Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 09 August 2013
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00037

What does the mediodorsal thalamus do?

  • Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, Oxford, UK

Dense amnesia can result from damage to the medial diencephalon in humans and in animals. In humans this damage is diffuse and can include the mediodorsal nuclei of the thalamus. In animal models, lesion studies have confirmed the mediodorsal thalamus (MD) has a role in memory and other cognitive tasks, although the extent of deficits is mixed. Anatomical tracing studies confirm at least three different subgroupings of the MD: medial, central, and lateral, each differentially interconnected to the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Moreover, these subgroupings of the MD also receive differing inputs from other brain structures, including the basal ganglia thus the MD subgroupings form key nodes in interconnected frontal-striatal-thalamic neural circuits, integrating critical information within the PFC. We will provide a review of data collected from non-human primates and rodents after selective brain injury to the whole of the MD as well as these subgroupings to highlight the extent of deficits in various cognitive tasks. This research highlights the neural basis of memory and cognitive deficits associated with the subgroupings of the MD and their interconnected neural networks. The evidence shows that the MD plays a critical role in many varied cognitive processes. In addition, the MD is actively processing information and integrating it across these neural circuits for successful cognition. Having established that the MD is critical for memory and cognition, further research is required to understand how the MD specifically influences these cognitive processing carried out by the brain.

Introduction

It is now more widely recognized that the episodic memory processes disrupted in anterograde amnesia involve interactions between the medial temporal lobes and the medial diencephalon (Aggleton and Brown, 1999). However, understanding how the medial thalamus contributes to memory and other cognitive functions has been much overlooked in cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. This mainly stems from the theoretical notion held over the past 50 years that the medial temporal lobes act exclusively as the brain's long-term declarative memory center. Despite this prevailing account, human patients can also suffer dense amnesia following damage in the medial diencephalon (diencephalic amnesia, thalamic amnesia). Brain damage that causes memory loss and other cognitive deficits in this region occurs after traumatic head injury, stroke, hemorrhage, thiamine deficiency, or chronic alcoholism (Korsakoff's syndrome). However, this brain damage is not circumscribed in clinical patients as many of the structures of the medial diencephalon (medial thalamus, mammillary bodies, and mammillothalamic tract) suffer combined damage due to their small size, close proximity to one another and fibers of passage coursing through the region. The medial thalamic structures most frequently identified as being critical for the memory deficits are anterior (AT), mediodorsal (MD), and the intralaminar (IL)/midline thalamic nuclei. The mammillary bodies and white matter fiber tracts, particularly the internal medullary lamina and the mammillothalamic tract, are also strongly implicated in human amnesic cases and animal models. Thus, the neural basis of the memory deficits associated with the medial diencephalon continues to be debated in the literature (Harding et al., 2000; Kopelman, 2002; Van der Werf et al., 2003a; Cipolotti et al., 2008; Aggleton et al., 2011; Carlesimo et al., 2011; Pergola et al., 2012; Vann, 2013).

Animal models of diencephalic amnesia are critical in helping to determine the structures that are important for memory and other cognitive processes as well as understanding the neural circuitry of this region. The emphasis of this review is on the experiments in animal models (monkeys and rodents mainly) that assess the role of the MD in memory and other cognitive processes. The review will show how this research can extend our understanding about the functions of the MD that when damaged cause some of the symptoms of the human amnesic syndrome. There is also a section on anatomy of the MD and its interconnections with other brain structures: detailing the communication within these regions is critical for understanding their overall functioning. It is important to remember that lesion studies do not show what the area of the brain that has been lesioned does, rather they show how the rest of the brain functions and compensates after brain injury to a particular region has occurred. Furthermore, we know that a single region of the brain does not act alone. Thus, the brain structures of the medial thalamus are interconnected with other brain structures, together forming integrated neural networks of cognition. The review concludes with an overview of some of the theories of MD involvement in cognition and memory, current perspectives and possible future directions to investigate.

It is an exciting time to be studying the medial thalamus and its role in cognitive processing as the work of many is challenging the long held beliefs that the thalamus is only passively relaying information from the basal ganglia, midbrain and brainstem onto the prefrontal cortex (PFC). For example, more recent neuroanatomical and neuromodulatory studies highlight how the thalamus is providing a critical role in integrating communication between the basal ganglia, thalamus, and cortex, which is challenging many long standing theoretical ideas related to the passive role of the thalamus (Haber and McFarland, 2001; Guillery and Sherman, 2002; Sanchez-Gonzalez et al., 2005; Sherman and Guillery, 2005, 2011; Sherman, 2007; Haber and Calzavara, 2009).

In addition, with advances in neuroimaging and its analyses, and different electrophysiology techniques that can help investigate functional and anatomical connectivity, the medial thalamus and specifically the MD has now been shown to influence many cognitive processes including memory, decision-making, and executive functions with comparative data across numerous species. The MD is also a critical structure linked to many neurological disorders (e.g., stroke, dementia, schizophrenia, major depressive disorder, Parkinson's disease, and Alzheimer's disease). Clearly, further research is needed on the MD to develop greater understanding of the neural mechanisms of its functioning and how it contributes to many neurological disorders.

Anatomy of the MD

Many of the structures in the brain can go by several names and this is the case with the MD, which is also referred to as medial dorsal thalamic nuclei, nucleus medialis dorsalis, and the dorsomedial thalamus. For the purposes of this review, the structure will be referred to as the mediodorsal thalamus (MD) and at some points will be distinguished by some of its subdivisions, that is, the magnocellular mediodorsal thalamus (MDmc) or medial MD, the parvocellular mediodorsal thalamus (MDpc) or central MD, and a lateral grouping that will include the densocellular (MDdc) and pars multiforms (MDmf) mediodorsal thalamic nuclei or lateral MD (MDl).

Cytoarchitecture

The MD is considered the largest of the nuclear structures in the medial thalamus, and it is most developed in primates, especially humans. The increase in the size of the MD in phylogenetic evolution parallels that of prefrontal, association and cingulate cortices (Bentivoglio et al., 1993; Jones, 1998). In rats, the MD is relatively heterogeneous with four main subdivisions identified (see Figure 1). These are the medial, central, lateral, and paralamellar segments (Krettek and Price, 1977; Groenewegen, 1988). The boundaries of each segment are somewhat well defined, especially between the central and lateral segments. The dendrites of the cells in each of these two segments tend to be confined to their respective regions and the lateral segment stains more heavily for acetylcholinesterase (Price, 1995). In primates, the four subdivisions are more easily recognizable (see Figure 2): a magnocellular subdivision (MDmc) occupies the most medial and rostral part of the MD and is considered equivalent to the medial segment in rats. The parvocellular (MDpc) subdivision is located within the central part of MD throughout the rostrocaudal extent. The other two subdivisions, densocellular (MDdc) and pars multiforms (MDmf) are located in the lateral part of MD with the MDmf situated in the rostral part and MDdc situated in the caudal part of the MD (Jones, 1985; Bentivoglio et al., 1993; Bachevalier et al., 1997).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic diagram (and enlargement) of the medial aspects (Bregma—2.56 mm) of the medial thalamus in the rodent brain. Abbreviations: CeM, center median nucleus, part of the midline nuclei; CL, centrolateral nucleus, part of the intralaminar nuclei; IMD, intermediodorsal nucleus, part of the midline nuclei; LD, laterodorsal nucleus; MDc, central subdivision of mediodorsal thalamus; MDl, lateral subdivision of mediodorsal thalamus; MDm, medial subdivision of mediodorsal thalamus; MDpl, paralamellar subdivision of the mediodorsal thalamus; PC, paracentral nucleus, part of the intralaminar nuclei; PV, paraventricular nucleus, part of the midline nuclei; Re, reuniens. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1998).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic diagrams of some of the coronal sections located approximately IA+9.7, +7.9, +6.6, and +5.0 through the rostrocaudal extent of the medial thalamus in the non-human primate brain. Abbreviations: CM, centromedian nucleus; Fx, fornix; Pul, pulvinar; Sm, stria medullaris. Adapted from the atlas of Olszewski (1952).

Neural Connections of the Mediodorsal Thalamus

Prefrontal cortex afferents and efferents

In rodents and non-human primates, there are substantial reciprocal interconnections between the PFC and the MD (Krettek and Price, 1977; Goldman-Rakic and Porrino, 1985; Groenewegen, 1988; Ray and Price, 1993; McFarland and Haber, 2002; Xiao et al., 2009). Higher order thalamic structures, like the MD (and the pulvinar) receive inputs from different cortical layers. The majority of projection neurons to the MD originate from layer VI and V (Giguere and Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Yeterian and Pandya, 1994; Xiao et al., 2009); mainly from within the deep regions of these layers. The cortical layer V pyramidal neurons also have branches of long descending axons going to motor centers (Guillery, 1995; Guillery and Sherman, 2002). Guillery also proposed that these higher order thalamic nuclei play a key role in cortico-cortical communication and higher cortical functioning (Guillery, 1995). Thalamic neurons innervated by cortical layer VI project focally to the middle cortical layers and thalamic neurons innervated by cortical layer V project widely to the superficial cortical layers which are involved in cortico-cortical communications (Jones, 1985; Xiao et al., 2009). In addition, there are nonreciprocal components to the thalamo-cortical links, indicating a dual role for the MD in integrating basal ganglia outputs within specific cortical circuits (see below) (McFarland and Haber, 2002; Haber and Calzavara, 2009), as well as mediating information flow between cortico-cortical structures via this transthalamic route (Guillery and Sherman, 2002; Sherman, 2005, 2007; Sherman and Guillery, 2005). Glutamate is the main neurotransmitter of communication between thalamus and cortex (Sherman, 2013).

