 10.1177/03057356221115461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazey, J. A., and Hardy, L. (1988). “The inverted-U hypothesis: A catastrophe for sport psychology” in British Association of Sports Sciences, monograph no. 1 (Leeds: National Coaching Foundation).

Google Scholar

Fehm, L., and Schmidt, K. (2006). Performance anxiety in gifted adolescent musicians. J. Anxiety Disord. 20, 98–109. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernholz, I., Mumm, J. L. M., Plag, J., Noeres, K., Rotter, G., Willich, S. N., et al. (2019). Performance anxiety in professional musicians: A systematic review on prevalence, risk factors and clinical treatment effects. Psychol. Med. 49, 2287–2306. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finch, K., and Moscovitch, D. (2016). Imagery-based interventions for music performance anxiety: an integrative review. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 31, 222–231. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2016.4040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finch, K. K., and Oakman, J. M. (2022). “Applied mental imagery and music performance anxiety” in Music and mental imagery. eds. Mats, B. Küssner, Liila, Taruffi, and Georgia, A. Floridou (Abingdon: Routledge), 221–230.

Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., Middlestadt, S. E., Ottati, V., Straus, S., and Ellis, A. (1988). Medical problems among ICSOM musicians: overview of a national survey. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 3, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Ford, J., and Arvinen-Barrow, M. (2019). Exploring the use of psychological skills training interventions in a music domain: A systematic review. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 34, 222–229. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2019.4033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., and Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 14, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90181-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gannon, P. (2019). Is it anxiety or arousal that can facilitate musical performance? Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 34, 118–119. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2019.2018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gembris, H., and Heye, A. (2012). Älter werden im Orchester. Institut Für Begabungsforschung in Der Musik 18, 371–391. doi: 10.1177/1029864914548912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gembris, H., Heye, A., and Seifert, A. (2018). Health problems of orchestral musicians from a life-span perspective: results of a large-scale study. Music Sci. 1, 1–20. doi: 10.1177/2059204317739801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsborg, J. (2019). Managing music performance anxiety. Am Music Teacher 68, 16–18.

Google Scholar

Ginsborg, J., Kreutz, G., Thomas, M., and Williamon, A. (2009). Healthy behaviours in music and non-music performance students. Health Educ. 109, 242–258. doi: 10.1108/09654280910955575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez, P., Nielsen, C., Studer, R. K., Hildebrandt, H., Klumb, P. L., Nater, U. M., et al. (2018). Prolonged performance-related neuroendocrine activation and perseverative cognition in low- and high-anxious university music students. Psychoneuroendocrinology 95, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González, A., Blanco-Piñeiro, P., and Díaz-Pereira, M. P. (2018). Music performance anxiety: exploring structural relations with self-efficacy, boost, and self-rated performance. Psychol. Music 46, 831–847. doi: 10.1177/0305735617727822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goren, L. (2014). A meta-analysis of nonpharmacologic psychotherapies for music performance anxiety [Doctoral Dissertation]. San Francisco, CA: California Institute of Integral Studies.

Google Scholar

Greene, D. (2012). Performance success. Abingdon: Routledge Ltd.

Google Scholar

Grindea, C. (1984). Tensions in the performance of music: A symposium. New York: Alexander Broude.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, S. A., and Musgrave, G. (2016). Can music make you sick? Music and depression (A Study into the Incidence of Musicians’ Mental Health). pp. 1–69). London: Help Musicians UK.

Google Scholar

Guyon, A. J. A. A., Cannavò, R., Studer, R. K., Hildebrandt, H., Danuser, B., Vlemincx, E., et al. (2020). Respiratory variability, sighing, anxiety, and breathing symptoms in low- and high-anxious music students before and after performing. Front. Psychol. 11:303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halleland, H. B., Harris, A., Sørnes, S., Murison, R., and Ursin, H. (2009). Subjective health complaints, stress, and coping in orchestra musicians. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 24, 58–62. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2009.2014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, D. L. (1982). An assessment of anxiety in instrumental and vocal performances. J. Res. Music. Educ. 30, 77–90. doi: 10.2307/3345040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, D. L. (1985). The other side of stage fright. Music. Educ. J. 71, 26–28. doi: 10.2307/3396494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, D. L., and Sobaje, M. (1983). Anxiety and the college musician: a study of performance conditions and subject variables. Psychol. Music 11, 37–50. doi: 10.1177/0305735683111005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, L., and Parfitt, G. (1991). A catastrophe model of anxiety and performance. Br. J. Psychol. 82, 163–178. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02391.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, B. S. (2002). Workplace and health: a survey of classical orchestral musicians in the United Kingdom and Germany. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 17, 83–92. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2002.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. R. (1986). A psychologist Viéws musical performance anxiety. Am. Music Teacher 35:24.

Google Scholar

Hatfield, J. L. (2016). Performing at the top of One’s musical game. Front. Psychol. 7:1356. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hays, K. F. (2009). Performance psychology in action: A casebook for working with athletes, performing artists, business leaders, and professionals in high-risk occupations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Helding, L. (2016). Music performance anxiety. J. Sing. 73, 83–90.

Google Scholar

Hernández, S. O., Zarza-Alzugaray, F. J., and Casanova, O. (2018). Music performance anxiety. Substance use and career abandonment in Spanish music students. Int. J. Music. Educ. 36, 460–472. doi: 10.1177/0255761418763903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewitt, P. L., and Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 456–470. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hildebrandt, H. (2009). “Teaching music physiology and motor learning processes at a university: experience and evaluation” in Art in motion. Musical and athletic motor learning and performance. ed. A. Mornell (Frankfurt: Peter Lang), 191–222.

