Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 19 May 2023
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Bodily Self and Its Alterations: Psychopathological, Neural and Theoretical Aspects View all 6 articles

Trauma-related disorders and the bodily self: current perspectives and future directions

  • Department of Philosophy, Social Sciences and Education, University of Perugia, Perugia, Italy

Trauma-related disorders are debilitating psychiatric conditions that influence people who have directly or indirectly witnessed adversities. Dramatic brain/body transformations and altered person's relationship with self, others, and the world occur when experiencing multiple types of traumas. In turn, these unfortunate modifications may contribute to predisposition to trauma-related vulnerability conditions, such as externalizing (aggression, delinquency, and conduct disorders) problems. This mini-review analyzes the relations between traumatic experiences (encoded as implicit and embodied procedural memories) and bodily self, sense of safety for the own body, and relationship with others, also in the presence of externalizing conducts. Furthermore, an emerging research area is also considered, highlighting principles and techniques of body-oriented and sensorimotor therapies designed to remodel bodily self-aspects in the presence of trauma, discussing their potential application with individuals showing externalizing problems.

1. Introduction

Embodied cognition perspective suggests that high-order mental processes are based on low-order integration of signals associated with visual, auditory, vestibular, visceral, somatosensory, and motor functions (Fernandino and Iacoboni, 2010). Low-level (bottom-up) sensorimotor processes and high-level (top-down) representations of the body combine with each other, leading to an inclusive and ongoing bodily domain (Balconi, 2010), involving body ownership and a sense of self (Gallagher, 2000; Tsakiris, 2010). Thus, the self is rooted in the body. Starting from the body borders that differentiate between inside and outside (Ogden, 2018), bodily self-consciousness encompasses self-location, self-identification with the body, and the first-person perspective (Aspell et al., 2012). All these aspects are interconnected to create a sense of self that is embodied, supporting the sense of agency over one's own body and over the space surrounding the body (peripersonal space, PPS) (Rizzolatti et al., 1997; Brozzoli et al., 2012; Serino et al., 2013; Salomon et al., 2017). Moreover, it is important also to consider interoceptive bodily awareness, defined as the representation of the body's internal state (Cameron, 2001; Craig, 2003; Farb et al., 2015).

Traumatic events, especially at an early age and in an interpersonal context, may deteriorate the experience of the body as a safe entity (Van der Kolk, 2014; Laricchiuta et al., 2023) in all domains. Early trauma, involving sexual, physical, or emotional abuse, and/or neglect, may therefore disrupt the orientation toward the body (Bernstein et al., 2003; He et al., 2019). In some cases, sensations of comfort and trust in bodily experiences may be substituted by bodily perceptions associated with the implicit memory of the trauma. In accordance with the dual representation theory (Brewin, 2011), explicit and implicit memories are crucial for storing traumatic information. Explicit memory refers to semantic, episodic, and autobiographic memory that can be verbally accessed. Implicit memory cannot be retrieved, and therefore, is unconscious. Such a memory typically involves bottom-up somatosensory manifestations and intense re-experiencing of somatic sensations of the traumatic event (Hellawell and Brewin, 2004). Van der Kolk (2014) articulated that the “somatic memory” of trauma may conduct to estrangement from own body, with bodily signals perceived as alarming. Thus, the somatic experiencing approach to trauma focuses on the hypothesis that trauma is “blocked” in the body (Van der Kolk, 1994; Levine, 1997). All individuals are physiologically programmed to flee, fight, or freeze in the face of adversity. If the natural responses to danger are impeded, the unfinished defensive actions become blocked in the body, even long after the event has passed. Continued mobilizing (fight or flight responses) or immobilizing (freeze response) defenses are reflected by physiologic states of autonomic hyper- and hypo-arousal, respectively, very often considered as the hallmark symptoms of trauma (Siegel, 1999). Inflexibility between the defensive systems and their expression in the absence of danger involves chronic dysregulated arousal, contributes to maintain the traumatization, and allows to put into action externalizing and internalizing post-traumatic conducts (Panuccio et al., 2022). Internalizing problems feature mood or emotion as their primary characteristics including symptoms such as anxiety, depression, anhedonia, and withdrawal. Externalizing problems include aggressiveness, delinquency, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder (Achenbach et al., 1991; Kovacs and Devlin, 1998; Laricchiuta et al., 2023).

What is being reported so far consents to re-think at post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This pervasive pathological condition includes re-experiencing, avoidance, hyperarousal symptoms, and negative alterations in mood and cognition (APA, 2013). At the level of defensive actions, it is mainly characterized by bodily manifestations of fight-and-flight defensive systems. PTSD symptomatology implies an association between high-level and low-level processes, namely, the bottom-up multisensory mechanism is mainly involved during re-experiencing and hyperarousal symptoms, in which the trauma is relived as if it was re-occurring at the present moment, with concurrent bodily reactions. Conversely, avoidance symptoms have been associated with a top-down over-modulation of emotional reaction, characterized by emotional detachment/restricted affect coping style (Frewen et al., 2008, 2012; Lanius, 2010), which has also been posited to underlie the development and manifestation of callous and aggressive tendencies (Kosson et al., 2018).

