Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 13 February 2024
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Pro-environmental behavior, personality and emotional intelligence in adolescents: a systematic review

  • 1CRETUS, Interdisciplinary Research Center in Environmental Technologies, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Galicia, Spain
  • 2Department of Social Psychology, Basic Psychology and Methodology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 3Department of Education and Psychology, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, Vila Real, Portugal
  • 4Centre for Educational Research and Intervention, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal

Introduction: Human behavior significantly contributes to environmental problems, making the study of pro-environmental behavior an important task for psychology. In this context, it is crucial to understand the pro-environmental behavior of adolescents, as young people play a fundamental role in facilitating long-term changes in environmental consciousness and encouraging decision-makers to take action. However, little is currently known about the pro-environmental behavior of adolescents. Recently, there has been growing interest in examining the influence of personality traits and emotional intelligence on pro-environmental behavior.

Methods: We conducted a systematic review to enhance our understanding of adolescent pro-environmental behavior. Thus, this systematic review was designed to enhance understanding of adolescent’s pro-environmental behavior by summarizing existing evidence on how it relates to personality and emotional intelligence.

Results: Our findings suggest associations between specific personality traits and dimensions of emotional intelligence with adolescent pro-environmental behavior, aligning with similar studies conducted on adults.

Discussion: While our findings offer valuable insights, further research is needed to establish causality and deepen our understanding of the interplay between multiple variables influencing pro-environmental behavior among adolescents.

Systematic review registration: [https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42023387836], identifier [CRD42023387836].

1 Introduction

Human behavior is considered one of the main factors responsible for environmental problems (Steg and de Groot, 2012; Cook et al., 2016; Vicedo-Cabrera et al., 2021). Therefore, pro-environmental behavior (PEB), defined as behavior that protects the environment (Stern, 2000; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002) or at least does not harm it (Lange and Dewitte, 2019), plays a key role in reducing environmental problems. For this reason—and to help define policies and programs that effectively promote PEB—much research has been conducted to identify the factors that influence people to take action (or not) to benefit the environment (for a review, see: Bamberg and Moser, 2007; Steg and Vlek, 2009; Gifford and Nilsson, 2014; Blankenberg and Alhusen, 2019). Considering that the severity of environmental problems keeps increasing (IPCC, 2023), this kind of research still needed.

The study of adolescents’ PEB is particularly important for the future of environmental protection (Balundė et al., 2020), as the behaviors of young people are key indicators of long-term changes in both environmental consciousness and action (Wray-Lake et al., 2010; Jovanović et al., 2016; Koessler et al., 2022). However, the nature of adolescents’ PEB is under-investigated (Palupi and Sawitri, 2018; Lee et al., 2020; Gong and Zheng, 2021).

The scientific literature has shown that PEB is determined by various factors. For example, Blankenberg and Alhusen (2019) present a review outlining 22 such factors, each categorized as socio-economic, psychological, habits, and contextual factors. The sheer number of factors highlights the difficulty of defining PEB-promotion policies, as numerous variables must be considered. Due to the multifaceted nature of PEB and this complex and evolving research landscape (Gifford and Nilsson, 2014), investigation has focused on identifying those factors that have the strong impact on PEB. In recent years, has gained relevance research about the role of variables such as gratitude (Sun et al., 2023), mindfulness (Panno et al., 2018; Apaolaza et al., 2022), connectedness to nature (see meta-analysis: Whitburn et al., 2020), use of smartphones (Fang et al., 2021), personality (e.g. Soutter and Mõttus, 2020; Gibbon and Douglas, 2021), or emotional intelligence (EI; e.g.: Aguilar-Luzón et al., 2014; Carrieri and Fermani, 2018).

This systematic review focuses on two of these variables: personality and EI. These constructs were chosen because psychological variables have made significant contributions to understanding of PEB (Li et al., 2019). Previous research has identified a link between PEB and some personality traits (for a meta-analysis, see Soutter et al., 2020), affect (Carrus et al., 2008; Coelho et al., 2017) and emotion (Durán et al., 2007; Robina-Ramírez et al., 2020), indicating that EI may be associated with PEB. In addition, these two variables develop substantially during adolescence. Research suggests that personality is continually maturing during adolescence (Van Dijk et al., 2020; Tetzner et al., 2023) and that an individual’s EI undergoes complex changes during this period, increasing in some dimensions and decreasing in others (Keefer et al., 2013; Azpiazu et al., 2022). Therefore, it is essential to explore how these two factors interact with adolescents’ PEB. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review is to summarize the available evidence on personality and EI are related to adolescents’ PEB, thereby making a valuable Contribution To The Field of environmental psychology.

1.1 Why adolescents?

The research into children’s environmental attitudes and behavior began some decades ago (Otto et al., 2019), with the goal of clarifying the origins and development of both ecological sense and environmental behavior. Hahn and Garrett (2017) demonstrated that children of 3 years were already able to evaluate actions as environmentally harmful, showing moral attitudes. In a more recent study, Geraci et al. (2023) investigated the environmental morality of 7-month-old infants. The authors highlight the results of well-established studies indicating that young children make moral judgments of actions that harm the environment and seek to clarify when this moralization begins. Of importance to the current study, their results show that, by the age of 7 months, children are able to make moral evaluations. This may suggest that PEB can be part of an innate propensity (Otto et al., 2021; Geraci et al., 2023), crucial for environmental protection. Indeed, empirical evidence suggests that by the time a child has reached the age of 7 years their attitude and behavior have formed (Otto et al., 2019).

In a study of adolescents’ PEB, Böhme et al. (2018) found that adolescents seem to be potentially influenced, in the case by mindfulness, to actively engage in sustainable consumption. Collado et al. (2019) report that young people influence one another to environmentalist behavior. Similarly, Žukauskienė et al. (2021) conclude that adolescents may be important agents to influence their families and communities to adopt pro-environmental attitudes and PEB. We have witnessed the widespread participation of teenagers in the “Fridays for the Future” initiative, a movement initiated by Greta Thunberg, which may indicate greater concern with environmental issues.