Google Scholar

Hildebrandt, H., and Nübling, M. (2004). Providing further training in musicophysiology to instrumental teachers: do their professional and preprofessional students derive any benefit? Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 19, 62–69. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2004.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hildebrandt, H., Nübling, M., and Candia, V. (2012). Increment of fatigue, depression, and stage fright during the first year of high-level education in music students. MPPA 27, 43–48. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2012.1008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hitchcock, P. F., Martin, L. M., Fischer, L., Marando-Blanck, S., and Herbert, J. D. (2016). Popular conceptions of mindfulness: awareness and emotional control. Mindfulness 7, 940–949. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0533-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, S. L., and Hanrahan, S. J. (2012). Mental skills for musicians: managing music performance anxiety and enhancing performance. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 1, 17–28. doi: 10.1037/a0025409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, J. (2001). An orchestra musician’s perspective on 20 years of performing arts medicine. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 16, 102–108. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2001.3018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, W.-L., and Song, B. (2021). How do college musicians self-manage musical performance anxiety: strategies through time periods and types of performance. Int. J. Music. Educ. 39, 95–118. doi: 10.1177/0255761421990800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, I. (1998). Western orchestral musicians are highly stressed. Resonance Int. Music Council 26, 19–20.

Google Scholar

James, I. M. (2000). “Survey of orchestras” in Medical problems of the instrumentalist musician. eds. R. Tubiana and P. C. Amadio (London: Martin Dunitz), 195–201.

Google Scholar

James, I., and Savage, I. (1983). A comparison of the effects on musical performance of two methods designed to reduce situational anxiety. J. Int. Society Study Tension Perform. 1, 34–38.

Google Scholar

Jones, G., and Swain, A. (1992). Intensity and direction as dimensions of competitive state anxiety and relationships with competitiveness. Percept. Mot. Skills 74, 467–472. doi: 10.2466/pms.1992.74.2.467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juncos, D. G., and Markman, E. J. (2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy for the treatment of music performance anxiety: A single subject design with a university student. Psychology of Music, 44, 935–953.

Google Scholar

Juncos, D. G., and de Paiva e Pona, E. (2018). Acceptance and commitment therapy as a clinical anxiety treatment and performance enhancement program for musicians: towards an evidence-based practice model within performance psychology. Music Sci. 1:205920431774880. doi: 10.1177/2059204317748807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juncos, D. G., and de Paiva e Pona, E. (2022). ACT for musicians: A guide for using acceptance and commitment training to enhance performance, overcome performance anxiety, and improve well-being. Irvine, CA: Universal-Publishers.

Google Scholar

Juncos, D. G., Heinrichs, G. A., Towle, P., Duffy, K., Grand, S. M., Morgan, M. C., et al. (2017). Acceptance and commitment therapy for the treatment of music performance anxiety: A pilot study with student vocalists. Front. Psychol. 8:986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kageyama, N. J. (2022). Why trying to calm down backstage could backfire. Bulletproof Musician. Available at: https://bulletproofmusician.com/what-leads-to-better-performances-telling-yourself-to-calm-down-or-get-excited/

Google Scholar

Kantor-Martynuska, J., Domaradzka, E., Kenny, D. T., and Holmes, J. (2018). Performance anxiety: the need for an integrative approach. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 19, 272–282. doi: 10.24425/119495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaspersen, M., and Götestam, K. G. (2002). A survey of music performance anxiety among Norwegian music students. Europ. J. Psychiatry 16, 69–80.

Google Scholar

Kelley, J., and Farley, A. (2019). Self-compassion levels in music and non-music students. Contrib. Music. Educ. 44, 167–184.

Google Scholar

Kendrick, M. J., Craig, K. D., Lawson, D. M., and Davidson, P. O. (1982). Cognitive and behavioral therapy for musical-performance anxiety. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 50, 353–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.50.3.353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2004). Music performance anxiety: is it the music, the performance or the anxiety. Music Forum 10, 38–43.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2005). A systematic review of treatments for music performance anxiety. Anxiety Stress Coping 18, 183–208. doi: 10.1080/10615800500167258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2006). Music performance anxiety: origins, phenomenology, assessment and treatment. Context J. Music Res. 31, 51–64.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2009). “Negative emotions in music making: performance anxiety” in Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. eds. P. N. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 425–451.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. (2011). The psychology of music performance anxiety. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D., and Ackermann, B. (2015). Performance-related musculoskeletal pain, depression and music performance anxiety in professional orchestral musicians: A population study. Psychol. Music 43, 43–60. doi: 10.1177/0305735613493953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T., Davis, P., and Oates, J. (2004). Music performance anxiety and occupational stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with state and trait anxiety and perfectionism. J. Anxiety Disord. 18, 757–777. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D., Driscoll, T., and Ackermann, B. (2014). Psychological well-being in professional orchestral musicians in Australia: A descriptive population study. Psychol. Music 42, 210–232. doi: 10.1177/0305735612463950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D., Driscoll, T., and Ackermann, B. (2016). Is playing in the pit really the pits?: pain, strength, music performance anxiety, and workplace satisfaction in professional musicians in stage, pit, and combined stage/pit orchestras. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 31, 1–7. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2016.1001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T., and Holmes, J. (2015). Exploring the attachment narrative of a professional musician with severe performance anxiety: A case report. J. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 1–6. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T., and Holmes, J. (2018). Attachment quality is associated with music performance anxiety in professional musicians: an exploratory narrative study. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 49, 283–298. doi: 10.24425/119496