In this framework, trauma-related disorders can alter the representation of, and relation with, the self at multiple levels, including cognitive, bodily, and social levels. As theories proliferate and research accumulates in the area, a concise yet comprehensive review can represent useful-to-go resources for scholars and practitioners working in the field and can facilitate developments of research agendas across disciplines. In the next sections, we attempted to synthesize the largely separate literatures on the impact of traumatic experiences on the bodily self as construed in the foregoing, also integrating the sparse investigations into externalizing syndromes. Finally, body-oriented and sensorimotor therapies, designed to remodel bodily self-aspects, are proposed as emerging care approaches in the presence of traumatization and externalizing problems.

2. Post-traumatic repercussions on bodily self

In the presence of traumatization, the subjective perception and experience of the body may be catastrophically and fearfully oriented (Sullivan et al., 1995). Catastrophic and fearful orientation refers to an amplified negative perception of bodily signals associated with the tendency to ruminate upon, magnify, and feel helpless when facing adverse bodily sensations (McNally, 2002). Body vigilance is high in order to monitor interoceptive bodily signals and perturbations in search of illness signs (Zvolensky and Forsyth, 2002; Pieritz et al., 2015; Tsur et al., 2017, 2018). This tendency increases psychosomatic symptoms (Lamela and Figueiredo, 2013) and body shame (Talmon and Ginzburg, 2018).

Nevertheless, post-traumatic catastrophic orientation to bodily signals may not be the only long-term and self-related psychopathological reaction to trauma. Traumatized children may exhibit a developmental trauma disorder, which includes multifaceted biopsychosocial responses to attachment disruption (D'Andrea et al., 2012; Ford et al., 2018). Furthermore, during adulthood, a complex PTSD (CPTSD) may transpire (van der Kolk et al., 2005), characterized by the abolishment of self-organization, negative self-concept, and affective dysregulation capacities (Cloitre et al., 2018). Tsur (2020) reported that prolonged experiences of interpersonal trauma during childhood were associated with a lack of trust in one's own body. Based on these findings, they suggested that the reaction to trauma that characterizes CPTSD—rather than the exposure to trauma per se—is consequential for one's orientation to the body.

Very often, traumatized individuals with CPTSD respond to potential or experienced danger with dissociation, which may have a negative impact on the wellbeing of their bodies (Haven and Pearlman, 2004; Haven, 2009). Intrapsychic functions of dissociation—which refers here to barring some aspects of mental functioning from conscious awareness—may reflect the need not to feel, know, be oneself, and mismanage the threat that connection poses. In turn, all these needs reflect the necessity to separate oneself from intolerable effects, traumatic memories and knowledge, unacceptable aspects of oneself, and dangerous interpersonal relationships. Critically, the dissociative subtype of PTSD (PTSD + DS) is characterized by depersonalization (feeling parts of the body or the entire body as detached and out of control) and derealization (feeling external surroundings as unreal, dreamlike, or distorted) (Lanius et al., 2012; Spiegel et al., 2013).

Interestingly, Rabellino et al. (2018) explored the rubber hand illusion (RHI), an experimental paradigm utilized to manipulate the sense of body ownership through a temporary illusion (Ehrsson et al., 2004), in subjects with PTSD and PTSD + DS, as compared to healthy controls. The illusion effect was lower in the subjects with PTSD and more variable in subjects with PTSD + DS, as compared to the controls. These findings indicate that subjects with PTSD may have a rigid body representation as an avoidance strategy, with top-down cognitive processes that weaken the impact of manipulation of body ownership. Conversely, the response elicited in PTSD + DS subjects appeared to be related to an increased vulnerability to manipulation of embodiment, which in turn associated with a disruption of multisensory integration processes. In view of the neurocognitive model, the mechanisms of embodiment and its manipulation are related to feelings of owning and controlling the body that arise as an interaction between the current multisensory input and internal models of the body (Tsakiris, 2010; Ratcliffe and Newport, 2017). Accordingly, neuroimaging studies of subjects with PTSD + DS have suggested altered activity in brain regions involved in multisensory integration (Simeon et al., 2000; Lanius et al., 2002; Felmingham et al., 2008), such as modified vestibular nuclei functional connectivity with key cortical vestibular regions (Harricharan et al., 2017) and changed functional connectivity of the left flocculus (the cerebellar component of the vestibular system) with key regions of the default mode network (the cortical component of the attentional system) (Rabellino et al., 2022).

