Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 26 May 2022
Sec. Avian Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neural Mechanisms of Cognitive Control and Emotion in Birds View all 22 articles

Activation of the Nucleus Taeniae of the Amygdala by Umami Taste in Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus)

  • 1Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Jena, Germany
  • 2Institute for Biology, Karl-Franzens-University Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 3Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy

In chickens, the sense of taste plays an important role in detecting nutrients and choosing feed. The molecular mechanisms underlying the taste-sensing system of chickens are well studied, but the neural mechanisms underlying taste reactivity have received less attention. Here we report the short-term taste behaviour of chickens towards umami and bitter (quinine) taste solutions and the associated neural activity in the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala, nucleus accumbens and lateral septum. We found that chickens had more contact with and drank greater volumes of umami than bitter or a water control, and that chicks displayed increased head shaking in response to bitter compared to the other tastes. We found that there was a higher neural activity, measured as c-Fos activation, in response to umami taste in the right hemisphere of the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala. In the left hemisphere, there was a higher c-Fos activation of the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala in response to bitter than in the right hemisphere. Our findings provide clear evidence that chickens respond differently to umami and bitter tastes, that there is a lateralised response to tastes at the neural level, and reveals a new function of the avian nucleus taeniae of the amygdala as a region processing reward information.

Introduction

Taste perception provides animals with information about the quality of food via five basic tastes: sweet, umami, bitter, salty and sour (Jiang et al., 2012). Bitter can indicate toxic and harmful substances, and aversion to bitter tastes likely evolved as a protective mechanism (Davis et al., 2010). Umami, on the other hand, reflects amino acids in protein (Chandrashekar et al., 2006), and is known to increase appetite, but also to increase satiety (Masic and Yeomans, 2014). Research on the mechanism(s) involved in taste perception will increase our understanding of how animals use taste to make adaptive foraging decisions.

To fully understand taste perception requires comparative physiological studies ranging from receptor mechanisms to brain circuitry, but also psychophysical studies that quantitatively describe the perceptual output of the system (Green, 2021). These types of research can lead to the discovery of new phenomena such as the functional loss or neofunctionalization of taste receptors (Baldwin et al., 2014), which generate new insights and hypotheses about the mechanisms and evolution of taste perception (Zhao et al., 2010; Jiang et al., 2012; Toda et al., 2021). The comparative genetic linkage between taste perception and feeding specializations are well studied (Li and Zhang, 2014; Wang and Zhao, 2015; Liu et al., 2016), whereas the cognitive mechanisms that underlie taste perception in animals other than humans, non-human primates, and rodents is less studied.

Among birds, domestic chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) are probably the most intensively investigated species with regards to taste receptor physiology and associated behaviour, which can be attributed to their commercial importance (for a review see Roura et al., 2013). Chickens lack the receptor for sweet taste (Cheled-Shoval et al., 2015), but possess umami and bitter receptors in their oral cavity (Shi and Zhang, 2006; Yoshida et al., 2015). The behavioural responses of chickens to sour, salty, umami, and bitter can be observed during the first post hatch day (Ganchrow et al., 1990). The typical response to bitter includes prolonged head shaking, beak wiping and sticking out the tongue (Nicol, 2004). Chicks also develop conditioned taste aversions and exhibit foraging biases in response to bitter (Skelhorn and Rowe, 2005a; Skelhorn and Rowe, 2005b; Skelhorn et al., 2008). Umami, on the other hand, is palatable for chickens (Roura et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2018; Niknafs and Roura, 2018) and it is suggested that chicks may perceive umami as a salty-and sweet-like taste (Yoshida et al., 2018).

While there has been some research into the neural mechanisms underlying reward in domestic chickens (Yanagihara et al., 2001; Izawa et al., 2002; Aoki et al., 2003; Matsushima et al., 2003; Ichikawa et al., 2004; Amita and Matsushima, 2014), there has been less attention given to the brain regions involved in palatable taste perception. In mammals, the pathway for the gustatory sensory information runs through the nucleus of the solitary tract to the parabranchial nucleus, which in turn projects to various forebrain regions, including the amygdaloid arcopallium and nucleus accumbens (Arends et al., 1988; Wild et al., 1990; Berk et al., 1993). Palatable taste perception involves the mesolimbic rewards system (Dayan and Balleine, 2002; Ramírez-Lugo et al., 2007; Berridge et al., 2009), which includes the nucleus accumbens, ventral pallidum, basolateral and medial amygdala (Ramírez-Lugo et al., 2006; Ramírez-Lugo et al., 2007; Wassum et al., 2009; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013; Baumgartner et al., 2021; Hu et al., 2021). Although the mesolimbic reward system is highly conserved in vertebrates (O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011), investigating whether these brain areas are also involved in taste perception in domestic chicks will add to our understanding of comparative taste perception and reward behaviour.

Here we present three different groups of young domestic chicks with either a solution containing monosodium glutamate and inosine 5′-monophosphate (umami taste), quinine solution (bitter taste), or to pure water (as a control) (Figure 1). Behavioural reactions to these stimuli were measured in terms of: 1) frequency of contact with the solution, 2) volume of liquid consumed, 3) frequency of head shakes and 4) beak cleaning episodes. Brain activation was measured by immunohistochemical detection of the Immediate Early Gene (IEG) product c-Fos in three interconnected subpallial brain regions: nucleus taeniae of the amygdala (TnA), nucleus accumbens (Ac) and lateral septum (LS) (Figure 2) that may directly modulate taste-responsive neurons of the parabrachial complex through an anatomical pathway using neurotensin (Bálint et al., 2011; Bálint et al., 2016), or receive gustatory information indirectly.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Experimental setup. Chicks were habituated in metal cages with food and water ad libitum. Prior to the presentation of the experimental taste, chicks were water deprived for 45 min by removing their water dish through an opening in the occluder. Then, an identical water dish with the experimental taste was presented for 10 minutes. A camera was placed above each cage to record chicks behavioural responses to the taste.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Typical placements of cell counting zones (green rectangles) in the regions of interest (pink). (A) Schematic representation of a coronal section at the level of the nucleus accumbens. (B) Coronal section at the level of the lateral septum. (C) Coronal section at the level of nucleus taeniae of the amygdala. TnA—nucleus taeniae of the amygdala; LS—lateral septum; Ac—nucleus accumbens; HF—hippocampal formation; M—mesopallium; N—nidopallium; Str - Striatum.