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T., and Osborne, M. S. (2006). Music performance anxiety: new insights from young musicians. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2, 103–112. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0049-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. B. S., Shorter, S. M., Cope, S., Wyshak, G., and Sklar, E. (2009). Yoga ameliorates performance anxiety and mood disturbance in young professional musicians. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 34, 279–289. doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9103-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiik-Salupere, V., and Ross, J. (2020). “How professional and student singers Cope with performance anxiety” in The Routledge companion to interdisciplinary studies in singing, vol. 2, Eds. Helga, R. Gudmundsdottir, Carol, Beynon, Karen, Ludke, and Annabel, J.Cohen (Abingdon: Routledge), 241–252.

Google Scholar

Kim, Y. (2008). The effect of improvisation-assisted desensitization, and music-assisted progressive muscle relaxation and imagery on reducing pianists’ music performance anxiety. J. Music. Ther. 45, 165–191. doi: 10.1093/jmt/45.2.165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirchner, J. M. (2003). A qualitative inquiry into musical performance anxiety. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 18, 78–82. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2003.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsner, J., Wilson, S. J., and Osborne, M. S. (2023). Music performance anxiety: the role of early parenting experiences and cognitive schemas. Front. Psychol. 14:1185296. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1185296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kivimäki, M., and Jokinen, M. (1994). Job perceptions and well-being among symphony orchestra musicians: A comparison with other occupational groups. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 9, 73–76.

Google Scholar

Kobori, O., Yoshie, M., Kudo, K., and Ohtsuki, T. (2011). Traits and cognitions of perfectionism and their relation with coping style, effort, achievement, and performance anxiety in Japanese musicians. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 674–679. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreutz, G., Ginsborg, J., and Williamon, A. (2008). Music students’ health problems and health-promoting behaviours. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 23, 3–11. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2008.1002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langendörfer, F., Hodapp, V., Kreutz, G., and Bongard, S. (2006). Personality and performance anxiety among professional orchestra musicians. J. Individ. Differ. 27, 162–171. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.27.3.162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, M. (2019). Music performance anxiety as hidden desire and emerging self: The development and exploration of a conceptual lens for performers and practitioners [Doctoral Dissertation]. London: Guildhall School of Music & Drama.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (2006). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Springer publishing company.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, A. A., and Abramovitz, A. (2004). A multimodal behavioral approach to performance anxiety. J. Clin. Psychol. 60, 831–840. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer publishing company.

Google Scholar

Lederman, R. J. (1999). Medical treatment of performance anxiety: A statement in favor. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 14, 117–121.

Google Scholar

Lehrer, P. M. (1987). A review of the approaches to the Management of Tension and Stage Fright in music performance. J. Res. Music. Educ. 35, 143–153. doi: 10.2307/3344957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrer, P. M., Goldman, N. S., and Strommen, E. F. (1990). A principal components assessment of performance anxiety among musicians. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5, 12–18.

Google Scholar

Lehrer, P. M., and Woolfolk, R. L. (1982). Self-report assessment of anxiety: somatic, cognitive, and behavioral modalities. Behav. Assess. 4, 167–177.

Google Scholar

Lin, P., Chang, J., Zemon, V., and Midlarsky, E. (2008). Silent illumination: A study on Chan (zen) meditation, anxiety, and musical performance quality. Psychol. Music 36, 139–155. doi: 10.1177/0305735607080840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liston, M., Frost, A. A., and Mohr, P. B. (2003). The prediction of musical performance anxiety. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 18, 120–125. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2003.3021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockwood, A. H. (1989). Medical problems of musicians. N. Engl. J. Med. 320, 221–227. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198901263200405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, E. C. (2022). Managing music performance anxiety: A performer’s perspective. Kelowna, BC: University of British Columbia.

Google Scholar

Lupiáñez, M., Ortiz, F. D. P., Vila, J., and Muñoz, M. A. (2022). Predictors of music performance anxiety in conservatory students. Psychol. Music 50, 1005–1022. doi: 10.1177/03057356211032290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyon, N., and Plisco, M. (2020). The effects of self-compassion and mindfulness on performance anxiety and flow in elite athletes. J. Sport Behav. 43, 426–441.

Google Scholar

MacAfee, E., and Comeau, G. (2022). Teacher perspective on music performance anxiety: an exploration of coping strategies used by music teachers. Br. J. Music Educ. 40, 34–53. doi: 10.1017/S0265051722000146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacNamara, Á., Holmes, P., and Collins, D. (2008). Negotiating transitions in musical development: the role of psychological characteristics of developing excellence. Psychol. Music 36, 335–352. doi: 10.1177/0305735607086041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahony, S., Juncos, D. G., and Winter, D. (2022). Acceptance and commitment coaching for music performance anxiety: piloting a six-week group course with undergraduate dance & musical theatre students. Front. Psychol. 13:230. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.830230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manchester, R. A. (2011). The biopsychosocial model and performing arts medicine. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 26, 121–122. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2011.3020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matei, R., Broad, S., Goldbart, J., and Ginsborg, J. (2018). Health education for musicians. Front. Psychol. 9:1137. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matei, R., and Ginsborg, J. (2017). Music performance anxiety in classical musicians – what we know about what works. BJPsych. Int. 14, 33–35. doi: 10.1192/S2056474000001744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzarolo, I., Burwell, K., and Schubert, E. (2023). Teachers’ approaches to music performance anxiety management: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1205150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1205150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarty, R. (2016). “Chapter 4- the fight-or-flight response: A cornerstone of stress research” in Stress: Concepts, cognition, emotion, and behavior. ed. G. Fink, vol. 1 (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 33–37.