Taken together, these results are a positive signs for the environmental-protection cause, as it seems that—from as young as 7 months old and throughout infant–juvenile development—young people may engage in environmental protection as a development task. However, research also indicates a decline in PEB during adolescence (Collado et al., 2015; Krettenauer, 2017; Wray-Lake et al., 2017; Krettenauer et al., 2019; Otto et al., 2019; Keith et al., 2021). Additionally, a discrepancy between environmental attitudes and behaviors has been observed in adolescents, with their concerns not always translating into corresponding behaviors (Huoponen, 2023; Thomaes et al., 2023). Therefore, it remains unclear whether adolescents’ involvement in environmental issues indicates a genuine commitment to environmental protection or whether it is a demand for governmental action rather than an assertion of individual responsibility (Wray-Lake et al., 2010). Given these mixed results, further investigation is vital to enhance our understanding of the factors driving adolescents’ PEB.

Although we are aware of the view that adolescence should be defined by the age range 10–24 years (e.g., Sawyer et al., 2018), our study focuses on adolescents aged between 11 and 18 years. This decision was taken because, in several domains of psychology, investigations of adolescents focus on this age range (e.g., Böhme et al., 2018; Gómez-López et al., 2019; Balundė et al., 2020; Habib et al., 2023; Neurohr et al., 2023). Furthermore, we argue that extending the age range to 24 years could make comparison and comprehension of the behaviors under study more difficult, as it would introduce greater variability into the population under study (e.g., by including university students and participants already in the job market).

1.2 Personality and pro-environmental behavior

Personality traits play a fundamental role in shaping an individual’s beliefs, attitudes, and values, which, in turn, influence their behaviors and decisions (Markowitz et al., 2012; Soutter et al., 2020). On that basis, there is a growing body of research into the relationship between personality and PEB (Soutter and Mõttus, 2020). These studies tend to apply the “five-factor model of personality,” which comprises the so-called “Big Five”: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness to experience. Alternatively, some use the HEXACO model and its six dimensions: Honesty–Humility, Emotionality, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience. Some researchers have applied the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa and McCrae, 1992), in one of its several versions (e.g., Zhou et al., 2019), while others have employed several versions of the HEXACO–Personality Inventory–Revised (Lee and Ashton, 2004; Ashton and Lee, 2009; Brick and Lewis, 2016; Pavalache-Ilie and Cazan, 2018; Panno et al., 2021) or the International Personality Item Pool (Johnson, 2014; Soutter and Mõttus, 2020). These studies usually examine the relationships between personality and PEB, pro-environmental attitude (PEA) or pro-environmental intention (PEI).

Soutter et al. (2020) conducted a meta-analysis of investigations in which the “Big Five” or HEXACO models had been used to examine the relationship between personality, PEB, and PEA. Their results show that Openness, Honesty–Humility, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion are associated with both PEA and PEB, with Openness and Honesty–Humility having the strongest correlations. Panno et al. (2021) also found that Openness and Honesty–Humility were significant predictors of PEB, and these traits were linked to PEB both directly and indirectly through moral anger. Similarly, research by Markowitz et al. (2012) identified a moderate positive relationship between Openness and PEB, while Brick and Lewis (2016) found that Openness and Conscientiousness independently predict emission-reduction behavior and that their effects are mediated by PEA. Openness was also found to be correlated with PEB in a study by Puech et al. (2019), while Gibbon and Douglas (2021) found that Openness/Intellect significantly predict PEB. Analyzing personality facets, Soutter and Mõttus (2020) found that Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness—along with certain aspects of Extraversion—positively predicted both PEA and PEB, while certain facets of Neuroticism had a negative association with PEB. An investigation among village leaders in China found that leaders with higher levels of Agreeableness and Neuroticism were more willing to adopt environmental protection measures (Zhou et al., 2019). These findings suggest that personality may be one explanation for engagement in PEB during adolescence.

Of special interest to the current systematic review is the development of this propensity to environmental protection, as well the possibility that one indicator of this propensity may be the personality trait of Honesty–Humility (Otto et al., 2021). Moreover, although personality develops across the lifespan, adolescence is a period that sees major development of the personality (Soto et al., 2011; de Moor et al., 2022), as well as its maturation (McCrae et al., 2002; Van Dijk et al., 2020). In addition, mean levels of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness generally rise during adolescence (Soto et al., 2011; Van Dijk et al., 2020). As a result, it would be beneficial to clarify the extent to which personality influences PEB during that period.

1.3 Emotional intelligence and pro-environmental behavior

In the quest to investigate factors that influence PEB, researchers have explored the crucial role of emotions. In this context, EI is a promising variable. Research in this domain has used the Wong and Law (2002) Emotional Intelligence Scale (e.g., Aziz et al., 2021); the Salovey et al. (1995) Trait Meta-Mood Scale (e.g., Aguilar-Luzón et al., 2014); and The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (Petrides, 2009a,b) in its various forms (e.g., Ntanos et al., 2017; Giancola et al., 2022). Aguilar-Luzón et al. (2014) studied the role of EI as a moderating variable in the relationship between anthropocentric beliefs, ecocentric beliefs, and PEB. Their results show that those people with stronger ecocentric beliefs and a better ability to manage their emotions tend to have more favorable attitude toward behavior, greater intention to have PEB and more engagement in PEB. Furthermore, when anthropocentric beliefs are less pronounced and EI is higher, there is a greater intention to perform PEB. Chowdhury (2017), in a study of the relationship between EI and ethical consumption, found that an ability to appraise and recognize other’s emotions was positively related to pro-environmental buying actions. Ntanos et al. (2017) found a positive correlation between trait EI and willingness to invest in renewable energy sources. Likewise, Carrieri and Fermani (2018) show a positive correlation between EI and sustainable-hospitality choices through social well-being, and using a cluster analysis, they found that higher scores for IE correspond to a stronger sustainable-hospitality orientation. Further, Aziz et al. (2021) found that EI mediated the relationship between pro-environmental intention and PEB. Together, these results suggest that the role of EI in the understanding and promotion of PEB warrants further study.