The major outputs of the MD are to the medial and lateral prefrontal and orbital frontal (OFC) cortices, and in some neuroanatomical tracing studies in rats, the medial PFC is said to be defined by the projections received from the MD nucleus (Groenewegen, 1988; Negyessy et al., 1998). Thus, these interconnections between the MD and PFC are segregated based on the subdivisions within the MD (see Figures 3A–C). The MDmc-PFC projections are almost exclusively reciprocal between the MDmc and the OFC and ventromedial PFC (vmPFC: areas 14, 25, 11, 13, and 12) but there is also a nonreciprocal input from ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC: area 45) and medial PFC (dACC: area 32 from the ventral and caudal aspects) (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Russchen et al., 1987; Barbas et al., 1991; Bachevalier et al., 1997; McFarland and Haber, 2002). Some of the midline nuclei [e.g., the intermediodorsal (IMD) and the paraventricular (PV) nucleus in rodents, see Figure 1] are also reciprocally connected to the OFC (Groenewegen, 1988). Thus, the MDmc and these midline nuclei have been regarded as a neuroanatomically functioning unit in rodents (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005). The MDpc has reciprocal connections with the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC; areas 9 and 46) and area 10. There is also non-reciprocal inputs to MDpc from OFC (area 12, 13), VLPFC and the dACC (supracallosal area 24 and from the dorsal and rostral aspects of precallosal area 32 and 14) (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Russchen et al., 1987; Barbas et al., 1991; Bachevalier et al., 1997; Haber and McFarland, 2001; Erickson and Lewis, 2004). The most lateral parts of the MD that are combined with the ILn diffusely project to the PFC and dACC (supracallosal area 24) and exclusively to the frontal eye fields (FEF); the most prominent projection however is the topographically organized input to the basal ganglia (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Barbas et al., 1991; Bachevalier et al., 1997; Erickson and Lewis, 2004; Erickson et al., 2004).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Schematic illustrations of the main connections of the (A) MDmc, (B) MDpc and (C) MDl in the brain. Abbreviations are provided in the text.

Medial temporal lobes (MTL) afferents and efferents

In non-human primates there are connections from the association cortex of the temporal lobes [i.e., the entorhinal (ERh) and perirhinal (PRh) cortices] to the MDmc (see Figure 3A) and midline thalamic nuclei (Aggleton et al., 1986; Russchen et al., 1987; Saunders et al., 2005). These mainly course through the ventroamygdalofugal pathway and the inferior thalamic peduncle. In the rodent brain, it appears that only the rostral portion of area 35 of the perirhinal cortex projects to the MD (Burwell et al., 1995).

There are also amygdala (Amyg) projections to the MD (see Figure 3A). The central nucleus and basolateral nuclei project densely to the MDmc in monkeys and medial segment of MD in non-primates (Aggleton and Mishkin, 1984; Groenewegen et al., 1990; Krettek and Price, 1977). These projections from the amygdala to the medial MD are much sparser than the amygdala projections to the striatum and PFC (Jones, 1985). Krettek and Price (1977) reported that the fibers from the caudal part of amygdala terminate rostrally in medial MD and those from the rostral part terminate more caudally and ventrally in the medial MD. In turn, it has been documented in rodents that medial parts of MD project back to the basal grouping and anterior cortical nuclei of the amygdala, while the midline nuclei project to the central nucleus and the rostral part of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (Groenewegen et al., 1990).

In contrast, the central MDpc and more lateral parts of the MD (see Figures 3B,C) do not directly interact with the MTL and amygdala.

Subcortical Afferents and Efferents

Some of the thalamic projections to the PFC represent in many instances the final link in fronto-striatal-thalamic circuits (Alexander et al., 1986; Groenewegen et al., 1990, 1999a; Haber and McFarland, 2001; Haber and Calzavara, 2009; Haber and Knutson, 2010). There are no neuroanatomical tracing studies that document direct projections from the caudate-putamen (C-Pu) to the medial thalamus in rats or in primates. Instead the C-Pu projects to the output structures of the pallidum via either direct or indirect pathways and then onto the medial and ventral thalamus. The direct pathway comprises of striatal projections to the internal segment of the globus pallidus (GP) and the reticular part of the substantia nigra (SNr: located in the midbrain) then to the thalamus. The indirect pathway comprises of striatal projections to the external segment of the GP, then to the subthalamic nucleus (STN), which in turn project to the internal segment of the GP and the SNr, and then to the thalamus1 (Haber et al., 1985; Groenewegen et al., 1990, 1993, 1997, 1999a,b; Tekin and Cummings, 2002; Haber and Calzavara, 2009). The internal segment of the GP projects predominantly to MDpc and MDl, while the ventral pallidum (VP) projects densely to MDmc. The most lateral subdivisions of the MD and many midline nuclei project to the basal ganglia (Berendse and Groenewegen, 1990; Groenewegen et al., 1990, 1999a; Gimenez-Amaya et al., 1995; Haber and McFarland, 2001; Haber and Calzavara, 2009).

From the brainstem in the rat, the locus coeruleus projects to all segments of the MD (Groenewegen, 1988). The median raphe projects most heavily to the MDl, whereas the dorsal raphe is strongly connected with the MDmc (Groenewegen, 1988).

The MDpc and MDl receive non-dopaminergic projections from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and SNr (Groenewegen et al., 1990). The MDmc also receive projections from the VTA and SNr, but these projections are dopaminergic (Groenewegen, 1988). In addition, the reticular formation projects to all segments of the MD (Groenewegen, 1988).

A significant amount of thalamic neuromodulatory input is also received from the basal forebrain. Amongst many studies it is reported that the largest amount of basal forebrain inputs reaching the medial thalamus terminate in the reticular nucleus, with moderate terminal fields in the MDmc and sparse terminals in other sites (Hallanger et al., 1987; Groenewegen, 1988). These basal forebrain projections to the MD are predominantly GABAergic, while brainstem projections provide cholinergic inputs (Hallanger et al., 1987). For example, in cats, only 7–20% of basal forebrain neurons projecting to the MD are cholinergic (Bentivoglio et al., 1993).

Based on these differences in cortical-subcortical connectivity patterns among the MD, PFC, MTL, and basal ganglia, at least three separate MD thalamic neural circuits can be identified: a medial subdivision, including some of the midline nuclei and the MDmc, reciprocally connected to the OFC and vmPFC with further inputs from the VLPFC, rhinal cortex, amygdala, VS, and VP (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005); a central subdivision, the MDpc, reciprocally connected to the DLPFC and area 10 with further inputs from the OFC, dACC, dorsal striatum, and GP; and a lateral subdivision (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005; Lopez et al., 2009) including the intralaminar nuclei that is also interconnected to the dorsal striatum, GP and more diffusely to the PFC and FEF.

Animal Lesion Studies

Studying memory and cognition with animal models is extremely insightful, in addition to being a useful way to overcome some of the limitations that are inherent in the clinical evidence. There are many advantages to developing animal models of memory processing. Surgical lesions in animals can normally be somewhat more circumscribed and involve subtotal, complete or even contra-lateral neuronal damage to connected structures. These planned lesions, if produced with a high degree of selectivity to the target structures of interest, can encourage a greater certainty about identifying the critical locus and also the particular kinds of memory deficits than are evident in comparative human cases. In addition, direct comparisons are possible between control and lesion animals, within pre- vs. post-operative testing or between subtotal lesions to one structure vs. another nearby structure.

Despite the benefits of experimental thalamic lesions, animal studies have, like the clinical evidence, also encountered difficulties and produced conflicting findings. This has resulted from the use of different techniques to create lesions in the MD, differences in the size and location of these lesions, and the extent of atrophy to surrounding target structures due to the inherent complexity of the medial and “non-specific” regions of the thalamus. Fortunately, the extent of brain damage in the medial thalamus has been minimized more recently by using neurotoxins that produce selective lesions to the individual structures that make up the medial diencephalon in animals. Thus, recent studies in rodents and non-human primates with very selective lesions to the mediodorsal thalamus using neurotoxins have been most insightful (Chudasama et al., 2001; Corbit et al., 2003; Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005; Gibb et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 2007a,b, 2008; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008; Ostlund and Balleine, 2008; Pickens, 2008; Wolff et al., 2008; Lopez et al., 2009; Cross et al., 2012; Moreau et al., 2013).

Standardization of memory tasks and testing procedures for animals has also met with difficulties. Interpreting findings across studies and species can be problematic. Nevertheless, it is widely accepted that some cognitive tests provide adequate measures of animal memory that are analogous to human episodic recall tasks (Aggleton and Pearce, 2001). In addition, over the past few years, memory research has linked the work done in rats with the work of humans and non-human primates to a greater extent (Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Aggleton et al., 2000; Aggleton and Pearce, 2001; Morris, 2001; Uylings et al., 2003).

Experimental Mediodorsal Thalamus Lesions

Earlier work in animals focused on determining the one critical structure within the medial thalamus that was causing the memory deficits associated with thalamic amnesia. As mentioned, there are many candidates within the medial thalamus to fulfill this critical role. Neuropathological evidence reported in clinical cases of Wernicke-Korsakoff's syndrome supported a role for the MD in memory (Victor et al., 1971; Kopelman, 1995). However, Wernicke-Korsakoff's patients invariably suffer extensive neural damage due to the widespread effects of alcohol in the brain (Kril and Halliday, 1999), thus less equivocal evidence can only be obtained from experimental lesion studies involving circumscribed damage conducted in animal models. Table 1 details what we believe to be the extent of the experiments that have investigated cognitive and memory impairments after MD lesions over the past 40 years in rodents and monkeys collected using searches on pubmed involving mediodorsal, medial dorsal, dorsomedial, dorsalis medialis, and thalam*. Some of these studies and their conclusions are discussed below.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies involving MD thalamic lesions assessing performance in an array of memory tasks over the past 40 years.

Non-human primates

Monkey studies have demonstrated that aspiration lesions to the MD (i.e., typically including the magnocellular and the parvocellular subdivisions and other medial thalamic structures as well as potential fibers of passage passing through this region) cause impairments in recognition memory, deficits in new learning of object-in-place (OIP) discriminations and object-reward associations. These lesions also produce impaired performance in the spatial delayed alternation task and delayed response task but not in object reversal (associative memory task) and visual pattern discrimination (Isseroff et al., 1982; Aggleton and Mishkin, 1983a,b; Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985; Gaffan and Murray, 1990; Gaffan and Watkins, 1991; Parker et al., 1997; Gaffan and Parker, 2000). Other studies have also highlighted how interactions between interconnected structures of the amygdala, vmPFC and MD are important for postoperative new learning of two-choice visual discriminations associated with differing amounts of food reward (Gaffan et al., 1993). Despite this extensive range of deficits linked to damage in the MD, it did not appear that MD lesions by themselves were the critical source of dense amnesia linked to cases of thalamic and diencephalic amnesia suffered in patients. For example, Parker et al. (1997) found that bilateral ablations to MDmc did not produce recognition memory deficits as severe as those reported after bilateral perirhinal cortex ablations, and the animals were also not as markedly impaired as amnesic patients with recognition memory deficits (Aggleton and Shaw, 1996).