Google Scholar

McGinnis, A. M., and Milling, L. S. (2005). Psychological treatment of musical performance anxiety: current status and future directions. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. Train. 42, 357–373. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.42.3.357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGrath, C. (2012). Music performance anxiety therapies: A review of the literature [Doctoral Dissertation]. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

Google Scholar

McNeil, D. G., Loi, N. M., and Bullen, R. (2022). Investigating the moderating role of coping style on music performance anxiety and perfectionism. Int. J. Music. Educ. 40, 587–597. doi: 10.1177/02557614221080523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Middlestadt, S. E. (1990). Medical problems of symphony orchestra musicians: from counting people with problems to evaluating interventions. Revista Interamericana de Psicología 24, 159–172.

Google Scholar

Miller, S. R., and Chesky, K. (2004). The multidimensional anxiety theory: an assessment of and relationships between intensity and direction of cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence over multiple performance requirements among college music majors. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 19, 12–20. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2004.1003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, N. (2011). Evaluation and performance anxiety in music study. Can. Music. Educ. 53, 32–34.

Google Scholar

Montello, L. (1989). Utilizing music therapy as a mode of treatment for the performance stress of professional musicians [Doctoral Dissertation]. New York: New York University.

Google Scholar

Montello, L., Coons, E. E., and Kantor, J. (1990). The use of music therapy as a treatment for musical performance stress. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5:49.

Google Scholar

Mor, S., Day, H., Flett, G. L., and Hewitt, P. L. (1995). Perfectionism, control, and components of performance anxiety in professional artists. Cogn. Ther. Res. 19, 207–225. doi: 10.1007/bf02229695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno-Gutierréz, J. A., López-González, M. V., de Rojas Leal, C., Chao-Ecija, A., and Dawid-Milner, M. S. (2023). Impact of music performance anxiety on cardiovascular blood pressure responses, autonomic tone and baroreceptor sensitivity to a western classical music piano-concert. Front. Neurosci. 17:1213117. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1213117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mornell, A., and Wulf, G. (2019). Adopting an external focus of attention enhances musical performance. J. Res. Music. Educ. 66, 375–391. doi: 10.1177/0022429418801573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moura, N., and Serra, S. (2021). Listening to teachers’ voices: constructs on music performance anxiety in artistic education. J. Sci. Technol. Arts 13, 99–117. doi: 10.34632/jsta.2021.9853

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumm, J., Fernholz, I., Ströhle, A., Plag, J., and Schmidt, A. (2020). Performance anxiety among musicians. Z. Neuropsychol. 31, 76–80. doi: 10.1024/1016-264X/a000294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagel, J. J. (1990). Performance anxiety and the performing musician: A fear of failure or a fear of success. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5, 37–40.

Google Scholar

Nagel, J. J. (1993). Stage fright in musicians: A psychodynamic perspective. Bull. Menn. Clin. 57, 492–503.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nagel, J. J. (2010). Treatment of music performance anxiety via psychological approaches. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 25, 141–148. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2010.4031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagel, J. J., Himle, D. P., and Papsdorf, J. D. (1989). Cognitive-behavioural treatment of musical performance anxiety. Psychol. Music 17, 12–21. doi: 10.1177/0305735689171002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neftel, K. A., Adler, R. H., Kappeli, L., Rossi, M., Dolder, M., Kaser, H. E., et al. (1982). Stage fright in musicians: A model illustrating the effect of beta blockers. Psychosom. Med. 44, 461–469. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198211000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholson, D. R., Cody, M. W., and Beck, J. G. (2015). Anxiety in musicians: on and off stage. Psychol. Music 43, 438–449. doi: 10.1177/0305735614540018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, C., Studer, R. K., Hildebrandt, H., Nater, U. M., Wild, P., Danuser, B., et al. (2018). The relationship between music performance anxiety, subjective performance quality and post-event rumination among music students. Psychol. Music 46, 136–152. doi: 10.1177/0305735617706539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, B. K., Pratt, R. R., and Maughan, M. L. (1993). Biofeedback training, selected coping strategies, and music relaxation interventions to reduce debilitative musical performance anxiety. Int. J. Arts Med. 2, 7–15.

Google Scholar

Nordin-Bates, S. M. (2012). “Performance psychology in the performing arts” in The Oxford handbook of sport and performance psychology, Eds. S. M. Murphy (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 81–114.