1.4 Strategic question

Although there is growing evidence of relationships between personality and PEB and between EI and PEB, there are gaps in this knowledge—namely, regarding the role of the variables in the PEB of adolescents. The present research examines the studies in this field and considers their conclusions in relation to adolescents to gain new insights. The findings of the current paper could support the design of PEB-promotion programs catered to specified personality traits and aspects of EI.

2 Methods

2.1 Databases and search strategy

Searches were conducted in seven electronic databases: Proquest, which included PsycARTICLES, ERIC and Psychology Database; MEDLINE; PubMed; Scopus; and Web of Science. In each database, the following combinations of keywords was searched: (Adolesc* OR “Young people” OR Youth* OR Teen*) AND (“Proenvironmental behavior” OR “pro-environmental behavior” OR “ecological behavior” OR “sustainable behavior” OR “environmentally friendly behavior” OR “green behavior”) AND (Personality OR “big five” OR “five factor model” OR extravers* OR neurotic* OR “emotional stability” OR openness OR agreeableness OR conscientiousness) AND (“Emotional Intelligence” OR “Self-Emotion Appraisal” OR “Others’ Emotion Appraisal” OR “Use of Emotion” OR “Regulation of Emotion”).

2.2 Screening and detailed assessment process

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines for systematic reviews were followed (Page et al., 2021) and the systematic review protocol was registered through the PROSPERO—International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (CRD42023387836).

Papers were downloaded from a Mendeley library. A search of the databases yielded a total of 262 records (Figure 1), and 25 duplicates were removed from these. Researchers, independently and simultaneously, screened the titles and abstracts of the remaining 217 articles. The following inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied throughout the screening process. All discrepancies were discussed to reach final decisions based on consensus among the evaluators.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA diagram, which collects the different steps followed in the process for the final selection of studies included in the systematic review.

2.3 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

To select the articles for the review, the following inclusion criteria were used: (1) only peer-reviewed and empirical articles in full-text form were considered acceptable. All articles were published in English, Spanish, or Portuguese. All dates were acceptable. (2) The participants in the studies were to be adolescents aged 11–18 years. (3) The studies must have applied quantitative, qualitative, or mixed methods, but in the case of meta data, only quantitative methods—such as meta-analysis—would be analyzed.

The reasons for excluding an article were as follows: (1) it was not available in full text; (2) it was published in a language other than English, Spanish, or Portuguese; (3) it did not use a defined tool to directly measure PEB, personality, or emotional intelligence; and (4) it was a review, opinion, editorial, case study, or empirical study in which the relationship between PEB and personality or between PEB and emotional intelligence were merely mentioned or superficially discussed, without in-depth analysis.

2.4 Final selection process

On the basis of this initial screening of the abstracts, 20 articles were deemed relevant for further assessment. All 20 articles were screened at the full-text stage by two researchers who chose the papers for final inclusion. All of the uncertainties (i.e., five highlighted by one researcher and nine by the other) were discussed with the two other researchers, and agreement was reached on the five articles included in this systematic review. (For the reasons for the exclusions, see Figure 1).

2.5 Quality and risk bias of selected studies

The methodological quality of the included studies was assessed using an Excel spreadsheet that listed all of the studies, duly coded. Extraction of relevant information from the selected primary studies was performed in the referred excel spreadsheet. Specifically, final analysis consisted in verifying two items: (1) whether the sample included only adolescents and (2) whether the variables under study included PEB, personality, or emotional intelligence. The two evaluators discussed their respective analyses, and once all four researchers had discussed the disagreements, an agreement was reached on the final decision.

3 Results

This systematic review included a total of five studies, and a summary of the main findings can be found in Table 1. The small number of articles included in the final selection reflects the scarcity of the research on these topics within the literature on PEB. All the included studies were published in the English language and had been conducted between 2014 and 2020. Although the studies analyzed the relationships between several variables and PEB, only those results concerning PEB, PEA, or PEI will be reported in this review. Despite our primary focus on PEB, we consider it worthwhile to include data on PEA and PEI because one of the most widely applied theories in PEB studies—the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991)—postulates that attitude directly influences intention, which in turn leads to the behavior. Although research shows that attitude not always turns in behavior, inconsistency known as the “attitude–behavior gap” and studied extensively in the field of environmental behavior (Siegel et al., 2018)—evidence also shows that intention is the strongest determinant of behavior (Klöckner, 2013).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characterization of articles selected for analysis of the systematic literature review (n = 5).

An assessment of the methodologies revealed that the variables had been assessed using self-reported measures. There were a total of 6,170 participants across the studies, with an average of 1,234 per study. In one study, the age of the participants was not specified, while the other four studies focused on adolescents with an average age of 15.11 years (ranging from 12 to 18 years). In one study, only 24% of the participants were female, whereas the remaining four studies had gender-balanced samples, with an average across the samples of 48.87% female (ranging from 42 to 54%). Regarding the geographic context, one study was conducted in Australia and the others in Europe.

3.1 Evidence of the effect of personality on adolescents’ PEB

One aim of the current systematic review was to examine the role of personality in influencing adolescents’ PEB. Four of the five studies evaluated this. One study (Quintelier, 2014) used the “Big Five” personality model to examine the correlation between personality and political consumer behavior, specifically the intention to boycott products for social, political, or environmental reasons. It found a correlation between Openness to experience and political consumption behaviors. A regression analyses showed that Openness has a positive effect on the intent to boycott, while Extraversion has a negative effect: that is, participants who are open-minded, imaginative, curious, creative, and insightful (McCrae and John, 1992) are more likely to boycott products for social, political, and environmental reasons. In contrast, participants with characteristics of sociability, talkativeness, assertiveness, and excitability (McCrae and John, 1992) are less likely to boycott.