Parker and Gaffan (1998) proposed that ablation of the MDmc in primates produces hypoactivity in the PFC and, therefore, the deficits in cognitive testing after MD lesions might be ascribed to frontal dysfunction. Given the extent of dense reciprocal connections between the MD and PFC, it makes sense to propose that damage to the MD may result in dysfunction within the PFC and that this disruption causes deficits in cognition and memory (Isseroff et al., 1982). The PFC is associated with higher order cognitive functioning, often labeled “executive functioning” in humans. It has been suggested that lesions to the MD could disrupt pathways leading to the PFC and may affect processes that are typically governed by the PFC, including attention, inhibition, planning, coordination, and strategy selection, which could then produce memory impairments on tasks (Gaffan and Parker, 2000). Certainly all of the above tasks that produce deficits after MD lesions are also sensitive to damage in the PFC (Fuster, 2008; Chudasama, 2011).

More recently, selective neurotoxic lesions to the MDmc have confirmed the importance of this medial subdivision in new learning of OIP discriminations and in a reward satiety devaluation task, as neurotoxic lesions of the MDmc produce impaired performance on these tasks (Mitchell et al., 2007a,b, 2008; Izquierdo and Murray, 2010). However, the same selective lesions to MDmc do not impair the retention of pre-operatively acquired information (Mitchell et al., 2007a; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). One such task that assesses retention of pre-operative information is the strategy implementation task (Gaffan et al., 2002). In this task, animals learn a specific strategy for responding to objects presented on a touchscreen in order to receive reward. Pre-operative performance for individual animals is compared with post-operative performance. Animals with crossed unilateral lesions that disconnect the whole of PFC in one hemisphere from inferotemporal cortex in the contralateral hemisphere (PFC × IT) cause impairments on this task (Gaffan et al., 2002) as do bilateral ablations to the VLPFC (Baxter et al., 2009). However, damage to the MDmc did not disrupt the animals' ability to implement the strategy that they had acquired pre-operatively (Mitchell et al., 2007a), despite the strong reciprocal interconnections with PFC and input from IT.

In the same study, another task, the OIP discrimination learning task, was also learnt pre-operatively although in this task animals learn 20 new pairs of OIP discriminations (see Figure 4 for example stimuli of the OIP discriminations) during each session across eight concurrent repetitions of the set of 20 pairs of OIP discriminations. To assess impairments in this task, the performance of each animal is compared during a pre-operative test of 10 sessions and compared, after recovery from neurosurgery, to a post-operative test of 10 sessions. The neurotoxic MDmc lesions caused animals to make more errors during postoperative testing, so it could be concluded that deficits were linked to new learning of information as opposed to the retention of specific information acquired pre-operatively (Mitchell et al., 2007a). Critically though, this evidence demonstrated that the damage to the MDmc was not simply causing widespread PFC dysfunction to account for the observed cognitive deficits. Furthermore, the extent of the deficits on the OIP task were similar across two different lesion studies provided convincing evidence that the neurotoxin lesion technique used by Mitchell et al. (2007a) worked as effectively as the previously used ablation method (Gaffan and Parker, 2000).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Two examples of object-in-place (OIP) discrimination problems. Each discrimination problem had two different “objects” (one rewarded and one non-rewarded) embedded within a unique colored and patterned background akin to a “scene”; the objects are the differently colored typographic characters “B” and “m” in the left panel and “J” and “h” in the right panel.

In addition, the types of errors made in learning the OIP discriminations produced after bilateral MDmc lesions are not suggestive of problems with perseverative responding during learning (Mitchell et al., 2007a). However, bilateral ablations to the VLPFC do produce perseverative responding during new learning of OIP discriminations (Baxter et al., 2008). This evidence further confirms that damage to the MDmc does not simply produce a generalized impairment in memory by causing dysfunction of prefrontal functioning as had been previously proposed.

However, other studies (e.g., Parker et al., 1997) have shown that damage to the MD does not impair new learning or retention on recognition memory tasks when the stimulus set size is small. So it could also be argued that as the stimulus set size (four pairs of objects) is small in the strategy implementation task, no further learning is occurring. This theory was further tested (Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008) by comparing retrograde amnesia and anterograde amnesia within the same animals using the same types of OIP discrimination stimuli for both types of memory (see Figure 4) from a large sample size of 400 pairs of discriminations. In addition, a one-trial retention test was used to assess memory retention; this test is a pure measure of postoperative retention, uncontaminated by post-operative re-learning (Dean and Weiskrantz, 1974). Interestingly, damage to the MDmc using neurotoxins caused no impairment in the one-trial postoperative retention test. That is, the monkeys with bilateral MDmc neurotoxic lesions showed good retention (i.e., no retrograde amnesia) of the 300 pairs of OIP discriminations that they had acquired pre-operatively, when the errors made during their pre-operative retention test were compared with errors made during their post-operative retention test (Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). In contrast, the same animals were markedly impaired in new postoperative learning (anterograde amnesia) of a further set of 100 novel pairs of OIP discriminations presented concurrently across sessions (Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). It was concluded from this evidence that the MDmc is critical for the processing of new information more so than in the retention of information acquired prior to damage.

Further research from our laboratory has extended our understanding about some of the brain regions involved in retrograde amnesia and anterograde amnesia using this OIP retention and new learning task. The effects of lesions to different subcortical and cortical structures have been assessed (Mitchell et al., 2008; Mitchell and Buckley, submitted). One such study assessed the effects of combining neurotoxic MDmc lesions with bilateral fornix transection [a lesion that produces more widespread brain damage to medial diencephalic structures as well as disrupting interconnections with the PFC and medial temporal lobes (Mitchell et al., 2008)]. Interestingly these combined lesions produced dense amnesia for new learning as well as retention, yet still they confirmed that subcortical damage produces more severe deficits in anterograde amnesia than in retrograde amnesia.

Clearly animals with bilateral MDmc lesions have provided greater understanding of the critical role that the MDmc plays in differing forms of memory processing. Animals with MDmc lesions have also been assessed on tasks investigating other cognitive processes e.g., reward (satiety) devaluation. For example, animals with selective bilateral neurotoxic lesions to the MDmc are also impaired on a computerized version of a classic food satiety devaluation task (Malkova et al., 1997) demonstrating the importance of the MDmc within the neural circuit crucial for reward devaluation (Mitchell et al., 2007b) that also includes the OFC and amygdala (Malkova et al., 1997; Baxter et al., 2000; Izquierdo and Murray, 2010). Interestingly though, lesions to MDmc did not impair the animals' ability to learn the 60 pairs of object-reward associations presented concurrently over successive sessions during initial postoperative acquisition training before performing the reward satiety devaluation testing in this task. In this reward devaluation paradigm, the monkeys must first learn postoperatively to link one of the two pairs of objects in each presentation with a peanut or chocolate candy reward, 50% of one of the stimuli from the pairs of objects was rewarded for a correct choice with a peanut (while the other object was not associated with any food) and the other 50% of the pairs of objects with a chocolate candy. Presumably this lack of deficit in new learning during the initial acquisition was linked to the smaller set size of stimuli (Parker et al., 1997) as well as to the salience of the rewards [e.g., bilateral lesions to the OFC and the amygdala also do not impair the initial postoperative acquisition of the object-reward associations in this task (Malkova et al., 1997; Baxter et al., 2000)]. Other studies though have shown that bilateral MD lesions do impair the ability of the animals to associate pairs of stimuli with differing amounts of food rewards (Gaffan and Watkins, 1991).

In contrast, MD lesions do produce deficits in new learning of larger sized samples of concurrent object-reward association problems over sessions, although the lesion does not impair the retention of pre-operatively acquired object-reward associations (Gaffan and Parker, 2000). Damage to parts of the PFC (e.g., crossed unilateral disconnection of PFC × IT lesions) does not impair concurrent learning of visual object discriminations, where the animal learns to associate visually presented objects with food (or no) reward and the presentation of each object pair is separated in time by presentation of other pairs of objects (Gaffan et al., 2002). Interestingly, this same PFC × IT disconnection lesion does, however, severely impair serial learning of visual discriminations, where the animal learns to associate single pairs of objects with food (or no) reward and the presentation of each object pair occurs immediately after one another (Browning et al., 2007). Concurrent object-reward association learning is qualitatively different to learning serial presentations of visual discriminations (Murray and Gaffan, 2006). It remains to be fully tested whether bilateral MD lesions produce dissociable deficits in learning concurrent vs. serial visual discriminations, although as previously shown MD lesions produce deficits in within-session new learning of OIP discriminations (Gaffan and Parker, 2000; Mitchell et al., 2007a).

Rodents

In rats, many studies assess the rats' ability to forage for food using T-mazes, water mazes and radial arm mazes, taking advantage of their natural curiosity to explore novel environments for food. Many strategies can be used by the animals to complete these tasks successfully (Dudchenko, 2001). One strategy involves spatial navigation based on the use of extra maze cues (e.g., door, windows, lights, posters, experimenter, etc.) within the testing environment (spatial cues) to help guide their optimal exploration and ensure they do not return to the same location twice. Animals with selective neurotoxic lesions to the MD show comparable performance to control animals when they use spatial cues to guide their navigation in radial arm maze tasks (Beracochea et al., 1989; Hunt and Aggleton, 1998a; Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005), unless they incorporate a delay (Harrison and Mair, 1996; Floresco et al., 1999) or produce more widespread damage that also includes the AT (Stokes and Best, 1988, 1990a,b; Hunt and Aggleton, 1991, 1998a). It has been proposed that MD deficits in delay tasks are presumably a consequence of widespread disruption to PFC functioning (Hunt and Aggleton, 1998b). Floresco et al. (1999) contrasted working memory performance using a spatial delayed responding task and non-delayed spatial tasks to show that the interaction between the PFC and the MD mediates “context-dependent retrieval and manipulation of recently acquired information.” Furthermore, this study provided evidence, via lidocaine infusions into the MD, to show that MD alone is not sufficient to affect episodic-like memory processing on spatial memory tasks (Floresco et al., 1999). Instead, it is widely accepted that spatial memory processing deficits related to the medial thalamus are governed by the anterior thalamic nuclei and their interconnections to the extended hippocampal system, also known as the Delay-Bryon neural circuit (Aggleton and Brown, 1999).