Thus, significant disruption of multisensory integration processes manifests as either a hyper-rigid or extremely weak representation of the body that is critical for understanding the relationship with bodily self and surroundings. Depersonalization and derealization prevent the individual from creating a stable space as a defensive zone around the body, corresponding to an unstable self-other distinction (Rabellino et al., 2020).

3. Implications for externalizing disorders

Some of the consequences of trauma described above—such as shame related to the body and the self as well as affective dysregulation—are robust correlates of externalizing behavior in community, clinical, and correctional samples (Velotti et al., 2014, 2017; Garofalo et al., 2018). This stresses the importance of increased attention to the body and mind/body relations in individuals at risk for externalizing behavior. One reason why attention to these matters in offending populations has been limited may be due to the fact that a portion—and perhaps the most severe—of this population does not apparently show typical signs of bodily alterations. Therefore, scholars and practitioners may operate under the assumption that these individuals have either not been exposed to early traumatic experiences or have been resilient against them. However, few but convincing evidence suggests that this may be far from the truth. First, because of the direct evidence that offenders report, on average, higher levels of traumatic experiences across the lifespan compared to non-offenders (Wolff et al., 2009; Adshead and Ferrito, 2015; Gueta et al., 2021; for a recent review, see Pettus, 2023). Second, in offenders with personality disorders, early, versatile, and chronic antisocial tendencies have been associated with reduced interoceptive awareness (Nentjes et al., 2013), suggesting that the expression of antisocial traits and behaviors may be influenced by an attenuated sensitivity to one's own bodily signal. The most severe forms of externalizing disorders in adulthood (e.g., psychopathy; DeLisi, 2009; Gillespie et al., 2023) can also be associated with hyposensitivity to threat and reduced ability to detect sources of threat (Hoppenbrouwers et al., 2016; Fanti et al., 2020; Driessen et al., 2021). At the same time, these individuals are also prone to interpret ambiguous situations as threatening and over-react activating fight responses in the form of aggressive behavior (Smeijers et al., 2017, 2019). Notably, findings concerning interoceptive awareness and threat detection are consistent—albeit indirectly—with a pattern of previous or current victimization.

Third, research suggests that violent offenders possess an enlarged perception of their PPS coupled with a hyper-sensitivity and an exaggerated neural response to perceptions of personal space invasion, a pattern that has been attributed to a tendency to hostile interpretation bias (Schienle et al., 2017) that is also consistent with traumatization. Fourth, recent findings of specific bodily dysfunctions linked reduced pain distress to externalizing (i.e., impulsive and irresponsible) traits and also to reduced estimation of others' pain distress, which is a purported causal antecedent of aggressive behavior (Brazil et al., 2022).

It is noteworthy that calls to place more emphasis on the assessment and therapy of mind/body relations have been historically produced in the criminology literature although these calls have often fallen in a vacuum. For instance, Ferrell (1999) argued that “Perhaps the most critical of situations, the most intimate of cultural spaces in which crime and crime control intersect are those in and around the physical and emotional self” (p. 413). Accordingly, Ferrell advocated for a “criminology of the skin” that could explain criminal behavior investigating embodied and affective meanings of crime for the self. From this perspective, externalizing behaviors can be interpreted as attempts to fill an emptied bodily self with experiences that have a strong physiological resonance (e.g., Lyng, 2004). Arguments consistent with a focus on the body and the self-body relations have been reinvigorated by Vaughn and DeLisi (2018)'s criminal energetics theory of criminal enhancement and attenuation, which suggests that several key tenets of criminal career paradigms can be accounted for by focusing on indices pertaining to the body and the individual's experience of the body. Taken together, these empirical and theoretical perspectives align with a broad understanding that the self and the body have a crucial yet often unattended relevance in the understanding of externalizing conduct.

4. Body-oriented interventions for traumatized individuals: extension to externalizing disorders

Therapists who work with traumatized individuals recognize that trauma-related disorders have extreme complexity. Traumatized subjects do not just suffer memories of distressing events, but they show bodily responses to dysregulated emotions (Ogden et al., 2006).

Traditional “talk-therapy” approaches tend to address the explicit and declarative components of trauma. In these approaches, it is presupposed that change occurs in a top-down direction. However, as the explicit memory is recalled, the somatosensory traces of the trauma are simultaneously activated, frequently leading to a re-experiencing of somatoform symptoms that can include autonomic dysregulation, dissociative defenses associated with hyper- and hypo-arousal states, intrusive sensory experiences, and involuntary movements (Aposhyan, 2004). The body sensations are interpreted as current rather than past data. In turn, the intensity of trauma-related emotions and sensorimotor reactions disorganizes the cognitive capacities for top-down regulation (LeDoux, 2002; Schore, 2002).