Results

Behaviour

Contact with the liquid significantly varied among treatments (ANOVA: F33,2 = 11.76, p < 0.001). Chicks had more contact with umami (mean ± s.e.m.: 38.5 ± 4.63) than with bitter (14.5 ± 2.54; p < 0.001) or control (25.3 ± 2.98; p = 0.012), and more contact with control than with bitter (p = 0.03) (Figure 3A). The amount of liquid consumed was also significantly different among treatments (F33,2 = 8.25, p = 0.001). Chicks consumed significantly more umami (0.05 ± 0.003) than control (0.03 ± 0.002; p = 0.041) or bitter (0.02 ± 0.003; p < 0.005) (Figure 3B). Head shaking was significantly different among treatments (F33,2 = 3.47, p = 0.042). Chicks shook their head significantly more frequently after having contact with bitter (1.43 ± 0.22) than with umami (0.92 ± 0.14; p = 0.04) or control (0.84 ± 0.13, p = 0.02) (Figure 3C). Beak cleaning was also significantly different among treatments (Kruskal–Wallis: X2 = 11.629, d.f. = 2, p = 0.002). Chicks cleaned their beak more frequently after having contact with bitter (1.83 ± 0.57) than with control (0.30 ± 0.12; p = 0.008) or umami (0.21 ± 0.12; p = 0.001) (Figure 3D).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Behavioural responses of chicks according to experimental tastes. (A) Contact with the liquid, (B) liquid consumed/body weight, (C) head shaking in relation to contact with the liquid and (D) beak cleaning in relation to contact with the liquid. Bar plots indicate mean ± s.e.m. (∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001).

Brain Activity

All 36 brains (n = 12 in each treatment) were successfully stained for c-Fos (Figure 4). However, the accumbens region of one brain from the quinine group was damaged and was excluded from further analysis.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. An example of c-Fos staining within the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala. The nuclei of c-Fos immunoreactive cells are stained black (red arrow). The c-Fos-negative cells (black arrow) are green due to the methyl-green counterstaining.

In nucleus taeniae of the amygdala there was a significant interaction between treatment and hemisphere (p = 0.008), meaning that in one of the hemispheres, treatment had an effect on c-Fos activation as revealed by ANOVA (Table 1). Only in the right nucleus taeniae was c-Fos activation higher in the umami group (Mean ± s.e.m: 1058.33 ± 90.92 cells/mm2) than in the control (813.75 ± 49.38 cells/mm2; p = 0.032), or bitter (742.81 ± 61.39 cells/mm2; p = 0.007) (Table 1). Such differences between treatments were not present in the left nucleus taeniae (control 851.45 ± 60.73 cells/mm2; umami: 952.08 ± 97.90 cells/mm2; bitter: 946.35 ± 86.28 cells/mm2) (Figure 5A). Moreover, an additional lateralised response was found in nucleus taeniae. c-Fos activation of the bitter group was higher in the left nucleus taeniae than in the right (p = 0.014). Control and umami treatments did not present such differences (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Summary of the statistical results of brain activation.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Measured c-Fos-ir (immunoreactive) cell densities of the different treatments across regions of interest, in the left and right hemisphere. (A) nucleus taeniae of the amygdala, (B) nucleus accumbens, and (C) lateral septum. Bar plots indicate mean ± s.e.m. (∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01).

In the other two brain regions (accumbens and lateral septum), there were no main effects of treatment or hemisphere and no interactions (Table1; Figure 5). However, patterns of activation qualitatively similar to the right nucleus taeniae of the amygdala were present also in right accumbens (control: 889.89 ± 122.50 cells/mm2; umami: 1104.99 ± 129.71 cells/mm2; bitter: 745.59 ± 82.49 cells/mm2), although the interaction was not significant at the alpha 0.05 level (p = 0.06, Table 1). The activation pattern in the left accumbens did not differ (control 848.30 ± 97.34 cells/mm2; umami: 986.27 ± 143.08 cells/mm2; bitter: 1030.36 ± 137.95 cells/mm2), nor did the left lateral septum (control: 536.97 ± 67.73 cells/mm2; umami: 535.52 ± 115.02 cells/mm2; bitter: 589.06 ± 103.74 cells/mm2), or right lateral septum (control: 563.67 ± 54.40 cells/mm2; umami: 562.57 ± 82.53 cells/mm2; bitter: 490.52 ± 62.03 cells/mm2).

Correlations

There were no significant correlations between c-Fos activation and any of the measured behaviours in any of the brain regions analysed (Table S1).

Discussion

We show that the avian nucleus taeniae of the amygdala responds to gustatory information: at the brain level, c-Fos activation was higher in the right nucleus taeniae in chicks that consumed umami, compared to those that consumed bitter or water control. This is the first evidence to show that the avian nucleus taeniae of the amygdala responds to umami taste. We also show a clear behavioural preference for umami and an aversion to bitter: chicks consumed more umami than bitter and water, and shook their heads and cleaned their beaks more after contact with bitter than with umami or water (see also Gentle, 1975; Ganchrow et al., 1990; Yoshida et al., 2015; Yoshida et al., 2018). The higher activation in the right nucleus taeniae of the amygdala in response to umami, and the greater consumption of umami is likely indicative of reward processing. The avian nucleus taeniae of the amygdala resembles a homolog brain region to the mammalian medial amygdala (Reiner et al., 2004; Yamamoto et al., 2005; Abellán et al., 2009), which in humans and other non-human primates is associated with reward experiences, such as a palatable tastes (Azuma et al., 1984; De Araujo et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2021). Our results suggest a conserved function of avian nucleus taeniae of the amygdala and the medial amygdala in mammals, and the involvement of the mesolimbic reward network in processing taste perception.