Another study (Poškus and Žukauskienė, 2017) focused on adolescents’ recycling behavior. Taking a person-oriented approach, the study was designed to verify not only the relationship between recycling behavior and personality traits, but also to cluster the participants into groups of similar people using the “Big Five” personality traits. Specifically, the goal was to investigate how different configurations of personality traits influenced, among other variables, attitude, intention, behavior. The first analyses found that Agreeableness and Conscientiousness have moderate correlations with recycling attitudes and intention, while Agreeableness is also related to self-reported recycling behavior. Further analysis indicates four clusters: (1) “Positive,” which includes adolescents who score highly for Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness, and Neuroticism; (2) “Negative,” which includes those with high Neuroticism and low Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness; (3) “Extravert and Open,” which includes adolescents with high scores for Extraversion and Openness, moderate scores for Conscientiousness and Neuroticism, and low scores for Agreeableness; and (4) “Agreeable and Closed groups,” which include those with high scores for Agreeableness and low for Openness. Adolescents in the positive cluster reported more recycling behavior than those in the other three clusters, although this difference was only marginal. In addition, the adolescents in this cluster differed significantly from the others in terms of their intention to recycle and attitudes toward recycling.

In another sample, Poškus (2020a), using the same person-oriented approach, grouped the adolescents into personality-trait clusters to study the effect of personality on five pro-environmental intentions and behaviors: recycling, water conservation, electricity conservation, sustainable consumption, and sustainable-transportation use. The four final clusters differed slightly from those in the previous study (Poškus and Žukauskienė, 2017): (1) “Positive,” grouping individuals who are stable, social, friendly, responsible, and open to new ideas and experiences; (2) “Conservative,” whose members are low in Openness and relatively emotionally stable; (3) “Outgoing,” grouping adolescents who score highly for Extraversion; and (4) “Negative,” comprising individuals who are moderately disagreeable and unconscientious, without refusing novel ideas. The results reveal that the Positive cluster displayed the highest pro-environmental scores, while the Conservative cluster had the lowest. The Negative and Outgoing clusters received average scores for their pro-environmental tendencies, with the Negative cluster being slightly more open to PEB.

Finally, in another study (Poškus, 2020b) examined the association between personality and reaction to persuasive messages encouraging pro-environmental behavior such as recycling and the conservation of water and electricity. This was a large intervention study, predating the previously summarized work (Poškus, 2020a), and it included the same clusters of participants. However, the goal of this research was to verify whether people with different personality patterns were differently affected by persuasive messages in terms of the effects on their respective PEI and PEA. At the starting point, the Positive cluster exhibited the highest level of PEB, while the Conservative cluster displayed the lowest, and the Negative and Outgoing clusters in the range between. After the intervention, the Positive cluster continued to have higher scores, being more influenced by the persuasive messages. Conversely, the Negative cluster was least affected by the persuasive messages. Interestingly, participants in the Conservative cluster, despite their characteristic resistance to adopting new behaviors, did evidence some changes. The strongest effect was seen for the Outgoing cluster, meaning that those adolescents who are more reactant to salient social norms will react more strongly to interventions that promote pro-environmental behavior as a salient social norm, which has been done in this study.

3.2 Evidence on the effect of emotional intelligence on adolescents’ pro-environmental behavior

The second aim of this review is to examine the relationship between EI and PEB during adolescence. A search for studies of this relationship found just one article (Robinson et al., 2019). That study involved the Adolescent Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test, which measures four EI traits: (1) Emotional Recognition and Expression (ERE), (2) Understand Emotions of Others (UEO); (3) Emotions Direct Cognition (EDC); and (4) Emotional Management and Control (EMC). The aim was to evaluate whether EI was associated with PEB and PEA and whether the relationship between PEA and PEB was moderated by EI.

The correlation analyses in this study revealed a moderate positive relationship between PEA and PEB, indicating that stronger pro-environmental attitudes are related to higher levels of pro-environmental behavior. Conversely, EMC is inversely correlated with PEA, suggesting that individuals with better emotional management and control may have weaker pro-environmental attitudes. Concerning the potential moderating effect of EI on the relationship between PEA and PEB, the study found that only UEO and EMC had the potential to act as moderators, occurring when both UEO and EMC are low or high. A hierarchical multiple regression confirmed that EI explains an additional 3% of the variance in the model and that higher EMC predicts higher PEB. The results confirm the presence of moderation, suggesting that these dimensions of EI—in particular EMC—play a role in influencing the relationship between pro-environmental attitudes and behavior.

Overall, this study provides evidence that emotional intelligence—particularly the dimension of emotional management and control—is associated with pro-environmental behavior during adolescence. It also suggests that certain aspects of emotional intelligence may moderate the relationship between PEA and behavior.

4 Discussion

The commitment of the younger generation to PEB is vital for environmental protection, so it is equally important to identify the factors that could increase that commitment. In recent years, various studies have focused on the psychological variables contributing to this, although investigations with adolescents remain rare. This systematic review synthesized the evidence related to adolescents’ PEB and the association with both personality and emotional intelligence. To our knowledge, no other systematic reviews have specifically addressed these concepts in the context of adolescent PEB. In summary, the main findings of this review are as follows: (1) personality and EI traits can be empirically distinguished in adolescence, (2) certain personality traits or clusters and specific dimensions of EI are more strongly associated with environmentalism than others, and (3) the data could underpin suggestions for promoting the development of PEB in adolescence.

Although the results indicate a relationship between adolescents’ personality, EI, and PEB, it is important to note that the current literature does not establish a causal relationship. Only one of the five studies included in this review employed an experimental design, while the others were correlational in nature. Therefore, more research is needed to deepen our understanding in this area. As noted previously, a range of variables is associated with PEB and it may be interesting to research the interplay between those variables and personality and EI. Nevertheless, the results of this review indicate that these findings from studies with adolescent samples are consistent with those from studies of adults.