Other researchers have observed in rats with bilateral MD lesions certain behavioral deficits that could result in memory impairments, for example, an inability to adopt different strategies, or changes in activity and exploration levels or deficits in withholding spatial responses (Hunt and Aggleton, 1998a,b; Floresco et al., 1999; Block et al., 2007; Ostlund and Balleine, 2008). All of these types of deficits are also observed in rats with damage to regions of the PFC (Chudasama, 2011).

Rodent studies have been instrumental in demonstrating the distinct, interdependent involvement of adjacent medial thalamic structures in memory and other cognitive deficits. Dissociable deficits between the MD and adjacent anterior thalamus (AT) have been reported (Chudasama and Muir, 2001; Chudasama et al., 2001; Corbit et al., 2003; Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005, 2006). Corbit et al. (2003) assessed the effects of highly selective MD and AT lesions in rats on instrumental conditioning. Rats with either MD or AT lesions were both able to acquire the instrumental performance but during the degradation of the action-outcome contingency test, the rats with MD lesion were unable to demonstrate reliable devaluation effects. This deficit shown by the rats with MD lesions was distinct from the rats with AT lesions and controls, which did not differ, and suggests that the MD contributes to deficits in encoding and/or utilizing the action-outcome association (Corbit et al., 2003). Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford (2005) have also demonstrated dissociable impairments in rats with lesions of the medial MD compared to the lateral MD or to the AT on various cognitive tests. The damage to the medial MD impairs go/no-go reward value discriminations and recency memory with a 2-h delay, but had no impact on spatial memory processing using an 8-arm radial maze or spontaneous object recognition (SOR) memory (see below). Lateral MD lesions produced mild deficits in 8-arm radial maze performance and recency memory but had no impact on go/no-go reward value discriminations or SOR memory (see below). In contrast, AT lesions produced deficits on 8-arm radial maze performance but they had no effect on recency memory using a 2-h delay. Interestingly, AT lesions do impair the ability to remember the pseudorandom order of six odors (Wolff et al., 2006). This deficit may be linked to the nature of the associative memory processing involving reward that is required in this particular task but that is not present in the spontaneous exploration paradigm used in the recency memory task (see below). Further dissociations in performance have been reported in rats with lesions to the lateral MD or to the AT, with only damage to the lateral MD impairing the rats' memory in a delayed-match-to-sample task using an egocentric (body-turn) response in a cross-maze; damage to the AT left performance intact (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2006). Further studies from the same laboratory have provided more insight into the dissociable effects of lesions to the AT and lateral MD combined with ILN lesions in learning and memory processing (Gibb et al., 2006; Wolff et al., 2008; Lopez et al., 2009; Moreau et al., 2013). These authors concluded from this and the above evidence that no single medial thalamic structure is critical for all of the memory and other cognitive deficits associated with thalamic amnesia. Instead many subdivisions of medial thalamic nuclei are contributing to independent neural networks via subcortical and cortical interactions and are integrating information for successful cognition (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005, 2006). Other research (e.g., Eleore et al., 2011) has also documented similar roles for other thalamic nuclei, namely the reuniens, in supporting the acquisition of associative learning using a classical eyeblink conditioning task with a trace paradigm, because high frequency train stimulation directed at the reuniens in behaving mice prevented the proper acquisition of the task.

The experiment demonstrating bilateral MD involvement in recency memory (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005) has been further confirmed by Cross et al. (2012). The medial PFC (mPFC) is involved in recency memory processing in rodents (Mitchell and Laiacona, 1998; Hannesson et al., 2004; Cross et al., 2012) and patients with Korsakoff's syndrome and frontal lobe damage have problems with temporal processing and recency discriminations (Kopelman et al., 1997; Kopelman, 2002; Fuster, 2008).

Cross et al. (2012) have demonstrated in rodents the importance of neural communication in MD-mPFC circuitry for successful recency recognition memory. After combining crossed unilateral lesions of the MD and mPFC, a lesion that disconnects the structures in both hemispheres, animals were impaired in the recency recognition memory task. In contrast, after a combined ipsilateral unilateral lesion of the MD and mPFC (essentially a control lesion of these two structures that leaves one hemisphere functioning) recency recognition memory performance was left intact. The authors proposed that “during associative or recency recognition memory tasks, the MD–mPFC connection might be necessary to direct ongoing behavior toward, for example, the novel object–place configuration” (Cross et al., 2012). This study highlights how the interplay of communication within and between MD-mPFC networks is clearly critical for cognition.

After neurotoxic lesions to MD, rodents are not impaired at SOR tasks (Hunt and Aggleton, 1998a; Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005; Cross et al., 2012). As already noted above, conflicting evidence exists for the role of MD in various recognition memory tests. Several studies have reported deficits in object recognition using rats involving delay non-matching-to-sample tasks that involve object-reward associations (Hunt and Aggleton, 1991; Mumby et al., 1993), while others have reported no deficits on various unrewarded recognition memory tasks that rely on spontaneous exploration instead (M'Harzi et al., 1991; Hunt and Aggleton, 1998b; Kornecook et al., 1999; Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005). The most parsimonious explanation is that the task demands related to reward are different, as SOR does not involve reward but rather relies on spontaneous exploration while delayed-matching or non-matching to sample tasks normally reward the animal for a correct response, thus engaging associative memory networks instead (Parker et al., 1997; Gaffan and Parker, 2000). In addition, it is now known that pre-operative training is a critical factor in learning and memory tasks, as damage to the MD does not impair retention of pre-operatively acquired information associating objects and rewards (Gaffan and Parker, 2000; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). Also, there may be cross species differences in neuroanatomy. That is, in macaques there is a distinct projection from the rhinal cortices (perirhinal and entorhinal) to the MDmc (Aggleton et al., 1986; Saunders et al., 2005), while a similar projection is not as robust in rodents (Burwell et al., 1995).

As in monkey studies, researchers have investigated the devaluation effects after bilateral MD lesions in rodents. Pickens (2008) has systematically assessed rats with MD lesions on many variants of devaluation testing using Pavlovian and operant contingencies, and single and multiple reinforcement paradigms. Pickens concluded from this series of experiments, that the MD is important in devaluation circuits only “in cases in which previous associations need to be suppressed in order for new associations to be learned and control behavior, otherwise the devaluation circuit does not require MD” (Pickens, 2008).

Thus, through experimental testing in both rats and non-human primates it has been shown that the different subdivisions of the MD provide critical contributions to successful cognitive processing in many different tasks. Principally, the MD in conjunction with its neuroanatomical connections is important for some forms of recognition memory, recency memory processing, and further prospective integration of the rewards associated with successful responses to govern additional responses, as well as new learning of OIP discriminations, but not their retention. The subdivisions of the MD provide key roles in helping integrate object/reward/response information for successful new learning and successful additional (future) responding. Furthermore, and most importantly, it has been demonstrated that the MD contributes to successful cognition, rather than causing memory and other cognitive deficits by simply causing a generalized dysfunction of the PFC.

Electrophysiology

A recent review of single unit recordings in macaques (Watanabe and Funahashi, 2012) provides insight into how the MD contributes to successful performance during working memory (delayed oculomotor response) tasks. The review highlights clear interplay between the MD and PFC, as suggested by other studies. For example, neurons in the MD have shown cue-, delay- and response-period activity, similar to the discharge patterns observed in DLPFC, although most neurons exhibited sustained excitatory response during the delay period (Tanibuchi and Goldman-Rakic, 2005; Sommer and Wurtz, 2006; Watanabe and Funahashi, 2012). One study (Alexander and Fuster, 1973) in particular showed attenuation in magnitude of the delay-period response following cooling of the DLPFC suggesting that the projection neurons of PFC control task-related activity of the MD.

Further experiments have shown that the MD seems to contribute to prospective encoding more so than DLPFC during the delay period (Funahashi et al., 2004; Watanabe and Funahashi, 2012). Watanabe and Funahashi (2012) have proposed that the MD is the major area that provides information regarding impending behavior to the DLPFC. In contrast, retrospective sensory information is maintained during the delay period in the DLPFC and this could play an important role in helping to generate prospective motor information (Watanabe and Funahashi, 2012). The response-period active neurons were more frequent in MD than in DLPFC reflecting a bias toward processing motor aspects of the task by these thalamic nuclei, confirmed further by population vector analyses (Watanabe and Funahashi, 2012).

Other electrophysiology studies have shown that the MDmc of primates contain neuronal populations that signal information concerning prior stimulus occurrence (Fahy et al., 1993), that is linked to interconnected regions of the medial PFC and the perirhinal cortex (Brown and Xiang, 1998; Xiang and Brown, 2004), although the role of the MD within this neural circuitry is still uncertain.

Finally, another study has used single unit recording to demonstrate how the PFC and MD interact in cognitive tasks. Recent work by Kellendonk and colleagues (Parnaudeau et al., 2013) using a mouse model of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia has shown a subtle decrease in MD activity to disrupt the thalamic-PFC neural circuitry and cognition. They recorded single units in MD neurons during choice phase vs. reward phase of the T-maze task and demonstrated decreased MD activity interfered with task-dependent modulation of MD-PFC synchrony, which correlated with the cognitive deficits of the mice.

Theories About MD Involvement in Memory Processing

Aggleton and Brown (1999) suggested that the MD, and the perirhinal cortex of the medial temporal lobe, may play a role in a system responsible for familiarity-based recognition processes. However, this proposal remains debated because the direct neural connections between the MD and perirhinal cortex are sparse and clinical evidence (Pergola et al., 2012) and animal lesion evidence (as detailed above) remains equivocal. While it is widely accepted that the perirhinal cortex contributes to recognition memory, the contribution attributed to the MD remains uncertain. The evidence in the clinical cases of deficits in recognition memory following damage in the MD is mixed (Cipolotti et al., 2008) with some researchers reporting no such impairments (Shuren et al., 1997; Edelstyn et al., 2002). However, given that the majority of evidence supports the MD being involved in memory, and that its role is not just confined to familiarity judgments, further models of MD functioning in memory processes are required.