In mice, the medial amygdala is associated with processing reward information, at least in a social context (Hu et al., 2021). And in domestic chickens the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala has been reported to respond to social stimuli (Mayer et al., 2019), to novel environments (Morandi-Raikova and Mayer, 2020) and in phobic reactions to novel foods and objects (Perez et al., 2020). In our study we can exclude neophobia from explaining the response of nucleus taeniae of the amygdala because both bitter and umami tastes were novel, only umami was consumed significantly more by chicks, and only umami induced high activation of the right nucleus taeniae of the amygdala. The reward function of nucleus taeniae of the amygdala is further supported by a similar (although not significant) pattern of activity observed in the accumbens (Figure 5B). Accumbens in vertebrates is involved in processing reward information and plays an important role in learning and motivation (Day and Carelli, 2007). In domestic chicks the role of accumbens in processing reward information has been investigated only in a few studies (e.g. Yanagihara et al., 2001; Izawa et al., 2002; Aoki et al., 2003; Izawa et al., 2003; Ichikawa et al., 2004; Amita and Matsushima, 2014) which have suggested a role in the control of impulsivity when choosing a predictable food reward (Cardinal et al., 2001). Further studies should assess whether avian nucleus taeniae and nucleus accumbens work together in processing food reward information.

We also found higher c-Fos expression in the left nucleus taeniae of the amygdala than in the right in the chicks presented with bitter taste. This could indicate that some lateralised processing of bitter taste is happening in the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala. In humans, the amygdala plays a role in the recognition, evaluation and response to aversive stimuli (Zald et al., 1998). However, an alternative explanation is that this lateralisation pattern in the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala is not related to the recognition of bitter taste at all. Task independent higher expression of c-Fos in the left nucleus taeniae of the amygdala compared to right was present in our previous study (Corrales Parada et al., 2021), and spontaneous lateralisation of c-Fos activity in young chicks without performing any task has also been found in other brain areas (Lorenzi et al., 2019). Therefore, our results could be indicative of a baseline lateralisation of c-Fos expression independent of bitter taste. These explanations could be teased apart by testing chicks with different concentrations of bitter tastes, and different bitter tastants, and comparing the responses to a water control group.

In chicks, accumbens has been proposed to be involved in the formation of aversive taste memories in a passive avoidance learning paradigm (Patterson and Rose, 1992; Stewart and Rusakov, 1995; Csillag, 1999; Rose, 2000; Bálint et al., 2011). In our study, we did not find a response of accumbens to bitter taste. The paradigm used in our study had some important differences to the classic passive avoidance learning in which chicks are naïve to any visual stimuli, and spontaneously peck on a coloured bead and learn to associate the bitter taste with the colour of the bead. In our study, all chicks were habituated with water, and likely formed a positive association of water taste with the water container. Chicks in our study also had restricted access to water prior to the test to motivate them to drink. When we presented the experimental taste in the same container that was used during habituation, it is possible that the chicks anticipated a rewarding outcome. In humans, altering expectancy reduces neural responses of the gustatory cortex to aversive taste when participants were led to believe that a highly aversive bitter taste would be less distasteful than it actually was (Nitschke et al., 2006). If chicks expected a rewarded experience, this could explain why we did not observe activation of the brain regions associated with aversive taste as we expected. The lack of neural response to quinine in our study is in line with what has been reported in rats (Yasoshima et al., 2006), where the rats accumbens showed changes in c-Fos expression only during taste aversion retrieval but not after consumption of quinine hydrochloride. Future studies could compare naïve to experienced chicks in the response of the accumbens to bitter tastes, or present different concentration of quinine to naïve newly hatched chicks and investigate c-Fos activation in earlier stations of the gustatory system, such as nucleus of the solitary tract and parabrachial nucleus. In our study, the lateral septum did not show differences between treatments, which confirms a region-specific activation to rewarding taste in nucleus taeniae. To the best of our knowledge, this area has never been reported to process gustatory information. We also found no correlation between the behaviours expressed and c-Fos activation in any of the measured brain areas, which support the conclusion that exposure to appetitive and aversive taste, is associated with activation of the mesolimbic reward system in birds, and that this represents taste perception and not differences in motoric behaviour.

So far, we have only considered the role of gustatory information as a source of our effects. However, olfactory and gustatory information are usually combined in the sensory experience associated with food consumption. Since birds can perceive olfactory information (Jones and Roper, 1997; Krause et al., 2018), it is important to consider also the potential role of olfaction in our results. Chicks respond to odours predominantly when the right nostril is used (Vallortigara and Andrew, 1994; Rogers et al., 1998; Burne and Rogers, 2002). This has been interpreted as reflecting a predominant use of the right side of the brain to process olfactory information (Vallortigara and Andrew, 1994; Rogers et al., 1998; Burne and Rogers, 2002). The primary olfactory sensory area (the olfactory bulb) is a bilateral structure located within the frontal telencephalon of both hemispheres. The bulb projects to further telencephalic areas, without passing the thalamus (a unique feature of the olfactory pathway). It is reasonable to assume that the right bulb would predominantly project to the right hemisphere. However, projections to contralateral areas have also been reported (Reiner and Karten, 1985; Atoji and Wild, 2014). Intriguingly, the nucleus taeniae of the amygdala receives a direct projection from the olfactory bulb (Reiner and Karten, 1985). Thus, the similar rightward bias found in behavioural olfaction studies and in nucleus taeniae’s responses to umami, could indeed suggest a role of olfactory cues in our results. While we cannot exclude this interpretation, at present there is no evidence that chicks are actually able to smell umami. It is also not known if the nucleus taeniae process odour information at all. To date, only one study investigated if this structure would respond to odours in any bird species (Golüke et al., 2019). While in this study hippocampus responded to social odours, no evidence was found for the involvement of nucleus taeniae in odour processing (Golüke et al., 2019). Moreover, it has been shown that in another galliform species, gustatory information plays a more prominent role than olfactory information in responses to aposematic signals associated with aversive taste (Marples et al., 1994). Thus, while we cannot exclude an involvement of olfactory information in the activation of nucleus taeniae, we still believe that gustatory information played a major role in this effect. Future studies should be devoted to clarify this issue, e.g., by testing anosmic animals. In any case, whether through purely gustatory information or a combination of taste and olfaction, our results demonstrate that exposure to umami was a rewarding experience for chicks and that nucleus taeniae is involved in the processing of this rewarding information.