Thus, a different approach to treatment may be helpful: adding bottom-up approaches to top-down therapy. In sensorimotor psychotherapy, bodily experience becomes the primary entry point of therapy, attending to the patient's body directly and working on implicit memories in order to modify procedural learning and dysregulated autonomic arousal (Ogden and Minton, 2000; Ogden et al., 2006). Traumatized patients are helped to rediscover their unfinished defensive actions via tracking their bodily movements and sensations. Somatic bottom-up interventions that address the repetitive, unbidden, physical sensations of hyper- or hypo-arousal can then be integrated with more traditional top-down interventions that help to transform the narrative of the trauma and facilitate the development of a reorganized somatic sense of self.

Recently, Classen et al. (2021) provided support for sensorimotor psychotherapy, a powerful body-oriented approach aimed to address chronic fear states in the body related to complex trauma, namely, significant improvements were found in awareness of somatic experience, anxiety, and soothing receptivity when comparing body-oriented treated women to no-treated women. The improvements resembled to shift from experiencing the body as a source of hurt to a place of healing.

In the same line, the broad concept of body awareness has been described as a key element and a mechanism of action for other mind/body approaches, such as mindfulness-based therapies and basic body awareness therapy (Mehling et al., 2011). In a safe setting, these approaches permit the subject to progressively shift the attentional focus away from thinking about threatening the body toward immediately feeling body sensations (e.g., muscular effort, joint activity, breathing, heart rate acceleration, and without judgments) to further reduce rumination and arousal (Farb et al., 2015). Mehling et al. (2018) developed an integrative exercise program for PTSD veterans that combined aerobic and resistance exercises with yoga movements and postures as well as mindfulness-based principles. This mindfulness training may support decoupling the usual reactions to unpleasant experiences and thoughts, possibly increasing the ability to downregulate hyperarousal (Kearney et al., 2012; Stephenson et al., 2017). Participants in the integrative exercise group gave high ratings for feasibility and acceptability and demonstrated greater improvement in PTSD symptom severity compared with a control group.

Finally, physical therapists from 13 countries working with the basic body awareness therapy method in mental health care were interviewed in six focus groups about what effects they have experienced in their work with patients, reporting that the therapy mainly worked by helping the patients to be in better contact with their bodily self (Gyllensten et al., 2019).

Although we are not aware of attempts to apply sensorimotor psychotherapy to clients with externalizing disorders such as offenders in correctional or forensic psychiatric hospitals, several studies from different countries have provided preliminary testing of the feasibility, acceptability, and effectiveness of mindfulness-based interventions for youth and adults in correctional settings, with a special emphasis on reducing aggression (Fix and Fix, 2013; Barrett, 2017; Simpson et al., 2018; Bouw et al., 2019; Davies et al., 2021). By and large, these approaches were well-received by staff and patients, but there was no convincing evidence that these interventions promoted clinically meaningful improvements in patients' wellbeing and reductions in maladaptive behavior compared to treatment as usual. Crucially, the scientific rigor of most studies on the topic was severely hampered by methodological and practical limitations that make generalization of the preliminary promising results challenging (Fix and Fix, 2013; Simpson et al., 2018). Importantly, preliminary findings based on more robust investigations of interventions that make use of mind/body approaches with these populations show promise. For instance, prisoners who participated in a 10-week yoga course—compared to a wait list control group—showed improvements in positive affect, perceived stress, and psychological symptoms, as well as improvements in cognitive and behavioral control in a “Go/No-Go Task” (Bilderbeck et al., 2013). Based on a systematic review, scholars have suggested that the positive effects of these kinds of mind/body interventions could be due to alterations in gene expression that contribute to a reduced risk of inflammatory reactions to stress (Buric et al., 2017).

The approaches proposed here for individuals with trauma-related externalizing disorders are also consistent with promising results obtained with trauma-informed treatments in justice-involved youth with histories of trauma (for a review, see Zettler, 2021) and could be combined with such treatments to enhance effectiveness. Specifically, trauma-informed adaptations of several therapeutic approaches (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy, family functional therapy, and aggression replacement training) as well as trauma-informed programs specifically developed for justice-involved youth [e.g., skills-based programs, trauma affect regulation guide for education and therapy (TARGET), think trauma training, and sanctuary model] have been proven effective in reducing behavioral infractions and institutional violence although evidence of positive effects in the longer term (e.g., recidivism or post-release adjustment) are scarce. Of note, these treatments all share a focus on integrating physiological and psychological processes disrupted by traumatic experiences, hence lending themselves to a more explicit integration of sensorimotor techniques.