Other researchers have proposed that the MD has a deferential role in memory processing caused by disruptions in executive functioning which is processed by the PFC. It has been suggested that the memory impairments resulting from lesions to the MD are secondary to the primary disruptions in executive functioning, e.g., deficits in attention or withholding responses/inhibition and perseverative responding in both humans and animals (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985; Hunt and Aggleton, 1998b; Floresco et al., 1999; Van der Werf et al., 2000; Schmahmann, 2003).

Van der Werf et al. (2003b) in a review of clinical evidence suggest that the AT and MD each has a functional role in declarative memory processes. The authors propose that the different nuclei of the thalamus play different roles at varying levels of declarative memory functioning, namely the AT and MD are involved in processing the contents of the stimuli for storage and recall. The AT influences the selection of material to be stored and remembered, whereas the MD is involved in the coordination and selection of strategies used to retrieve material. The intralaminar and midline nuclei maintain a necessary state of arousal amongst the cortical regions involved in the ongoing memory processes. These groupings of nuclei then work in parallel to mediate and allow memory functioning.

In contrast to these proposals, Gaffan, Mitchell and colleagues have proposed that the MD, in particular MDmc has an important integrative role in conjunction with the PFC in episodic-like declarative memory, due to the prominent interconnections among these structures (Gaffan and Parker, 2000). The MDmc has a specific role in supporting new learning of information, contributing to the successful acquisition rather than the retention of previously acquired information (Mitchell et al., 2007a, 2008; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). As highlighted above, the MD plays a key role in helping integrate object/reward/response information for successful new learning and successful additional (future) responding. Furthermore, Mitchell and colleagues have suggested that the role of MD in learning and memory is not simply a consequence of causing generalized disruption to PFC functioning (Mitchell et al., 2007a; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008).

Aggleton et al. (2011) have revised their model of MD involvement in recognition memory. Their latest model, the multi-effect multi-nuclei model, asserts that the MD can contribute to both familiarity and recollective processes either directly via an interaction with the PFC or indirectly as a result of cortical diaschisis (Aggleton et al., 2011). This model is supported by recent findings regarding associative recognition (Cross et al., 2012), along with recent clinical results (Pergola et al., 2012) that point to contributions from the parvocellular MD for recollective aspects of recognition.

Re-Evaluating Mediodorsal Thalamus in Memory and Where to from Here

As indicated from the above survey of the contribution of the MD to specific forms of memory and decision-making, some conclusions have been drawn but much debate remains. Nevertheless, the evidence thus far provides some understanding and certainly helps with future directions. Thus, the animal evidence (and also the clinical evidence although not reviewed here) simply doesn't support the notion that there is a single structure within the medial diencephalon that is responsible for the extent of anterograde and retrograde memory deficits associated with diencephalic (or thalamic) amnesia. Furthermore, given the extent of variability in other cognitive deficits observed after damage to the MD it is not possible that one specific structure or subdivision of the MD is the critical locus of these deficits. Instead, the evidence suggests that the subdivisions of the MD, and subdivisions of other medial thalamic structures, are each functioning within independent but integrated neural circuits, all of which are important for specific aspects of cognitive processing, and together they form a group of critical networks in the brain that are important for learning and memory as well as many other forms of cognition.

The current evidence points to the role of higher order thalamic structures, in our case the MD, in mediating the complex functioning within the PFC, via the transthalamic route (Sherman and Guillery, 2002). Neuroanatomical tracing studies have positioned the various subdivisions of the MD within separate but integrated neural circuits based on their respective interconnections. Moreover, as reviewed here, animal models of complete bilateral lesions to the MD as well as more selective lesions to individual subgroupings of the MD (i.e., medial MD, central MD and lateral MD) have demonstrated deficits in various tasks that assess new learning, recognition memory associated with reward, reward devaluation and recency memory processing, but not retention of previously acquired information. Manifestations of such deficits are often similar, but often can also be dissimilar to deficits seen after damage within the PFC (Fuster, 2008; Chudasama, 2011).

Thus, it may be proposed that the transthalamic connections linking the MD to the cognitive PFC are more important for supporting the learning of new information than for retention of previously acquired information (Mitchell et al., 2007a; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008), perhaps by way of regulating cortical synchrony between regions of the PFC and MTL that support acquisition of new information. Others (Saalmann et al., 2012) have demonstrated how the pulvinar (another higher order thalamic relay structure) regulates cortico-cortical communication based on attention demands. This group combined simultaneous neural recordings in the pulvinar, V4 and area TEO (in the medial temporal lobes) while monkeys performed a visuospatial attention task. Precise interconnected target regions were identified via diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). The findings showed that the pulvinar regulates cortical synchrony between these connected structures according to the attentional allocation of the task (Saalmann et al., 2012).

In contrast to deficits in new learning, the evidence suggests that cortical structures are more important for the retention of information learnt prior to brain injury (retrograde amnesia). Impairments in retention are reported after restricted damage to selective cortical structures highlighting how some of these cortical regions are more important for memory of previously acquired information (Dean and Weiskrantz, 1974; Thornton et al., 1997; Mitchell et al., 2008). This evidence supports recent proposals that learning and retention are performed by different networks of the brain Thus, such memory processing may not require the regulation of cortical synchrony provided by the transthalamic pathways via the MDmc (at the least). Instead the direct cortico-cortical connections coursing within the PFC and across the medial temporal lobes are sufficient to support retention memory.

Widespread global amnesia associated with anterograde and retrograde memory deficits may be caused by widespread damage to subcortical structures. For example, the combined bilateral lesion damage to MDmc and fornix results in both retrograde and anterograde amnesia of OIP discriminations (Mitchell et al., 2008). This combined damage would have very likely resulted in extensive damage to interconnected regions of the medial diencephalon, medial temporal lobes, cingulate cortex and the PFC. In other primate animal models, similar types of global amnesia are also reported after combined lesions causing disconnection to the temporal stem, amygdala, and fornix (Gaffan et al., 2001; Easton et al., 2002; Gaffan, 2005). These lesions combining gray matter and white matter tracts disrupt widespread cortical—subcortical interconnections from basal forebrain, medial thalamus, and the midbrain, as well as cortico-cortical communication linking temporal and prefrontal cortices. Similar types of global amnesia are reported following widespread damage in the brain [e.g., in Korsakoff's syndrome patients (Kopelman et al., 1999; Harding et al., 2000)].

Future Directions

Further and combined behavioral, cognitive, and electrophysiology studies are required to gain greater understanding of the impact of disconnection lesions to the PFC, MD, and other interconnected structures. This research may also have clinical application in understanding the roles of the different subdivisions of the MD in many neuropsychological disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive disorder, and major depression). For example, recent studies across different species (Leal-Campanario et al., 2007, 2013; Cross et al., 2012; Parnaudeau et al., 2013) have highlighted the importance of MD-PFC communication within these interconnected neural circuits for successful cognition. Furthermore, many other studies have shown how different types of damage to brain structures interconnected to the MD can produce surprising results across species. Schoenbaum and colleagues (Stalnaker et al., 2007) have shown in rodents how the orbital frontal cortex (OFC) and amygdala interact in reversal learning tasks, with amygdala lesions abolishing the OFC dependent reversal impairments. Interestingly though, in macaques, amygdala lesions do not impair reversal learning (Izquierdo and Murray, 2007), nor do excitotoxic lesions to the OFC, however, transection of the white matter tract fibers leading into the OFC do disrupt reversals and inhibitory control (Rudebeck et al., 2013). It remains an empirical question about the extent of reversal learning deficits linked to the MD and how the MD interacts within this neural network.

There needs to be more research on the understanding of the functional consequences of the communication links between the MD and PFC related to this higher order information transfer (Guillery and Sherman, 2002). For example, how does the MD influence the neural circuitry involved for new learning yet appear to have little impact on retention. The importance of understanding the metabotropic glutamate communication between the MD and the PFC may be particularly relevant for answering this, given that glutamate invokes synaptic plasticity and potentially learning and memory due to the prolonged response of the metabotropic glutamate receptor activation (Sherman, 2013).

Finally, advances in neuroimaging are also illustrating the interconnections of the subcortical brain structures in vivo. For example, the fiber pathways from ventral PFC to MD have recently been documented using magnetic resonance scanning (Lehman et al., 2011). Recent DTI studies have started revealing structural connectivity of MD to PFC and limbic cortical areas and the subcortical caudate nucleus suggestive of the existence of basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuits in humans in vivo (Draganski et al., 2008; Metzger et al., 2010; Eckert et al., 2012). These advances in neuroimaging and future research that combines different behavioral and cognitive neuroscience techniques in humans and in animal models will further advance our understanding of the key roles that the subdivisions of the MD contribute to cognition.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Anna S. Mitchell is a recipient of a UK MRC Career Development Award—G0800329.

Footnotes

1. ^In the direct pathway, the neurotransmitter GABA is activated, which inhibits pallidal and nigral neurons and consequently disinhibits the thalamus and midbrain targets. In the traditional model, it is proposed that this pathway facilitates thalamocortical activity and behavioral and motor outputs. The cells express mainly dopamine D1 receptors in the direct pathway. In the indirect pathway, the neurotransmitters GABA and glutamate are activated. The GABA inhibits the pallidal neurons, which leads to less inhibition at the subthalamic level where glutamate has stronger activity levels as a result. The glutamate then influences GABA in the output neurons of the internal segment of globus pallidus and pars reticulata of the SN, which results in stronger inhibition of the thalamic and midbrain targets. It is proposed this indirect pathway then exerts an inhibitory influence on the thalamus and midbrain, equating to suppression of behavioral and motor outputs. The cells express mainly dopamine D2 receptors in the indirect pathway. These two hypotheses on the direct and indirect pathway modulation are particularly relevant to theories of motor output problems associated with Parkinson's and Huntingdon's diseases. Tekin and Cummings (2002).