In conclusion, we show that umami taste is associated with higher activation of the right nucleus taeniae of the amygdala in domestic chicks. Our results allows us to speculate that reward processing in birds nucleus taeniae is an ancestral function shared with the mammalian medial amygdala and provides new insights into the regulation of gustatory rewards.

Methods

Subjects

We tested 36 four days old female chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) of the Aviagen ROSS 308 strain. Fertilized eggs were obtained from a commercial hatchery (CRESCENTI Societ`a Agricola S.r.l. –Allevamento Trepola–cod. Allevamento127BS105/2). Eggs were incubated from embryonic day one (E0) to seventeen (E17) in complete darkness. On the morning of E18 to the evening of E19, eggs were light stimulated following (Lorenzi et al., 2019). This procedure has an important impact on the lateralisation of chicks visual system (Rogers, 1982; Rogers and Sink, 1988; Rogers, 1990; Rogers and Bolden, 1991; Rogers and Deng, 1999), on the maturation of visual responses (Costalunga et al., 2021) and on several aspects of chicks behaviour (Daisley et al., 2009; Salva et al., 2009). Chicks hatched on E21 in darkness at a constant temperature of 37.7°C and a humidity of 60%. On the day of hatching, chicks were placed in individual metal cages (28 cm× 32 cm× 40 cm; W × H × L), where they were housed for three days with food and water ad libitum at a room temperature of 30–32°C and light conditions of 14 h light and 10 h dark.

Apparatus

On the third day after hatching (24 h prior to the experiment), chicks were taken to the experimental room and housed individually in metal cages (Figure 1), identical to their home cages. The experimental cages differed to the home cage only by the addition of a black plastic wall (occluder) that allowed us to make two compartments in the cage. Chicks were located on the larger section of the cage (25 cm L); while the smaller section (15 cm L) was used for keeping the water dishes separated from the chicks until the start of the experiment, and prevented chicks from observing the experimenter (see below). The two compartments were connected by a closable opening (5 × 4.8 cm) in the plastic wall. Above every cage (76 cm), there was a video camera (Microsoft LifeCam Cinema for Business) for recording the experiment. Illumination of the cages was provided by top lights (25 W warm light) at 67.5 cm above the cage, while the rest of the experimental room was dark.

Habituation

Chicks were left in their individual experimental cages for 24 h prior to the experiment with water and food ad libitum. Room temperature was 26.4–28.7°C, humidity 24–41%, and light conditions of 14 h light and10 h dark.

Experiment

For the experiment, we assigned 12 chicks to each treatment. Treatments consisted of three experimental tastes (control, umami and bitter). We used water as a control, since chicks were habituated to drink it. As a palatable taste, we used umami in a combination of 2.5% monosodium glutamate (MSG, AJI-NO-MOTO >99%, CEE: E621, France) + 0.25% inosine 5′-monophosphate (IMP, ≥98%, Sigma-Aldrich I2879-1G diluted in water). Yoshida et al. (2018) showed that at this concentration, MSG is an enhancer of the umami taste (IMP) in chicks. As an unpalatable bitter taste, we used quinine (Alfa Aesar A10459 99%) 10 mM dissolved in water. The experimental solutions were prepared the day prior to the experiment and kept in the experimental room to adjust their temperature to the room temperature. Experimental tastes were colourless and were presented to chicks in identical containers with 80 ml solution in each. Before the experiment chicks were deprived of water for 45 min. After this period, the experimental taste was presented for 10 min, the behaviour of the chicks was recorded, and after that, the experimental container was removed. The containers with the experimental solutions were weighted before and after the experiment in order to estimate the amount of liquid consumed by each chick. We ran the experiment in 4 days, testing nine chicks per day. Every testing day, we assigned three individuals to each treatment in randomized order.

Video Analysis

We analysed the videos for seven minutes after the first contact with the experimental taste. In this period, we recorded the total number of the following events: contact with the liquid (when chicks peck the experimental solution), head shaking, and beak cleaning. The category ‘beak cleaning’ includes two behaviours: beak wiping, when the animal wipes its beak on the floor and beak scratching, performed with a foot. Video analysis was conducted blinded to the experimental conditions using the VLC media player software (v. 3.0.12).

Histology and Immunohistochemistry

Seventy min after the first contact with the experimental taste, all chicks were weighted and received a lethal dose (0.8 ml) of a ketamine/xylazine solution (1:1 ketamine 10 mg/ml + xylazine 2 mg/ml). They were then perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; 0.1 mol, pH = 7.4, 0.9% sodium chloride, 4°C) and paraformaldehyde (4% PFA in PBS, 4°C). The head was post-fixed and stored in 4% PFA for at least 8 days until processing. All the following brain processing steps were performed blind to the experimental treatments. To ensure that the subsequent coronal brain sections would have the same orientation (°45), brains were removed from the skulls following the procedures described in the chick brain atlas (Kuenzel and Masson, 1988). The left and the right hemispheres were separated and processed independently. Each hemisphere was embedded in a 7% gelatine in PBS solution containing egg yolk at 40°C and post-fixated for 48h at 5°C in 20% sucrose in 4% PFA/PBS and further 48 h in 30% sucrose in 0.4% PFA/PBS solution. Four series of 45 μm coronal sections were cut on a Cryostat (Leica CM1850 UV) at −20°C. Two out of the four series were collected in cold PBS. The sections of the first series were used for labelling. The sections of the second series were kept as backup. For free-floating immunostaining, sections were incubated in 0.3% H2O2 in PBS for 20 min to deplete endogenous peroxidase activity. Between every of the following steps of the procedure the sections were washed in PBS. After the sections were treated with 3% normal goat serum (S-1000, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in PBS for 30 min at room temperature, they were transferred to the c-Fos antibody solution (c-Fos antibody, 1:2000; rabbit, polyclonal K-25, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) containing 0.1% Bovine Serum Albumin (BSA, SP-5050, Vector Laboratories) in PBS and incubated for 48 h at 5°C on a rotator. The secondary antibody reaction was carried out using a biotinylated anti-rabbit solution (1:200, BA- 1000, Vector Laboratories) in PBS for 60 min at room temperature on a rotator. The ABC method was used for signal amplification (Vectastain Elite ABC Kit, PK-6100, Vector Laboratories). Cells with concentrated c-Fos protein were visualized with the VIP substrate kit for peroxidase (SK-4600, Vector Laboratories), at a constant temperature of 25°C for 25 min. This produced a dark purple reaction product confined to the nuclei of c-Fos immunoreactive (-ir) cells. Brain sections of right and left hemispheres were mounted with the same orientation on gelatine-coated slides, enabling the coder to be blind to the hemisphere’s identity while counting. Slides were then dried at 50°C, counterstained with methyl green (H-3402, Vector Laboratories) and cover slipped with Eukitt (FLUKA).