Google Scholar

Norton, G. R., MacLean, L., and Wachna, E. (1978). The use of cognitive desensitization and self-directed mastery training for treating stage fright. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2, 61–64. doi: 10.1007/BF01172513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orejudo, S., Zarza-Alzugaray, F. J., Casanova, O., Rodríguez-Ledo, C., and Mazas, B. (2017). The relation of music performance anxiety (MPA) to optimism, self-efficacy, and sensitivity to reward and punishment: testing Barlow’s theory of personal vulnerability on a sample of Spanish music students. Psychol. Music 45, 570–583. doi: 10.1177/0305735616674791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., and Franklin, J. (2002). Cognitive processes in music performance anxiety. Aust. J. Psychol. 54, 86–93. doi: 10.1080/00049530210001706543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M., Glasser, S., and Loveridge, B. (2022). ‘It’s not so scary anymore. It’s actually exhilarating’: A proof-of-concept study using virtual reality technology for music performance training under pressure. Sydney: ASCILITE Publications. e22116.

Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., Greene, D. J., and Immel, D. T. (2014). Managing performance anxiety and improving mental skills in conservatoire students through performance psychology training: A pilot study. Psychol. Well-Being 4:18. doi: 10.1186/s13612-014-0018-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., and Kenny, D. T. (2005). Development and validation of a music performance anxiety inventory for gifted adolescent musicians. J. Anxiety Disord. 19, 725–751. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., and Kenny, D. T. (2008). The role of sensitizing experiences in music performance anxiety in adolescent musicians. Psychol. Music 36, 447–462. doi: 10.1177/0305735607086051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., Kenny, D. T., and Cooksey, J. (2007). Impact of a cognitive-behavioural treatment program on music performance anxiety in secondary school music students: A pilot study. Music. Sci. 11, 53–84. doi: 10.1177/10298649070110S204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., and Kirsner, J. (2022). “Music performance anxiety” in The Oxford handbook of music performance. ed. G. E. McPherson, vol. 2 (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 204–231.

Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., and McPherson, G. E. (2019). Precompetitive appraisal, performance anxiety and confidence in conservatorium musicians: A case for coping. Psychol. Music 47, 451–462. doi: 10.1177/0305735618755000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, M. S., Munzel, B., and Greenaway, K. H. (2020). Emotion goals in music performance anxiety. Front. Psychol. 11:1138. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oudejans, R. R. D., Spitse, A., Kralt, E., and Bakker, F. C. (2017). Exploring the thoughts and attentional focus of music students under pressure. Psychol. Music 45, 216–230. doi: 10.1177/0305735616656790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oyan, S. (2006). Mindfulness meditation: Creative musical performance through awareness. [DMA dissertation]. Available at: https://www.proquest.com/openview/06698276f6c99dc535fd28ed6c1ef4c4/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y

Google Scholar

Papageorgi, I. (2020). Prevalence and predictors of music performance anxiety in adolescent learners: contributions of individual, task-related and environmental factors. Music. Sci. 26:3128. doi: 10.1177/1029864920923128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papageorgi, I., Haddon, E., Creech, A., Morton, F., Bezenac, C. D., Himonides, E., et al. (2010). Institutional culture and learning II: inter-relationships between perceptions of the learning environment and undergraduate musicians’ attitudes to performance. Music. Educ. Res. 12, 427–446. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2010.520432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papageorgi, I., Hallam, S., and Welch, G. F. (2007). A conceptual framework for understanding musical performance anxiety. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 28, 83–107. doi: 10.1177/1321103X070280010207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parasuraman, S., and Purohit, Y. S. (2000). Distress and boredom among orchestra musicians: the two faces of stress. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 74–83. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J.-E. (2010). The Relationshiop between musical performance anxiety, healthy lifestyle factors, and substance use among young adult classical musicians: Implications for training and education [PhD Thesis]. New York, NY: Columbia University.

Google Scholar

Patson, T., and Loughlan, T. (2014). Playing with performance: the use and abuse of beta-blockers in the performing arts. Victorian J. Music Educ. 1, 3–10.

Google Scholar

Pecen, E. (2019). Development of an evidence-based psychological skills training program for professional music performance [Doctoral]. Preston: University of Central Lancashire.

Google Scholar

Pecen, E., Collins, D., and Mac Namara, Á. (2016). Music of the night: performance practitioner considerations for enhancement work in music. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 5, 377–395. doi: 10.1037/spy0000067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pecen, E., Collins, D. J., and Mac Namara, Á. (2018). “It’s your problem. Deal with it.” performers’ experiences of psychological challenges in music. Front. Psychol. 8:2374. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pell, D. R. (2020). Insulating musical motor skills against music performance anxiety [PhD Thesis]. Toronto: University of Toronto (Canada).

Google Scholar

Perdomo-Guevara, E. (2014). Is music performance anxiety just an individual problem? Exploring the impact of musical environments on performers’ approaches to performance and emotions. Psychomusicology 24, 66–74. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, E., Stolterfoth, C., Spahn, C., Schmidt, H. U., Timmermann, T., and Wittmann, M. (2020). Preventing music performance anxiety (MPA): music students judge combined depth relaxation music therapy (DRMT) and silence to be an effective methodology. Music and Medicine 12:148. doi: 10.47513/mmd.v12i3.688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plott, T. M. (1986). An investigation of the hypnotic treatment of music performance anxiety [PhD Thesis]. University of Tennessee, Knoxville.

Google Scholar

Powell, D. H. (2004). Treating individuals with debilitating performance anxiety: an introduction. J. Clin. Psychol. 60, 801–808. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reubart, D. (1985). Anxiety and musical performance: On playing the piano from memory. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.