References

Aggleton, J. P., and Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 425–444. discussion: 444–489. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., Desimone, R., and Mishkin, M. (1986). The origin, course, and termination of the hippocampothalamic projections in the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 243, 409–421. doi: 10.1002/cne.902430310

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., Dumont, J. R., and Warburton, E. C. (2011). Unraveling the contributions of the diencephalon to recognition memory: a review. Learn. Mem. 18, 384–400. doi: 10.1101/lm.1884611

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., McMackin, D., Carpenter, K., Hornak, J., Kapur, N., Halpin, S., et al. (2000). Differential cognitive effects of colloid cysts in the third ventricle that spare or compromise the fornix. Brain 123(Pt 4), 800–815.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Mishkin, M. (1983a). Memory impairments following restricted medial thalamic lesions in monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 52, 199–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Mishkin, M. (1983b). Visual recognition impairment following medial thalamic lesions in monkeys. Neuropsychologia 21, 189–197.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Mishkin, M. (1984). Projections of the amygdala to the thalamus in the cynomolgus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 222, 56–68. doi: 10.1002/cne.902220106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Pearce, J. M. (2001). Neural systems underlying episodic memory: insights from animal research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 1467–1482. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0946

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Shaw, C. (1996). Amnesia and recognition memory: a re-analysis of psychometric data. Neuropsychologia 34, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/0028-393200150-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alexander, G. E., and Fuster, J. M. (1973). Effects of cooling prefrontal cortex on cell firing in the nucleus medialis dorsalis. Brain Res. 61, 93–105. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90518-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., and Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alexinsky, T. (2001). Differential effect of thalamic and cortical lesions on memory systems in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 122, 175–191. doi: 10.1016/S0166-432800182-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bachevalier, J., Meunier, M., Lu, M. X., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1997). Thalamic and temporal cortex input to medial prefrontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 430–444. doi: 10.1007/PL00005713

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbas, H., Henion, T. H., and Dermon, C. R. (1991). Diverse thalamic projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 313, 65–94. doi: 10.1002/cne.903130106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxter, M. G., Browning, P. G., and Mitchell, A. S. (2008). Perseverative interference with object-in-place scene learning in rhesus monkeys with bilateral ablation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Learn. Mem. 15, 126–132. doi: 10.1101/lm.804508

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxter, M. G., Gaffan, D., Kyriazis, D. A., and Mitchell, A. S. (2009). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex is required for performance of a strategy implementation task but not reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 2049–2059. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06740.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxter, M. G., Parker, A., Lindner, C. C. C., Izquierdo, A. D., and Murray, E. A. (2000). Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, 4311–4319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bentivoglio, M., Kultas-Ilinsky, K., and Ilinsky, I. (1993). “Limbic thalamus: structure, intrinsic organization, and connections,” in Neurobiology of Cingulate Cortex and Limbic Thalamus: a Comprehensive Handbook, eds B. A. Vogt and M. Gabriel (Boston, MA: Birkhauser), 71–122.

Beracochea, D. J., Jaffard, R., and Jarrard, L. E. (1989). Effects of anterior or dorsomedial thalamic ibotenic lesions on learning and memory in rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 51, 364–376. doi: 10.1016/S0163-104791000-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berendse, H. W., and Groenewegen, H. J. (1990). Organization of the thalamostriatal projections in the rat, with special emphasis on the ventral striatum. J. Comp. Neurol. 299, 187–228. doi: 10.1002/cne.902990206

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Block, A. E., Dhanji, H., Thompson-Tardif, S. F., and Floresco, S. B. (2007). Thalamic-prefrontal cortical-ventral striatal circuitry mediates dissociable components of strategy set shifting. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1625–1636. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl073

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, M. W., and Xiang, J. Z. (1998). Recognition memory: neuronal substrates of the judgement of prior occurrence. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 149–189. doi: 10.1016/S0301-008200002-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Browning, P. G., Easton, A., and Gaffan, D. (2007). Frontal-temporal disconnection abolishes object discrimination learning set in macaque monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 17, 859–864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burk, J. A., and Mair, R. G. (1998). Thalamic amnesia reconsidered: excitotoxic lesions of the intralaminar nuclei, but not the mediodorsal nucleus, disrupt place delayed matching-to-sample performance in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Neurosci. 112, 54–67. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.54

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burwell, R. D., Witter, M. P., and Amaral, D. G. (1995). Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: a review of the neuroanatomical literature and comparison with findings from the monkey brain. Hippocampus 5, 390–408. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450050503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carlesimo, G. A., Lombardi, M. G., and Caltagirone, C. (2011). Vascular thalamic amnesia: a reappraisal. Neuropsychologia 49, 777–789. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chauveau, F., Celerier, A., Ognard, R., Pierard, C., and Beracochea, D. (2005). Effects of ibotenic acid lesions of the mediodorsal thalamus on memory: relationship with emotional processes in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 215–223. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chauveau, F., Pierard, C., Corio, M., Celerier, A., Christophe, T., Vouimba, R. M., et al. (2009). Mediodorsal thalamic lesions block the stress-induced inversion of serial memory retrieval pattern in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 203, 270–278. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chudasama, Y. (2011). Animal models of prefrontal-executive function. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 327–343. doi: 10.1037/a0023766

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chudasama, Y., Bussey, T. J., and Muir, J. L. (2001). Effects of selective thalamic and prelimbic cortex lesions on two types of visual discrimination and reversal learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 1009–1020. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01607.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chudasama, Y., and Muir, J. L. (2001). Visual attention in the rat: a role for the prelimbic cortex and thalamic nuclei. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 417–428.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cipolotti, L., Husain, M., Crinion, J., Bird, C. M., Khan, S. S., Losseff, N., et al. (2008). The role of the thalamus in amnesia: a tractography, high-resolution MRI and neuropsychological study. Neuropsychologia 46, 2745–2758. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbit, L. H., Muir, J. L., and Balleine, B. W. (2003). Lesions of mediodorsal thalamus and anterior thalamic nuclei produce dissociable effects on instrumental conditioning in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 1286–1294. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02833.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cross, L., Brown, M. W., Aggleton, J. P., and Warburton, E. C. (2012). The medial dorsal thalamic nucleus and the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat function together to support associative recognition and recency but not item recognition. Learn. Mem. 20, 41–50. doi: 10.1101/lm.028266.112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dean, P., and Weiskrantz, L. (1974). Loss of preoperative habits in Rhesus monkeys with inferotemporal lesions: recognition failure or relearning deficit. Neuropsychologia 12, 299–311.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dolleman-van der Weel, M. J., Morris, R. G., and Witter, M. P. (2009). Neurotoxic lesions of the thalamic reuniens or mediodorsal nucleus in rats affect non-mnemonic aspects of watermaze learning. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 329–342. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0200-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Draganski, B., Kherif, F., Kloppel, S., Cook, P. A., Alexander, D. C., Parker, G. J., et al. (2008). Evidence for segregated and integrative connectivity patterns in the human basal ganglia. J. Neurosci. 28, 7143–7152. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1486-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dudchenko, P. A. (2001). How do animals actually solve the T maze. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 850–860.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Easton, A., Ridley, R. M., Baker, H. F., and Gaffan, D. (2002). Unilateral lesions of the cholinergic basal forebrain and fornix in one hemisphere and inferior temporal cortex in the opposite hemisphere produce severe learning impairments in rhesus monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 12, 729–736. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.7.729

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eckert, U., Metzger, C. D., Buchmann, J. E., Kaufmann, J., Osoba, A., Li, M., et al. (2012). Preferential networks of the mediodorsal nucleus and centromedian-parafascicular complex of the thalamus–a DTI tractography study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2627–2637. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21389

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edelstyn, N. M., Ellis, S. J., Jenkinson, P., and Sawyer, A. (2002). Contribution of the left dorsomedial thalamus to recognition memory: a neuropsychological case study. Neurocase 8, 442–452. doi: 10.1076/neur.8.5.442.16180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eleore, L., Lopez-Ramos, J. C., Guerra-Narbona, R., and Delgado-Garcia, J. M. (2011). Role of reuniens nucleus projections to the medial prefrontal cortex and to the hippocampal pyramidal CA1 area in associative learning. PLoS ONE 6:e23538. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023538

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, S. L., and Lewis, D. A. (2004). Cortical connections of the lateral mediodorsal thalamus in cynomolgus monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 473, 107–127. doi: 10.1002/cne.20084

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, S. L., Melchitzky, D. S., and Lewis, D. A. (2004). Subcortical afferents to the lateral mediodorsal thalamus in cynomolgus monkeys. Neuroscience 129, 675–690. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fahy, F. L., Riches, I. P., and Brown, M. W. (1993). Neuronal signals of importance to the performance of visual recognition memory tasks: evidence from recordings of single neurones in the medial thalamus of primates. Prog. Brain Res. 95, 401–416. doi: 10.1016/S0079-612360384-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floresco, S. B., Braaksma, D. N., and Phillips, A. G. (1999). Thalamic-cortical-striatal circuitry subserves working memory during delayed responding on a radial arm maze. J. Neurosci. 19, 11061–11071.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Funahashi, S., Takeda, K., and Watanabe, Y. (2004). Neural mechanisms of spatial working memory: contributions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 409–420. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.4.409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuster, J. M. (2008). The Prefrontal Cortex, 4th Edn. London: Academic Press.