Brain Anatomy

Brain sections were analysed blind to the experimental condition and hemispheres. c-Fos-ir cells were stained purple-black and were easily discerned from the background and non-activated cells, which were stained light-green (Figure 4). Photos were taken with a Zeiss microscope (objective magnification: ×20 with a numerical aperture of 0.5) connected to a digital camera (Zeiss AxioCam MRc5) and the Zeiss imaging software ZEN. Exposure time of the camera and the light conditions of the microscope were kept constant for all photos, while the contrast was slightly adjusted for individual photos to match their visual appearance if it was needed.

To analyse brain activation within the regions of interest a spot with the highest number of c-Fos-ir cells was chosen, by visual observation under the microscope. Then a counting area (400 × 400 µm) was positioned over this spot by keeping a minimum distance of 10 µm from the borders. A snapshot was taken, cropped around the counting area and saved. Automatic counting of the c-Fos immunoreactive cells was performed using the “analyse particle” function of the ImageJ software (Schneider et al., 2012). All photos were analysed with a predefined macro, where the image was transformed into 8bit, threshold was set to 120, circularity of particles to 0.5–1.0 and particle size to 2–200.

For the analysis of nucleus taeniae of the amygdala five sections of both hemispheres were selected from an area corresponding to the A8.8 to A6.4 of the brain atlas (Kuenzel and Masson, 1988) (Figure 2C). To quantify c-Fos-ir cells in accumbens (Bálint et al., 2016), five sections of both hemispheres were selected from A10.0 to A9.2 (Kuenzel and Masson, 1988; Figure 2A). Lateral septum was measured on five selected sections from A8.8 to A7.6 (Kuenzel and Masson, 1988) in the lower half of septum starting from the ventral border of the lateral ventricle (Figure 2B). The lateral septum was further delineated from the medial septum based on anatomical landmarks that are visible after methyl green counterstain. The cell densities obtained from the different brain sections were averaged to estimate overall activity in each measured area. These individual bird means were employed for further statistical analysis.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analysis were conducted with R v. 4.0.4 (R Core Team, 2021). We analysed whether behavioural responses varied according to treatment. The response variables were contact with the liquid, liquid consumed per body weight, head shaking and beak cleaning. The raw values were divided by the number of contact with liquid episodes. This allowed to reveal how much the contact with liquid elicited the behaviours of interest. ANOVA was run on the first three variables with Fischer’s LSD tests for post-hoc analysis. Given that for beak cleaning the residuals were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test, W = 0.7382, p < 0.05), even after square root or logarithmic transformation, a non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test was used. Significance between the comparisons was assessed with post-hoc Dunn tests.

In order to test whether c-Fos activation (response variable) varied according to treatment and hemisphere (factors), we conducted a repeated measures ANOVA for each brain region. We used a between-subject factors with three levels (Treatment: control, umami, bitter), and within-subject factor with two levels (Hemisphere: left and right). Given that for nucleus taeniae of the amygdala and accumbens residuals were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk Test; nucleus taeniae: W71 = 0.955, p = 0.013; accumbens: W71 = 0.952, p = 0.009), we used a square root transformation. This procedure significantly improved the normality of residuals (nucleus taeniae: W71 = 0.979, p = 0.272; accumbens: W71 = 0.982, p = 0.393), and further analysis were conducted with transformed data. Between treatment comparisons were assessed with independent post-hoc t-tests, while between hemispheres comparisons for each treatment was assessed with paired t-tests. In order to test whether behaviour and motoric activity is correlated with c-Fos expression, we performed Pearson correlations between c-Fos expression in the brain regions analysed and the behaviours recorded for each of the treatments and hemispheres. All figures were created or postprocessed using Adobe Photoshop and Illustrator software.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Ministero della Salute, Dipartimento Alimenti, Nutrizione e Sanita’ Pubblica Veterinaria (permit number 60/2020-PR).

Author Contributions

FP-S: conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, writing–original draft. CDCP: software, investigation, formal analysis. UM: conceptualisation, methodology, validation, formal analysis, resources, data curation, writing–review and editing, visualization, supervision, project administration. HMR: conceptualisation, methodology, validation, resources, writing–review and editing, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition.

Funding

FP‐S is funded by an IMPRS doctoral fellowship. HMR is funded by the Max Planck Society. UM is funded by the University of Trento.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Anastasia Morandi-Raikova for her support with lab work.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2022.897931/full#supplementary-material