In line with the increased attention to this kind of approach to reduce externalizing behavior, Pettus (2023) has cogently advocated the implementation of trauma-responsive approaches to re-entry after imprisonment; we argue that such approaches should rely (also) on mind/body interventions delivered before re-entry as they can intervene in the aftermath of trauma-related externalizing psychopathology and in turn reduce recidivism risk.

5. Conclusion

In this mini-review, we have integrated the largely separate literature on trauma, trauma-related disorders, and bodily and mind/body dysfunctions, extending to the often-neglected area of externalizing psychopathology and closing with future treatment perspectives. In doing so, we have supported the relevance of mind/body interventions to address the multifarious negative consequences of traumatic experiences that crystallize over time and generate immense suffering to trauma survivors and—especially in the case of externalizing behavior and offending—to their environment as well. These interventions hold promise to be effective trans-diagnostic approaches addressing the traumatic roots of diverse symptomatic presentations. We hope this study will inspire future multidisciplinary research into basic mechanisms that connect traumatic experiences and mind-body alterations, externalizing behavior to further optimism that tailor interventions offered more timely and with more precision.

Author contributions

DL conceptualized the article and performed the first literature data curation. CM acquired the funding to administer the project. All authors wrote the original draft, reviewed, and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by grants from the basic research fund of the authors.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Quay, H. C., and Conners, C. K. (1991). National survey of problems and competencies among four- to sixteen-year-olds: parents' reports for normative and clinical samples. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 56, 1–131. doi: 10.2307/1166156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adshead, G. M., and Ferrito, S. B. (2015). Recovery after homicide narrative shifts in therapy with homicide perpetrators. Crim. Justice Behav. 42, 70–81. doi: 10.1177/0093854814550030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Aposhyan, S. (2004). Bodymind Paychotherapy: Principles, Techniques and Practical Applications. New York, NY: WW Norton.

Google Scholar

Aspell, J. E., Lenggenhager, B., and Blanke, O. (2012). “Chapter 24. Multisensory perception and bodily self-consiousness. From out-of- body to inside-body experience,” in: The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, edsM. M. Murray and M. T. Wallace (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; Taylor and Frances). doi: 10.1201/b11092-30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M. (2010). Neuropsychology of the Sense of Agency. Milan: Springer Verlag Italia.