Agreeableness and Openness to experience were shown by Quintelier (2014) to lead to more political consumption, and the same “positive” cluster is also more pro-environmental (Poškus and Žukauskienė, 2017; Poškus, 2020a,b). These results are consistent with those for adult samples (Markowitz et al., 2012; Brick and Lewis, 2016; Puech et al., 2019; Soutter and Mõttus, 2020; Gibbon and Douglas, 2021; Panno et al., 2021). Furthermore, Poškus (2020b) shows that individuals in the Positive cluster, after exposure to persuasive messages, become even more environmentalist. This could mean that treating PEB as something new may increase PEB, even in those who are already pro-environmentalist. Quintelier (2014) concludes that the extraversion trait has a negative effect on the political consumption. Conversely, the clusters “Extravert and Open” (Poškus and Žukauskienė, 2017) and “Outgoing” (Poškus, 2020a,b)—which comprise those with high scores for Extraversion—are associated with average levels of PEB. This might suggest that the associations between personality and PEB could be clarified by research that—rather than analyzing traits—takes a person-oriented approach, grouping individuals holistically and according to the patterns between them (Poškus, 2018). This suggestion becomes more significant when considering the research by Poškus (2020b) concerning the “Conservative” cluster. Being characterized by low openness, average neuroticism, and average scores in other traits, these individuals tend to be resistant to novel ideas and values—attributes required for the adoption of PEB (Poškus, 2018). However, surprisingly, interventions have shown that even individuals in this cluster can experience improvements in their PEB levels in response to pro-PEB messaging. This highlights the potential of to promote PEB among all individuals, regardless of their personality traits. Different personality clusters react differently to persuasive messaging, and these results suggest that personalizing messages to align with different traits or personality profiles could make them more effective. Indeed, previous research has shown that a message’s persuasiveness is influenced by the personality of the recipient (Pangbourne et al., 2020) and that personalized messages are likely to be more successful than generic messaging (Jylhä et al., 2013). Empirical evidence shows that personality traits can change (Jach et al., 2023)—throughout the lifespan or in response to interventions. In adolescence, these changes are very pronounced (Bleidorn et al., 2018). Therefore, the need for personality-change interventions and personalized learning (Jach et al., 2023) may be another practical implication of these results. The idea is to combine environmental science with behavioral change to designing interventions to promote, for example, the traits needed to become more open to the novelty of PEBs or less resistant to their costs (Niu et al., 2023) and that usually prevent it.

As noted earlier, the dimensions of Emotional Intelligence most strongly correlated with PEB in the sample of adolescents are “Emotions Management and Control” and “Understanding Emotions of Others.” Although this correlation is moderate, there is evidence—from both the adult samples (Mattingly and Kraiger, 2019) and the children and adolescent samples (Durlak et al., 2011)—that EI can be enhanced with training. Given this promising connection, it could be beneficial to incorporate EI development into environmental education programs. By linking sustainability with emotional skills, we can strengthen the relationship between pro-environmental behavior and emotional intelligence. The data obtained from this systematic review provides support for the claim that personality and EI are related to PEB, and furthermore, these findings could contribute to environmental-education initiatives, PEB programs, and interventions to deal with eco-anxiety.

EI includes the ability to perceive, understand, monitor, and regulate one’s own and others’ feelings and to use this information to guide one’s own actions. In the context of this study, a plausible hypothesis is that the ability to manage the emotions associated with environmental protection will allow a person to engage in mechanisms of adaption, such as PEB. For example, approaches to managing eco-anxiety include the provision of emotional support and emotional-focused interventions. (For a scoping review, see Baudon and Jachens, 2021). This assigns to EI the important role of not only intervening to mitigate the negative impact of environmental problems on mental health, but also linking EI and the ability to take action for the environmental protection. Future research might examine this potentiality by creating environmental education programs that acknowledge this link.

5 Limitations and strengths

The small number of studies (n = 5) included in this review is the greatest limitation of this work. To overcome this, future studies could include both peer-reviewed journals and so-called “gray literature.” Still regarding the number of studies, we are aware that when this happens a meta-analyses could be considered. However, that was not possible in this case, as the studies were too heterogeneous to be comparable (Lee, 2019) and the data was not sufficient to permit generalization (Allen, 2020). Nevertheless, this limitation might reflect the under-researched nature of this topic, explained by the novelty of the area of study or the focus on these particular variables —aspects which could also be considered strengths. To measure both personality traits and PEB, a plurality of instruments was used, which may also be a limitation. Although all the studies in the review use the “Big Five” model, Quintelier (2014) used a different scale to Poškus and Žukauskienė (2017), Poškus (2020a), and Poškus (2020b). In the same way, the study by Robinson et al. (2019) applied a scale to measure PEB, but in all the other studies, no specific instruments were used to measure PEB.

Despite these limitations, the goal and findings of this review can be considered strengths, as this is—to the best of our knowledge—the first systematic review to consider the relationship between adolescents’ personality, emotional intelligence, and pro-environmental behaviors and how this influences the processes that drive adolescents’ PEB.

6 Conclusion

In conclusion, the findings of this review demonstrate the need for further research on the relationship between personality, emotional intelligence, and PEB in adolescents. This review suggests that certain personality traits and dimensions of emotional intelligence are associated with PEB, affecting adolescents in a manner consistent with that shown by previous studies to affect adults. This has important implications for research and practice, including for policymakers and/or educational programs’ designers to how to design interventions to promote PEB in different traits or clusters of personality and EI. However, more experimental studies and an exploration of the interplay between multiple variables are necessary to establish causality and deepen our understanding of these relationships in the context of adolescent PEB.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

PL: Conceptualization. Writing−original draft, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology. CG-R: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing−review and editing. LG: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing−review and editing. AM: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing−review and editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research has received funding from the Galician Department of Culture, Education, Professional Training, and Universities (grant number ED431B 2022/35). Three of the authors belong to the Galician Competitive Research Group COSOYPA (GPC2022 GI-1456).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aguilar-Luzón, M. C., Calvo-Salguero, A., and Salinas, J. M. (2014). Beliefs and environmental behavior: the moderating effect of emotional intelligence. Scand. J. Psychol. 55, 619–629. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M. (2020). Understanding the practice, application, and limitations of meta-analysis. Am. Behav. Sci. 64, 74–96. doi: 10.1177/0002764219859619