Google Scholar

Rider, M. S. (1987). Music therapy: therapy for debilitated musicians. Music. Ther. Perspect. 4, 40–43. doi: 10.1093/mtp/4.1.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rife, N. A., Lapidus, L. B., and Shnek, Z. M. (2000). Musical performance anxiety, cognitive flexibility, and field independence in professional musicians. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 15, 161–167.

Google Scholar

Roland, D. (1994). How professional performers manage performance anxiety. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 2, 25–35. doi: 10.1177/1321103X9400200105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruggiero, M. (2012). Using attentional control theory to account for anxiety-related errors on musical performance tasks [Doctoral Dissertation]. Perth: Curtin University.

Google Scholar

Rumsey, H. (2015). Anxiety’s effect on muscle activation and fatigue in trumpet players: A pilot study. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 30, 203–210. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2015.4038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabino, A. D., Chagas, M. H. N., and Osório, F. L. (2020). Acute effects of oxytocin in music performance anxiety: A crossover, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology 237, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s00213-020-05493-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadler, M. E., and Miller, C. J. (2010). Performance anxiety: A longitudinal study of the roles of personality and experience in musicians. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 1, 280–287. doi: 10.1177/1948550610370492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safirstein, L. (1962). Stage fright in a musician: a segment of an analysis. Am. J. Psychoanal. 22, 15–42. doi: 10.1007/BF01871614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmon, P. G. (1990). A psychological perspective on musical performance anxiety: a review of the literature. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5, 2–11.

Google Scholar

Salmon, P. G. (1992). Performance anxiety. In Comprehensive casebook of cognitive therapy (A. FreemanIn and F. M. Dattilio (eds), pp. 61–69). New York: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Salmon, P. G., and Meyer, R. G. (1992). Notes from the green room: Coping with stress and anxiety in musical performance. Princeton, NJ: Maxwell Macmillan Int.

Google Scholar

Sarıkaya, M., and Kurtaslan, Z. (2018). Prediction of musical performance anxiety according to music teacher candidates’ perfectionism and self-efficacy beliefs. Int. Online J. Educ. Sci. 10:10. doi: 10.15345/iojes.2018.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sataloff, R. T., Rosen, D. C., and Levy, S. (1999). Medical treatment of performance anxiety: A comprehensive approach. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 14, 122–126.

Google Scholar

Schanche, E., Vøllestad, J., Binder, P.-E., Hjeltnes, A., Dundas, I., and Nielsen, G. H. (2020). Participant experiences of change in mindfulness-based stress reduction for anxiety disorders. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well Being 15:1776094. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2020.1776094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuele, S. U., and Lederman, R. J. (2004). Occupational disorders in instrumental musicians. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 19, 123–128. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2004.3021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senyshyn, Y. (1999). Perspectives on performance and anxiety and their implications for creative teaching. Canadian J. Educ. 24, 30–41. doi: 10.2307/1585769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senyshyn, Y., and O’Neill, S. A. (2001). Subjective experience of anxiety and musical performance: A relational perspective. Philos. Music Educ. Rev. 9, 42–53.

Google Scholar

Shaw, T. A., Juncos, D. G., and Winter, D. (2020). Piloting a new model for treating music performance anxiety: training a singing teacher to use acceptance and commitment coaching with a student. Front. Psychol. 11:882. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simoens, V. L., Puttonen, S., and Tervaniemi, M. (2015). Are music performance anxiety and performance boost perceived as extremes of the same continuum? Psychol. Music 43, 171–187. doi: 10.1177/0305735613499200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinden, L. M. (1999). Music performance anxiety: Contributions of perfectionism, coping style, self-efficacy, and self-esteem (60, 590. Tempe, AZ: Arizona State University.

Google Scholar

Skoogh, F., and Frisk, H. (2019). Performance values – an artistic research perspective on music performance anxiety in classical music. J. Res. Arts Sports Educ. 3, 1–15. doi: 10.23865/jased.v3.1506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. J., and Rickard, N. S. (2004). Prediction of music performance anxiety via personality and trait anxiety in young musicians. Aust. J. Music. Educ. 1, 3–12.

Google Scholar

Sokoli, E., Hildebrandt, H., and Gomez, P. (2022). Classical music students’ pre-performance anxiety, catastrophizing, and bodily complaints vary by age, gender, and instrument and predict self-rated performance quality. Front. Psychol. 13:905680. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.905680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sousa, C. M., Machado, J. P., Greten, H. J., and Coimbra, D. (2016). Occupational diseases of professional orchestra musicians from northern Portugal: A descriptive study. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 31, 8–12. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2016.1002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spahn, C., Richter, B., and Zschocke, I. (2002). Health attitudes, preventive behavior, and playing-related health problems among music students. Medical Problems of Performing Artists. 17, 22–28. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2002.1004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spahn, C. (2015). “Treatment and prevention of music performance anxiety” in Music, neurology, and neuroscience: Evolution, the musical brain, medical conditions, and therapies. eds. E. Altenmueller, S. Finger, and F. Boller, vol. 217 (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 129–140.

Google Scholar

Spahn, C., Echternach, M., Zander, M. F., Voltmer, E., and Richter, B. (2010). Music performance anxiety in opera singers. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocol. 35, 175–182. doi: 10.3109/14015431003720600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spahn, C., Walther, J.-C., and Nusseck, M. (2016). The effectiveness of a multimodal concept of audition training for music students in coping with music performance anxiety. Psychol. Music 44, 893–909. doi: 10.1177/0305735615597484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-trait anxiety inventory for adults (STAI-AD) [database record]. APA PsycTests. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (2013). Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Stanson, N. (2019). The effectiveness of mindfulness training on young adult musicians experiencing music performance anxiety [Master’s thesis]. Ottawa: University of Ottawa.