Google Scholar

Barrett, C. J. (2017). Mindfulness and rehabilitation: teaching yoga and meditation to young men in an alternative to incarceration program. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 61, 1719–1738. doi: 10.1177/0306624X16633667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., et al. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilderbeck, A. C., Farias, M., Brazil, I. A., Jakobowitz, S., and Wikholm, C. (2013). Participation in a 10-week course of yoga improves behavioural control and decreases psychological distress in a prison population. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 1438–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouw, N., Huijbregts, S. C. J., Scholte, E., and Swaab, H. (2019). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in prison: experiences of inmates, instructors, and prison staff. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 63, 2550–2571. doi: 10.1177/0306624X19856232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazil, I. A., Atanassova, D. V., and Oosterman, J. M. (2022). Own pain distress mediates the link between the lifestyle facet of psychopathy and estimates of pain distress in others. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 824697. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.824697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewin, C. R. (2011). The nature and significance of memory disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 7, 203–227. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brozzoli, C., Makin, T. R., Cardinali, L., Holmes, N. P., and Farnè, A. (2012). “Peripersonal space,” in The Neural Bases of Mulitsensory Processes, eds M. Murray and M. Wallace (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 1–19. doi: 10.1201/b11092-29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buric, I., Farias, M., Jong, J., Mee, C., and Brazil, I. A. (2017). What is the molecular signature of mind–body interventions? A systematic review of gene expression changes induced by meditation and related practices. Front. Immunol. 8, 670. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2017.00670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, O. G. (2001). Interoception: the inside story–a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom. Med. 63, 697–710. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200109000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Classen, C. C., Hughes, L., Clark, C., Hill Mohammed, B., Woods, P., and Beckett, B. (2021). A pilot RCT of a body-oriented group therapy for complex trauma survivors: an adaptation of sensorimotor psychotherapy. J. Trauma Dissociation 22, 52–68. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2020.1760173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloitre, M., Shevlin, M., Brewin, C. R., Bisson, J. I., Roberts, N. P., Maercker, A., et al. (2018). The International Trauma Questionnaire: development of a self-report measure of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 138, 536–546. doi: 10.1111/acps.12956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 500–505. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Andrea, W., Ford, J., Stolbach, B., Spinazzola, J., and van der Kolk, B. A. (2012). Understanding interpersonal trauma in children: why we need a developmentally appropriate trauma diagnosis. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 82, 187–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, J., Ugwudike, P., Young, H., Hurrell, C., and Raynor, P. (2021). A pragmatic study of the impact of a brief mindfulness intervention on prisoners and staff in a category b prison and men subject to community-based probation supervision. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 65, 136–156. doi: 10.1177/0306624X20944664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLisi, M. (2009). Psychopathy is the unified theory of crime. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 7, 256–273. doi: 10.1177/1541204009333834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driessen, J. M. A., Brazil, I. A., Lorenzo, E. D., Herwaarden, A. E., Olthaar, A. J., Potamianou, H., et al. (2021). Psychopathic traits influence threat avoidance in a community sample independent of testosterone. Pers. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 12, 428–436. doi: 10.1037/per0000481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., and Passingham, R. E. (2004). That's my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305, 875–877. doi: 10.1126/science.1097011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A., Konikou, K., Cohn, M., Popma, A., and Brazil, I. A. (2020). Amygdala functioning during threat acquisition and extinction differentiates antisocial subtypes. J. Neuropsychol. 14, 226–241. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farb, N., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Gard, T., Kerr, C., Dunn, B. D., et al. (2015). Interoception, contemplative practice, and health. Front. Psychol. 6, 763. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felmingham, K., Kemp, A. H., Williams, L., Falconer, E., Olivieri, G., Peduto, A., et al. (2008). Dissociative responses to conscious and non-conscious fear impact underlying brain function in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Med. 38, 1771–1780. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandino, L., and Iacoboni, M. (2010). Are cortical motor maps based on body parts or coordinated actions? Implications for embodied semantics. Brain Lang. 112, 44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrell, J. (1999). Cultural criminology. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 25, 395–418. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.25.1.395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fix, R. L., and Fix, S. T. (2013). The effects of mindfulness-based treatments for aggression: a critical review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 18, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, J. D., Spinazzola, J., van der Kolk, B., and Grasso, D. J. (2018). Toward an empirically based developmental trauma disorder diagnosis for children: factor structure, item characteristics, reliability, and validity of the developmental trauma disorder semi-structured interview. J. Clin. Psychiatry 79, 17m11675. doi: 10.4088/JCP.17m11675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., Lane, R. D., Densmore, M., Stevens, T. K., et al. (2012). Emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 73, 431–436. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frewen, P. A., Lanius, R. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., Pain, C., Hopper, J. W., et al. (2008). Clinical and neural correlates of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 171–181. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, I. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garofalo, C., Velotti, P., and Zavattini, G. C. (2018). Emotion regulation and aggression: The incremental contribution of alexithymia, impulsivity, and emotion dysregulation facets. Psychol. Viol. 8, 470–483. doi: 10.1037/vio0000141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillespie, S., Jones, A., and Garofalo, C. (2023). Psychopathy and dangerousness: an umbrella review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 100, 102240. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gueta, K., Gila, C., and Romel, N. (2021). Trauma-oriented recovery framework with offenders a necessary missing link in offenders' rehabilitation. Aggress. Violent Behav. 63, 101678. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2021.101678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gyllensten, A. L., Jacobsen, L. N., and Gard, G. (2019). Clinician perspectives of Basic Body Awareness Therapy (BBAT) in mental health physical therapy: an international qualitative study. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 23, 746–751. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2019.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harricharan, S., Nicholson, A. A., Densmore, M., Théberge, J., McKinnon, M. C., Neufeld, R. W. J., et al. (2017). Sensory overload and imbalance: resting-state vestibular connectivity in PTSD and its dissociative subtype. Neuropsychologia 106, 169–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haven, T., and Pearlman, L. A. (2004). “Minding the body: the intersection of dissociation and physical health in relational trauma psychotherapy,” in Health Consequences of Abuse in the Family: A Clinical Guide for Evidence-Based Practice, ed K. A. Kendall Tackett (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 215–232. doi: 10.1037/10674-012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haven, T. J. (2009). “That part of the body is just gone”: understanding and responding to dissociation and physical health. J. Trauma Dissociation 10, 204–218. doi: 10.1080/15299730802624569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, J., Zhong, X., Gao, Y., Xiong, G., and Yao, S. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ-SF) among undergraduates and depressive patients. Child Abuse Negl. 91, 102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellawell, S. J., and Brewin, C. R. (2004). A comparison of flashbacks and ordinary autobiographical memories of trauma: content and language. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00088-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., Bulten, B. H., and Brazil, I. A. (2016). Parsing fear: a reassessment of the evidence for fear deficits in psychopathy. Psychol. Bull. 142, 573–600. doi: 10.1037/bul0000040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kearney, D. J., McDermott, K., Malte, C., Martinez, M., and Simpson, T. L. (2012). Association of participation in a mindfulness program with measures of PTSD, depression and quality of life in a veteran sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 101–116. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosson, D. S., McBride, C. K., Miller, S. A., Riser, N. R. E., and Whitman, L. A. (2018). Attentional bias following frustration in youth with psychopathic traits: emotional deficit versus negative preception. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 9, 1–21. doi: 10.5127/jep.060116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovacs, M., and Devlin, B. (1998). Internalizing disorders in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 47–63. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamela, D., and Figueiredo, B. (2013). Parents' physical victimization in childhood and current risk of child maltreatment: the mediator role of psychosomatic symptoms. J. Psychosom. Res. 75, 178–183. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A. (2010). “Early life trauma: impact on health and disease,” in The Impact of Early Life Trauma on Health and Disease: The Hidden Epidemic, eds R. Lanius, E. Vermetten, and C. Pain (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1–92. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511777042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A., Brand, B., Vermetten, E., Frewen, P. A., and Spiegel, D. (2012). The dissociative subtype of posttraumatic stress disorder: rationale, clinical and neurobiological evidence, and implications. Depress. Anxiety 29, 701–708. doi: 10.1002/da.21889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore, M., Gupta, M., Neufeld, R. W. J., et al. (2002). Brain activation during script-driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 305–311. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01367-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laricchiuta, D., Panuccio, A., Picerni, E., Biondo, D., Genovesi, B., and Petrosini, L. (2023). The body keeps the score: the neurobiological profile of traumatized adolescents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 145, 105033. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (2002). Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York, NY: Penguin Putnam, Inc.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Levine, P. A. (1997). Waking the Tiger: Healcitying Trauma. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lyng, S. (2004). Crime, edgework and corporeal transaction. Theor Criminol 8, 359–375. doi: 10.1177/1362480604044614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNally, R. J. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 938–946. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01475-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Chesney, M. A., Metzler, T. J., Goldstein, L. A., Maguen, S., Geronimo, C., et al. (2018). A 12-week integrative exercise program improves self-reported mindfulness and interoceptive awareness in war veterans with posttraumatic stress symptoms. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 554–565. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Wrubel, J., Daubenmier, J. J., Price, C. J., Kerr, C. E., Silow, T., et al. (2011). Body Awareness: a phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philos. Ethics Humanit. Med. 6, 6. doi: 10.1186/1747-5341-6-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nentjes, L., Meijer, E., Bernstein, D., Arntz, A., and Medendorp, W. (2013). Brief communication: investigating the relationship between psychopathy and interoceptive awareness. J. Pers. Dis. 27, 617–624. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2013_27_105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, P. (2018). The Primitive Edge of Experience. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780429482823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, P., and Minton, K. (2000). Sensorimotor psychotherapy: one method for processing traumatic memory. Traumatology 6, 149–173. doi: 10.1177/153476560000600302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, P., Minton, K., and Pain, C. (2006). Trauma and the Body: A Sensorimotor Approach to Psychotherapy. Scranton, PA: WW Norton and Co.