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Apaolaza, V., Paredes, M. R., Hartmann, P., Barrutia, J. M., and Echebarria, C. (2022). How does mindfulness relate to proenvironmental behavior? the mediating influence of cognitive reappraisal and climate change awareness. J. Cleaner Production 357:131914. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2022.131914

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashton, M. C., and Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO–60: a short measure of the major dimensions of personality. J. Pers. Assessment 91, 340–345. doi: 10.1080/00223890902935878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aziz, F., Rami, A. A., Zaremohzzabieh, Z., and Ahrari, S. (2021). Effects of emotions and ethics on pro-environmental behavior of university employees: a model based on the theory of planned behavior. Sustainability 13:7062. doi: 10.3390/su13137062

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Azpiazu, L., Fernández-Zabala, A., and Rodríguez-Fernández, A. (2022). Perceived emotional intelligence and subjective well-being during adolescence: the moderating effect of age and sex. Curr. Psychol. 42, 31048–33106. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-04128-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balundė, A., Perlaviciute, G., and Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I. (2020). Sustainability in youth: environmental considerations in adolescence and their relationship to pro-environmental. Behavior. Front. Psychol. 11:582920. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.582920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamberg, S., and Moser, G. (2007). Twenty years after hines, hungerford, and tomera: a new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 14–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baudon, P., and Jachens, L. (2021). A scoping review of interventions for the treatment of eco-anxiety. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:9636. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18189636

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blankenberg, A., and Alhusen, H. (2019). On the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behavior: a Literature Review and Guide for the Empirical Economist (February 21, 2019). Germany: Center for European, Governance, and Economic Development Research. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3473702

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J., and Lucas, R. E. (2018). Life events and personality trait change. J. Pers. 86, 83–96. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12286

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Böhme, T., Stanszus, L., Geiger, S., Fischer, D., and Schrader, U. (2018). Mindfulness training at school: a way to engage adolescents with sustainable consumption? Sustainability 10, 1–22. doi: 10.3390/su10103557

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brick, C., and Lewis, G. J. (2016). Unearthing the “green” personality: core traits predict environmentally friendly behavior. Environ. Behav. 48, 635–658. doi: 10.1177/0013916514554695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrieri, A., and Fermani, A. (2018). Sustainable accommodation choice in tourism and emotional intelligence connected: an exploratory study looking for evidence. Cogent Psychol. 5:1485474. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2018.1485474

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Passafaro, P., and Bonnes, M. (2008). Emotions, habits and rational choices in ecological behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chowdhury, R. (2017). Emotional intelligence and consumer ethics: the mediating role of personal moral Philosophies. J. Bus. Ethics 142, 527–548. doi: 10.1007/s10551-015-2733-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coelho, F., Pereira, M. C., Cruz, L., Simões, P., and Barata, E. (2017). Affect and the adoption of pro-environmental behaviours: a structural model. J. Environ. Psychol. 54, 127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.10.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collado, S., Evans, G. W., Corraliza, J. A., and Sorrel, M. A. (2015). The role played by age on children’s pro-ecological behaviors: an exploratory analysis. J. Environ. Psychol. 44, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.09.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collado, S., Staats, H., and Sancho, P. (2019). Normative influences on adolescents’ self-reported pro-environmental behaviors: the role of parents and friends. Environ. Behav. 51, 288–314. doi: 10.1177/0013916517744591

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P. T., Anderegg, W. R. L., Verheggen, B., Maibach, E. W., et al. (2016). Consensus on consensus: a synthesis of consensus estimates on human-caused global warming. Environ. Res. Lett. 11:048002. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/11/4/048002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). The five-factor model of personality and its relevance to personality disorders. J. Pers. Disord. 6, 343–359. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1992.6.4.343

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Moor, E. L., Nelemans, S. A., Becht, A. I., Meeus, W. H. J., and Branje, S. (2022). Personality development across adolescence and young adulthood: the role of life transitions and self-concept clarity. Eur. J. Pers. 37, 587–604. doi: 10.1177/08902070221119782

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Durán, M., Alzate, M., López, W., and Sabucedo, J. M. (2007). Emociones y comportamiento pro-ambiental. Rev. Latinoamericana Psicol. 39, 287–296.

Google Scholar

Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., and Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 82, 405–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, W. T., Ng, E., Liu, S., Chiang, Y., and Chang, M. (2021). Determinants of pro-environmental behavior among excessive smartphone usage children and moderate smartphone usage children in Taiwan. PeerJ 9:e11635. doi: 10.7717/peerj.11635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., Franchin, L., and Benavides-Varela, S. (2023). Evaluations of pro-environmental behaviors by 7-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 72:101865. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101865

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giancola, M., Palmiero, M., and D’Amico, S. (2022). Social sustainability in late adolescence: trait emotional intelligence mediates the impact of the dark triad on altruism and equity. Front. Psychol. 13:840113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.840113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbon, E., and Douglas, H. E. (2021). Personality and the pro-environmental individual: unpacking the interplay between attitudes, behaviour and climate change denial. Pers. Individ. Dif. 181:111031. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, R., and Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro- environmental concern and behaviour: a review. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 141–157. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez-López, M., Viejo, C., and Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2019). Psychological well-being during adolescence: stability and association with romantic relationships. Front. Psychol. 10:1772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, B., and Zheng, Y. (2021). More is not always better: a study of country-level factors associated with adolescents’ environmental attitudes using a multilevel analysis of PISA 2006. Educ. Policy Analysis Arch. 29:125. doi: 10.14507/epaa.29.4846

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Habib, M., Osmont, A., Tavani, J., Cassotti, M., and Caparos, S. (2023). Is adolescence believed to be a period of greater risk taking than adulthood? Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 28:1. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2023.2242469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, E. R., and Garrett, M. K. (2017). Preschoolers’ moral judgments of environmental harm and the influence of perspective taking. J. Environ. Psychol. 53, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huoponen, A. (2023). From concern to behavior: barriers and enablers of adolescents’ pro-environmental behavior in a school context. Environ. Educ. Res. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2023.2180374

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

IPCC (2023). “Summary for Policymakers,” in Climate Change 2023: Synthesis Report. Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, eds H. Lee and J. Romero (Geneva: IPCC).