Gaffan, D. (2005). Neuroscience. Widespread cortical networks underlie memory and attention. Science 309, 2172–2173. doi: 10.1126/science.1119445

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaffan, D., Easton, A., and Parker, A. (2002). Interaction of inferior temporal cortex with frontal cortex and basal forebrain: double dissociation in strategy implementation and associative learning. J. Neurosci. 22, 7288–7296.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gaffan, D., and Murray, E. A. (1990). Amygdalar interaction with the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in stimulus reward associative learning in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 10, 3479–3493.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gaffan, D., Murray, E. A., and Fabrethorpe, M. (1993). Interaction of the amygdala with the frontal-lobe in reward memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 5, 968–975. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1993.tb00948.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaffan, D., and Parker, A. (2000). Mediodorsal thalamic function in scene memory in rhesus monkeys. Brain 123, 816–827. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.4.816

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaffan, D., Parker, A., and Easton, A. (2001). Dense amnesia in the monkey after transection of fornix, amygdala and anterior temporal stem. Neuropsychologia 39, 51–70. doi: 10.1016/S0028-393200097-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaffan, D., and Watkins, S. (1991). Mediodorsal thalamic lesions impair long-term visual associative memory in macaques. Eur. J. Neurosci. 3, 615–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00847.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibb, S. J., Wolff, M., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2006). Odour-place paired-associate learning and limbic thalamus: comparison of anterior, lateral and medial thalamic lesions. Behav. Brain Res. 172, 155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.05.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giguere, M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Mediodorsal nucleus: areal, laminar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 277, 195–213. doi: 10.1002/cne.902770204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gimenez-Amaya, J. M., McFarland, N. R., de las Heras, S., and Haber, S. N. (1995). Organization of thalamic projections to the ventral striatum in the primate. J. Comp. Neurol. 354, 127–149. doi: 10.1002/cne.903540109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goldman-Rakic, P. S., and Porrino, L. J. (1985). The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe. J. Comp. Neurol. 242, 535–560. doi: 10.1002/cne.902420406

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J. (1988). Organization of the afferent connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal-prefrontal topography. Neuroscience 24, 379–431. doi: 10.1016/0306-452290339-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J., Berendse, H. W., and Haber, S. N. (1993). Organization of the output of the ventral striatopallidal system in the rat: ventral pallidal efferents. Neuroscience 57, 113–142. doi: 10.1016/0306-452290115-V

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J., Berendse, H. W., Wolters, J. G., and Lohman, A. H. (1990). The anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopallidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: evidence for a parallel organization. Prog. Brain Res. 85, 95–116. discussion: 116–118.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J., Galis-de Graaf, Y., and Smeets, W. J. (1999a). Integration and segregation of limbic cortico-striatal loops at the thalamic level: an experimental tracing study in rats. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 16, 167–185.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J., Wright, C. I., Beijer, A. V., and Voorn, P. (1999b). Convergence and segregation of ventral striatal inputs and outputs. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 877, 49–63.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Groenewegen, H. J., Wright, C. I., and Uylings, H. B. (1997). The anatomical relationships of the prefrontal cortex with limbic structures and the basal ganglia. J. Psychopharmacol. 11, 99–106. doi: 10.1177/026988119701100202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guillery, R. W. (1995). Anatomical evidence concerning the role of the thalamus in corticocortical communication: a brief review. J. Anat. 187(Pt 3), 583–592.

Guillery, R. W., and Sherman, S. M. (2002). Thalamic relay functions and their role in corticocortical communication: generalizations from the visual system. Neuron 33, 163–175. doi: 10.1016/S0896-627300582-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haber, S., and McFarland, N. R. (2001). The place of the thalamus in frontal cortical-basal ganglia circuits. Neuroscientist 7, 315–324. doi: 10.1177/107385840100700408

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haber, S. N., and Calzavara, R. (2009). The cortico-basal ganglia integrative network: the role of the thalamus. Brain Res. Bull. 78, 69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.09.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haber, S. N., Groenewegen, H. J., Grove, E. A., and Nauta, W. J. (1985). Efferent connections of the ventral pallidum: evidence of a dual striato pallidofugal pathway. J. Comp. Neurol. 235, 322–335. doi: 10.1002/cne.902350304

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haber, S. N., and Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hallanger, A. E., Levey, A. I., Lee, H. J., Rye, D. B., and Wainer, B. H. (1987). The origins of cholinergic and other subcortical afferents to the thalamus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 262, 105–124. doi: 10.1002/cne.902620109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hannesson, D. K., Vacca, G., Howland, J. G., and Phillips, A. G. (2004). Medial prefrontal cortex is involved in spatial temporal order memory but not spatial recognition memory in tests relying on spontaneous exploration in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 153, 273–285. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.12.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harding, A., Halliday, G., Caine, D., and Kril, J. (2000). Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 123, 141–154. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.141

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrison, L. M., and Mair, R. G. (1996). A comparison of the effects of frontal cortical and thalamic lesions on measures of spatial learning and memory in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 75, 195–206. doi: 10.1016/0166-432800173-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunt, P. R., and Aggleton, J. P. (1991). Medial dorsal thalamic lesions and working memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 55, 227–246. doi: 10.1016/0163-104780141-Z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunt, P. R., and Aggleton, J. P. (1998a). An examination of the spatial working memory deficit following neurotoxic medial dorsal thalamic lesions in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 97, 129–141.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hunt, P. R., and Aggleton, J. P. (1998b). Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsomedial thalamus impair the acquisition but not the performance of delayed matching to place by rats: a deficit in shifting response rules. J. Neurosci. 18, 10045–10052.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hunt, P. R., Neave, N., Shaw, C., and Aggleton, J. P. (1994). The effects of lesions to the fornix and dorsomedial thalamus on concurrent discrimination learning by rats. Behav. Brain Res. 62, 195–205. doi: 10.1016/0166-432890028-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Isseroff, A., Rosvold, H. E., Galkin, T. W., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1982). Spatial memory impairments following damage to the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in rhesus monkeys. Brain Res. 232, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0006-899390613-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izquierdo, A., and Murray, E. A. (2007). Selective bilateral amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys fail to disrupt object reversal learning. J. Neurosci. 27, 1054–1062. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3616-06.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izquierdo, A., and Murray, E. A. (2010). Functional interaction of medial mediodorsal thalamic nucleus but not nucleus accumbens with amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex is essential for adaptive response selection after reinforcer devaluation. J. Neurosci. 30, 661–669. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3795-09.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, E. G. (1985). The Thalamus. New York, NY: Plenum.

Jones, E. G. (ed.). (1998). The Thalamus of Primates. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Kopelman, M. D. (1995). The Korsakoff syndrome. Br. J. Psychiatry 166, 154–173. doi: 10.1192/bjp.166.2.154

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kopelman, M. D. (2002). Disorders of memory. Brain 125, 2152–2190. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf229

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kopelman, M. D., Stanhope, N., and Kingsley, D. (1997). Temporal and spatial context memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions. Neuropsychologia 35, 1533–1545. doi: 10.1016/S0028-393200076-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kopelman, M. D., Stanhope, N., and Kingsley, D. (1999). Retrograde amnesia in patients with diencephalic, temporal lobe or frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia 37, 939–958. doi: 10.1016/S0028-393200143-2

CrossRef Full Text

Kornecook, T. J., Anzarut, A., and Pinel, J. P. (1999). Rhinal cortex, but not medial thalamic, lesions cause retrograde amnesia for objects in rats. Neuroreport 10, 2853–2858. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199909090-00028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krazem, A., Beracochea, D., and Jaffard, R. (1995). Effects of mammillary bodies and mediodorsal thalamic lesions on the acquisition and retention of a learning set in mice: paradoxical effect of the intersession interval. Behav. Brain Res. 67, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/0166-432800103-M

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krettek, J. E., and Price, J. L. (1977). The cortical projections of the mediodorsal nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 171, 157–191. doi: 10.1002/cne.901710204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kril, J. J., and Halliday, G. M. (1999). Brain shrinkage in alcoholics: a decade on and what have we learned. Prog. Neurobiol. 58, 381–387.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Leal-Campanario, R., Delgado-Garcia, J. M., and Gruart, A. (2013). The rostral medial prefrontal cortex regulates the expression of conditioned eyelid responses in behaving rabbits. J. Neurosci. 33, 4378–4386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5560-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leal-Campanario, R., Fairen, A., Delgado-Garcia, J. M., and Gruart, A. (2007). Electrical stimulation of the rostral medial prefrontal cortex in rabbits inhibits the expression of conditioned eyelid responses but not their acquisition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 11459–11464. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704548104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lehman, J. F., Greenberg, B. D., McIntyre, C. C., Rasmussen, S. A., and Haber, S. N. (2011). Rules ventral prefrontal cortical axons use to reach their targets: implications for diffusion tensor imaging tractography and deep brain stimulation for psychiatric illness. J. Neurosci. 31, 10392–10402. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0595-11.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lopez, J., Wolff, M., Lecourtier, L., Cosquer, B., Bontempi, B., Dalrymple-Alford, J., et al. (2009). The intralaminar thalamic nuclei contribute to remote spatial memory. J. Neurosci. 29, 3302–3306. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5576-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malkova, L., Gaffan, D., and Murray, E. A. (1997). Excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala fail to produce impairment in visual learning for auditory secondary reinforcement but interfere with reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 17, 6011–6020.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

McFarland, N. R., and Haber, S. N. (2002). Thalamic relay nuclei of the basal ganglia form both reciprocal and nonreciprocal cortical connections, linking multiple frontal cortical areas. J. Neurosci. 22, 8117–8132.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Metzger, C. D., Eckert, U., Steiner, J., Sartorius, A., Buchmann, J. E., Stadler, J., et al. (2010). High field FMRI reveals thalamocortical integration of segregated cognitive and emotional processing in mediodorsal and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Front. Neuroanat. 4:138. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2010.00138

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

M'Harzi, M., Jarrard, L. E., Willig, F., Palacios, A., and Delacour, J. (1991). Selective fimbria and thalamic lesions differentially impair forms of working memory in rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 56, 221–239. doi: 10.1016/0163-104790364-V

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., Baxter, M. G., and Gaffan, D. (2007a). Dissociable performance on scene learning and strategy implementation after lesions to magnocellular mediodorsal thalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 27, 11888–11895.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., Browning, P. G., and Baxter, M. G. (2007b). Neurotoxic lesions of the medial mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus disrupt reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 27, 11289–11295.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., Browning, P. G., Wilson, C. R., Baxter, M. G., and Gaffan, D. (2008). Dissociable roles for cortical and subcortical structures in memory retrieval and acquisition. J. Neurosci. 28, 8387–8396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1924-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2005). Dissociable memory effects after medial thalamus lesions in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 973–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04199.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2006). Lateral and anterior thalamic lesions impair independent memory systems. Learn. Mem. 13, 388–396. doi: 10.1101/lm.122206

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., and Gaffan, D. (2008). The magnocellular mediodorsal thalamus is necessary for memory acquisition, but not retrieval. J. Neurosci. 28, 258–263. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4922-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, J. B., and Laiacona, J. (1998). The medial frontal cortex and temporal memory: tests using spontaneous exploratory behaviour in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 97, 107–113. doi: 10.1016/S0166-432800032-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moreau, P. H., Tsenkina, Y., Lecourtier, L., Lopez, J., Cosquer, B., Wolff, M., et al. (2013). Lesions of the anterior thalamic nuclei and intralaminar thalamic nuclei: place and visual discrimination learning in the water maze. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 657–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0419-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morris, R. G. (2001). Episodic-like memory in animals: psychological criteria, neural mechanisms and the value of episodic-like tasks to investigate animal models of neurodegenerative disease. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 1453–1465. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0945

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mumby, D. G., Pinel, J. P., and Dastur, F. N. (1993). Mediodorsal thalamic lesions impair object recognition in rats. Psychobiology 21, 27–36.