References

Abellán A., Legaz I., Vernier B., Rétaux S., Medina L. (2009). Olfactory and Amygdalar Structures of the Chicken Ventral Pallium Based on the Combinatorial Expression Patterns of LIM and Other Developmental Regulatory Genes. J. Comp. Neurol. 516, 166–186. doi:10.1002/cne.22102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amita H., Matsushima T. (2014). Competitor Suppresses Neuronal Representation of Food Reward in the Nucleus Accumbens/Medial Striatum of Domestic Chicks. Behav. Brain Res. 268, 139–149. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoki N., Izawa E.-I., Yanagihara S., Matsushima T. (2003). Neural Correlates of Memorized Associations and Cued Movements in Archistriatum of the Domestic Chick. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1935–1946. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02632.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arends J. J. A., Wild J. M., Zeigler H. P. (1988). Projections of the Nucleus of the Tractus Solitarius in the Pigeon (Columba livia). J. Comp. Neurol. 278, 405–429. doi:10.1002/cne.902780310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atoji Y., Wild J. M. (2014). Efferent and Afferent Connections of the Olfactory Bulb and Prepiriform Cortex in the Pigeon (Columba livia). J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 1728–1752. doi:10.1002/cne.23504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azuma S., Yamamoto T., Kawamura Y. (1984). Studies on Gustatory Responses of Amygdaloid Neurons in Rats. Exp. Brain Res. 56, 12–22. doi:10.1007/BF00237437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin M. W., Toda Y., Nakagita T., O'Connell M. J., Klasing K. C., Misaka T., et al. (2014). Evolution of Sweet Taste Perception in Hummingbirds by Transformation of the Ancestral Umami Receptor. Science 345, 929–933. doi:10.1126/science.1255097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bálint E., Balázsa T., Zachar G., Mezey S., Csillag A. (2016). Neurotensin: Revealing a Novel Neuromodulator Circuit in the Nucleus Accumbens-Parabrachial Nucleus Projection of the Domestic Chick. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 605–616. doi:10.1007/s00429-014-0928-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bálint E., Mezey S., Csillag A. (2011). Efferent Connections of Nucleus Accumbens Subdivisions of the Domestic Chicken (Gallus domesticus): An Anterograde Pathway Tracing Study. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 2922–2953. doi:10.1002/cne.22672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner H. M., Schulkin J., Berridge K. C. (2021). Activating Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Systems in the Nucleus Accumbens, Amygdala, and Bed Nucleus of Stria Terminalis: Incentive Motivation or Aversive Motivation? Biol. Psychiatry 89, 1162–1175. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berk M. L., Smith S. E., Mullins L. A. (1993). Distribution, Parabrachial Region Projection, and Coexistence of Neuropeptide and Catecholamine Cells of the Nucleus of the Solitary Tract in the Pigeon. J. Comp. Neurol. 327, 416–441. doi:10.1002/cne.903270308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge K. C., Kringelbach M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of Affect: Brain Mechanisms of Pleasure and Displeasure. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 294–303. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge K. C., Robinson T. E., Aldridge J. W. (2009). Dissecting Components of Reward: “Liking”, “Wanting”, and Learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burne T. H. J., Rogers L. J. (2002). Chemosensory Input and Lateralization of Brain Function in the Domestic Chick. Behav. Brain Res. 133, 293–300. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00009-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardinal R. N., Pennicott D. R., Lakmali C., Sugathapala , Robbins T. W., Everitt B. J. (2001). Impulsive Choice Induced in Rats by Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core. Science 292, 2499–2501. doi:10.1126/science.1060818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandrashekar J., Hoon M. A., Ryba N. J. P., Zuker C. S. (2006). The Receptors and Cells for Mammalian Taste. Nature 444, 288–294. doi:10.1038/nature05401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheled-Shoval S. L., Druyan S., Uni Z. (2015). Bitter, Sweet and Umami Taste Receptors and Downstream Signaling Effectors: Expression in Embryonic and Growing Chicken Gastrointestinal Tract. Poult. Sci. 94, 1928–1941. doi:10.3382/ps/pev152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costalunga G., Kobylkov D., Rosa-Salva O., Vallortigara G., Mayer U. (2021). Light-Incubation Effects on Lateralisation of Single Unit Responses in the Visual Wulst of Domestic Chicks. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 497–513. doi:10.1007/s00429-021-02259-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csillag A. (1999). Striato-Telencephalic and Striato-Tegmental Circuits: Relevance to Learning in Domestic Chicks. Behav. Brain Res. 98, 227–236. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00088-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daisley J. N., Mascalzoni E., Rosa-Salva O., Rugani R., Regolin L. (2009). Lateralization of Social Cognition in the Domestic Chicken (Gallus gallus). Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 965–981. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis J. K., Lowman J. J., Thomas P. J., ten Hallers B. F. H., Koriabine M., Huynh L. Y., et al. (2010). Evolution of a Bitter Taste Receptor Gene Cluster in a New World Sparrow. Genome Biol. Evol. 2, 358–370. doi:10.1093/gbe/evq027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day J. J., Carelli R. M. (2007). The Nucleus Accumbens and Pavlovian Reward Learning. Neuroscientist 13, 148–159. doi:10.1177/1073858406295854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan P., Balleine B. W. (2002). Reward, Motivation, and Reinforcement Learning. Neuron 36, 285–298. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00963-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Araujo I. E. T., Kringelbach M. L., Rolls E. T., Hobden P. (2003). Representation of Umami Taste in the Human Brain. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 313–319. doi:10.1152/jn.00669.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganchrow J. R., Steiner J. E., Bartana A. (1990). Behavioral Reactions to Gustatory Stimuli in Young Chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Dev. Psychobiol. 23, 103–117. doi:10.1002/dev.420230202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentle M. J. (1975). Gustatory Hyposensitivity to Quinine Hydrochloride Following Diencephalic Lesions in Gallus domesticus. Physiol. Behav. 14, 265–270. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(75)90032-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golüke S., Bischof H.-J., Engelmann J., Caspers B. A., Mayer U. (2019). Social Odour Activates the Hippocampal Formation in Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Behav. Brain Res. 364, 41–49. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2019.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green B. G. (2021). From Receptors to the Brain: Psychophysical Clues to Taste Physiology. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 20, 154–158. doi:10.1016/j.cophys.2020.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu R. K., Zuo Y., Ly T., Wang J., Meera P., Wu Y. E., et al. (2021). An Amygdala-To-Hypothalamus Circuit for Social Reward. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 831–842. doi:10.1038/s41593-021-00828-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ichikawa Y., Izawa E.-I., Matsushima T. (2004). Excitotoxic Lesions of the Medial Striatum Delay Extinction of a Reinforcement Color Discrimination Operant Task in Domestic Chicks; A Functional Role of Reward Anticipation. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 76–83. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa E.-I., Zachar G., Aoki N., Koga K., Matsushima T. (2002). Lesions of the Ventro-Medial Basal Ganglia Impair the Reinforcement but Not the Recall of Memorized Color Discrimination in Domestic Chicks. Behav. Brain Res. 136, 405–414. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00179-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa E.-I., Zachar G., Yanagihara S., Matsushima T. (2003). Localized Lesion of Caudal Part of Lobus Parolfactorius Caused Impulsive Choice in the Domestic Chick: Evolutionarily Conserved Function of Ventral Striatum. J. Neurosci. 23, 1894–1902. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.23-05-01894.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang P., Josue J., Li X., Glaser D., Li W., Brand J. G., et al. (2012). Major Taste Loss in Carnivorous Mammals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 4956–4961. doi:10.1073/pnas.1118360109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones R. B., Roper T. J. (1997). Olfaction in the Domestic Fowl: A Critical Review. Physiol. Behav. 62, 1009–1018. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00207-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause E. T., Bischof H.-J., Engel K., Golüke S., Maraci Ö., Mayer U., et al. (2018). Olfaction in the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata): What Is Known and Further Perspectives. Adv. Study Behav. 50, 37–85. doi:10.1016/bs.asb.2017.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuenzel W. J., Masson M. (1988). A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Chick (Gallus domesticus). Available at: http://agris.fao.org/agris-search/search.do?recordID=US9000051.