Google Scholar

Jach, H. K., Bardach, L., and Murayama, K. (2023). How personality matters for education research. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 35:94. doi: 10.1007/s10648-023-09807-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. A. (2014). Measuring thirty facets of the five factor model with a 120-item public domain inventory: development of the IPIP-NEO-120. J. Res. Personality 51, 78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.05.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovanović, S., Gatarić, D., Prnjat, Z., Andjelković, G., Jovanović, J. M., Lukić, B., et al. (2016). Exploring proenvironmental behavior of serbian youth through environmental values, satisfaction, and responsibility. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 44, 1057–1068. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2016.44.7.1057

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jylhä, A., Nurmi, P., Sirén, M., Hemminki, S., and Jacucci, G. (2013). “MatkaHupi: a persuasive mobile application for sustainable mobility,” in Proceedings of the 2013 ACM conference on Pervasive and ubiquitous computing adjunct publication (UbiComp ’13 Adjunct). Association for Computing Machinery, (New York, NY). doi: 10.1145/2494091.2494164

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keefer, K. V., Holden, R. R., and Parker, J. D. A. (2013). Longitudinal assessment of trait emotional intelligence: measurement invariance and construct continuity from late childhood to adolescence. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1255–1272. doi: 10.1037/a0033903

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keith, R. J., Given, L. M., Martin, J. M., and Hochuli, D. F. (2021). Urban children’s connections to nature and environmental behaviors differ with age and gender. PLoS One 16:e0255421. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255421

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klöckner, C. A. (2013). A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour—a meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 1028–1038. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.05.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koessler, A., Vorlaufer, T., and Fiebelkorn, F. (2022). Social norms and climate-friendly behaviour of adolescents. PLoS One 17:e0266847. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krettenauer, T. (2017). Pro-environmental behavior and adolescent moral development. J. Res. Adolesc. 27, 581–593. doi: 10.1111/jora.12300

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krettenauer, T., Wang, W., Jia, F., and Yao, Y. (2019). Connectedness with nature and the decline of pro-environmental behavior in adolescence: a comparison of Canada and China. J. Environ. Psychol. 71:101348. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101348

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, F., and Dewitte, S. (2019). Measuring pro-environmental behavior: review and recommendations. J. Environ. Psychol. 63, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K., and Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behav. Res. 39, 329–358. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K., Gjersoe, N., O’Neill, S., and Barnett, J. (2020). Youth perceptions of climate change: a narrative synthesis. WIREs Clim. Change 11:e641. doi: 10.1002/wcc.641

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. H. (2019). Strengths and limitations of meta-analysis. Korean J. Med. 4, 391–395. doi: 10.3904/kjm.2019.94.5.391

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Zhao, L., Ma, S., and Zhang, L. (2019). What influences an individual’s pro-environmental behavior? a literature review. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 146, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2019.03.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, E. M., Goldberg, L. R., Ashton, M. C., and Lee, K. (2012). Profiling the “pro-environmental individual”: a personality perspective. J Pers. 80, 81–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00721.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattingly, V., and Kraiger, K. (2019). Can emotional intelligence be trained? a meta-analytical investigation. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 29, 140–155. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2018.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., Parker, W. D., Mills, C. J., De Fruyt, F., et al. (2002). Personality trait development from age 12 to age 18: longitudinal, cross-sectional and cross-cultural analyses. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1456–1468. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1456

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J. Pers. 60, 175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neurohr, A., Pasch, N., Otto, S., and Möller, A. (2023). Measuring adolescents’ level of interest in nature: a promising psychological factor facilitating nature protection. Front. Psychol. 14:1186557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1186557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, N., Fan, W., Ren, M., Li, M., and Zhong, Y. (2023). The role of social norms and personal costs on pro-environmental behavior: the mediating role of personal norms. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 16, 2059–2069. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S411640

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntanos, S., Arabatzis, G., and Chalikias, M. (2017). “The role of emotional intelligence as an underlying factor towards social acceptance of green investments,” in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Information and Communication Technologies in Agriculture, Food and Environment, HAICTA, Chaina, (Greece)

Google Scholar

Otto, S., Evans, G. W., Moon, M. J., and Kaiser, F. G. (2019). The development of children’s environmental attitude and behavior. Glob. Environ. Change 58:101947. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.101947

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Otto, S., Pensini, P., Zabel, S., Diaz-Siefer, P., Burnham, E., Navarro-Villarroel, C., et al. (2021). The prosocial origin of sustainable behavior: a case study in the ecological domain. Glob. Environ. Change 69:102312. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2021.102312

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 134, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palupi, T., and Sawitri, D. (2018). The importance of pro-environmental behavior in adolescent. E3S Web Conf. 31:09031. doi: 10.1051/e3sconf/20183109031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pangbourne, K., Bennett, S., and Al Baker. (2020). Persuasion profiles to promote pedestrianism: effective targeting of active travel messages. Travel Behav. Soc. 20, 300–312. doi: 10.1016/j.tbs.2020.04.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panno, A., De Cristofaro, V., Oliveti, C., and Donati, M. (2021). Personality and environmental outcomes: the role of moral anger in channeling climate change action and pro-environmental behavior. Analyses Soc. Issues Public Policy 21, 853–873. doi: 10.1111/asap.12254

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panno, A., Giacomantonio, M., Carrus, G., Maricchiolo, F., Pirchio, S., and Mannetti, L. (2018). Mindfulness, pro-environmental behavior, and belief in climate change: the mediating role of social dominance. Environ. Behav. 50, 864–888. doi: 10.1177/0013916517718887