Google Scholar

Stanton, H. E. (1993). Alleviation of performance anxiety through hypnotherapy. Psychol. Music 21, 78–82. doi: 10.1177/030573569302100106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephenson, H., and Quarrier, N. F. (2005). Anxiety sensitivity and performance anxiety in college music students. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 20, 119–125. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2005.3024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A. (1982). Performance anxiety. Recent developments in its analysis and management. The Musical Times 123, 537–541. doi: 10.2307/962763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A. (1989). Stress, coping and stage fright in professional musicians. Psychol. Music 17, 3–11. doi: 10.1177/0305735689171001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A. (2001). “Negative emotions in music making: the problem of performance anxiety” in Music and emotion: Theory and research, eds. P. N. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 291–307.

Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., and Fidler, H. (1987). Stage fright in orchestral musicians: A study of cognitive and behavioural strategies in performance anxiety. Br. J. Psychol. 78, 241–249. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1987.tb02243.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, J. R., Khalsa, S. B. S., and Hofmann, S. G. (2012). A yoga intervention for music performance anxiety in conservatory students. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 27, 123–128. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2012.3023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternbach, D. J. (1993). Addressing stress-related illness in professional musicians. Maryland Med. J. 42, 283–288.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sternbach, D. J. (2008). Stress in the lives of music students. Music. Educ. J. 94, 42–48. doi: 10.1177/002743210809400309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., and Eismann, U. (2007). Perfectionism in young musicians: relations with motivation, effort, achievement, and distress. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 2182–2192. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.036u

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., and Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: approaches, evidence, challenges. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 295–319. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studer, R. K., Danuser, B., Wild, P., Hildebrandt, H., and Gomez, P. (2014). Psychophysiological activation during preparation, performance, and recovery in high-and low-anxious music students. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 39, 45–57. doi: 10.1007/s10484-014-9240-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studer, R., Gomez, P., Hildebrandt, H., Arial, M., and Danuser, B. (2011). Stage fright: its experience as a problem and coping with it. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 84, 761–771. doi: 10.1007/s00420-010-0608-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney, G. A., and Horan, J. J. (1982). Separate and combined effects of cue-controlled relaxation and cognitive restructuring in the treatment of musical performance anxiety. J. Couns. Psychol. 29, 486–497. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.29.5.486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syme, K. L., and Hagen, E. H. (2020). Mental health is biological health: why tackling “diseases of the mind” is an imperative for biological anthropology in the 21st century. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 171, 87–117. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.23965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talbot-Honeck, C. (1994). Excellence in the performing arts: A study of elite musicians’ mental readiness to perform. Ottawa: University of Ottawa.

Google Scholar

Tang, Y., and Ryan, L. (2020). Music performance anxiety: can expressive writing intervention help? Front. Psychol. 11:1334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. P., and Nettelbeck, T. (2014). Performance anxiety in adolescent musicians. Psychol. Music 42, 624–634. doi: 10.1177/0305735613485151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurber, M. R., Bodenhamer-Davis, E., Johnson, M. L., Chesky, K., and Chandler, C. K. (2010). Effects of heart rate variability coherence biofeedback training and emotional management techniques to decrease music performance anxiety. Biofeedback 38, 28–40. doi: 10.5298/1081-5937-38.1.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomlinson, C. (2018). Music from the inside out. Market Harborough: Troubador Publishing Ltd.

Google Scholar

Topoğlu, O., Karagülle, D., Keskin, T., Abacigil, F., and Okyay, P. (2018). General health status, music performance anxiety, and coping methods of musicians working in Turkish state symphony orchestras: A cross-sectional study. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 33, 118–123. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2018.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremayne, P., and Morgan, A. (2016). “Attention, centering, and being mindful: medical specialties to the performing arts” in Mindfulness and performance. ed. A. L. Baltzell (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 389–411.

Google Scholar

Turan, B., Hurst-Wajszczuk, K., and Edwards, D. A. (2022). Hormone and enzyme reactivity before, during, and after a music performance: cortisol, testosterone, and alpha-amylase. Comprehens. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 9:100111. doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2022.100111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., Dancu, C. V., and Stanley, M. A. (1989). An empirically derived inventory to measure social fears and anxiety: the social phobia and anxiety inventory. Psychol. Assess. 1, 35–40. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.35

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, E. R. (2002). “The fear of performance. In musical performance: A guide to understanding” in Musical performance: A guide to understanding. ed. J. Rink (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 168–182.

Google Scholar

Valentine, E. R., Fitzgerald, D. F., Gorton, T. L., Hudson, J. A., and Symonds, E. R. (1995). The effect of lessons in the Alexander technique on music performance in high and low stress situations. Psychol. Music 23, 129–141. doi: 10.1177/0305735695232002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Fenema, E., Julsing, J. E., Carlier, I. V., van Noorden, M. S., Giltay, E. J., van der Wee, N. J., et al. (2013). Musicians seeking psychiatric help: A preliminary study of psychiatric characteristics. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 28, 9–18. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2013.1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Kemenade, J. F., Van Son, M. J., and Van Heesch, N. C. (1995). Performance anxiety among professional musicians in symphonic orchestras: A self-report study. Psychol. Rep. 77, 555–562. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.77.2.555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van McKinney, H. (1984). The effects of thermal biofeedback training on musical performance and performance anxiety [PhD Thesis]. Greeley, CO: University of Northern Colorado, School of Music.