Google Scholar

Li D., Zhang J. (2014). Diet Shapes the Evolution of the Vertebrate Bitter Taste Receptor Gene Repertoire. Mol. Biol. Evol. 31, 303–309. doi:10.1093/molbev/mst219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu H.-X., Rajapaksha P., Wang Z., Kramer N. E., Marshall B. J. (2018). An Update on the Sense of Taste in Chickens: A Better Developed System Than Previously Appreciated. J. Nutr. Food Sci. 08, 686. doi:10.4172/2155-9600.1000686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu Z., Liu G., Hailer F., Orozco-terWengel P., Tan X., Tian J., et al. (2016). Dietary Specialization Drives Multiple Independent Losses and Gains in the Bitter Taste Gene Repertoire of Laurasiatherian Mammals. Front. Zool. 13, 28. doi:10.1186/s12983-016-0161-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi E., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Morandi-Raikova A., Vallortigara G. (2019). Spontaneous and Light-Induced Lateralization of Immediate Early Genes Expression in Domestic Chicks. Behav. Brain Res. 368, 111905. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marples N. M., van Veelen W., Brakefield P. M. (1994). The Relative Importance of Colour, Taste and Smell in the Protection of an Aposematic Insect Coccinella Septempunctata. Anim. Behav. 48, 967–974. doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masic U., Yeomans M. R. (2014). Umami Flavor Enhances Appetite but Also Increases Satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 100, 532–538. doi:10.3945/ajcn.113.080929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsushima T., Izawa E.-I., Aoki N., Yanagihara S. (2003). The Mind Through Chick Eyes : Memory, Cognition and Anticipation. Zoolog. Sci. 20, 395–408. doi:10.2108/zsj.20.395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Loveland J. L., Vallortigara G. (2019). Selective Response of the Nucleus Taeniae of the Amygdala to a Naturalistic Social Stimulus in Visually Naive Domestic Chicks. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–10. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-46322-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morandi-Raikova A., Mayer U. (2020). The Effect of Monocular Occlusion on Hippocampal C-Fos Expression in Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus). Sci. Rep. 10, 1–13. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-64224-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicol C. J. (2004). Development, Direction, and Damage Limitation: Social Learning in Domestic Fowl. Learn. Behav. 32, 72–81. doi:10.3758/bf03196008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niknafs S., Roura E. (2018). Nutrient Sensing, Taste and Feed Intake in Avian Species. Nutr. Res. Rev. 31, 256–266. doi:10.1017/S0954422418000100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitschke J. B., Dixon G. E., Sarinopoulos I., Short S. J., Cohen J. D., Smith E. E., et al. (2006). Altering Expectancy Dampens Neural Response to Aversive Taste in Primary Taste Cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 435–442. doi:10.1038/nn1645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. (2011). The Vertebrate Mesolimbic Reward System and Social Behavior Network: A Comparative Synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599–3639. doi:10.1002/cne.22735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrales Parada C. D., Morandi-Raikova A., Rosa-Salva O., Mayer U. (2021). Neural Basis of Unfamiliar Conspecific Recognition in Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Behav. Brain Res. 397, 112927. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson T. A., Rose S. P. (1992). Memory in the Chick: Multiple Cues, Distinct Brain Locations. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 465–470. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.106.3.465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez E. C., Meurisse M., Hervé L., Georgelin M., Constantin P., Cornilleau F., et al. (2020). Object and Food Novelty Induce Distinct Patterns of C-Fos Immunoreactivity in Amygdala and Striatum in Domestic Male Chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Behav. Brain Res. 381, 112453. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Found. Stat. Comput. Available at: http://www.r-project.org.

Google Scholar

Ramírez-Lugo L., Núñez-Jaramillo L., Bermúdez-Rattoni F. (2007). Taste Memory Formation: Role of Nucleus Accumbens. Chem. Senses 32, 93–97. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjl023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramírez-Lugo L., Zavala-Vega S., Bermúdez-Rattoni F. (2006). NMDA and Muscarinic Receptors of the Nucleus Accumbens Have Differential Effects on Taste Memory Formation. Learn. Mem. 13, 45–51. doi:10.1101/lm.103206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiner A., Karten H. J. (1985). Comparison of Olfactory Bulb Projections in Pigeons and Turtles. Brain Behav. Evol. 27, 11–27. doi:10.1159/000118717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiner A., Perkel D. J., Bruce L. L., Butler A. B., Csillag A., Kuenzel W., et al. (2004). Revised Nomenclature for Avian Telencephalon and Some Related Brainstem Nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 473, 377–414. doi:10.1002/cne.20118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L., Andrew R. J., Burne T. H. J. (1998). Light Exposure of the Embryo and Development of Behavioural Lateralisation in Chicks, I: Olfactory Responses. Behav. Brain Res. 97, 195–200. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00043-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L. J., Bolden S. W. (1991). Light-Dependent Development and Asymmetry of Visual Projections. Neurosci. Lett. 121, 63–67. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90650-I