Google Scholar

Panuccio, A., Biondo, D., Picerni, E., Genovesi, B., and Laricchiuta, D. (2022). Trauma-related internalizing and externalizing behaviors in adolescence: a bridge between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. Adolescents 2, 413–423. doi: 10.3390/adolescents2040032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettus, C. A. (2023). Trauma and prospects for reentry. Annu. Rev. Criminol. 6, 423–446. doi: 10.1146/annurev-criminol-041122-111300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pieritz, K., Rief, W., and Euteneuer, F. (2015). Childhood adversities and laboratory pain perception. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 11, 2109–2116. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S87703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabellino, D., Burin, D., Harricharan, S., Lloyd, C., Frewen, P. A., McKinnon, M. C., et al. (2018). Altered sense of body ownership and agency in posttraumatic stress disorder and its dissociative subtype: a rubber hand illusion study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 163. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabellino, D., Frewen, P. A., McKinnon, M. C., and Lanius, R. A. (2020). Peripersonal space and bodily self-consciousness: implications for psychological trauma-related disorders. Front. Neurosci. 14, 586605. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.586605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabellino, D., Thome, J., Densmore, M., Théberge, J., McKinnon, M. C., and Lanius, R. A. (2022). The vestibulocerebellum and the shattered self: a resting-state functional connectivity study in posttraumatic stress disorder and its dissociative subtype. Cerebellum 11, 392–410. doi: 10.1007/s12311-022-01467-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliffe, N., and Newport, R. (2017). The effect of visual, spatial and temporal manipulations on embodiment and action. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 227. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science 277, 190–191. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5323.190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomon, R., Noel, J.-P., Łukowska, M., Faivre, N., Metzinger, T., Serino, A., et al. (2017). Unconscious integration of multisensory bodily inputs in the peripersonal space shapes bodily self-consciousness. Cognition 166, 174–183. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schienle, A., Wabnegger, A., Leitner, M., and Leutgeb, V. (2017). Neuronal correlates of personal space intrusion in violent offenders. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 454–460. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9526-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schore, A. N. (2002). Dysregulation of the right brain: a fundamental mechanism of traumatic attachment and the psychopathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 36, 9–30. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2002.00996.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serino, A., Alsmith, A., Costantini, M., Mandrigin, A., Tajadura-Jimenez, A., and Lopez, C. (2013). Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 1239–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, D. (1999). The developing mind. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Hazlett, E. A., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Hollander, E., and Buchsbaum, M. S. (2000). Feeling unreal: a PET study of depersonalization disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 1782–1788. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, S., Mercer, S., Simpson, R., Lawrence, M., and Wyke, S. (2018). Mindfulness-based interventions for young offenders: a scoping review. Mindfulness 9, 1330–1343. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0892-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeijers, D., Bulten, E. B. H., and Brazil, I. A. (2019). The Computations of hostile biases (CHB) model: grounding hostility biases in a unified cognitive framework. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 73, 101775. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeijers, D., Rinck, M., Bulten, E., van den Heuvel, T., and Verkes, R.-J. (2017), Generalized hostile interpretation bias regarding facial expressions: characteristic of pathological aggressive behavior. Aggr. Behav. 43, 386–397. doi: 10.1002/ab.21697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, D., Lewis-Fernández, R., Lanius, R., Vermetten, E., Simeon, D., and Friedman, M. (2013). Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 299–326. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephenson, K. R., Simpson, T. L., Martinez, M. E., and Kearney, D. J. (2017). Changes in mindfulness and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among veterans enrolled in mindfulness-based stress reduction. J. Clin. Psychol. 73, 201–217. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, M. J. L., Bishop, S. R., and Pivik, J. (1995). The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol. Assess. 7, 524–532. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talmon, A., and Ginzburg, K. (2018). “Body self” in the shadow of childhood sexual abuse: the long-term implications of sexual abuse for male and female adult survivors. Child Abuse Negl. 76, 416–425. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 48, 703–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsur, N. (2020). “My own flesh and blood”: the implications of child maltreatment for the orientation towards the body among dyads of mothers and daughters. Child Abuse Negl. 104, 104469. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsur, N., Defrin, R., and Ginzburg, K. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder, orientation to pain, and pain perception in ex-prisoners of war who underwent torture. Psychosom. Med. 79, 655–663. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsur, N., Defrin, R., Lahav, Y., and Solomon, Z. (2018). The traumatized body: long-term PTSD and its implications for the orientation towards bodily signals. Psychiatry Res. 261, 281–289. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). The body keeps the score: memory and the emerging psychobiology of post traumatic stress. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 1, 253–265. doi: 10.3109/10673229409017088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Kolk, B. A. (2014). The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma. New York, NY: VIking.

Google Scholar

van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S., Pelcovitz, D., Sunday, S., and Spinazzola, J. (2005). Disorders of extreme stress: the empirical foundation of a complex adaptation to trauma. J. Trauma. Stress 18, 389–399. doi: 10.1002/jts.20047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaughn, M. G., and DeLisi, M. (2018). Criminal energetics: a theory of antisocial enhancement and criminal attenuation. Aggress. Violent Behav. 38, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2017.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velotti, P., Elison, J., and Garofalo, C. (2014). Shame and aggression: different trajectories and implications. Aggress. Violent Behav. 19, 454–461. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2014.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velotti, P., Garofalo, C., Bottazzi, F., and Caretti, V. (2017). Faces of shame: implications for self-esteem, emotion regulation, aggression, and well-being. J. Psychol. 151, 171–184. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2016.1248809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, N., Shi, J., and Siegel, J. A. (2009). Patterns of victimization among male and female inmates: evidence of an enduring legacy. Violence Vict. 24, 469–484. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.24.4.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zettler, H. R. (2021). Much to do about trauma: a systematic review of existing trauma-informed treatments on youth violence and recidivism. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 19, 113–134. doi: 10.1177/1541204020939645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zvolensky, M. J., and Forsyth, J. P. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity dimensions in the prediction of body vigilance and emotional avoidance. Cognit. Ther. Res. 26, 449–460. doi: 10.1023/A:1016223716132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: trauma, post-traumatic stress disorder, externalizing behaviors, aggression, body-oriented therapy, sensorimotor therapy

Citation: Laricchiuta D, Garofalo C and Mazzeschi C (2023) Trauma-related disorders and the bodily self: current perspectives and future directions. Front. Psychol. 14:1166127. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1166127

Received: 14 February 2023; Accepted: 28 April 2023;
Published: 19 May 2023.

Edited by:

Gabriella Martino, University of Messina, Italy

Reviewed by:

Chiara Spatola, University of Messina, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Laricchiuta, Garofalo and Mazzeschi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniela Laricchiuta, daniela.laricchiuta@unipg.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.