Google Scholar

Vervainioti, A., and Alexopoulos, E. C. (2015). Job-related stressors of classical instrumental musicians: A systematic qualitative review. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 30, 197–202. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2015.4037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivas, E., Nascimento, M., and Rocha, S. (2021). The performance anxiety influence on the motor coordination levels: A literature mini-review. Int. J. Educ. Res. 3, 1–5.

Google Scholar

Voltmer, E., Zander, M., Fischer, J. E., Kudielka, B. M., Richter, B., and Spahn, C. (2012). Physical and mental health of different types of orchestra musicians compared to other professions. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 27, 9–14. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2012.1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, R. (1995). Phenomenological mapping: A method for describing and comparing states of consciousness. J. Transpers. Psychol. 27:25.

Google Scholar

Walton, C. C., Osborne, M. S., Gilbert, P., and Kirby, J. (2022). Nurturing self-compassionate performers. Aust. Psychol. 57, 77–85. doi: 10.1080/00050067.2022.2033952

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardle, A. (1969). Behavioral modification by reciprocal inhibition of instrumental music performance anxiety [Doctoral thesis]. Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University.

Google Scholar

Wells, R., Outhred, T., Heathers, J. A. J., Quintana, D. S., and Kemp, A. H. (2012). Matter over mind: A randomised-controlled trial of single-session biofeedback. PLoS One 7:e46597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesner, R. B., Noyes, R. Jr., and Davis, T. L. (1990). The occurence of performance anxiety among musicians. J. Affect. Disord. 18, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(90)90034-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WHO. (2004). Management of mental disorders (treatment protocol project, Vol. 1). Darlinghurst, NSW: World Health Organization collaborating Centre for Evidence in mental health policy (secondary source, cited in Osborne & Kirsner, 2022).

Google Scholar

Wiedemann, A., Vogel, D., Voss, C., and Hoyer, J. (2021). How does music performance anxiety relate to other anxiety disorders? Psychol. Music 50, 204–217. doi: 10.1177/0305735620988600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiedemann, A., Vogel, D., Voss, C., Nusseck, M., and Hoyer, J. (2020). The role of retrospectively perceived parenting style and adult attachment behaviour in music performance anxiety. Psychol. Music 48, 707–723. doi: 10.1177/0305735618817877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamon, A. (2004). Musical excellence: Strategies and techniques to enhance performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Williamon, A., Aufegger, L., Wasley, D., Looney, D., and Mandic, D. P. (2013). Complexity of physiological responses decreases in high-stress musical performance. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20130719. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.0719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, G. D. (1994). Psychology for performing artists: Butterflies and bouquets. London: Jessica Kingsley.

Google Scholar

Wolfe, M. L. (1989). Correlates of adaptive and maladaptive musical performance anxiety. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 4, 49–56.

Google Scholar

Wolfe, M. L. (1990). Coping with musical performance anxiety: problem-focused and emotion-focused strategies. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 5, 33–36.

Google Scholar

Yadigaroğlu, Z. (2021). Examination of music performance anxiety of music teacher candidates. Int. J. Educ. Technol. Scient. Res. 6, 2064–2104. doi: 10.35826/ijetsar.398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Schroeder, F., and Rodger, M. (2022). “The role of perfectionism in music performance anxiety within university piano majors in China” in Advances in design, music and arts II. eds. D. Raposo, J. Neves, R. Silva, L. Correia Castilho, and R. Dias (New York: Springer International Publishing), 674–689.

Google Scholar

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshie, M., Kudo, K., Murakoshi, T., and Ohtsuki, T. (2009). Music performance anxiety in skilled pianists: effects of social-evaluative performance situation on subjective, autonomic, and electromyographic reactions. Exp. Brain Res. 199, 117–126. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1979-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakaria, J. B., Musib, H. B., and Shariff, S. M. (2013). Overcoming performance anxiety among music undergraduates. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 90, 226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.07.086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhukov, K. (2019). Current approaches for Management of Music Performance Anxiety: an introductory overview. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 34, 53–60. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2019.1008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinn, M., McCain, C., and Zinn, M. (2000). Musical performance anxiety and the high-risk model of threat perception. Med. Probl. Perform. Art. 15, 65–71. doi: 10.21091/mppa.2000.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zyl, M. (2021). The effects of virtual reality on music performance anxiety among university-level music majors. Visions Res. Music Educ. 35

Google Scholar

Keywords: music performance anxiety, MPA, musicians, interventions, theoretical literature review

Citation: Herman R and Clark T (2023) It’s not a virus! Reconceptualizing and de-pathologizing music performance anxiety. Front. Psychol. 14:1194873. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1194873

Received: 27 March 2023; Accepted: 02 October 2023;
Published: 10 November 2023.

Edited by:

Michiko Yoshie, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Japan

Reviewed by:

Horst Hildebrandt, Zurich University of the Arts, Switzerland
Roberta Antonini Philippe, Université de Lausanne, Switzerland

Copyright © 2023 Herman and Clark. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rebecca Herman, Rebecca.herman@rcm.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.