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavalache-Ilie, M., and Cazan, A. M. (2018). Personality correlates of pro-environmental attitudes. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 28, 71–78. doi: 10.1080/09603123.2018.1429576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V. (2009a). “Psychometric properties of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire,” in Advances in the Assessment of Emotional Intelligence, eds C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske, and J. D. Parker (Berlin: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V. (2009b). Technical Manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue), 4th printing, 1st Edn. London: London Psychometric Laboratory. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poškus, M. (2018). Personality and pro-environmental behaviour. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 72, 969–970. doi: 10.1136/jech-2018-210483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poškus, M. (2020a). What works for whom? investigating adolescents’ pro-environmental behaviors. Sustainability 12:7313. doi: 10.3390/su12187313

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poškus, M. (2020b). Normative influence of pro-environmental intentions in adolescents with different personality types. Curr. Psychol. 39, 263–276. doi: 10.1007/s12144-017-9759-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poškus, M., and Žukauskienė, R. (2017). Predicting adolescents’ recycling behavior among different big five personality types. J. Environ. Psychol. 54, 57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puech, C., Dougal, J., Deery, C., Waddell, C., and Mottus, R. (2019). Openness is related to proenvironmental behavior both within and across families. Environ. Behav. 52:001391651985329. doi: 10.1177/0013916519853294

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quintelier, E. (2014). The influence of the Big 5 personality traits on young people’s political consumer behavior. Young Consum. Insight Ideas Responsible Marketers 15, 342–352. doi: 10.1108/YC-09-2013-00395

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robina-Ramírez, R., Medina, J., and McCallum, S. (2020). What role do emotions play in transforming students’ environmental behaviour at school? J. Cleaner Production 258, 120638. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.120638

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, A. C., Downey, L. A., Ford, T. C., Lomas, J. E., and Stough, C. (2019). Green teens: investigating the role of emotional intelligence in adolescent environmentalism. Pers. Individ. Dif. 138, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., and Palfai, T. P. (1995). “Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the trait meta-mood scale,” in Emotion, Disclosure, and Health, ed. J. W. Pennebaker (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 125–154. doi: 10.1037/10182-006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D., and Patton, G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health. 2, 223–228. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, L., Cutter-Mackenzie-Knowles, A., and Bellert, A. (2018). Still ‘Minding the Gap’ sixteen years later: (Re)Storying pro-environmental behaviour. Australian J. Environ. Educ. 34, 189–203. doi: 10.1017/aee.2018.32

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., and Potter, J. (2011). Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 330–348. doi: 10.1037/a0021717

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soutter, A. R. B., Bates, T. C., and Mõttus, R. (2020). Big Five and HEXACO personality traits, pro-environmental attitudes, and behaviors: a meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 913–941. doi: 10.1177/1745691620903019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soutter, A. R. B., and Mõttus, R. (2020). Big Five facets’ associations with pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. J. Pers. 89, 203–215. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and de Groot, J. (2012). “Environmental values,” in The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology, ed. S. D. Clayton (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 81–92. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199733026.013.0005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 56, 407–424. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00175

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, J., Ma, B., and Wei, S. (2023). Same gratitude, different pro-environmental behaviors? Effect of the dual-path influence mechanism of gratitude on pro-environmental behavior. J. Cleaner Production 415:137779. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2023.137779

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tetzner, J., Becker, M., and Bihler, L.-M. (2023). Personality development in adolescence: examining big five trait trajectories in differential learning environments. Eur. J. Pers. 37, 744–764. doi: 10.1177/08902070221121178

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomaes, S. C. E., Grapsas, S., van de Wetering, J., Spitzer, J. E., and Poorthuis, A. M. G. (2023). Green teens: Understanding and promoting adolescents’ sustainable engagement. One Earth 6, 352–361. doi: 10.1016/j.oneear.2023.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dijk, M. P. A., Hale, W. W., Hawk, S. T., Meeus, W., and Branje, S. (2020). Personality development from age 12 to 25 and its links with life transitions. Eur. J. Pers. 34, 322–344. doi: 10.1002/per.2251

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicedo-Cabrera, M., Scovronick, N., Sera, F., Royé, D., Schneider, R., Tobias, A., et al. (2021). The burden of heat-related mortality attributable to recent human-induced climate change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 11, 492–500. doi: 10.1038/s41558-021-01058-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitburn, J., Linklater, W., and Abrahamse, W. (2020). Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behaviour. Conserv. Biol. 34, 180–193. doi: 10.1111/cobi.13381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, C., and Law, D. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: an exploratory study. Leadersh. Q. 13, 243–274. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wray-Lake, L., Flanagan, C. A., and Osgood, D. W. (2010). Examining trends in adolescent environmental attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors across three decades. Environ. Behav. 42, 61–85. doi: 10.1177/0013916509335163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wray-Lake, L., Metzger, A., and Syvertsen, A. K. (2017). Testing multidimensional models of youth civic engagement: model comparisons, measurement invariance, and age differences. Appl. Dev. Sci. 21, 266–284. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1205495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, M., Qiu, M., Huang, L., and Nuse, B. (2019). Personality traits and village cadre adoption of rural environmental protection measures: a case study from China. J. Environ. Plann. Manag. 63, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/09640568.2019.1687429

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Žukauskienė, R., Truskauskaitė-Kunevićienė, I., Gabė, V., and Kaniušonytė, G. (2021). My words matter”: the role of adolescents in changing pro-environmental habits in the family. Environ. Behav. 53, 1140–1162. doi: 10.1177/0013916520953150

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pro-environmental behavior, adolescents, personality, emotional intelligence, systematic review

Citation: Lisboa PV, Gómez-Román C, Guntín L and Monteiro AP (2024) Pro-environmental behavior, personality and emotional intelligence in adolescents: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 15:1323098. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1323098

Received: 17 October 2023; Accepted: 15 January 2024;
Published: 13 February 2024.

Edited by:

Christoph Steinebach, Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

M. Carmen Aguilar-Luzón, University of Granada, Spain
Alessandra Geraci, Dante Alighieri University for Foreigners, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Lisboa, Gómez-Román, Guntín and Monteiro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paulo Vítor Lisboa, paulo.lisboa@rai.usc.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.