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L. J., Deng C. (1999). Light Experience and Lateralization of the Two Visual Pathways in the Chick. Behav. Brain Res. 98, 277–287. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00094-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L. J. (1982). Light Experience and Asymmetry of Brain Function in Chickens. Nature 297, 223–225. doi:10.1038/297223a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L. J. (1990). Light Input and the Reversal of Functional Lateralization in the Chicken Brain. Behav. Brain Res. 38, 211–221. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(90)90176-F

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers L. J., Sink H. S. (1988). Transient Asymmetry in the Projections of the Rostral Thalamus to the Visual Hyperstriatum of the Chicken, and Reversal of its Direction by Light Exposure. Exp. Brain Res. 70, 378–384. doi:10.1007/BF00248362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose S. P. R. (2000). God's Organism? the Chick as a Model System for Memory Studies. Learn. Mem. 7, 1–17. doi:10.1101/lm.7.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roura E., Baldwin M. W., Klasing K. C. (2013). The Avian Taste System: Potential Implications in Poultry Nutrition. Anim. Feed Sci. Technol. 180, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.anifeedsci.2012.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salva O. R., Daisley J. N., Regolin L., Vallortigara G. (2009). Lateralization of Social Learning in the Domestic Chick, Gallus gallus domesticus: Learning to Avoid. Anim. Behav. 78, 847–856. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider C. A., Rasband W. S., Eliceiri K. W. (2012). NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 Years of Image Analysis. Nat. Methods 9, 671–675. doi:10.1038/nmeth.2089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi P., Zhang J. (2006). Contrasting Modes of Evolution between Vertebrate Sweet/umami Receptor Genes and Bitter Receptor Genes. Mol. Biol. Evol. 23, 292–300. doi:10.1093/molbev/msj028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skelhorn J., Griksaitis D., Rowe C. (2008). Colour Biases Are More Than a Question of Taste. Anim. Behav. 75, 827–835. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skelhorn J., Rowe C. (2005a). Frequency-dependent Taste-Rejection by Avian Predation May Select for Defence Chemical Polymorphisms in Aposematic Prey. Biol. Lett. 1, 500–503. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skelhorn J., Rowe C. (2005b). Tasting the Difference: Do Multiple Defence Chemicals Interact in Müllerian Mimicry? Proc. R. Soc. B. 272, 339–345. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart M. G., Rusakov D. A. (1995). Morphological Changes Associated with Stages of Memory Formation in the Chick Following Passive Avoidance Training. Behav. Brain Res. 66, 21–28. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(94)00119-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toda Y., Ko M.-C., Liang Q., Miller E. T., Rico-Guevara A., Nakagita T., et al. (2021). Early Origin of Sweet Perception in the Songbird Radiation. Science 373, 226–231. doi:10.1126/science.abf6505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallortigara G., Andrew R. J. (1994). Olfactory Lateralization in the Chick. Neuropsychologia 32, 417–423. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(94)90087-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang K., Zhao H. (2015). Birds Generally Carry a Small Repertoire of Bitter Taste Receptor Genes. Genome Biol. Evol. 7, 2705–2715. doi:10.1093/gbe/evv180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassum K. M., Ostlund S. B., Maidment N. T., Balleine B. W. (2009). Distinct Opioid Circuits Determine the Palatability and the Desirability of Rewarding Events. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 12512–12517. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905874106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wild J. M., Arends J. J. A., Philip Zeigler H. (1990). Projections of the Parabrachial Nucleus in the Pigeon (Columba livia). J. Comp. Neurol. 293, 499–523. doi:10.1002/cne.902930402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu B., Eldeghaidy S., Ayed C., Fisk I. D., Hewson L., Liu Y. (2021). Mechanisms of Umami Taste Perception: From Molecular Level to Brain Imaging. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr.. doi:10.1080/10408398.2021.1909532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto K., Sun Z., Wang H. B., Reiner A. (2005). Subpallial Amygdala and Nucleus Taeniae in Birds Resemble Extended Amygdala and Medial Amygdala in Mammals in Their Expression of Markers of Regional Identity. Brain Res. Bull. 66, 341–347. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanagihara S., Izawa E.-I., Koga K., Matsushima T. (2001). Reward-Related Neuronal Activities in Basal Ganglia of Domestic Chicks. Neuroreport 12, 1431–1435. doi:10.1097/00001756-200105250-00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasoshima Y., Scott T. R., Yamamoto T. (2006). Memory-Dependent C-Fos Expression in the Nucleus Accumbens and Extended Amygdala Following the Expression of a Conditioned Taste Aversive in the Rat. Neuroscience 141, 35–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida Y., Kawabata F., Kawabata Y., Nishimura S., Tabata S. (2018). Short-Term Perception of and Conditioned Taste Aversion to Umami Taste, and Oral Expression Patterns of Umami Taste Receptors in Chickens. Physiol. Behav. 191, 29–36. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida Y., Kawabata Y., Kawabata F., Nishimura S., Tabata S. (2015). Expressions of Multiple Umami Taste Receptors in Oral and Gastrointestinal Tissues, and Umami Taste Synergism in Chickens. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 466, 346–349. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2015.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zald D., Lee J. T., Fluegel K. W., Pardo J. V. (1998). Aversive Gustatory Stimulation Activates Limbic Circuits in Humans. Brain 121, 1143–1154. doi:10.1093/brain/121.6.1143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao H., Yang J.-R., Xu H., Zhang J. (2010). Pseudogenization of the Umami Taste Receptor Gene Tas1r1 in the Giant Panda Coincided with its Dietary Switch to Bamboo. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27, 2669–2673. doi:10.1093/molbev/msq153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reward, bitter, taste perception, Immediate early genes (IEGs), accumbens (NAc), lateral septum (LS), c-Fos

Citation: Protti-Sánchez F, Corrales Parada CD, Mayer U and Rowland HM (2022) Activation of the Nucleus Taeniae of the Amygdala by Umami Taste in Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus). Front. Physiol. 13:897931. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.897931

Received: 16 March 2022; Accepted: 11 April 2022;
Published: 26 May 2022.

Edited by:

Toshiya Matsushima, Hokkaido University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Lesley J. Rogers, University of New England, Australia
Brian McCabe, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Protti-Sánchez, Corrales Parada, Mayer and Rowland. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francesca Protti-Sánchez, fprotti@ice.mpg.de

These authors share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.