Murray, E. A., and Gaffan, D. (2006). Prospective memory in the formation of learning sets by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 32, 87–90. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.87

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neave, N., Sahgal, A., and Aggleton, J. P. (1993). Lack of effect of dorsomedial thalamic lesions on automated tests of spatial memory in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 55, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/0166-432890005-B

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Negyessy, L., Hamori, J., and Bentivoglio, M. (1998). Contralateral cortical projection to the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus: origin and synaptic organization in the rat. Neuroscience 84, 741–753. doi: 10.1016/S0306-452200559-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olszewski, J. (1952). The Thalamus of the Macaca mulatta. An Atlas for Use with the stereotaxic Instrument. Basel: Karger.

Ostlund, S. B., and Balleine, B. W. (2008). Differential involvement of the basolateral amygdala and mediodorsal thalamus in instrumental action selection. J. Neurosci. 28, 4398–4405. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5472-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parker, A., and Gaffan, D. (1998). Interaction of frontal and perirhinal cortices in visual object recognition memory in monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 3044–3057. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00306.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parker, A., Eacott, M. J., and Gaffan, D. (1997). The recognition memory deficit caused by mediodorsal thalamic lesion in non-human primates: a comparison with rhinal cortex lesion. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, 2423–2431. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01659.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parnaudeau, S., O'Neill, P. K., Bolkan, S. S., Ward, R. D., Abbas, A. I., Roth, B. L., et al. (2013). Inhibition of mediodorsal thalamus disrupts thalamofrontal connectivity and cognition. Neuron 77, 1151–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (1998). The Rat Brain: in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Sydney: Academic Press.

Peinado-Manzano, M. A., and Pozo-Garcia, R. (1991). The role of different nuclei of the thalamus in processing episodic information. Behav. Brain Res. 45, 17–27. doi: 10.1016/S0166-432880176-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peinado-Manzano, M. A., and Pozo-Garcia, R. (1996). Retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsomedial thalamic damage. Behav. Brain Res. 80, 177–184. doi: 10.1016/0166-432800033-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pergola, G., Gunturkun, O., Koch, B., Schwarz, M., Daum, I., and Suchan, B. (2012). Recall deficits in stroke patients with thalamic lesions covary with damage to the parvocellular mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychologia 50, 2477–2491. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pickens, C. L. (2008). A limited role for mediodorsal thalamus in devaluation tasks. Behav. Neurosci. 122, 659–676. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.3.659

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preuss, T. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Crossed corticothalamic and thalamocortical connections of macaque prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 257, 269–281. doi: 10.1002/cne.902570211

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Price, J. L. (ed.). (1995). Thalamus, 2nd Edn. Sydney, NSW: Academic Press, Inc.

Ray, J. P., and Price, J. L. (1993). The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in Macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 337, 1–31. doi: 10.1002/cne.903370102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ridley, R. M., Baker, H. F., Cummings, R. M., Green, M. E., and Leow-Dyke, A. (2005). Mild topographical memory impairment following crossed unilateral lesions of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus and the inferotemporal cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 518–525. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.518

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ridley, R. M., Maclean, C. J., Young, F. M., and Baker, H. F. (2002). Learning impairments in monkeys with combined but not separate excitotoxic lesions of the anterior and mediodorsal thalamic nuclei. Brain Res. 950, 39–51. doi: 10.1016/S0006-899302984-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rudebeck, P. H., Saunders, R. C., Prescott, A. T., Chau, L. S., and Murray, E. A. (2013). Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral flexibility, emotion regulation and value updating. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1140–1145. doi: 10.1038/nn.3440

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Russchen, F. T., Amaral, D. G., and Price, J. L. (1987). The afferent input to the magnocellular division of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the monkey, Macaca fascicularis. J. Comp. Neurol. 256, 175–210. doi: 10.1002/cne.902560202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., Wang, L., Li, X., and Kastner, S. (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanchez-Gonzalez, M. A., Garcia-Cabezas, M. A., Rico, B., and Cavada, C. (2005). The primate thalamus is a key target for brain dopamine. J. Neurosci. 25, 6076–6083. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0968-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saunders, R. C., Mishkin, M., and Aggleton, J. P. (2005). Projections from the entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, presubiculum, and parasubiculum to the medial thalamus in macaque monkeys: identifying different pathways using disconnection techniques. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2361-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D. (2003). Vascular syndromes of the thalamus. Stroke 34, 2264–2278. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000087786.38997.9E

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherman, S. M. (2005). Thalamic relays and cortical functioning. Prog. Brain Res. 149, 107–126. doi: 10.1016/S0079-612349009-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherman, S. M. (2007). The thalamus is more than just a relay. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 417–422. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherman, S. M. (2013). The function of metabotropic glutamate receptors in thalamus and cortex. Neuroscientist doi: 10.1177/1073858413478490. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (2002). The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos Trans R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 357, 1685–1708. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1161

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (2005). Exploring the Thalamus and its Role in Cortical Function. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (2011). Distinct functions for direct and transthalamic corticocortical connections. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1068–1077. doi: 10.1152/jn.00429.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shuren, J. E., Jacobs, D. H., and Heilman, K. M. (1997). Diencephalic temporal order amnesia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 62, 163–168. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.62.2.163

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sommer, M. A., and Wurtz, R. H. (2006). Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature 444, 374–377. doi: 10.1038/nature05279

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stalnaker, T. A., Franz, T. M., Singh, T., and Schoenbaum, G. (2007). Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments. Neuron 54, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stokes, K. A., and Best, P. J. (1988). Mediodorsal thalamic lesions impair radial maze performance in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 102, 294–300. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.102.2.294

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stokes, K. A., and Best, P. J. (1990a). Mediodorsal thalamic lesions impair “reference” and “working” memory in rats. Physiol. Behav. 47, 471–476.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Stokes, K. A., and Best, P. J. (1990b). Response biases do not underlie the radial maze deficit in rats with mediodorsal thalamus lesions. Behav. Neural Biol. 53, 334–345.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tanibuchi, I., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2005). Comparison of oculomotor neuronal activity in paralaminar and mediodorsal thalamus in the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 614–619. doi: 10.1152/jn.00969.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tekin, S., and Cummings, J. L. (2002). Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. J. Psychosom. Res. 53, 647–654. doi: 10.1016/S0022-399900428-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thornton, J. A., Rothblat, L. A., and Murray, E. A. (1997). Rhinal cortex removal produces amnesia for preoperatively learned discrimination problems but fails to disrupt postoperative acquisition and retention in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 17, 8536–8549.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Uylings, H. B., Groenewegen, H. J., and Kolb, B. (2003). Do rats have a prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 146, 3–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Van der Werf, Y. D., Jolles, J., Witter, M. P., and Uylings, H. B. (2003a). Contributions of thalamic nuclei to declarative memory functioning. Cortex 39, 1047–1062.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Van der Werf, Y. D., Scheltens, P., Lindeboom, J., Witter, M. P., Uylings, H. B., and Jolles, J. (2003b). Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following infarction in the thalamus; a study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, 1330–1344.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Van der Werf, Y. D., Witter, M. P., Uylings, H. B., and Jolles, J. (2000). Neuropsychology of infarctions in the thalamus: a review. Neuropsychologia 38, 613–627. doi: 10.1016/S0028-393200104-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vann, S. D. (2013). Dismantling the Papez circuit for memory in rats. Elife 2:e00736. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00736

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Victor, M., Adams, R. D., and Collins, G. H. (1971). The Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. A clinical and pathological study of 245 patients, 82 with post-mortem examinations. Contemp. Neurol. Ser. 7, 1–206.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Watanabe, Y., and Funahashi, S. (2012). Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 134–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winocur, G. (1985). The hippocampus and thalamus: their roles in short- and long-term memory and the effects of interference. Behav. Brain Res. 16, 135–152. doi: 10.1016/0166-432890088-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winocur, G. (1990). Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or dorsomedial thalamic lesions. Behav. Brain Res. 38, 145–154. doi: 10.1016/0166-432890012-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolff, M., Gibb, S. J., Cassel, J. C., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2008). Anterior but not intralaminar thalamic nuclei support allocentric spatial memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 90, 71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.01.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolff, M., Gibb, S. J., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2006). Beyond spatial memory: the anterior thalamus and memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odor cues. J. Neurosci. 26, 2907–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5481-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xiang, J. Z., and Brown, M. W. (2004). Neuronal responses related to long-term recognition memory processes in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 42, 817–829. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.05.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xiao, D., Zikopoulos, B., and Barbas, H. (2009). Laminar and modular organization of prefrontal projections to multiple thalamic nuclei. Neuroscience 161, 1067–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yeterian, E. H., and Pandya, D. N. (1994). Laminar origin of striatal and thalamic projections of the prefrontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 99, 383–398. doi: 10.1007/BF00228975

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Young, H. L., Stevens, A. A., Converse, E., and Mair, R. G. (1996). A comparison of temporal decay in place memory tasks in rats (Rattus norvegicus) with lesions affecting thalamus, frontal cortex, or the hippocampal system. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 1244–1260. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.6.1244

CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, Y., Burk, J. A., Glode, B. M., and Mair, R. G. (1998). Effects of thalamic and olfactory cortical lesions on continuous olfactory delayed nonmatching-to-sample and olfactory discrimination in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Neurosci. 112, 39–53. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.39

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zola-Morgan, S., and Squire, L. R. (1985). Amnesia in monkeys after lesions of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Ann. Neurol. 17, 558–564. doi: 10.1002/ana.410170605

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: prefrontal cortex, memory, executive function, macaque, rodent, animal models, learning

Citation: Mitchell AS and Chakraborty S (2013) What does the mediodorsal thalamus do? Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:37. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00037

Received: 04 May 2013; Accepted: 17 July 2013;
Published online: 09 August 2013.

Edited by:

Yuri B. Saalmann, Princeton University, USA

Reviewed by:

José M. Delgado-García, University Pablo de Olavide, Seville, Spain
Ysbrand Van Der Werf, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Netherlands

Copyright © 2013 Mitchell and Chakraborty. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna S. Mitchell, Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford, OX1 3UD, UK e-mail: anna.mitchell@psy.ox.ac.uk

Download